Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 08 June 2021
Sec. Public Health Education and Promotion
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights in Public Health Education and Promotion: 2021 View all 39 articles

Identification of Demographic Variables Influencing Dementia Literacy and Risk Perception Through a Global Survey

  • 1Exercise Mobility and Brain Health Lab, Department of Neuroscience, Western University, London, ON, Canada
  • 2Exercise Mobility and Brain Health Lab, School of Kinesiology, Western University, London, ON, Canada

Dementia literacy is important for risk mitigation and preventative strategies before disease onset. The aim of our study was to investigate dementia literacy and how demographic characteristics influence these perceptions in order to provide evidence for how dementia-centered public health initiatives should structure their focus. We conducted a globally administered online survey, through Amazon Mechanical Turk (mTurk). Survey items evaluated: (1) personal perception on the preventability of dementia, and (2) risk awareness of lifestyle factors. Differences in risk scoring between the 598 respondents were compared using Kruskal-Wallis testing factored by demographic categorizations. Most of the sample demonstrated understanding that lifestyle factors contribute some risk toward dementia, though these risk scores were generally low. Differences in risk scoring varied by demographic characteristics. Women, older adults, those with non-post-secondary attainment, below average income, and White background tended to report lower risk scores. Public health education and initiatives for dementia prevention should focus on lifestyle risk factors, in addition to considering the barriers related to the demographic factors identified that may prevent populations from accessing programs and information.

Introduction

Nearly 50 million people worldwide are living with some form of dementia, with 10 million new cases occurring each year (1). Despite the widespread impact of dementia, public awareness surrounding dementia continues to demonstrate large gaps, especially for public knowledge concerning the prevention and risk reduction of dementia. A recent systematic review indicated that nearly 50% of the general population, from various countries surveyed, viewed dementia as a normal part of aging and a non-preventable part of living (2). Similarly, in the Netherlands (3) and United States (4) individuals demonstrated difficulty in identifying risk factors related to dementia. Despite the general populations' beliefs, dementia is not a normal part of aging and certain factors have been linked to increasing dementia risk. It is evident that dementia literacy in the general population continues to be a critical area for improvement for public health education.

Dementia literacy, one's beliefs regarding dementia and application of beliefs, is an important public health concept for risk mitigation and preventative strategies before disease onset (5). The improvement of dementia literacy can aid individuals in both identifying risk factors related to dementia, but also increase recognition that many risk factors are also modifiable. Improving dementia literacy for modifiable risk factors, such as those related to lifestyle (e.g., physical activity, diet, mental health) can be a critical tool in encouraging individuals to change their behaviors in order to decrease the risk of dementia or slow disease progression (6, 7).

Evaluations of dementia beliefs in the general population have begun to identify where these literacy shortfalls lie. However, dementia beliefs may be influenced by a variety of factors such as education, ethnicity, income, and sex. Identifying where the general public's dementia beliefs falters, and how demographic characteristics further influence these opinions, can better inform the content and audience that public health initiatives should target for improved dementia literacy, especially regarding modifiable risk factors.

The aim of our study was to investigate perceptions surrounding risk factors and beliefs related to dementia. Specifically, we aimed to identify the underlying demographics that influence these perceptions in order to provide evidence for how dementia-centered public health initiatives should structure their focus in order to improve dementia literacy. Therefore, we conducted a globally administered online survey in order to evaluate these perceptions on a large scale across varying demographic profiles.

Methods

An online survey was conducted through Amazon Mechanical Turk (mTurk), allowing English-speaking mTurk users aged 18 years and older to participate. The use of mTurk has been shown to be a valid platform for collecting diverse demographic respondents (8, 9) and reliable data compared to lab-based studies (10). Prior to the survey, users were provided with an online letter of information and consent. Those that consented were directed to the questionnaire hosted via the online survey software Qualtrics. All procedures of this study were approved by the Non-Medical Research Ethics Board at Western University, ID #113040.

Questions to assess dementia literacy were developed based on information from the Alzheimer Society of Canada website (11) and previous dementia literacy surveys (12, 13). Survey items evaluated: (1) personal perception on the preventability of dementia, and (2) risk awareness of lifestyle factors that have the potential to increase dementia risk. Participants were asked to rate, on a scale of 0 to 100%, their perception of dementia preventability based on the statement, “to what extent do you believe that Alzheimer's disease and dementia are preventable?.” Additionally, each potential risk factor was individually scored on a seven-point scale for its possibility of increasing dementia risk. Values ranged from one, representing “no risk,” to seven, representing “high risk.” We included 14 risk factors for evaluation: 11 of which are strongly supported as factors for increasing dementia risk (old age, genetics, physical inactivity, unhealthy diet, stress, heart disease, head injury, mental illness, high alcohol consumption, smoking, and social isolation). In addition, we included two factors that have limited supported associations to dementia risk (exposure to toxins, and viral/bacterial infections), and one non-empirical factor (God's will).

Socio-demographic variables of sex, age, education, and income were also included in the questionnaire in order to assess their relationships to dementia literacy. Age demographics were stratified into young adults (18–39 years old), middle adulthood (40–59 years old), and older adults (60+ years old). Education was obtained by report of highest level of education completed and then categorized into post-secondary education (completion of bachelors or graduate degree), vocational education (completion of trade's apprenticeship or college diploma), and high school education (no post-secondary education completed of any kind). Income level categorization was determined by self-report as to whether individuals earned below, at, or above their country of residence's national average.

Statistical Analysis

A two-tailed independent sample t-test bootstrapped at 1,000 permutations was conducted to examine sex differences in ratings of perception of dementia preventability. Additionally, one-way ANOVAs were conducted to evaluate the differences within age group, level of education, income, ethnicity, and country corrected for multiple comparisons using Tukey. To assess differences in risk scoring for the 11empirically supported risk factors, Kruskal-Wallis tests were conducted factored by demographic categorizations. Initial Kruskal-Wallis testing p-threshold was evaluated at p < 0.005 to account for repeated testing of the eleven empirically supported risk factors being evaluated. Post-hoc testing was also corrected for multiple comparisons using the same Bonferroni method as the omnibus testing. All analyses were conducted in SPSS Statistics (ver 24, 2019).

Results

In total, 617 survey responses were collected which were further assessed for quality of responses. Six were excluded due to participants self-reporting at the end of the questionnaire that they did not feel they answered questions accurately. An additional five were excluded due to randomness of responses, verified by “failed catch trials” or incongruent responses and independently identified by the authors (B.R.H., J.A.F., and M.Y.S.W.). A further eight were excluded due to more than two demographic variables being unreported by the participant, as decided upon as exclusion criteria prior to data collection. Thus, a total of 598 responses were included in our analysis.

Demographics

Demographic information for our sample is reported in Table 1. Of the total sample (n = 598), 388 respondents were classified as young adults, 149 as middle aged, 31 as older adults, and 30 declining to provide their age. The mean reported age was 39.2 (± 11.5) years old, and over half of our sample (60.7%, n = 362) reported their biological sex as male. Most of our sample was categorized as completing post-secondary education (81.4%, n = 487) or vocational education (4.7%, n = 28). Reported level of income indicated 29.4% (n = 176) reporting above average income, 44.6% (n = 267) at average income, and 22.9% (n = 137) reporting below average income. Reported ethnicity was 53.2% (n = 318) White and 45.7% (n = 268) Black, Indigenous or Person of Color (BIPOC). The sample was further characterized by 65.7% (n = 393) residing in the USA.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of sample.

Belief of Dementia Prevention

Almost all participants reported dementia as somewhat preventable, with average preventability score being 46.6% (±27.5%). Only 5% of the respondents indicated that dementia is completely unpreventable; comparably, 1.5% of the respondents indicated that they believed dementia is 100% preventable. Demographic subgroups demonstrated differences in their beliefs in the preventability of dementia (Figure 1). On average, women reported that dementia was less preventable compared to men (t(596) = 5.676, p = 0.001). White individuals reported dementia as less preventable than BIPOC individuals (t(584) = 6.841, p < 0.001). Omnibus testing for age (F(2, 565) = 10.884, p < 0.001), education level (F(2, 594) = 8.122, p < 0.001), income (F(2, 577) = 11.120, p < 0.001), and country of residence (F(2, 595) = 36.330, p < 0.001) all returned as significant. Post-hoc comparisons indicated younger adults (M = 49.6, SD = 26.6) believed dementia to be more preventable than middle aged adults (M = 38.15, SD = 25.7, p < 0.001). Respondents with post-secondary education (M = 48.7, SD = 27.4) believed dementia to be more preventable than both vocational education (M = 35.3, SD = 23.3, p = 0.030) and high school education (M = 37.9, SD = 27.1, p = 0.003). Those with below average income (M = 38.7, SD = 27.8) believed dementia was less preventable than what was reported by those with both average (M = 49.5, SD = 26.4, p < 0.001) and above average income (M = 51.2, SD = 27.5, p < 0.001). Respondents from India (M = 60.7, SD = 21.5) believed dementia to be more preventable than both residents of the USA (M = 41.0, SD = 27.4, p < 0.001) and other countries (M = 38.69, SD = 31.2, p < 0.001).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Belief ratings (0–100%) based on question “to what extent do you believe that Alzheimer's disease and dementia are preventable,” responses displayed by demographic subgroups. *Indicates p-value < 0.05, **indicates p-value < 0.01, ***indicates p-value < 0.001. Education was self-reported and categorized as follows: ahigh school education (some high school, high school diploma), vocational education (trade school or college diploma) and post-secondary education (bachelor's or graduate degree). bEthnicity was self-reported and categorized as follows: White and Black, Indigenous or Person of Color (BIPOC). cOther countries included: Brazil, Canada, France, Germany, Italy, Nigeria, Portugal, and Vietnam.

General Population Dementia Risk Factor Beliefs

Of the entire sample, 63.9% were correctly able to identify all 11 empirically supported factors as having at least some risk for increasing dementia risk, while 9.1% were able to identify between one and five of the supported risk factors, and only 0.8% reported “no risk” for all 11 of the supported factors. Table 2 displays the relative frequencies of scoring for each of the 14 factors evaluated. Of the 11 supported risk factors, heart disease and smoking were the most frequently scored as having “no risk” (23.2 and 16.6%, respectively), while comparatively, old age and genetics were the least common to be reported as no risk (3.0 and 3.8%, respectively). The average score for each of the 11 supported risk factors is reported in Table 4. Notably, average risk score did not exceed 4.7, representing moderate risk, for any of these risk factors. Heart disease reported the lowest average score at 3.3, representing low risk.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Frequency of risk scoring for the 14 potential risk factors for dementia.

Demographic Differences in Scoring of Risk Perception

After correction for multiple comparisons, all 11empirically supported risk factors demonstrated at least one relationship with a demographic characteristic (Table 3). Factors that were more frequently scored as low or no risk demonstrated more demographic relationships than those that were scored as high risk. Specific scoring differences for the 11 empirically supported factors are reported below by demographic category, with mean risk score reported in Table 4.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Scoring distribution relationships with demographic variables using Kruskal-Wallis omnibus testing.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mean and standard deviations per demographic variable for risk score of the 11 empirically supported risk factor for increasing dementia risk.

Age

Scoring for the risk factors of physical inactivity (H (2) = 13.890, p = 0.001), social isolation (H (2) = 11.476, p = 0.003), alcohol (H (2) = 11.949, p < 0.001), mental illness (H (2) = 20.587, p < 0.001), smoking (H (2) = 15.940, p < 0.001), and heart disease (H (2) = 17.744, p < 0.001) were significantly related with age. Significant rank differences were seen post-hoc between younger and middle aged adults for physical inactivity (p = 0.002), social isolation (p = 0.001), alcohol (p = 0.003), mental illness (p < 0.001), smoking (p < 0.001), and heart disease (p < 0.001), where the young adult group ranked these factors as higher risk for dementia. The remaining risk factors of old age, genetics, head injury, stress, and unhealthy diet were not observed to have a significant relationship with age after adjusting for multiple testing (p > 0.005).

Sex

Male respondents were found to indicate higher risk of dementia for head injury (H (1) = 8.196, p = 0.004), stress (H (1) = 15.442, p < 0.001), social isolation (H (1) = 9.687, p < 0.001), alcohol (H (1) = 12.269, p <0.001), mental illness (H (1) = 19.675, p < 0.001), smoking (H (1) = 16.229, p < 0.001), and heart disease (H (1) = 17.345, p < 0.001) compared to female respondents. The remaining supported risk factors of old age, genetics, unhealthy diet, and physical inactivity were not observed to have a significant relationship with sex after adjusting for multiple testing (p > 0.005).

Education

Scoring for the risk factors of physical inactivity (H(2) = 11.476, p = 0.003), social isolation (H (2) = 11.844, p = 0.003), alcohol (H(2) = 18.571, p < 0.001), mental illness (H (2) = 16.531, p = 0.001), smoking (H (2) = 31.028, p < 0.001), and heart disease (H (2) = 20.289, p < 0.001) were significantly related with education. Significant rank differences, after adjustment for multiple comparisons, were seen post-hoc between high school education and post-secondary education for physical inactivity (p = 0.003), social isolation (p = 0.004), mental illness (p = 0.001), smoking (p < 0.001), and heart disease (p < 0.001), as well as between high school education and vocational education groups for alcohol (p < 0.001) and smoking (p = 0.023). Specifically, those with high school education attainment ranked the above factors as having lower risk for dementia. Only heart disease was found post-hoc to have rank differences between vocational education and post-secondary education groups with vocational education scoring lower (p = 0.001). Old age, genetics, head injury, stress, and unhealthy diet were not significantly related to education (p > 0.005).

Income

Scoring for the risk factors of social isolation (H (2) = 12.200, p = 0.002), mental illness (H (2) = 11.528, p < 0.001), smoking (H (2) = 14.114, p < 0.001), and heart disease (H (2) = 20.289, p < 0.001) were observed to have a relationship with income. Those with lower income ranked the above factors as having lower risk for dementia in comparison to greater income earners. Significant rank differences were seen post-hoc between below average income and above average income for social isolation (p = 0.002), mental illness (p = 0.014), smoking (p = 0.001), and heart disease (p < 0.001), as well as, between below average income and average income for social isolation (p = 0.048), mental illness (p = 0.007), smoking (p = 0.035), and heart disease (p = 0.045). Further, in the post-hoc comparison for heart disease, average income earners reported lower risk scores compared to above average income earners (p = 0.045). Risk scores for old age, genetics, head injury, unhealthy diet, stress, physical inactivity, and alcohol were not significantly related to income after multiple testing corrections (p > 0.005).

Ethnicity

Risk rankings for all supported risk factors were observed to have a relationship with ethnicity. Head injury (H (1) = 8.070, p = 0.004), unhealthy diet (H (1) = 8.397, p = 0.004), stress (H (1) = 25.959, p < 0.001) physical inactivity (H (1) = 16.871, p < 0.001), social isolation (H (1) = 15.490, p < .001), alcohol (H (1) = 31.923, p < 0.001), mental illness (H (1) = 44.062, p < 0.001), smoking (H (1) = 29.832, p < 0.001), and heart disease (H (1) = 47.930, p < 0.001) were ranked as higher risk factors for dementia by BIPOC than Whites. Comparatively, old age (H (1) = 8.070, p = 0.004) and genetics were ranked as higher risk factors for dementia by Whites than BIPOC.

Country

Risk rankings for unhealthy diet (H (1) = 23.916, p < 0.001), stress (H (2) = 33.042, p < 0.001), physical inactivity (H (2) = 36.334, p <0.001), social isolation (H (2) = 27.848, p <0.001), alcohol (H (2) = 39.171, p < 0.001), mental illness (H (1) = 35.882, p < 0.001), smoking (H (2) = 46.294, p < 0.001), and heart disease (H (2) = 65.152, p < 0.001) were observed to have a relationship with country of residence. Follow-up post-hoc comparisons for the omnibus testing, after adjusting for multiple comparisons, showed that respondents from India ranked unhealthy diet, stress, physical inactivity, social isolation, smoking, mental illness, alcohol, and heart disease, with higher risk scores when compared to those from the United States (all comparisons p < 0.001). India compared to other countries indicated significantly higher risk scoring for social isolation (p = 0.004), mental illness (p = 0.020), alcohol (p < 0.001), heart disease (p < 0.001), physical inactivity, smoking, and unhealthy diet (p = 0.001). No significant differences were found for the comparison of the United States to other countries.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to identify whether underlying demographic factors influenced dementia literacy and perceptions of risk factors related to dementia. Our survey captured the responses from 598 individuals of varying age, gender, education, income, ethnicity, and country of residence. Our results indicated that as an aggregated group the understanding that dementia is a preventable disease was high, but differences in the strength of this belief varied by demographic characteristics.

Overall, women reported lower belief ratings that dementia is a preventable disease compared to men. This result may be reflective of the historic narrative that women are at greater risk for dementia than men. However, this narrative has been challenged by large-cohort studies that have found no differences in the incidence of dementia before the age of 80 between sexes (1416). Continued dissemination of information to the public that reflects dementia is a disease that affects both men and women is a priority. Furthermore, differences in risk scoring for modifiable lifestyle factors were found for sex. Men reported greater risk scores than women for the modifiable risk factors of head injury, social isolation, alcohol, mental illness, smoking, and heart disease, but no differences in risk scores were found for the other modifiable risk factors. It is unclear why these differences in perception in risk scoring may exist in the general population between sexes, as even within the scientific community little consensus has been drawn whether certain lifestyle factors carry more risk for a specific sex compared to another. This gap in the literature surrounding dementia risk is largely due to the fact that studies that examine risk factors for dementia often adjust for sex during analysis, even though many scenarios exist in where sex and gender differences may affect risk (17, 18). Arguably, the differences in risk perception within our sample may be reflective of higher educational attainment of the men in the sample. However, this interaction was not explored within our analysis, nor can higher education be definitively confirmed as the reason for scoring differences, though it may be a contributing factor.

Our study also found that, in regard to specific factors related to increasing dementia risk, most of the sample was accurately able to recognize multiple empirically supported factors as having some contribution toward increasing dementia risk. This finding is in line with a previous review in which results suggested that over the past decade, the general public has increasingly become more informed and aware of risk factors related to increasing dementia risk (2, 13). In our sample specifically, old age and genetics were risk factors that the sample most frequently scored as having some associated risk for increasing dementia risk. Comparatively, associated risk scores were generally scored low indicating room for improvement, especially regarding perceptions of modifiable lifestyle risk factors.

Of the modifiable risk factors we included in our survey, heart disease was the most frequently scored factor as having no risk for increasing dementia, with 23.2% of the sample selecting the no risk scoring option and an additional 28.3% scoring heart disease as low risk. Smoking was similarly scored, with 16.6% of the sample selecting no risk for increasing dementia risk and 32.8% scoring smoking as low risk. Critically, both heart disease and smoking have increasingly been shown to be modifiable lifestyle factors responsible for increasing dementia risk (19, 20). Yet, there appears to be a disconnect between the public perception of heart disease and smoking as risk factors for dementia and what has been empirically supported. A similar disconnected relationship between perception of risk and empirical evidence existed in our sample for the other modifiable risk factors, where 38–46% of the sample used the no risk or low risk scoring option, depending on the risk factor being evaluated. Continued education surrounding the risks of modifiable lifestyle factors related to dementia should continue to be a priority for the general population. Additionally, further investigation into how various beliefs compare to specific metrics of population attributable risk or hazard ratios could provide more targeted evidence toward which factors to address for specific demographic characteristics. Notably, this is an important facet to consider since different risk factors will have various levels of influence on individual risk vs. a population-wide effect.

Our results indicate that certain demographic characteristics influence the scoring of how much risk individuals associate with modifiable risk factors. We found that younger adults scored the empirically supported modifiable risk factors, such as physical inactivity, social isolation, alcohol, mental illness, smoking, and heart disease, as higher risk for dementia compared to the older adult age groups. The fact that younger adults recognize these modifiable factors as higher risk is advantageous from a public health perspective, as implementation of lifestyle changes to decrease dementia risk should occur as early as possible. Dementia education targeting young and middle-aged adults should be prioritized for the prevention of dementia, as it is becoming increasingly recognized that the disease often starts 10 years before the onset of clinical symptoms (21).

Examining the scoring differences within education as well as income demographics indicated that varying levels of educational attainment had differing risk scoring for several modifiable risk factors. These differences mainly occurred between those with high school education and those with post-secondary education. However, for the overall lowest scored risk factors of heart disease and smoking differences in scoring were observed between all three groups. Similar relationships were also observed for level of income. Understandably from a socioeconomic perspective, individuals with higher levels of educational attainment and income tend to have access to resources and materials that may influence how they perceive risk factors associated with dementia. Nevertheless, it should be noted that within our sample even those who reported higher levels of educational attainment and income still scored many of the modifiable risk factors in the low to moderate range, contrary to level of risk that has been reported in empirical studies (6). Therefore, increasing risk perception awareness of modifiable risk factors appears to be a need for all levels of education and income, but specific attention should continue to be focused on individuals with lower incomes and education.

When comparing ethnicities, BIPOC participants scored all modifiable risk factors higher than White participants. Additionally, BIPOC participants also reported greater preventability beliefs than Whites, potentially indicating that there is an understanding of the link between modifiable risk factors and dementia preventability (22). Furthermore, country of residence demonstrated differences in risk scoring for modifiable lifestyle factors, except for head injury. Dementia continues to be a global issue, yet the urgency and prioritization of dementia education varies between countries (23). As the global prevalence of dementia continues to grow, public health education targeted toward modifiable risk factors should continue to be a priority, especially for low-to-middle-income countries where both life expectancy and cardiovascular risk factors are increasing rapidly (24).

Our study, although large in scale and including global responses, is not without limitations. First of which, our sample consisted mainly of White, American individuals who had high levels of educational attainment. A larger data set may be useful in capturing a larger representation of ethnicities and countries of residence that tend to have limited evaluations in regard to dementia perceptions (2). Furthermore, the respondent pool may also be limited to those who read and comprehend English, and those who are comfortable using computer technologies and the mTurk platform. However, evidence has shown that responses via mTurk are comparable to those done in a lab not via computer interface (25). Additionally, the demographic characteristics evaluated within this study were done so in isolation from each other. The interactions between these demographics and how they are interrelated should also be considered, especially when creating and implementing public health education strategies.

The results of our survey indicate that various demographic characteristics, such as, sex, age cohort, education, income, ethnicity and country of residence are related to how individuals perceive dementia as a preventable disease and how individuals perceive associated risk to various established risk factors of dementia. While specific demographic factors have been identified related to risk perception, it is also clear that, regardless of demographic variables, modifiable risk factors are areas that need continued education. Public health education and initiatives for dementia prevention should focus on modifiable risk factors, especially cardiovascular related elements, in addition to considering the barriers related to demographic factors that may prevent populations from accessing programs and information. Future research should investigate the relationship between risk factor literacy, participation in risk reducing behavior, and perceived barriers to participating in risk reducing behaviors.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Western Research Ethics Board—Non-Medical Research Ethics Board. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

LN conceived the study and was in charge of overall direction and planning. BRH was the lead on importing survey questions to Qualtrics, creating mTurk work task, analyzed the data with input from LN, and wrote the manuscript with input from all authors. All authors collaborated in the design and creation of survey content.

Funding

This work was supported by Western University to LN.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. World Health Organization. Dementia. Dementia: Key Facts. (2019). Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/dementia#:~:text=Dementia is a syndrome in,million new cases every year (accessed August 27, 2021).

2. Cations M, Radisic G, Crotty M, Laver KE. What does the general public understand about prevention and treatment of dementia? A systematic review of population-based surveys. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0196085. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0196085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Heger I, Deckers K, Van Boxtel M, De Vugt M, Hajema K, Verhey F, et al. Dementia awareness and risk perception in middle-aged and older individuals: Baseline results of the MijnBreincoach survey on the association between lifestyle and brain health. BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:678. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7010-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Maust DT, Solway E, Langa KM, Kullgren JT, Kirch M, Singer DC, et al. Perception of dementia risk and preventive actions among US adults aged 50 to 64 years. JAMA Neurol. (2020) 77:259–62. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.3946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Low LF, Anstey KJ. Dementia literacy: recognition and beliefs on dementia of the Australian public. Alzheimer's Dement. (2009) 5:43–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2008.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Patterson C, Feightner J, Garcia A, MacKnight C. General risk factors for dementia: a systematic evidence review. Alzheimer's Dement. (2007) 3:341–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bellou V, Belbasis L, Tzoulaki I, Middleton LT, Ioannidis JPA, Evangelou E. Systematic evaluation of the associations between environmental risk factors and dementia: an umbrella review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Alzheimer's Dement. (2017) 13:406–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.07.152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Huff C, Tingley D. “Who are these people? ” Evaluating the demographic characteristics and political preferences of MTurk survey respondents. Res Polit. (2015) 2. doi: 10.1177/2053168015604648

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Difallah D, Filatova E, Ipeirotis P. Demographics and dynamics of Mechanical Turk workers. In: WSDM '18: Proceedings of the Eleventh ACM International Conference on Web Search and Data Mining. Marina Del Ray, CA (2018).

Google Scholar

10. Thomas KA, Clifford S. Validity and mechanical turk: an assessment- of exclusion methods and interactive experiments. Comp Hum Behav. (2017) 77:184–97 doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Alzheimer Society of Canada. Risk factors. (2020). Available online at: https://alzheimer.ca/en/Home/About-dementia/Alzheimer-s-disease/Risk-factors (accessed July 7, 2020).

Google Scholar

12. Price AE, Corwin SJ, Friedman DB, Laditka SB, Colabianchi N, Montgomery KM. Older adults' perceptions of physical activity and cognitive health: Implications for health communication. Heal Educ Behav. (2011) 38:15–24. doi: 10.1177/1090198110369764

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Friedman DB, Becofsky K, Anderson LA, Bryant LL, Hunter RH, Ivey SL, et al. Public perceptions about risk and protective factors for cognitive health and impairment: a review of the literature. Int Psychogeriatr. (2015) 27:1263–75. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214002877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Chen P, Belle S, DeKosky ST. Ten-year incidence of dementia in a rural elderly US community population. The MoVIES project. Neurology. (2000). 54:1109–16. doi: 10.1212/WNL.54.5.1109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Edland SD, Rocca WA, Petersen RC, Cha RH, Kokmen E. Dementia and Alzheimer disease incidence rates do not vary by sex in Rochester, Minn. Arch Neurol. (2002) 59:1589–93. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.10.1589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Chêne G, Beiser A, Au R, Preis SR, Wolf PA, Dufouil C, et al. Gender and incidence of dementia in the Framingham Heart Study from mid-adult life. Alzheimer's Dement. (2015) 11:310–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Mielke MM. Sex and gender differences in Alzheimer's disease dementia. Psychiatr Times. (2018) 35:14–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

18. Mielke MM, Vemuri P, Rocca WA. Clinical epidemiology of Alzheimer's disease: assessing sex and gender differences. Clin Epidemiol. (2014) 6:37–48. doi: 10.2147/CLEP.S37929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Santos CY, Snyder PJ, Wu WC, Zhang M, Echeverria A, Alber J. Pathophysiologic relationship between Alzheimer's disease, cerebrovascular disease, and cardiovascular risk: a review and synthesis. Alzheimer Dement. (2017) 7:69–87. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Deal JA, Power MC, Palta P, Alonso A, Schneider ALC, Perryman K, et al. Relationship of cigarette smoking and time of quitting with incident dementia and cognitive decline. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2020) 68:337–45. doi: 10.1111/jgs.16228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Beason-Held LL, Goh JO, An Y, Kraut MA, O'Brien RJ, Ferrucci L, et al. Changes in brain function occur years before the onset of cognitive impairment. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:18008–14. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1402-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Mayeda ER, Glymour MM, Quesenberry CP, Whitmer RA. Inequalities in dementia incidence between six racial and ethnic groups over 14 years. Alzheimer Dement. (2016) 12:216–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. World Health Organization and Alzheimer's Disease International. Dementia: a public health priority. (2012). Available online at: https://www.who.int/mental_health/publications/dementia_report_2012/en/ (accessed August 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

24. Satizabal CL, Beiser AS, Chouraki V, Chêne G, Dufouil C, Seshadri S. Incidence of Dementia over three decades in the framingham heart study. N Engl J Med. (2016) 374:523–32. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1504327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Casler K, Bickel L, Hackett E. Separate but equal? A comparison of participants and data gathered via Amazon's MTurk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing. Comput Hum Behav. (2013) 29:2156–60. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dementia literacy, risk factors, demographics, public health, global

Citation: Horst BR, Furlano JA, Wong MYS, Ford SD, Han BB and Nagamatsu LS (2021) Identification of Demographic Variables Influencing Dementia Literacy and Risk Perception Through a Global Survey. Front. Public Health 9:660600. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.660600

Received: 29 January 2021; Accepted: 11 May 2021;
Published: 08 June 2021.

Edited by:

Sunjoo Kang, Yonsei University, South Korea

Reviewed by:

Moonju Lee, The University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, United States
James C. Vickers, University of Tasmania, Australia

Copyright © 2021 Horst, Furlano, Wong, Ford, Han and Nagamatsu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lindsay S. Nagamatsu, lindsay.nagamatsu@uwo.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.