Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 18 May 2022
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Coping with Pandemic and Infodemic Stress: a Multidisciplinary Perspective View all 32 articles

Design for Pandemic Information: Examining the Effect of Graphs on Anxiety and Social Distancing Intentions in the COVID-19

\nJing LuoJing Luo1Yaqi ZhangYaqi Zhang2Yao Song,
Yao Song3,4*
  • 1Department of Industrial Design, College of Art and Design, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 2School of Design, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong, China
  • 3Department of Advertising, College of Literature and Journalism, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China
  • 4Digital Convergence Laboratory of Chinese Cultural Inheritance and Global Communication, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China

To increase public awareness and disseminate health information, the WHO and health departments worldwide have been visualizing the latest statistics on the spread of COVID-19 to increase awareness and thus reduce its spread. Within various sources, graphs are frequently used to illustrate COVID-19 datasets. Limited research has provided insights into the effect of different graphs on emotional stress and ineffective behavioral strategies from a cross-cultural perspective. The result of current research suggests a graph with a high proportion size of the colored area (e.g., stacked area graph) might increase people's anxiety and social distancing intentions; people in collectivist culture might have a high level of anxiety and social distancing intentions; the effect of different graphs on social distancing intentions is mediated by anxiety experienced. Theoretical contribution and practical implications on health communication were also discussed in this study.

Introduction

In January 2020, the World Health Organization (WHO) announced the outbreak of the new coronavirus disease COVID-19 as a public health emergency of international concern and that the risk of COVID-19 spreading worldwide was high (14). In March 2020, COVID-19 was further listed as a pandemic. Though the latest medical technology has dramatically promoted health conditions (1, 5), it is still necessary to increase public awareness and disseminate health information. Indeed, the WHO and health departments worldwide have been visualizing the latest statistics on the spread of COVID-19 to increase awareness and thus reduce its spread (6).

Individuals are exposed to information on COVID-19 daily through media such as newspapers, television, and the internet from sources such as the WHO Coronavirus Disease Dashboard (6), Worldometer dashboard (7), or Johns Hopkins Corona Virus Resource Center (8). Within various sources, graphs are frequently used to illustrate COVID-19 datasets. The more commonly used graphs are line, bar, and stacked area graphs (9). However, individuals experience mental stress when processing risk information (10). The difference in the presentation of charts or graphics affects individuals' perceptions and behavioral intentions (11). For example, research has suggested a positive relationship between seeking coronavirus updates and anxiety, common mental stress (12). However, limited research has provided insights into the effect of different graphs on emotional reactions in the context of a pandemic. Considering that social distancing might play an indispensable role in controlling and suppressing the spread of COVID-19 (13), this study aims to investigate the effects of the most commonly used types of graphs (line graphs, bar graphs, and stacked area graphs) on individuals' mental health and preventive behaviors from a cross-cultural perspective.

Background

Graphs in Data Visualization

In 1786, William Playfair first introduced graphs to visualize data (14). Since then, an increasing number of researchers have attempted to adopt this graphical representation tool and explore its potential advantages and disadvantages among different graphs (15). As a critical statistical representation tool, graphs have been used as a critical visual communication solution in various fields, such as science, technology, business, education, and mass media (16).

However, graph design guidelines and their effect on viewers have largely been neglected in the literature: individuals have generally relied on their intuition or common sense to make a “good” graph, although this is not always “scientific” (17). Indeed, graphs are more attractive than numbers because they are visually stimulating and can be perceived in a quick, automatic manner, despite some graphs required extra cognitive effort to make estimations (18). For example, the interpretation and calculation of particular graphics may depend on cognitive processing (19). Thus, an ideal graph should be designed to exploit visual heuristics while decreasing cognitive load (20).

Graphs are also widely adopted in printed and electronic materials among various health areas, such as risk assessment, risk signaling, and risk communication (18). However, scant literature has discussed how individuals interpret these public health graphs and how the associated perceptions raised. The understanding of graphs is usually different from what the designer planned. Accordingly, it might be both theoretically and practically significant to investigate the particular visual effects of public health communication on individuals' mental health and related behavior.

Graphs, Anxiety, and Social Distancing

We reviewed the daily COVID-19 updates from official resources, such as WHO, and observed different types of graphs. When visualizing COVID-19 updates, the most common information resources frequently use two types of graphs: thematic mapping and time series graph (6). For COVID-19 updates, a thematic map shows the spatial distribution of confirmed cases or deaths for selected geographic areas, and a time-series graph is used, for example, to visualize trends of total or daily confirmed cases or deaths over a period of time.

Within time-series graphs, there are three common graphs used to visualize COVID-19 updates: line, bar, and stacked area graphs (Figure 1). Figure 1 illustrates trends of the global total of confirmed cases from Feb 1 to Jun 30, 2020: Figure 1A is a line graph that displays cases as a series of data points connected by straight line segments; Figure 1B is a bar graph that presents data by using rectangular bars with heights or lengths proportional to the cases per day; Figure 1C is a stacked area graph that uses the area between the axis and a line to graphically display data.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Graphs of daily confirmed cases from Feb 1 to Jun 30, 2020. (A) refers to a line graph; (B) refers to a bar graph; (C) refers to a stacked area graph.

Visually, the most significant difference among these three graphs is the different portion sizes of foreground colored area on the background-colored area. In Figure 1, the foreground color of the stacked area graph is gray, and its background-color is white; thus, it has the highest portion size of the colored area, followed by the bar and line graphs. When individuals observe the health updates graph, its information could be perceived as threatening: dissemination of pandemic information might not only improve public awareness but also potentially dampen social wellbeing, such as people's anxiety about the crisis (21). The reason might lie in the bias in the embodied cognition where numerical estimation did not follow a linear relationship with spatial estimation (22). To specify, people tend to overestimate the associated number for high-intensive space while underestimating the number for low-intensive space (23). Thus, people would like to have a stronger sense and overestimate for a large area (2426). Considering portion size has been demonstrated to have a significant positive impact on anxiety (27, 28), a reasonable prediction is that a graph with a large portion-size area, compared with a small area, could cause a higher level of anxiety in the context of COVID-19 updates.

In addition, many studies have shown that individuals' perceptions of risk and public awareness are positively correlated with higher intentions for ineffective behavioral strategies (5, 29, 30). Accordingly, anxiety might mediate the effect of different graphs on intentions for social distancing (31).

Cultural Difference in Anxiety and Social Distancing

The research has also suggested that the anxiety of specific populations may be affected by their personality traits, such as cultural elements. According to social identity theory, collectivist self-esteem refers to individuals' self-esteem in relation to the social network to which they belong, rather than respect for themselves (32). Specifically, collectivist self-esteem indicates the extent to which individuals evaluate social groups (33). Compared with individualistic cultures, such as the Caucasian culture in the United States (US), collectivist self-esteem plays a more significant role in collectivist cultures such as China (34). That is, individualistic cultures value the expression and proposition of individual desires, and collectivist cultures pay more attention to maintaining group harmony (34).

Collectivist self-esteem also has a significant impact on the mental health of individuals compared with individualistic cultural backgrounds (35). For example, residents of East Asian countries (36) and immigrants from East Asia to Western countries (37) tend to experience higher levels of social anxiety than individuals from individualistic cultures since collectivism requires people to feel their obligations and responsibilities to group members (38). When exposed to risk information, such as COVID-19 updates, collectivists have stronger emotional reactions and experience higher pressure than individualists (39, 40) because they might have a stronger intention to avoid the risk and return to normal, maintaining group harmony (41). Therefore, an expectation is that in the context of visualizing COVID-19 updates, stacked area graphs might aggravate the anxiety perception by collectivist people (42). In other words, it might be that graph types and cultures could jointly influence anxiety perception and social distancing intentions.

Study Hypothesis

Based on the literature shown above, hypotheses (H1a to H3b) are stated as follows:

H1a: A graph with a high (vs. low) proportion size of the colored area might result in a high (vs. low) level of anxiety.

H1b: Individuals in collectivist (vs. individualistic) cultures tend to experience a high (vs. low) level of anxiety.

H2a: A graph with a high (vs. low) proportion size of the colored area might have high (vs. low) social distancing intentions.

H2b: Individuals in collectivist (vs. individualistic) cultures tend to have high (vs. low) social distancing intentions.

H3a: Perceived anxiety mediates the effect of different graphs on social distancing intentions.

H3b: Perceived anxiety mediates the effect of different cultures on social distancing intentions.

H4: Graph types and cultures jointly influence anxiety and social distancing intentions.

Experiment Design

The experiment was designed to examine the main effect of different graphs and cultures on social distancing intentions and the mediating role of anxiety in this process (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Theoretical model of this study.

Participants and Design

A three (different graphs: line graph, bar graph, and stacked area graph) by two (different cultures: individualism vs. collectivism) between-participants experiment was conducted. Specifically, participants in this experiment were from two sources: the Chinese sample and the US sample. The Chinese sample was recruited from Wenjuanxing (43), and the US sample was recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT) (44). Both Wenjuanxing and AMT are crowdsourcing platforms to recruit individuals and conduct behavioral research because of their adequate reliability and validity (16, 45, 46). We posit that both platforms are good data collection sources, especially considering their widely distributed population, which can avoid sampling bias to a large extent (16, 45, 46).

Stimuli/Material

This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Shenzhen University (SZUDA20190901001). In the selection of an appropriate index to describe the trend of the COVID-19 crisis, total confirmed cases, daily confirmed cases, and deaths are the most common indicators (6). Nevertheless, these three indicators are of different magnitudes: total confirmed cases and deaths are cumulative time series data, and daily confirmed cases are daily time-series data, which vary frequently. On one hand, it might be inappropriate to introduce three indicators in one graph due to their significantly different magnitudes. On the other hand, total confirmed cases or deaths (total cumulative count) might focus more on the overall severity of the pandemic, while daily new cases might work as a more obvious indicator for trends and predictions regarding COVID-19 (47). Thus, we choose daily new cases as an indicator in this study. Because we investigated the role of cultural differences in response to different graphs, daily confirmed cases for the specific country might bias individuals' emotional status (48). Accordingly, we focused on the global COVID-19 daily new cases, rather than a particular country. To sum up, COVID-19 daily new cases (from Feb 1 to Jun 30, 2020) were extracted from the WHO data repository to illustrate the trends for COVID-19. Microsoft Excel, as one of the most data visualization tools, was used to visualize the COVID-19 data into three graphs (see Figure 1).

Measurement

As for measurements, since the graph of daily new cases is efficient to illustrate the latest trends of COVID-19, it might influence participants' current anxiety level and their anticipation of COVID-19. Thus, current anxiety level and anticipated anxiety level were measured separately by using the participants' responses to two items on a nine-point Likert scale (My current level of anxiety about COVID-19 is high; If I were to develop flu-like symptoms tomorrow, I would be anxious) (31). Since current anxiety level and anticipated anxiety level constituted the participants' overall anxiety level, overall anxiety was treated as the main factor and measured as the average of current anxiety and anticipated anxiety. In addition, social distancing intentions were measured with a single item (I will avoid going to crowded places in the next few days) on a nine-point rating scale (31). Cronbach's alphas of overall anxiety (0.73) were checked and achieved adequate reliability (49), suggesting it was appropriate for further analysis.

Sample Size Justification

Power analysis for ANVOA was performed to determine the sample size per group (50). Software G*Power was performed on the effect size of Cohen's d = 0.40 and results showed 30 participants per group in the current setting was adequate to attain 80% power (alpha was set at 0.05) (51). Thus, one hundred and eighty participants were recruited in the experiment (mean age = 33.43, SD = 10.47; 107 males and 73 females; 90 US participants and 90 Chinese participants; 14 participants with high school or below, 75 participants with some college, 91 participants with college graduate or above).

As for the analysis plan, the effect of cultures and graphs on current anxiety, anticipated anxiety, and overall anxiety was firstly explored with descriptive analysis, examining H1a and H1b. Then, the effect of cultures and graphs on social distancing intentions was further analyzed, testing H2a and H2b. In addition, the mediating analysis of anxiety in signaling social distancing intentions was conducted to confirm H3a and H3b. Further, to examine H4, we tested the interaction effect of cultures and graphs on anxiety and social distancing intentions. Last, a chi-square test was analyzed to investigate whether people would have a bias for different graphs. The significance level in the analysis was set at 0.05.

Procedure and Statistical Analysis

The procedure of this experiment involved four parts: pre-study, recruiting, introduction, and the main study. We initially recruited 60 participants via Wenjuanxing and conducted a pre-study to ensure the clarity and consistency of the questionnaires. The formal recruitment was followed up after confirmation of the appropriateness of the questionnaire design. Specifically, the experimental task was distributed via two channels: participants were recruited with the help of Wenjuanxing for collectivist culture and AMT for individualistic culture. After informing their unique ID will be recorded for research purposes, participants who consented to be enrolled in this study could click the checkbox, “I agree to participate in the research”, and proceeded. For the main study, they were first asked to provide demographic information and were then randomly assigned to one of three stimuli (each graph was seen by 30 participants). Subsequently, they were asked to pay attention to the specific COVID-19 graph for 5 s (52), and then, were instructed to answer a set of questions for anxiety evaluation. Last, they were instructed to estimate the daily confirmed cases on Jun 30, 2020, from four choices (A. approximately 140,000 per day; B. approximately 150,000 per day; C. approximately 160,000 per day; D. approximately 170,000 per day). According to the data from the WHO, 159,962 individuals were confirmed on Jun 30. Thus, approximately 160,000 per day (C) was the correct choice. After finishing all the questions, they were informed they have finished the work. SPSS software was used to perform statistical analysis.

Data Analysis and Results

To test for H1a and H1b, a two-way ANOVA was conducted with different graphs (line graph, bar graph, and stacked area graph) and different cultures (individualism vs. collectivism) as the independent variables, and current anxiety, anticipated anxiety, and overall anxiety level as the dependent variables. Supporting H1a, the results showed the significant main effect of different graphs: individuals exposed to the stacked area graph had the highest level of current anxiety [Mean = 6.52, SD = 1.79; F(2, 174) = 8.64, p < 0.01, Eta-squared = 0.09], anticipated anxiety [Mean = 6.83, SD = 2.10; F(2, 174) = 9.62, p < 0.01, Eta-squared = 0.10], and overall anxiety [Mean = 6.68, SD = 1.68; F(2, 174) = 11.73, p < 0.01, Eta-squared = 0.12]; the bar graph had a medium level of current anxiety (Mean = 5.80, SD = 1.84), anticipated anxiety (Mean = 6.25, SD = 1.86), and overall anxiety (Mean = 6.02, SD = 1.62); and the line graph had the lowest level of current anxiety (Mean = 5.08, SD = 2.05), anticipated anxiety (Mean = 5.17, SD = 2.42), and overall anxiety (Mean = 5.13, SD = 2.01). Further post hoc tests are illustrated in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Effect of different graphs and cultures on current anxiety (A), anticipated anxiety (B), and overall anxiety (C). **Significant at 0.05; * Significant at 0.10, ns, non-significant.

Partically supporting H1b, the results also indicated the marginally significant main effect of culture (significant for the anticipated and overall anxiety while non-significant for the current anxiety): individuals in China have a higher level of current anxiety [Mean = 6.07 vs. 5.53, SD = 1.42 vs. 2.39; F(1, 174) = 3.59, p = 0.06, Eta-squared = 0.02], anticipated anxiety [Mean = 6.48 vs. 5.69, SD = 2.12 vs. 2.30; F(1, 174) = 6.27, p = 0.01, Eta-squared = 0.04], and overall anxiety [Mean = 6.27 vs. 5.61, SD = 1.46 vs. 2.18; F(1, 174) = 6.35, p = 0.01, Eta-squared = 0.04] than US individuals. However, there is no significant interaction effects on current anxiety [F(2, 174) = 1.15, p = 0.31], anticipated anxiety [F(2, 174) = 1.10, p = 0.33], and overall anxiety [F(2, 174) = 0.55, p = 0.57].

Similarly, the two-way ANOVA was also performed on social distancing intentions. The results showed that individuals exposed to the stacked area graph experienced a higher social distancing intentions [Mean = 8.23, SD = 0.94; F(2, 174) = 10.99, p < 0.01, Eta-squared = 0.12] than for the bar graph (Mean = 7.42, SD = 1.74) and line graph (Mean = 6.97, SD = 1.70), and the difference in social distancing intentions between Chinese and US individuals were significant [Mean = 7.77 vs. 7.31, SD = 0.94 vs. 2.02; F(1, 174) = 4.15, p = 0.04, Eta-squared = 0.02]. Thus, H2a and H2b was supported (Figure 4). However, there was no significant interaction effect between different cultures and graphs [F(2, 174) = 0.52, p = 0.59]. H4 was not supported.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Effect of different graphs and cultures on social distancing intentions. **Significant at 0.05; ns, non-significant.

To test H3a and H3b, the mediating role of anxiety and social distancing intentions were regressed on different graphs and cultures with overall anxiety as the mediator (model 4; n = 5,000 resamples; Hayes, 2015). According to the results, we observe a significant mediation effect for graphs (β = 0.16, SE = 0.08, LLCI = 0.02, ULCI = 0.20) and cultures (β = 0.17, SE = 0.09, LLCI = 0.03, ULCI = 0.37), separately. Figures 5, 6 show the separate mediation analysis.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Mediation analysis of anxiety between different graphs and social distancing intentions. ***Significant at 0.01.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Mediation analysis of anxiety between different cultures and social distancing intentions. ***Significant at 0.01.

Last, for new case estimation, we observe that the participants in the stacked area graph tended to overestimate the daily cases (the most common response was option “D”), and individuals exposed to the line graph underestimated the daily cases (the most common responses were options “A” and “B”). A chi-square test (6, N = 180) = 9.04, p = 0.17, showed no significant difference between different graphs and different options (Figure 7).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Estimation counts of the new cases on Jun 30, 2020.

Discussion

Theoretical and Practical Implications

This study mainly makes two theoretical contributions to the literature. To begin with, this study contributes to data visualization theory in the context of COVID-19. The rapid speed of the information and communications technology evolution has fundamentally changed how individuals obtain the latest public health information (53). Indeed, an increasing number of individuals are searching for health-related information on the internet (54). Regarding the intuitive nature of the visual presentation, graphs on health information have been widely used (55). Research on health information visualization has mainly focused on two perspectives: the difference between graphs and numbers (e.g., a table), and different design features within one graph (e.g., the size of a graphic element) (18). Scant research has attempted to discuss the most common types of graphs and their impact on health information communication. Thus, it is theoretically significant to examine the relationship between different graphs and their influences on behavior or behavioral intentions. Consistent with the research on embodied cognition (23, 27, 28), the stacked area graph resulted in a higher level of anxiety and social distancing intentions, followed by the bar graph and line graph. Although a stacked area graph might work as a better visualization tool to improve public awareness, it still could increase people's anxiety and dampen social wellbeing. Accordingly, we might still face an ethical dilemma: stacked area graphs might help to communicate health information and decrease people's intentions to get crowded while it could also threaten some residents who have already experienced stress, anxiety, and even depression via various social media (56). Related authorities might need to strike a balance between increasing public awareness and its potential deficiency and implement necessary mental services, such as a hotline, to support public mental health, especially vulnerable people (57). Another intriguing thing is through a line graph might not promote public awareness and social distancing, individuals might still prefer and like it for its simplicity and familiarity (58).

In addition, the current study validates the finding that, compared with individualism cultures in Western countries, individuals living in collectivist cultures, such as that in China, might generally experience higher social anxiety (59). However, the cultural elements did not aggravate the anxiety perception and social distancing intentions for collectivists, namely, different cultures might similarly proportional influence the effect of graphs on anxiety perceptions. Moreover, regarding the effect size of cultures and graph types, results showed the effects of cultures on anxiety and social distancing intentions were relatively small (<0.06) while the effect of graph types was relatively medium (>0.06) (60). It might suggest that graph types might have a more significant impact on anxiety and social distancing, compared with different cultures.

As for social distancing intentions, we observed a significant difference between different cultures, which is consistent with previous research on cultural differences in social distancing: people from individualistic countries are less likely to commit the social distancing as the governmental suggestions (61). Accordingly, there seem tradeoffs between the degree of freedom and constraints from the various viewpoints across societies when committing social distancing (61).

This study also has the following managerial implications. First, because the outbreak of COVID-19 continues to pose significant challenges worldwide (6), it is critical to increase public awareness and encourage social distancing (62). Compared with line graphs and bar graphs, stacked area graphs might work as an efficient tool to increase public health awareness and social distancing toward the COVID-19 crisis, nevertheless, we cannot neglect its potential threats on social mental health and should further consider specific characteristics of the audience. For example, since the whole world is still currently within the COVID-19 pandemic, different countries or regions might face different outbreak stages of COVID-19. Thus, some people in a collectivist culture might generally experience a higher level of anxiety when exposed to the same health information. Considering striking a balance between public wellbeing and social distancing, it might be more appropriate to use different graphs for different residents based on their current situations.

Limitations and Further Research Directions

This study has limitations that require further investigations. First, although the line, bar, and stacked area graphs are the most common time-series graphs in communicating COVID-19 updates, there are other time-series graphs, such as scatter plots, which are seldom used for health risk communication (9). Thus, further research could comprehensively analyze different time series graphs' influence on public health awareness and risk-prevention behavior. In addition, the graphic proportion of different graphs is the main means for communicating risk-related information; however, there are many other design elements in graph design, such as color, font size, and animation, which might elicit public health awareness and social distancing (18). For example, the colors red and yellow could significantly elicit higher anxiety than the colors blue and green (63) because higher levels of Chroma might increase greater feelings of excitement and more intense behavioral reactions (64). Regarding the WHO COVID-19 dashboard's general use of the colors gray or blue in visualizing data (6), the effect of graph color and its interaction with different graph types on public health awareness and social distancing requires further research to determine the most appropriate combinations of graph design elements. Further, though social distancing intentions could be influenced by social anxiety, they might also be shaped by various factors, such as local restrictions and governmental regulations (65) and resident characteristics (66). Thus, a possibility that different cities within one country might have different social distancing policies, such as the limitation of public gatherings to four persons in Hong Kong (67), which might elicit individuals' social distancing intentions to some extent (65). Last, for the measurements of anxiety, we used a single-item scale (or the average of two items) to assess the related anxiety level. Although a single-item scale might enjoy relatively similar reliability and validity compared with the multiple-item scale, the multiple-item scale indeed outperformed the single-item scale in specific cases (68). A further study should consider local regulations and examines their impact on people's social distancing intentions and applied a multiple-item measurement of related anxiety to validate the current finding.

Conclusion

Considering COVID-19 information visualization is widely used in various media communications of the latest health updates, this study examined the effect of different graphs and cultures on individuals' anxiety levels and social distancing intentions. Specifically, we indicated the mediation effect of anxiety on the relationship between different graphs and social distancing intentions and the role of different cultures in responding to different graphs. The results of this study demonstrate the following: (1) the stacked area graph caused the highest level of anxiety and social distancing intentions, followed by the bar and then line graphs; (2) Collectivist residents tended to experience a higher level of anxiety and social distancing intentions than individualistic residents; (3) there were no interaction effects of different graphs and cultures on anxiety level and social distancing intentions; (4) the effect of different graphs and cultures on social distancing intentions was mediated by anxiety level; (5) individuals exposed to the stacked area graph tended to overestimate the daily confirmed cases, and those exposed to the line graph tended to underestimate the daily confirmed cases.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committee of Shenzhen University (SZUDA20190901001). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JL, YZ, and YS: conceptualization, methodology, and writing—original draft preparation. YS: formal analysis. YZ and JL: investigation. JL: resources. JL and YS: data curation. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by Shenzhen University, “The Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities” [YJ202203], Annual Program of Social Science Research of Sichuan Province for the Thirteenth Five-year Plan [SC20ZT006], and The Innovation Project of Shanghai Municipal Education Commission [2017-01-07-00-06-E00031].

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2022.800789/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Diab Z, Shara N, Chou J, Libin A, Kuhn A, Farmer-Dixon C, et al. The contrasting short-term effects of COVID-19 on dental care practices in the United States. Clin Exp Dent Res. (2021) 7:279–84. doi: 10.1002/cre2.398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Nie Y, Ma Y, Wu Y, Li J, Liu T, Zhang C, et al. Association between physical exercise and mental health during the COVID-19 outbreak in China: a nationwide cross-sectional study. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:722448. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.722448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Büssing A, Rodrigues Recchia D, Dienberg T, Surzykiewicz J, Baumann K. Dynamics of perceived positive changes and indicators of well-being within different phases of the COVID-19 pandemic. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:685975. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.685975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Xu J, Liu C. Infodemic vs. pandemic factors associated to public anxiety in the early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak: a cross-sectional study in China. Front Public Heal. (2021) 9:723648. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.723648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Libin E. Future competencies for digitally aligned specialties: Coping intelligently with global challenges. Int Conf High Educ Adv. (2020) 1119–125. doi: 10.4995/HEAd20.2020.11210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. WHO. WHO Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Dashboard. WHO (2020) Available online at: https://covid19.who.int/region/amro/country/br (accessed July 25, 2020).

7. Worldometer. Coronavirus Cases. Worldometer (2020).

Google Scholar

8. Johns Hopkins. COVID-19 Map. Johns Hopkins Coronavirus Resource Center. (2020) Available online at: https://coronavirus.jhu.edu/map.html (accessed July 30, 2020).

9. Samantha Lile. 44 Types of Graphs How to Choose the Best One for Your Data. (2017). Available online at: http://blog.visme.co/types-of-graphs/ (accessed July 25, 2020).

10. Zhao X, Cai X. The role of risk, efficacy, and anxiety in smokers' cancer information seeking. Health Commun. (2009) 24:259–69. doi: 10.1080/10410230902805932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Heer J, Kong N, Agrawala M. Sizing the horizon: the effects of chart size and layering on the graphical perception of time series visualizations. In: CHI '09: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. Boston, MA (2009). doi: 10.1145/1518701.1518897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Huang Y, Zhao N. Generalized anxiety disorder, depressive symptoms and sleep quality during COVID-19 outbreak in China: a web-based cross-sectional survey. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 288:112954. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Lewnard JA, Lo NC. Scientific and ethical basis for social-distancing interventions against COVID-19. Lancet Infect Dis. (2020) 20:631. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30190-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Playfair W. The Commercial and Political Atlas: Representing, by Means of Stained Copper-Plate Charts, the progress of the Commerce, Revenues, Expenditure and Debts of England During the Whole of the Eighteenth Century. London (1786).

Google Scholar

15. Eells WC. The relative merits of circles and bars for representing component parts. J Am Stat Assoc. (1926) 21:119–32. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1926.10502165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Song Y, Luximon Y, Luo J. A moderated mediation analysis of the effect of lettering case and color temperature on trustworthiness perceptions and investment decisions. Int J Bank Mark. (2020) 38:987–1005. doi: 10.1108/IJBM-09-2019-0315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Kruskal W. Visions of maps and graphs. In: Proceedings of the International Symposium on Computer-Assisted Cartography. Available online at: http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.526.702&rep=rep1&type=pdf (accessed July 30, 2020).

Google Scholar

18. Ancker JS, Senathirajah Y, Kukafka R, Starren JB. Design features of graphs in health risk communication: a systematic review. J Am Med Informatics Assoc. (2006) 13:608–18. doi: 10.1197/jamia.M2115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Cleveland WS, McGill R. Graphical perception and graphical methods for analyzing scientific data. Science. (1985) 229:828–33. doi: 10.1126/science.229.4716.828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Wickens CD, Carswell CM. The proximity compatibility principle: Its psychological foundation and relevance to display design. Hum Factor. (1995) 37:473–94. doi: 10.1518/001872095779049408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Paek HJ, Oh SH, Hove T. How fear-arousing news messages affect risk perceptions and intention to talk about risk. Health Commun. (2016) 31:1051–62. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2015.1037419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Newcombe NS, Levine SC, Mix KS. Thinking about quantity: The intertwined development of spatial and numerical cognition. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. (2015) 6:491–505. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Shapiro L. The Routledge Handbook of Embodied Cognition. New York, NY: Routledge (2014). doi: 10.4324/9781315775845

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Postigo Y, Pozo JI. On the road to graphicacy: the learning of graphical representation systems. Educ Psychol. (2004) 24:623–44. doi: 10.1080/0144341042000262944

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Pérez-Echeverría MP, Postigo Y, Marín C. Understanding of graphs in social science undergraduate students: selection and interpretation of graphs. Irish Educ Stud. (2018) 37:89–111. doi: 10.1080/03323315.2018.1440248

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Zacks J, Tversky B. Bars and lines: a study of graphic communication. Mem Cogn. (1999) 27:1073–9. doi: 10.3758/BF03201236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Curry NA, Kasser T. Can coloring mandalas reduce anxiety? Art Ther. (2005) 22:81–5. doi: 10.1080/07421656.2005.10129441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Herzog M, Douglas CR, Kissileff HR, Brunstrom JM, Halmi KA. Food portion size area mediates energy effects on expected anxiety in anorexia nervosa. Appetite. (2017) 112:17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bish A, Michie S. Demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviours during a pandemic: a review. Br J Health Psychol. (2010) 15:797–824. doi: 10.1348/135910710X485826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Libin E. Coping intelligence: efficient life stress management. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:302. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Liao Q, Cowling BJ, Lam WWT, Ng DMW, Fielding R. Anxiety, worry and cognitive risk estimate in relation to protective behaviors during the 2009 influenza A/H1N1 pandemic in Hong Kong: ten cross-sectional surveys. BMC Infect Dis. (2014) 14:169. doi: 10.1186/1471-2334-14-169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Luhtanen R, Crocker J. A collective self-esteem scale: self-evaluation of one's social identity. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. (1992) 18:302–18. doi: 10.1177/0146167292183006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Tajfel H. Social identity and intergroup behaviour. Soc Sci Inf. (1974) 13:65–93. doi: 10.1177/053901847401300204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Oyserman D, Coon HM, Kemmelmeier M. Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychol Bull. (2002) 128:3–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Steel P, Schmidt J, Shultz J. Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Psychol Bull. (2008) 134:138–61. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Heinrichs N, Rapee RM, Alden LA, Bögels S, Hofmann SG, Ja Oh K, et al. Cultural differences in perceived social norms and social anxiety. Behav Res Ther. (2006) 44:1187–97. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Schreier SS, Heinrichs N, Alden L, Rapee RM, Hofmann SG, Chen J, et al. Social anxiety and social norms in individualistic and collectivistic countries. Depress Anxiety. (2010) 27:1128–34. doi: 10.1002/da.20746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Eckhardt G. Culture's consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organisations across nations. Aust J Manag. (2002) 27:89–94. doi: 10.1177/031289620202700105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Xu S. When individual cultural orientation and mediated portrayal of risk intersect: Effects of individualism-collectivism and media framing on risk perception and attribution of responsibility. J Contingencies Cris Manag. (2018) 26:499–509. doi: 10.1111/1468-5973.12219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Zheng L. Does online perceived risk depend on culture? Individualistic versus collectivistic culture. J Decis Syst. (2017) 26:256–74. doi: 10.1080/12460125.2017.1351861

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Kowal M, Karwowski M, Rasmussen J, Lieberoth A, Eichel K, Studzi A, et al. Who is the most stressed during the COVID-19 pandemic? Data from 26 countries and areas. Wiley Online Libr. (2020) 2020:946–66. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Huang F, Ding H, Liu Z, Wu P, Zhu M, Li A, et al. How fear and collectivism influence public's preventive intention towards COVID-19 infection: a study based on big data from the social media. BMC Public Health. (2020) 20:963. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09674-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Wenjuanxing. Available online at: https://www.wjx.cn/index.aspx (accessed February 12, 2022).

44. Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT). Mturk. (2021) Available online at: https://www.mturk.com/ (accessed March 30, 2021).

45. Vakharia D, Lease M. Beyond mechanical turk : an analysis of paid crowd work platforms. In: iConference Proc. Newport Beach, CA (2014). p. 1–17.

Google Scholar

46. WHO. Mental Health and Psychosocial Considerations During the COVID-19 outbreak. World Health Organiztion (2020). Available online at: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/331490/WHO-2019-nCoV-MentalHealth-2020.1-eng.pdf (accessed July 14, 2020).

Google Scholar

47. Yuan X, Xu J, Hussain S, Wang H, Gao N, Zhang L. Trends and prediction in daily new cases and deaths of COVID-19 in the United States: an internet search-interest based model. Explor Res Hypothesis Med. (2020) 5:1–6. doi: 10.14218/ERHM.2020.00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Rotliman AJ, Schwarz N. Constructing perceptions of vulnerability: personal relevance and the use of experiential information in health judgments. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. (1998) 24:1053–64. doi: 10.1177/01461672982410003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Mugge R, Schoormans JPL. Product design and apparent usability. The influence of novelty in product appearance. Appl Ergon. (2012) 43:1081–8. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2012.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Næs T, Brockhoff PB, Tomic O. Statistics for Sensory and Consumer Science. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd (2010). doi: 10.1002/9780470669181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Song Y, Luximon Y. Design for sustainability: The effect of lettering case on environmental concern from a green advertising perspective. Sustain. (2019) 11:1333. doi: 10.3390/su11051333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Marschollek M, Mix S, Wolf K-H, Effertz B, Haux R, Steinhagen-Thiessen E. ICT-based health information services for elderly people: Past experiences, current trends, and future strategies. Med Inform Internet Med. (2007) 32:251–61. doi: 10.1080/14639230701692736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Li F, Li M, Guan P, Ma S, Cui L. Mapping publication trends and identifying hot spots of research on Internet health information seeking behavior: a quantitative and co-word biclustering analysis. J Med Internet Res. (2015) 17:e81. doi: 10.2196/jmir.3326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Wattenberg M, Kriss J. Designing for social data analysis. IEEE Trans Visual Comput Graph. (2006) 12:549–57. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2006.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gao J, Zheng P, Jia Y, Chen H, Mao Y, Chen S, et al. Mental health problems and social media exposure during COVID-19 outbreak. PLoS ONE. (2020) 15:e0231924. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kim S, Pjesivac I, Jin Y. Effects of message framing on influenza vaccination: understanding the role of risk disclosure, perceived vaccine efficacy, and felt ambivalence. Health Commun. (2019) 34:21–30. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2017.1384353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Parrott R, Dorgan K, Condit C, Harris T. Risk Comprehension Judgments of Statistical Evidentiary Appeals When a Picture is Not Worth a Thousand Words. (2005). Available online at: https://academic.oup.com/hcr/article-abstract/31/3/423/4331547 (accessed July 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

59. Biddlestone M, Green R, Douglas KM. Cultural orientation, power, belief in conspiracy theories, and intentions to reduce the spread of COVID-19. Br J Soc Psychol. (2020) 59:663–73. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Holliday R. Evolutionary biology of aging. Michael R. Rose. Q Rev Biol. (1993) 68:93–94. doi: 10.1086/417928

CrossRef Full Text

61. Huynh TLD. Does culture matter social distancing under the COVID-19 pandemic? Saf Sci. (2020) 130:104872. doi: 10.1016/j.ssci.2020.104872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Lee SA. Coronavirus Anxiety Scale: a brief mental health screener for COVID-19 related anxiety. Death Stud. (2020) 44:393–401. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2020.1748481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Jacobs KW, Suess JF. Effects of four psychological primary colors on anxiety state. Percept Mot Skills. (1975) 41:207–10. doi: 10.2466/pms.1975.41.1.207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Gorn GJ, Chattopadhyay A, Yi T, Dahl DW. Effects of color as an executional cue in advertising: they're in the shade. Manage Sci. (1997) 43:1387–400. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.43.10.1387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Adolph C, Amano K, Bang-Jensen B, Fullman N, Wilkerson J. Pandemic politics: timing state-level social distancing responses to COVID-19. medRxiv. (2020). doi: 10.1101/2020.03.30.20046326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Mahase E. Covid-19: UK starts social distancing after new model points to 260 000 potential deaths. BMJ. (2020) 368:m1089. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m1089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Cheng L, Chung K. Coronavirus: Hong Kong to Limit Public Gatherings to Four Shut Cinemas, Gyms Other Social Spots as Cases Jump by 65. South China Morning Post (2020) Available online at: https://www.scmp.com/news/hong-kong/health-environment/article/3077301/coronavirus-hong-kong-limit-restaurant-tables (accessed July 25, 2020).

68. Hayes AF. An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behav Res. (2015) 50:1–22. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.962683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: graph, culture, anxiety, social distancing, data visualization, COVID-19

Citation: Luo J, Zhang Y and Song Y (2022) Design for Pandemic Information: Examining the Effect of Graphs on Anxiety and Social Distancing Intentions in the COVID-19. Front. Public Health 10:800789. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.800789

Received: 24 October 2021; Accepted: 03 March 2022;
Published: 18 May 2022.

Edited by:

Alexander V. Libin, MedStar Health Research Institute (MHRI), United States

Reviewed by:

Carlos Miguel Rios-González, National University of Caaguazú, Paraguay
Elena V. Libin, Independent Researcher, Chevy Chase, United States
Chao Zhang, Eindhoven University of Technology, Netherlands
Xiaohui Wang, City University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Copyright © 2022 Luo, Zhang and Song. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yao Song, eWFvLnNvbmcmI3gwMDA0MDtzY3UuZWR1LmNu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.