Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 10 January 2023
Sec. Life-Course Epidemiology and Social Inequalities in Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Population Transition and Environmental Factors for Full Life-course Health View all articles

The association among SES, screen time, and outdoor play in children at different ages: The GECKO Drenthe study

\nCongchao Lu,,
Congchao Lu1,2,3*Rikstje WiersmaRikstje Wiersma4Eva CorpeleijnEva Corpeleijn4
  • 1School of Public Health, Tianjin Medical University, Tianjin, China
  • 2Tianjin Key Laboratory of Environment, Nutrition and Public Health, Tianjin, China
  • 3Center for International Collaborative Research on Environment, Nutrition and Public Health, Tianjin, China
  • 4Department of Epidemiology, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

Introduction: This study examined the association among socioeconomic status (SES), screen time, and outdoor play in children at different ages in the GECKO Drenthe birth cohort study.

Methods: Valid data were obtained from two surveys at ages 3–4 years and 10–11 years. Screen time (TV watching and computer use) and outdoor play were reported by parents. Childhood SES was derived by a synthetic “Equivalized Household Income Indicator,” an estimated disposable income. Quantile regression models (cross-sectional analysis) and linear regression models (change between 3–4 and 10–11 years) were used.

Results: In general, screen time increased strongly from a median of 51 min/day at 3–4 years (n = 888) to 122 min/day at 10–11 years (n = 1023), whereas time spent on outdoor play remained stable over age (77 min/day at 3–4 years and 81 min/day at 10–11 years). More time spent on outdoor play (50th quantile) was found in children with low SES families at 3–4 years, while at 10–11 years, more outdoor play was found in the high SES group. At 10–11 years, in the higher ranges of screen time, children from high SES had relatively lower screen time [50th quantile: −10.7 (−20.8; −0.6); 75th quantile: −13.6 (−24.4; −2.8)]. In the longitudinal analysis (n = 536), high SES was associated with an increasing time spent on outdoor play [11.7 (2.7; 20.8)].

Conclusion: Socioeconomic disparities in children's outdoor play and screen behavior may be more obvious with increasing age. Low SES may facilitate both outdoor play (at 3–4 years) and screen time (at 10–11 years); however, children from high SES families develop slightly more favorable behavior patterns with age.

1. Introduction

Human behaviors and activities are changed due to the revolution of information technology. For example, digital communications have influenced how people work, study, and spend their leisure time. In most countries, children are spending greater time engaged in screen-based entertainment, such as television and computers. It is well-recognized that excessive screen time is associated with an increasing trend of physical inactivity in most societies around the world (1). Furthermore, increasing screen time is associated with poorer sleep outcomes (2) and delayed motor development skills in preschool children (3), and with increased obesity risk in children and adolescents (4, 5).

The health benefits of outdoor play have been emphasized by several researchers in terms of reducing myopia and developing motor skills, along with improving social skills (6). In addition, encouraging outdoor play might prove to be an effective strategy in children for curbing physical inactivity, since it is a cheap and natural way (7). A study indicated that every additional hour spent outdoors per day was associated with 7 min less sedentary time on an average day among Dutch preschoolers (8). Meanwhile, evidence indicated that outdoor play has been replaced by more time using electronic media indoors (9, 10). The World Health Organization guidelines state that preschool children (3–4 years old) should spend no more than 1 h of screen time each day (11), whereas children and adolescents (5–17 years old) should limit the amount of recreational screen time (12). Due to the co-dependence of lifestyle behaviors, an increase in one behavior would be expected to result in a decline in another (13). However, few studies have reported the co-dependence of the relationships between changes in screen time and changes in outdoor play. Understanding the long-term changes in those behaviors throughout childhood contributes to the early evidence-based planning of public health interventions. Thus, it needs to be understood how screen time and outdoor play change with age.

As lifestyle behaviors, outdoor play, and screen behaviors depend on their societal context, socioeconomic status (SES) which represents the social, cultural, and economic features of a family is an important factor that affects opportunities for these behaviors (14). Assessing SES in early life is also essential to control for confounders when studying outcomes that are strongly socially shaped (15). Studies investigated the association between SES and screen time, and inconsistent results may be derived because various SES indicators were used (16). For example, there can be marked racial differences in income at a given educational level (17). Thus, caution is needed when evidence based on different single SES indicators were synthesized for informing social policy design to effectively reduce health disparities in a socially diverse society (18). However, accurately measuring family income through questionnaires is difficult due to several issues, and it is essential to have harmonized comparable SES indicators over different studies. In this study, a standardized income indicator, the “Equivalized Household Income Indicator (EHII)” that measures the equivalized disposable household income based on a cluster of indicators (including but not limited to maternal and paternal education level, housing type, and family size) was used as children's SES indicator (19). We are aiming to explore SES differences in screen time and outdoor play at different ages, and the changes in screen time and outdoor play between ages 3–4 and 10–11 years in the Groningen Expert Center for Kids with Obesity (GECKO) Drenthe birth cohort.

2. Methods

2.1. GECKO Drenthe birth cohort

Data were derived from the GECKO Drenthe birth cohort, which focuses on the development of overweight and obesity in children living in Drenthe, a northern province of the Netherlands. Details of the study have been reported elsewhere (20). All mothers of children born between April 2006 and April 2007 and living in Drenthe were invited to participate during the third trimester of their pregnancy. Almost 3,000 pregnant women were recruited. Monitoring of the children started in the last trimester of the pregnancy and is still ongoing. At the age of 10–11 years, 2,299 children were measured for follow-up.

2.2. Data collection

At baseline, child and family information, including birth dates of family members, parental country of birth, parental educational levels, parental occupational status, mother living with a partner, and dwelling type, was collected. When children were aged 3–4 years (2010–2011) and 10–11 years (2017–2018), the height and weight of children were measured by trained preventive child healthcare nurses as part of a regular health screening. Children's overweight and obesity aspects were classified according to the age-specific and gender-specific cutoffs of Cole and Lobstein (21). Questionnaires for parents when the child was 45 months were handed out during visits to the Well Baby Clinic, and questionnaires for children of 11 years were sent to the parents by post. Children who had valid data on screen time and outdoor play both at 3–4 years and/or 10–11 years and the synthesized indicator of household income estimation were selected as potential participants for this study. The flowchart of the participants used in this study is shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of the participants in the GECKO Drenthe study.

2.3. Socioeconomic status

Childhood SES in this study was defined by the “Equivalized Household Income Indicator (EHII),” which is a standardized, cross-cohort comparable income indicator developed by Pizzi et al (19). The EHII was derived by a prediction model combining household and personal variables to estimate the disposable household income. The following household and personal variables were included in the prediction model: “maternal age,” “maternal educational level,” “maternal occupational status,” “maternal country of birth,” “paternal age,” “paternal educational level,” “paternal occupational status,” “paternal country of birth,” “cohabitation status (living with/without a partner),” “dwelling type,” and “family size.” The prediction model was constructed using external data of the Netherlands from 2011 from the pan-European Union Statistics on Income and Living Conditions (EUSILC) surveys (22) and validated with data from 2015. The prediction model had a good overall performance (R2 = 0.455), and details have been reported elsewhere (23). The derived household income was interpreted as the log equivalized monthly total disposable household income that a person with those characteristics would have had in 2011. The values of the EHII back-transformed monthly total disposable household income are equal to 729 Euro−2943 Euro in this study. A positive association was found between the estimated disposable income by EHII and self-reported household income in 2,212 participants in the cohort (rs = 0.428, p < 0.001). Childhood SES was categorized into low SES (<1,717 Euro/month), middle SES (1,717–2,172 Euro/month), and high SES (>2,172 Euro/month) based on tertiles of the estimated household income.

2.4. Screen time and outdoor play

Parents/guardians reported the frequency and duration of their children's television (TV) time, computer time, and outdoor play, considering a typical week in the past month, both when the child was 3–4 years old and 10–11 years old. In those questionnaires, TV time was defined as time spent watching TV, video, or DVD, and computer time was defined as time spent using a game computer. Child outdoor play was defined as time spent playing outside, with questions based on a study by Aarts et al. (24). For example, parents were asked to report the duration of outdoor play during weekdays (response categories ranged from 0 to 5 days) and weekend days (response categories ranged from 0 to 2 days), with the answer options (no outdoor play, <30 min per day, 30 min−1 h per day, 1–2 h per day, and more than 2 h per day). To sum up the result, “no outdoor play” was recorded as 0, “ <30 min” was recorded as 15 min, “30 min−1 h” was recorded as 45 min, “1–2 h” was recorded as 90 min, and “more than 2 h” was recorded as 150 min. The average number of minutes per day was computed to obtain an overall outdoor play time average per day. TV time and computer time were asked and recorded in the same way, separately, and they were summed together as screen time. The outcomes were screen time (min/day) and outdoor play (min/day), both at 3–4 years and 10–11 years, and the changes in screen time (min/day) and outdoor play (min/day) between the two surveys are referred to as tracking data.

2.5. Parents' rules on child screen use and outdoor play

Parents' rules on “the duration” of child TV watching and game computer use were asked separately in questionnaires, and those rules were combined to make rules about “the duration” of screen use. For example, if there was a rule at home “about the duration of the child be allowed to watch TV” and/or there was a rule “about the duration of the child be allowed to use the game computer,” then it was defined as there was a rule at home “about the duration of child's screen use.” In the questionnaire, there were also rules about “the duration” of the child's outdoor play. All of those rules were asked at both 3–4 years and 10–11 years.

2.6. Statistical analysis

Continuous variables were presented as means with SDs or, if data were skewed, as the median with 25th−75th percentile. Categorical variables were presented as rates in number and percentages. To examine the differences in characteristics between children with tracking data included in the analyses to children lacking tracking data, a t-test was used for normally distributed continuous variables, and the Mann–Whitney U-test was used for non-normal distributed continuous variables. Differences in categorical variables were tested by the chi-square test. The cross-sectional correlation between screen time and outdoor play was checked by Pearson correlation (normal distribution), or Spearman's correlation was used if data were skewed. To determine the cross-sectional relationships among SES, child screen time, and outdoor play in both surveys, quantile regression analysis was used, with children from middle SES families set as the reference group. Regression coefficient estimates at the 25th, 50th, and 75th quantiles of each outcome were reported, and all models were adjusted for age, sex, and parents' rules on child screen use/outdoor play. The season was not added in those models, since there was no difference in the season of measurement between the three SES groups (survey of 3–4 years: χ2 = 7.388, p = 0.286; survey of 10–11 years: χ2 = 9.747, p = 0.136). To determine the relationships between SES and the changes in screen time and outdoor play between the two surveys, the linear regression analysis adjusted for age and sex was used. For the sensitivity analyses, the maternal level of education in three categories was checked as an indicator of SES by the same methods, with children whose mothers were at the middle educational level as the reference group. IBM SPSS Statistics V.26 for Windows was used for this study, with test level α = 0.05, and analyses were conducted in 2022.

3. Results

In the survey of 3–4 years, 926 children had valid questionnaire data on screen time and outdoor play, and this number is 1,058 at 10–11 years. Combined with SES information, 888 children (52.9% boys) at 3–4 years and 1,023 children (48.9% boys) at 10–11 years had valid data for cross-sectional analysis. Of these, 536 children (53.7% boys) had valid data on screen time and outdoor play tracking (Figure 1). Missing data from the questionnaire at 3–4 years were mainly attributable to logistical and organizational problems.

Children with data on screen time and outdoor play tracking showed a higher level of maternal education (n = 208, 38.8% of total 536) compared to children without tracking data (n = 116, 33.0% of total 352; χ2= 91.274, p = 0.000). As shown in Table 1, screen time at 3–4 years consisted mostly of TV time, and it increased from the median of 51 (39; 90) min/day to 122 (90; 165) min/day at 10–11 years. The time children spent playing outside did not change too much as children got older. More outdoor play was associated with less screen time in both surveys (ρ = −0.098, p < 0.001; r = −0.098, p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the study population in the GECKO Drenthe cohort study.

As shown in Table 2, the cross-sectional associations of SES, children's screen time, and outdoor play were analyzed by quantile regression analysis, with children from middle SES families set as the reference group. Regression coefficient estimates at the 25th, 50th, and 75th quantiles cut-points of each outcome are given in both surveys. Compared to the middle SES group, more time spent on outdoor play was found in children with low SES families at the 50th quantile in the survey of 3–4 years. At the same time, more time spent on outdoor play was found in children with high SES families at the 50th quantile in the survey of 10–11 years. A pattern of less time spent on screen time at higher quantiles was observed in the high SES group in the survey of 10–11 years; the regression coefficient estimate at the 50th quantiles was −10.7 (95% CI: −20.8; −0.6), and at the 75th quantile, it was −13.6 (95% CI: −24.4; −2.8). This indicates that when children watch a little TV, the SES differences are negligible, whereas the difference becomes more obvious in the higher ranges of screen time.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The estimated regression coefficients in screen time and outdoor play according to the level of socioeconomic status.

The associations of SES and changes in children's screen time and outdoor play are given in Table 3. The average of changes in children's screen time in low, middle, and high SES groups were 65 (95 CI of mean: 52; 77), 76 (68; 85), and 67 (60; 73) min/day, respectively (Figure 2). It also showed that boys showed a greater increase in time spent on screen behaviors compared to girls. There was no difference in changes in screen time between SES groups, adjusted by sex and age. For changes in time spent on outdoor play, older children were observed to spend slightly less time on outdoor play at 10–11 years compared to when they were younger (Figure 3). The mean change was −15 (−25; −5) min/day in the low SES group, −7 (−14; 0) min/day in the middle SES group, and 5 (−1; 11) min/day in the high SES group (Figure 3). It showed that high SES was associated with maintenance and even a slight increase in time spent on outdoor play [β and 95% CI: 11.7 (2.7; 20.8)], compared to middle SES and low SES groups. As children from low SES families used to have slightly more time spent in outdoor play (Table 2), and outdoor play was maintained and even slightly increased in children from high SES families (Table 3), the eventual time spent in outdoor play at 10–11 years of age was comparable between groups (Table 2).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Socioeconomic status and changes in children's screen time and outdoor play from 3–4 to 10–11 years.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Daily screen time at different ages, and changes in screen time, according to socioeconomic status.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Daily time spent on outdoor play at different ages, and changes in outdoor play, according to socioeconomic status.

To determine the co-dependence of the relationships between changes in screen time and changes in outdoor play between the two surveys, linear regression analysis adjusted for age, sex, and SES was used. It showed that a decrease in screen time {β and 95% CI: −0.1 [−0.2; (−0.1)]} was associated with increasing outdoor playtime between the two surveys.

The results of the sensitivity analyses using maternal education level as an alternative indicator of socioeconomic position are given in Supplementary Table 1. It showed that the findings were materially unchanged. However, no different findings were found for time spent on outdoor play at 10–11 years, or changes in screen time or outdoor play between children in different maternal educational level families (Supplementary Table 2).

4. Discussion

This study examined how screen time and outdoor play depend on SES at different ages in the GECKO Drenthe birth cohort study. In general, children's screen time increased strongly with age, whereas time spent on outdoor play did not change too much as children got older. At 3–4 years, more time spent on outdoor play was found in children with low SES families. At 10–11 years, more outdoor play was found in children with high SES families, and a pattern of less time spent on screen time at higher quantiles was also observed in the high SES group. In addition, as an indicator of SES, the EHII captured longitudinal variations in socioeconomic inequalities in child outdoor play.

In this study, 38.9% of children spent more than 1 h of screen time per day at 3–4 years of age, while 54.2% of children spent more than 2 h per day of screen time at 10–11 years of age. Interestingly, an international study of 10-year-old children reported a similar percentage (54.2% of 5,844 children) of children who spent more than 2 h of screen time per day from 12 countries around the world (25). Over the past century, people in most countries have gotten used to new lifestyles based on lack of PA, increased nutritional consumption, and have shifted away from nature's 24-h day/night rhythm through the development of artificial light, such as screens. In the meantime, there is a growing concern about the adverse impact of screen behaviors on children's health, including adiposity, unhealthy diet, and depressive symptoms (26). For example, the latest systematic review has summarized that there was strong evidence for the association between home media environment and adiposity in childhood (≤12 years) (27). Although the etiology of media exposure and adiposity is still unclear, evidence indicated that exposure to the light of shorter wavelengths by screen usage might impact human metabolism (28). Furthermore, as home media use continues to evolve, it is necessary to explore if children are at high risk of adverse screen behaviors based on their socioeconomic group. Because screen habits may track from as early as infancy (29), early childhood could be an opportune time to intervene to reduce excessive screen time (30). Previous studies found that higher SES was inversely associated with high screen time in children (29, 31). As we found in this study, the difference in SES becomes more obvious in the higher ranges of screen time. At the same time, many children from high SES families were also at risk of high screen time with increasing age. This indicated that screens have become more widely available to all families with various economic conditions. As differences in screen time were much larger within SES groups than between SES groups, our result emphasized the potential need for interventions on reducing screen time in most children irrespective of socioeconomic group.

Active outdoor play for children's health and development is of particular importance since it could improve children's wellbeing in the physical, emotional, social, and cognitive domains (32). This study showed that more time spent on outdoor play was found in children with low SES families in the survey of 3–4 years, compared to the middle SES group. For preschool children, outdoor time could be a good opportunity for children to be active (33). Outdoor play may be more attractive to those parents from low SES families, since it is cheap and natural, and young children are highly dependent on their parents to create opportunities for activities. Another possible explanation is that parents from low SES families may have more free time to accompany their children to play outside (34). For example, a higher proportion of unemployed mothers (25.1%) was found in the low SES group in this study, compared to the middle (6.3%) or high (n = 0) SES groups. We re-analyzed the data to test this hypothesis, and found that more time spent on outdoor play was found in children with low SES families at the 50th quantile [18.0 (6.4; 29.6)] and 75th quantile [30.0 (13.4; 46.6)] on weekdays, but not on weekends. However, the older child may have determined the frequency at which they played outdoors themselves because they have developed the ability of independent mobility, for example, going outside themselves and spending time playing outdoors around their communities (35). A systematic review found that children play outdoors more when there is less traffic, increased neighborhood greenness, and when they have access to a yard (ages 2–15 years) (36). One study found that Canadian children (10.2 ± 1.0 years) living in lower SES areas had a lower level of outdoor play on weekend days compared to their peers from higher SES areas (37). Perhaps high SES neighborhoods may facilitate children to play outside, for example, in their private yards. This could be the reason that children from families with higher SES spent more time on outdoor play in the survey of 10–11 years. Furthermore, we cannot exclude that the social desirability bias affected the responses in some way (38); since the responses of parents from low SES families during the survey of 10–11 years (19.6%) were lower than in the survey of 3–4 years (26.5%).

It is speculated that the time young people spend outdoors had declined dramatically with age because of engaging in screen-based behaviors indoors (25, 39). In this study, data showed that every additional 10 min more of screen time per day was associated with 1 min less of outdoor play. This indicated that the increase in screen time can only be explained by changes in outdoor play to a very limited extent. Although, in our study, we did not observe a dramatic decline in outdoor play with age in other studies. There may be some other reasons for the decreased outdoor play, such as increased academic pressures in school children (40). Furthermore, some researchers concluded that more screen time might be related to a lack of appealing nearby play opportunities or community destinations. One study of the GECKO Drenthe birth cohort found that families living in a supportive neighborhood might have more opportunities to be physically active, and they were more likely to make use of these facilities for themselves and their children (8). In addition, neighborhood outdoor play environments may vary by socioeconomic position, for example, a systematic review summarized that there was a positive association between area-level socioeconomic position and green space (41). Thus, future studies need to identify barriers and facilitators which appeal to children's interests that can help to enhance outdoor time and lower screen time in different socioeconomic areas.

In this study, we used the EHII as a synthetic SES indicator and captured longitudinal variations in socioeconomic inequalities in child outdoor play. This indicated that the EHII seems more sensitive to longitudinal changes in child behaviors, compared to other SES indicators, for example, maternal education (42). Furthermore, the EHII is of potential interest because it is expected to vary over time, capturing changes in SES (19). Moreover, household income can change over time but is difficult to estimate through questionnaires because of non-response. In aiming at reducing the intergenerational transmission of inequalities in socioeconomic resources, the identification of inequalities in health behaviors between different socioeconomic backgrounds in childhood is essential (43). Therefore, a standardized and comparable SES indicator is important for harmonizing comparable socioeconomic positions across populations and time. This is particularly relevant in the context of international collaborative studies (15), for example, combining data in birth cohort studies aiming at identifying risk factors leading to disease across the lifecycle across countries (44). Currently, the availability of the EHII as a marker of SES is increasing in European birth cohort studies, and more studies are needed to examine the availability of the EHII in future (19).

An important strength of this study was that the evidence was based on a birth cohort study, with a good representativeness population for the socioeconomic position (20). This child SES indicator uses multiple domains of SES, and the SES groups can be compared with other collaborative European birth cohort studies. Quantile regression analysis was used, which is an effective statistical method when the health outcomes by proxy report do not follow a normal distribution (45). This means that associations with SES could be studied for different absolute levels of screen time and outdoor play. Parents' rules on screen time and outdoor play were used to adjust for the analysis, since parental regulation may be an important behavioral determinant of a child's outdoor play (46) or screen use (47). A limitation is that due to incomplete data during follow-up, a considerable number of cases could not be included in this analysis. However, the selection bias was small, with a slight bias toward higher-educated families but still more representative of lower SES than in other birth cohorts. In addition, child screen time and outdoor play were reported by parents, and those outcomes may be overestimated or underestimated especially when children were more independent at an older age.

5. Conclusion

In conclusion, this study indicated that the socioeconomic disparities in children's outdoor play and screen behavior may be more obvious with age. Low SES may facilitate both outdoor play (at 3–4 years) and screen time (at 10–11 years). However, children from high SES families develop slightly more favorable behavior patterns with age. Therefore, special attention should be paid to early health interventions for children from low SES families, especially aiming at reducing screen time. Furthermore, the ‘Equivalized Household Income Indicator' as a child SES indicator seems more sensitive to reflect the socioeconomic disparities in changes in children's behaviors with increasing age, compared to maternal education.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and the study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the University Medical Center Groningen (UMCG), in accordance with the 1975 Declaration of Helsinki (as revised in 1983). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

EC, CL, and RW designed the study and were involved in the interpretation of data. CL performed the analysis and drafted the manuscript. All authors were involved in the revision of the manuscript and approved the final version of the submitted manuscript.

Funding

The GECKO Drenthe birth cohort was funded by an unrestricted grant by Hutchison Whampoa Ltd., Hong Kong, and supported by the University of Groningen, Well Baby Clinic Foundation Icare, Noordlease, Pediatric Association of the Netherlands, Youth Preventive Health Care Drenthe, and European Union's Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme (LifeCycle, Grant Agreement No. 733206, 2016). The authors declare that Hutchison Whampoa Ltd. was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the families who took part in the GECKO Drenthe study, to the midwives, gynecologists, nurses, and GPs for their help in the recruitment and measurement of participants as well as to the entire team of the GECKO Drenthe study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2022.1042822/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Hallal PC, Andersen LB, Bull FC, Guthold R, Haskell W, Ekelund U, et al. Global physical activity levels: surveillance progress, pitfalls, and prospects. Lancet. (2012) 380:247–57. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60646-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Janssen X, Martin A, Hughes AR, Hill CM, Kotronoulas G, Hesketh KR. Associations of screen time, sedentary time and physical activity with sleep in under 5s: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. (2020) 49:101226. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2019.101226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association between screen time and children's performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr. (2019) 173:244–50. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Fang K, Mu M, Liu K, He Y. Screen time and childhood overweight/obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Care Health Dev. (2019) 45:744–53. doi: 10.1111/cch.12701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Sijtsma A, Koller M, Sauer PJ, Corpeleijn E. Television, sleep, outdoor play and BMI in young children: the GECKO Drenthe cohort. Eur J Pediatr. (2015) 174:631–9. doi: 10.1007/s00431-014-2443-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. McCurdy LE, Winterbottom KE, Mehta SS, Roberts JR. Using nature and outdoor activity to improve children's health. Curr Probl Pediatr Adolesc Health Care. (2010) 40:102–17. doi: 10.1016/j.cppeds.2010.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fuzeki E, Engeroff T, Banzer W. Health benefits of light-intensity physical activity: a systematic review of accelerometer data of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES). Sports Med. (2017) 47:1769–93. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0724-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lu C, Huang G, Corpeleijn E. Environmental correlates of sedentary time and physical activity in preschool children living in a relatively rural setting in the Netherlands: a cross-sectional analysis of the GECKO Drenthe cohort. BMJ Open. (2019) 9:e027468. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-027468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bassett DR, John D, Conger SA, Fitzhugh EC, Coe DP. Trends in physical activity and sedentary behaviors of United States youth. J Phys Act Health. (2015) 12:1102–11. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2014-0050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Russ SA, Larson K, Franke TM, Halfon N. Associations between media use and health in US children. Acad Pediatr. (2009) 9:300–6. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2009.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. WHO Guidelines on Physical Activity Sedentary Sedentary Behaviour and Sleep for Children Under 5 Years of Age. Geneva: World Health Organization: Geneva: World Health Organization. (2019). Avilable online at: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664 (accessed December 28, 2022).

Google Scholar

12. Chaput JP, Willumsen J, Bull F, Chou R, Ekelund U, Firth J, et al. 2020 WHO guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour for children and adolescents aged 5-17 years: summary of the evidence. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2020) 17:141. doi: 10.1186/s12966-020-01037-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Chaput JP, Carson V, Gray CE, Tremblay MS. Importance of all movement behaviors in a 24 hour period for overall health. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2014) 11:12575–81. doi: 10.3390/ijerph111212575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Sallis JF, Cervero RB, Ascher W, Henderson KA, Kraft MK, Kerr J. An ecological approach to creating active living communities. Annu Rev Public Health. (2006) 27:297–322. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.27.021405.102100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Canova C, Cantarutti A. Population-Based Birth Cohort Studies in Epidemiology. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:5276. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17155276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Musić Milanović S, Buoncristiano M, KriŽan H, Rathmes G, Williams J, Hyska J, et al. Socioeconomic disparities in physical activity, sedentary behavior and sleep patterns among 6- to 9-year-old children from 24 countries in the WHO European region. Obes Rev. (2021) 22 Suppl 6:e13209. doi: 10.1111/obr.13209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Cheng TL, Goodman E. Committee on Pediatric Research. Race, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status in research on child health. Pediatrics. (2015) 135:e225–37. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-3109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Braveman PA, Cubbin C, Egerter S, Chideya S, Marchi KS, Metzler M, et al. Socioeconomic status in health research: one size does not fit all. JAMA. (2005) 294:2879–88. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.22.2879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Pizzi C, Richiardi M, Charles MA, Heude B, Lanoe JL, Lioret S, et al. Measuring child socio-economic position in birth cohort research: the development of a novel standardized household income indicator. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:1700. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. L'Abee C, Sauer PJ, Damen M, Rake JP, Cats H, Stolk RP. Cohort Profile: the GECKO Drenthe study, overweight programming during early childhood. Int J Epidemiol. (2008) 37:486–9. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Cole TJ, Lobstein T. Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut-offs for thinness, overweight and obesity. Pediatr Obes. (2012) 7:284–94. doi: 10.1111/j.2047-6310.2012.00064.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Arora VS, Karanikolos M, Clair A, Reeves A, Stuckler D, McKee M. Data Resource Profile: The European Union Statistics on Income and Living Conditions (EU-SILC). Int J Epidemiol. (2015) 44:451–61. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wiersma R, Rijnks RH, Bocca G, Boezen HM, Hartman E, Corpeleijn E. Regional variation in lifestyle patterns and BMI in young children: the GECKO Drenthe cohort. Int J Health Geogr. (2022) 21:7. doi: 10.1186/s12942-022-00302-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Aarts MJ, de Vries SI, van Oers HA, Schuit AJ. Outdoor play among children in relation to neighborhood characteristics: a cross-sectional neighborhood observation study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2012) 9:98. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-9-98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. LeBlanc AG, Katzmarzyk PT, Barreira TV, Broyles ST, Chaput JP, Church TS, et al. Correlates of total sedentary time and screen time in 9-11 year-old children around the world: the international study of childhood obesity, lifestyle and the environment. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0129622. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Stiglic N, Viner RM. Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and adolescents: a systematic review of reviews. BMJ Open. (2019) 9:e023191 doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Kininmonth AR, Smith AD, Llewellyn CH, Dye L, Lawton CL, Fildes A. The relationship between the home environment and child adiposity: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2021) 18:4. doi: 10.1186/s12966-020-01073-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Fleury G, Masís-Vargas A, Kalsbeek A. Metabolic implications of exposure to light at night: lessons from animal and human studies. Obesity. (2020) 28 (Suppl 1):S18–28. doi: 10.1002/oby.22807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Trinh MH, Sundaram R, Robinson SL, Lin TC, Bell EM, Ghassabian A, et al. Association of trajectory and covariates of children's screen media time. JAMA Pediatr. (2019) 174:71–8. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Downing KL, Hnatiuk JA, Hinkley T, Salmon J, Hesketh KD. Interventions to reduce sedentary behaviour in 0-5-year-olds: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Br J Sports Med. (2018) 52:314–21. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Tandon PS, Zhou C, Sallis JF, Cain KL, Frank LD, Saelens BE. Home environment relationships with children's physical activity, sedentary time, and screen time by socioeconomic status. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2012) 9:88. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-9-88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Burdette HL, Whitaker RC. Resurrecting free play in young children: looking beyond fitness and fatness to attention, affiliation, and affect. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2005) 159:46–50. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.1.46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Tremblay MS, Gray C, Babcock S, Barnes J, Bradstreet CC, Carr D, et al. Position Statement on Active Outdoor Play. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2015) 12:6475–505. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120606475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Wijtzes AI, Jansen W, Bouthoorn SH, Pot N, Hofman A, Jaddoe VW, et al. Social inequalities in young children's sports participation and outdoor play. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2014) 11:155. doi: 10.1186/s12966-014-0155-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Loebach J, Sanches M, Jaffe J, Elton-Marshall T. Paving the way for outdoor play: examining socio-environmental barriers to community-based outdoor play. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:3617. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18073617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Lambert A, Vlaar J, Herrington S, Brussoni M. What is the relationship between the neighbourhood built environment and time spent in outdoor play? A systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:3840. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16203840

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Delisle Nyström C, Barnes JD, Blanchette S, Faulkner G, Leduc G, Riazi NA, et al. Relationships between area-level socioeconomic status and urbanization with active transportation, independent mobility, outdoor time, and physical activity among Canadian children. BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:1082. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7420-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Sugianto R, Toh JY, Wong SF, Tint MT, Colega MT, Lee YS, et al. Dietary Patterns of Five-Year-Old Children and Their Correlates: Findings from a Multi-Ethnic Asian Cohort. Br J Nutr. (2021) 127:763–72. doi: 10.1017/S0007114521001434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Larson LR, Szczytko R, Bowers EP, Stephens LE, Stevenson KT, Floyd MF. Outdoor time, screen time, and connection to nature: troubling trends among rural youth? Environ Behav. (2019) 51:966–91. doi: 10.1177/0013916518806686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. van Schalkwijk FJ, Blessinga AN, Willemen AM, Van Der Werf YD, Schuengel C. Social support moderates the effects of stress on sleep in adolescents. J Sleep Res. (2015) 24:407–13. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Jacobs J, Alston L, Needham C, Backholer K, Strugnell C, Allender S, et al. Variation in the physical activity environment according to area-level socio-economic position-a systematic review. Obes Rev. (2019) 20:686–700. doi: 10.1111/obr.12818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Ball K, Cleland VJ, Timperio AF, Salmon J, Crawford DA. Socioeconomic position and children's physical activity and sedentary behaviors: longitudinal findings from the CLAN study. J Phys Act Health. (2009) 6:289–98. doi: 10.1123/jpah.6.3.289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Kim P, Evans GW, Chen E, Miller G, Seeman T. How Socioeconomic Disadvantages Get Under the Skin and into the Brain to Influence Health Development Across the Lifespan. In: Halfon N, Forrest CB, Lerner RM, Faustman EM, editors. Handbook of Life Course Health Development. Cham: Springe. (2018). p. 463–97.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

44. Jaddoe VWV, Felix JF, Andersen AN, Charles MA, Chatzi L, Corpeleijn E, et al. The LifeCycle Project-EU Child Cohort Network: a federated analysis infrastructure and harmonized data of more than 250,000 children and parents. Eur J Epidemiol. (2020) 35:709–24. doi: 10.1007/s10654-020-00662-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Staffa SJ, Kohane DS, Zurakowski D. Quantile regression and its applications: a primer for anesthesiologists. Anesth Analg. (2019) 128:820–30. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000004017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Puggina A, Aleksovska K, Buck C, Burns C, Cardon G, Carlin A, et al. Policy determinants of physical activity across the life course: a 'DEDIPAC' umbrella systematic literature review. Eur J Public Health. (2018) 28:105–18. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckx174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Totland TH, Bjelland M, Lien N, Bergh IH, Gebremariam MK, Grydeland M, et al. Adolescents' prospective screen time by gender and parental education, the mediation of parental influences. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2013) 10:89. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-89

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: socioeconomic inequalities in children, Equivalized Household Income Indicator, screen time (ST), outdoor play, birth cohort study

Citation: Lu C, Wiersma R and Corpeleijn E (2023) The association among SES, screen time, and outdoor play in children at different ages: The GECKO Drenthe study. Front. Public Health 10:1042822. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.1042822

Received: 13 September 2022; Accepted: 15 December 2022;
Published: 10 January 2023.

Edited by:

Jilei Wu, Population Research Institute, Peking University, China

Reviewed by:

Shirley Wyver, Macquarie University, Australia
J. Bruce Morton, Western University, Canada

Copyright © 2023 Lu, Wiersma and Corpeleijn. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Congchao Lu, yes lvcongchao@tmu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.