Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 03 March 2021
Sec. Children and Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Maternal-Perinatal Risk and Children-Adolescent Health View all 17 articles

Feeding Habits in the Cultural Domains of Child Care: Elements for Health Promotion

  • 1Academic Group of Public Health and Epidemiology, Institute of Scientific Research, Juárez University of the State of Durango, Durango, Mexico
  • 2Department of Public Health, University Center for Health Sciences, University of Guadalajara, Guadalajara, Mexico

Introduction: Family eating behavior is determined by the meaning that the caretaker gives to food and the act of eating in the domestic environment, as well as the beliefs and perceptions around those concepts.

Objective: Identify the place that nutrition has within the dimensions of child care, the specific weight that the caregiver gives to it within the range of actions deployed and if there are differences when the child exhibits neurodevelopmental disorders, as a contribution to the design of interventions in health promotion.

Methodology: Qualitative, exploratory, two-stage study, with the approach of cognitive anthropology; proposal sampling of maximum differences, 121 informants participated in three groups, caregivers of: (1) healthy children, (2) children who had been hospitalized between 3 and 6 months prior to the time of the interview, and (3) children with a diagnosis of permanent neurological injury and that express some type of neurodevelopmental disorder.

Results: Nourishment is the element that reaches the highest values of cultural relevance in the three groups, is located in different domains according to the condition of the care receiver.

Conclusion: The common domains are Well-being, Health Maintenance, Coexistence, and Security, in the 3rd group the domain of Socialization emerges, the elements that make up the conceptual dimensions were identified, the comparative design allowed to identify differences. The description of the domains can represent the cognitive spaces of educational intervention and the elements that configure them are the triggers of the interaction, due to the importance they are given in everyday life.

Introduction

The usual care that a young child receives includes actions that are aimed at guaranteeing survival, growth, and well-being. Nutrition, as an essential process for life, is a constitutive element of child care. There are multiple factors that intervene so that the diet that an infant receives meets her nutritional needs. Various studies have shown that socioeconomic conditions are strongly related to the ability to acquire food. It has also been observed that the existence of parental and social networks represent a support to mitigate precarious conditions and favor the nutritional conditions of groups with social and economic disadvantage (1, 2).

On the other hand, within the framework of the normative values of their environment, subjects generate cultural decisions and carry out actions in accordance with the information they have acquired and the way they have processed it. The meaning, beliefs and perceptions that the caregiver has of food and the act of eating in the family environment are related to the eating behavior of the family. It is important to highlight that from Anthropology common elements have been identified in the eating patterns, both of populations that inhabit different geographic contexts (3) and in others, with diverse historical antecedents, which is related to one of the most important characteristics that human beings have, their adaptive capacity to the natural and social conditions of the environment (4, 5).

The family environment is the scenario in which the caregiver decides the actions related to feeding, as a complex phenomenon, and thus, turns them into a stimulus and result of the integral child development process, in this way, in everyday life, she expresses, what Paris Aguilar mentions as “the capacity of the artisan production of facts and meanings” (6, 7).

Bronfenbrenner's theoretical approach (8, 9), referring to human development, is appropriate to understand the importance of food and the context in which it is carried out, both in its nutritional aspect and in its purpose of favoring the socialization of the child; processes carried out within the family and in which particularly the mother, constitutes an ideal mediating factor between the family nucleus and the school and social group. The close interaction with the mother in her immediate environment and the emotional attachment established between the mother-child dyad are elements that positively trigger the interconnection of the child's family microsystem with more complex environments, represented by the mesosystem, exosystem and macrosystem, structures social and cultural conditions that enable appropriate child development, and it is precisely the moment when the child is fed when the interaction, approach and communication of the mother-child dyad are closer.

In Mexico, food policies and programs have been designed that have allowed these highly complex structures, particularly health and education institutions, to interact favorably with the elements that are located in the immediate environment of school children and their mothers, with the purpose of maintaining health and promoting well-being. During the last 100 years, the actions of the food programs have been aimed at three fundamental aspects: (a) Support for food production, (b) Diversification and strengthening of the food collection, storage, and distribution network, and (c) Provision of subsidies and direct interventions to vulnerable groups to modify food consumption patterns, with a weak presence of educational interventions (10).

Despite the wealth of knowledge accumulated around the topic of infant nutrition, it contrasts with the difficult penetration observed in health promotion programs aimed at strengthening family eating practices, either to preserve favorable behaviors, or to modify or avoid negative behaviors. The approaches carried out about food and nutrition, both biomedical and epidemiological, which were pioneers in Mexico, were oriented more to the knowledge of environmental conditions, economic, and sociodemographic characteristics of individuals, than to the investigation of the aspects relevant social and cultural characteristics of the investigated communities (11, 12).

Although the relevance of these investigative approaches is indisputable, the information that would allow the design of health promotion programs was not used. In reality, the development of such programs has been based on the knowledge generated from the perspective of nutrition experts, that is, considering the dietary norm more than the social or cultural norm, so it is urgent to recover the look and testimony of those who care for and feed children, according to their beliefs and meanings (13, 14).

The interactions of social life are manifested in the small daily phenomena that progressively constitute the social fabric and that once all this has settled down can emerge a deeper knowledge closer to reality. This knowledge or familiarity is what Heidegger calls the world. The world is taken for granted and seen and is only noticed in situations of rupture; the worlds in which people live are not universal or timeless, on the contrary, they are different according to the culture, time or historical period and according to the family in which they are born. Qualitative distinctions give meaning to things recognized by the person in their daily life and are shaped by culture and language (15). Therefore, if health promotion programs are designed without taking into account the components of the cultural concept of food, without elucidating the reasons that underlie food preparation decisions, the health promotion programs will continue to advance in parallel lines promoting the health and behaviors of the population.

The model provided by the Cultural Consensus Theory explores how subjects generate cultural decisions and perform actions according to the information they have acquired and the way they have processed it, within their normative values. It allows this by recognizing the terms that the informants express as ideas or actions, the relevance that these terms have in the majority of the members of the group and the grouping pattern that is observed when looking for similarities between them, always from the point of view of the informants (16).

Weller and Romney define the cultural domain as a group of phrases or words linked by semantic relationships, which together refer to a specific conceptual sphere; they represent perceptions more than preferences; they derive their meaning, in part, from their position in a mutually interdependent system, which reflects the way in which a cultural group classifies the relevant conceptual sphere. It is possible to explore two aspects of the normative beliefs of the group: The qualitative aspect, through the grouping pattern of individual responses, while the quantitative aspect refers to the relevance attributed to each response as a result of the frequency of the response hierarchical.

There is consensus in the conceptual sphere when there is correspondence between the expressions of each informant with those of the rest of the group. Those informants who show greater correspondence with the group are classified as competent informants, due to the possibility that they contribute diverse and sufficient experiences as shared knowledge of the phenomenon under study.

To use the formal Cultural Consensus Model in the analysis of the information obtained by free lists and pile sort, it is necessary that the answers be words or short phrases, they are answered individually, they are oriented to the same topic with the same level of complexity, and respect the “fresh” answer, that is, without correction or transformation (17). The study of cultural domains has been used successfully before various health problems (1820).

In order to identify the location of food within the cultural domains of child care, according to the health condition of the care recipient, a two-stage, phenomenological qualitative study was designed within the framework of the cultural consensus theory.

General Objective

Identify the site and the relevance given to food within the cultural dimensions of child care.

Specific Objective

Know the differences in the configuration of cultural domains when the care recipient suffers temporary or permanent illness.

Design

Qualitative exploratory study, in resident population of the city of Durango, Dgo., Mexico.

Informants

Three groups of preschool child caregivers were formed by propositive sampling and maximum differences:

Group 1−38 healthy child caregivers,

Group 2−40 caregivers of children who had been hospitalized between 3 and 6 months prior to the time of the interview, and

Group 3−43 caregivers of children diagnosed with permanent neurological injury and who expressed some type of neurodevelopmental disorder.

In determining the number of participants per group, the assumption of the consensus model was taken as the main criterion, which suggests for studies of cultural description a minimum size of 17 informants to classify 95% of the questions correctly, because the correlation Average among informants tends to be high. The participants of each group were divided equally for the realization of the two stages of the work.

Operational Stages

The meeting scenarios for the collection of information were the pediatric service of the General Hospital of Durango, Dgo and the Integral Development Care Center “Dr. Isauro Venzor (CADI) of Durango. One hundred and twenty one semi-structured interviews were conducted, fifty-eight and sixty-three respectively, free listings were obtained. The response rate was 100%.

The information was analyzed with support of Anthropac 4.1 and Ucinet software.

Stage 1. Obtaining free listings. They were prepared by registering the first 10 short sentences or words that the informant mentioned when asked what she was looking for child care; once the free listings were collected and in a tabular form the particular elements were identified, the absolute and relative frequency with which each of these elements is mentioned and the place in which they appeared in the lists, the relevance index was determined cultural or smith index, which is the product of the relationship between the previous values, that score represents the specific weight that each element has as part of the concept of care, however, it does not allow to discover the relationship that each of these elements have with each other, nor the way in which are configured the cultural dimensions, for that the next stage is necessary.

Stage 2. Drawing lots. The 20 elements that reached the highest figures of the cultural relevance index (smith index) were selected and printed on cards; the cards were assigned the numerical code corresponding to the cultural relevance index, so that the components of each group could be identified and individual values assigned. The participants gathered the cards that had the terms that looked the most and formed 4–5 lots each.

Individual values were analyzed using a factorial model that estimates the likelihood that a homogeneous system of knowledge predominates in a culture, based on the correspondence between the knowledge of the group and those of the individual. The likelihood criterion for obtaining consensus in the models implies that the first factor reaches a ratio three times greater than that of the second factor, condition that is interpreted as concordance in the responses of the informants, it is also possible to identify the similarity and the difference between the dimensions that make up the cultural domains of each group (21, 22). The aggregate values were analyzed by hierarchical conglomerates, to identify the degree of perceived similarity among the participants and is represented in a tree diagram with correlation levels.

Results

The primary caregiver is the mother in the caregiver groups of the three groups of children, although the participation of the father is more frequent in the group of children with developmental disorders than in the other two groups of children (Table 1). Nourishment is the element that reaches the highest values of cultural relevance in the three groups of caregivers, it is appreciated that it occupies the first and second place among the first 20 elements (Table 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the participating groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Elements of greater cultural relevance, conformation of the domains by pile sort.

The conceptual dimensions of “child care” show homogeneity, the percentage of accumulated variance for the first factor is 81.3, 84.6, and 86.0, respectively, when it is >60%, it is homogeneous—and consensus is observed in the three groups, the ratio between the first and second factor is 3.67, 4.03, and 4.53—when it is >3, consensus is considered to exist (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cultural relevance of food and Consensus of cultural domains of “child care,” according to group of caregivers.

The dimensions that appear in the three groups show similarities because of the presence of the particular elements, however, when observing the arrangement and the relationship between them, the dimensions are configured differently.

In group 1, made up of the mothers of healthy children, food is part of the welfare dimension, which includes body hygiene, bathing and household cleaning, among others (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Conceptual dimensions of child care by hierarchical conglomerates. Group 1. Caregivers of healthy children.

In group 2, that of the caregivers of children who have had a period of hospitalization between 3 and 6 months prior to the time of the interview, feeding is located in the dimension of health conservation what is accompanied by the care in what they eat and also in buying the necessary medicines (Figure 2). While in group 3, caregivers of children with neurodevelopmental disorders place food in the socialization dimension, in which they share space with actions aimed at avoiding social isolation, and favors the incorporation of child to the family environment, the neighborhood, and the school (Figure 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Conceptual dimensions of child care by hierarchical conglomerates. Group 2. Caregivers of children hospitalized 6 months ago.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Conceptual dimensions of child care by hierarchical conglomerates. Group 3. Caregivers of children with developmental disorders.

Discussion

The use of an anthropological approach in the study of the feeding of preschool children with different health conditions, allows to have an appropriate cultural and social perspective, to understand the impact that the caregivers of these children have, in the care they grant on the nutritional and relational level, particularly when it comes to preschoolers with neurological damage.

Infant feeding is the field in which the mother performs one of the activities that represents a relevant dimension of parenting and that she perceives as a significant feature of “being a mother,” which involves participating and deciding on the child's development. It is important to mention that it is also a space for participation and decision of the baby about its immediate environment. It is a time of intense interaction where the needs of the baby and the care of the mother are faced.

The emotional symbolic universes make possible the cultural patterns and norms that modulate both the form of language acquisition and the behavior patterns that allow communication, the exchange between the subjects, and the reproduction and transformation of the social order (23).

The group of caregivers of children with permanent injuries identify feeding on the same level as “play, give love, family life, correct behavior, and education,” so that a nuance of socialization is appreciated; unlike what the other groups refer to, this is part of the dimension in which recreational, affective activities that promote discipline and education are incorporated; they give great cultural relevance to alimentation for different reasons than the caregivers of the other two groups, these caregivers have more experience in the home care process, they seem to have more complex cultural constructions on this phenomenon, they have turned feeding into a space for interaction, they consider it as the opportunity to modify or adapt the behaviors linked to maternage, particularly related to the conditions of the baby's social performance, of the mother and the rest of the family, allows them to identify problems in the relationship established by the child with those responsible for their care, it could be that they have left behind hospital crises, family crises and the grief of not having had a healthy child; the caregivers are located in another plane, which becomes more understandable from the theoretical perspective of Bronfenbrenner and Morris (24), who defines human development as an enduring change in the way in which the person perceives the environment and relates to it. Although the Bronfenbrenner model has the child as the central protagonist, the importance of the caregiver's role is unquestionable, as a character that is responsible for “creating” the environments and “building” relationships, which allow the child to be optimally incorporated into its environment; if this is important when it comes to a healthy child, in the case of children with impaired development, it is crucial. To understand the feeding habits from a cultural perspective, it is necessary to design methodological approaches that allow access to symbolic universes and the study of social processes that are articulated with their properties and meanings, particularly the internal meanings of alimentation (3).

Conclusion

The elements that make up the conceptual dimensions of child care were identified, as well as the prominent role given to food. The comparative design allowed to identify substantive differences in the perception that the caregiver has about what feeding means. When the child is healthy, she is fed to maintain well-being. If the child becomes ill, food becomes a necessary element to regain health and avoid illness, while when the child is faced with a developmental alteration as a permanent condition, the physical and temporary space in which the food, is used to promote social interaction. The meaning that the caregiver gives to food can be the starting point in the design of health promotion programs.

If the caregiver received guidance that allowed her to reinforce the perception of the usefulness of the diet, a bond of trust would be created with the health personnel, due to the consonance of the approaches and from there, the reception of the messages could be facilitated and practices aimed at establishing healthy eating habits.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/supplementary material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by this work is part of a Doctoral Thesis. The ethical aspects of the project were reviewed and approved by the Doctoral Program Tutorial Committee of the University of Guadalajara, Mexico. Participating mothers were informed and invited to participate in the study. They expressed their acceptance and signed an informed consent letter. The research consisted of conducting interviews with the participating mothers. The questionnaires are anonymous and confidential. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

The contributions of YM-L, JS-M, and NA-A were similar in theoretical, method, and results analysis aspects. YM-L wrote the article. All authors contributed to the revision of the manuscript, read it, discussed, and approved the final version.

Funding

We want to express our gratitude to the University of Guadalajara and the Juárez University of the State of Durango, for the support provided for the development of this research. YM-L had a fellowship from the National Council of Science and Technology (CONACyT, MX) for the PhD Public Health through the National Quality Postgraduate Program (Code student E04140435). JS-M received support from of the Secretariat of Public Education through the Academic Group of Public Health and Epidemiology UJED-CA-85.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Sandra Lucía Restrepo M, Maya Gallego, M. La familia y su papel en la formación de los hábitos alimentarios en el escolar. Un acercamiento a la cotidianidad. Bol Antropol. (2005) 19:127–48.

Google Scholar

2. Barker M, Lawrence WT, Skinner TC, Haslam CO, Robinson SM, Inskip HM, et al. Constraints on food choices of women in the UK with lower educational attainment. Public Health Nutr. (2008) 11:1229–37. doi: 10.1017/S136898000800178X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bertran M. Significados socioculturales de la alimentación en la Ciudad de México. In: Bertran M, Arroyo P, editors. Antropología y Nutrición, Fondo Nestlé para la Nutrición. Funsalud. México: Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana -Xochimilco (2006). p. 221–35.

Google Scholar

4. Pérez S, De los Ángeles M. History of the food's History. Chron Nova (2009) 35:105–62.

5. Fischler, C. El (h)omnívoro. El gusto, la cocina y el cuerpo. Barcelona: Anagrama (1995). p. 28–87.

6. Moreno AC, Carbajal MMM, Larios NG, Villarreal LI, Addalid SH. Crianza, nutrición y desarrollo infantil en niños de Sonora y del Estado de México. Rev Mex Trastor Aliment. (2018) 9:170–82. doi: 10.22201/fesi.20071523e.2018.2.480

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Piña PA. Cultura y alimentación. Aspectos fundamentales para una visión comprensiva de la alimentación humana Anal Antrop. (2014) 48:11–31. doi: 10.1016/S0185-1225(14)70487-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris PA. The bioecological model of human development. In: Lerner RM, Damon W, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology: Theoretical Models of Human Development. John Wiley & Sons Inc (2006). p. 793–828. doi: 10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0114

CrossRef Full Text

9. Vélez-Agosto NM, Soto-Crespo JG, Vizcarrondo-Oppenheimer M, Vega-Molina S, Coll CG. Bronfenbrenner's bioecological theory revision: moving culture from the macro into the micro. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2017) 12:900–10. doi: 10.1177/1745691617704397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Barquera S, Rivera-Dommarco J, Gasca-García A. Food and nutrition policies and programs in Mexico. Salud Publ Mex. (2001) 43:464–77. doi: 10.1590/S0036-36342001000500011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Aguirre-Arenas J, Escobar-Pérez M, Chávez-Villasana A. Evaluación de los patrones alimentarios y la nutrición en cuatro comunidades rurales. Salud Públ Méx. (1998) 40:398–407. doi: 10.1590/S0036-36341998000500003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Saucedo Arteaga G, Chávez Villasana A, Merino Zeferino B, Madrigal Fritsch H. Cambios en el déficit de talla de los preescolares en la población rural mexicana. Estud Antropol Biol. (2001) 10:159–72.

Google Scholar

13. Pérez Gil S. Percepciones y prácticas alimentarias de un grupo de mujeres en el México rural. In: Bertran M, Arroyo P, editors. Antropología y Nutrición. México: Fondo Nestlé para la Nutrición. Funsalud; Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana -Xochimilco (2006). p. 137–55.

Google Scholar

14. Contreras J, Gracia Arnaiz M. Del dicho al hecho: las diferencias entre las normas y las prácticas alimentarias. In: Antropología y Nutrición. México: Fondo Nestlé para la Nutrición. Funsalud; Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana -Xochimilco (2006). p. 75–115.

Google Scholar

15. Esguerra Lozada MM. The world in the Heidegger's Phenomenology. An approach to the world from the History of the Concept of Time: Prolegomena. Praxis Filosófica. (2018) 46:151–69. doi: 10.25100/pfilosofica.v0i46.6165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Weller SC. Cultural consensus theory: applications and frequently asked questions. Field Methods. (2007) 19:339–68. doi: 10.1177/1525822X07303502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Weller SC, Romney K. Systematic Data Collection. Newbury Park, CA: Sage (1988).

Google Scholar

18. Hoyos RC, López TT, Villarreal FC, Lucatero AP, González MA, Coutiño BL. Concepciones culturales sobre el dengue en contextos urbanos de México. Rev Saúde Públ. (2006) 40:126–33. doi: 10.1590/S0034-89102006000100020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Aguilera V, de los Ángeles M, López TMT, Rodríguez García RM, Acosta Fernández M. Dimensiones culturales del concepto de salud en trabajadores jubilados de guadalajara, México. Hacia Prom Salud. (2010) 15:13–27.

Google Scholar

20. Kalra N, Pelto G, Tawiah C, Zobrist S, Milani P, Manu G, et al. Patterns of cultural consensus and intracultural diversity in Ghanaian complementary feeding practices. Matern Child Nutr. (2017) 14:e12445. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Sturrock K, Rocha JM. A multidimensional scaling stress evaluation table. Field Methods. (2000) 12:49–60. doi: 10.1177/1525822X0001200104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Weller S. Assesing rater performance without a gold standard using consensus theory. Med Decis Mak. (1997) 17:71–9. doi: 10.1177/0272989X9701700108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Calderón E. El papel de la dimensión afectiva en la adquisición del lenguaje materno. Alteridades. (2017) 27:11–22.

Google Scholar

24. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris PA. The ecology of cognitive developmental processes. In: Damon W, Lerner RM, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology. Vol. 1. Theoretical Models of Human Development. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. (1998). p. 993–1028.

Keywords: infant feeding, qualitative research, child care, health promotion, cultural domains

Citation: Martínez-López Y, Salvador-Moysén J and Alfaro-Alfaro N (2021) Feeding Habits in the Cultural Domains of Child Care: Elements for Health Promotion. Front. Public Health 9:536176. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.536176

Received: 18 February 2020; Accepted: 07 January 2021;
Published: 03 March 2021.

Edited by:

Julian Alberto Herrera, University of Valle, Colombia

Reviewed by:

Meghana Wadnerkar Kamble, University of East Anglia, United Kingdom
Nihar Ranjan Mishra, Veer Surendra Sai Medical College and Hospital, India

Copyright © 2021 Martínez-López, Salvador-Moysén and Alfaro-Alfaro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jaime Salvador-Moysén, jsmoysen@ujed.mx

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.