Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychiatry, 13 November 2024
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Affiliative Touch and Sense of Self: Theoretical Foundations and Innovative Treatments View all 4 articles

Affiliative touch, sense of self and psychosis

  • 1Department of Philosophy, Social Sciences, Humanities and Education, University of Perugia, Perugia, Italy
  • 2Gaetano Benedetti Institute of Existential Psychoanalysis, Perugia, Italy

Introduction

The affiliative touch represents a revolutionary innovation in the course of evolution, as it has enhanced the capacity of organisms that have adopted it to form social bonds. This is achieved through the provision of a natural mechanism for strengthening social cohesion, reducing stress and promoting mutual support within groups (1). The formation of social bonds increases the likelihood of individual survival and adaptation, and also contributes to the development of the human sense of self (24). The capacity to comprehend one’s own and others’ subjectivity, and to establish intersubjective bonds has facilitated social cooperation, enabling humans to modify their environment and reduce their dependence on external factors, thereby exerting a significant influence on human evolutionary development (1).

Affiliative touch has been the subject of extensive neuroscientific studies in recent decades, resulting in significant evidence concerning the neurobiological mechanisms through which it affects the formation and maintenance of attachment relationships (5, 6). These studies have also revealed its role in reducing stress experienced in social interactions (7, 8). Furthermore, its function in the development of the bodily basis for the sense of self has been clarified (912).

Affiliative touch, defined by slow and gentle gestures such as caresses, represents a fundamental form of interpersonal sensorimotor interaction. It plays a pivotal role in a range of evolutionary primary functions, including attachment, stress modulation, affective body representation, differentiation between self and others, the body’s ownership, and embodied simulation (415).

Affective bonds and modulation of the stress axis

Affiliative touch is a pivotal factor in the formation of affective bonds and the modulation of stress. It stimulates the release of oxytocin and endorphins in the limbic system (5), which in turn increase pleasure, reduce pain, and facilitate attachment between child and caregiver (6). Furthermore, oxytocin lowers corticosteroid levels during stressful situations, thereby promoting calm and protection and strengthening attachment to the caregiver (7, 8).

Affiliative touch is most effective when performed in a repeated manner. Studies have demonstrated that oxytocin production reaches its peak after approximately half an hour of continuous, rhythmic stroking in both adults and children (14, 15).

The bodily self and its boundaries: a multisensory affective representation

Affective touch simultaneously stimulates two distinct classes of tactile receptors: β-exteroceptors, which are connected to the somatosensory cortex, and tactile C-interoceptors, which are connected to the posterior insula (10, 11). The latter integrates information from both external and internal sources, thereby promoting a multisensory representation of the bodily self (912). Repeated stimulation of these receptors by affiliative touch facilitates the exchange of information between the inner and outer worlds, thereby contributing to the definition of the boundaries of the self (8, 9).

The formation of bodily self-representation is influenced by affiliative touch, which simultaneously activates the limbic system and elicits experiences of pleasure and attachment (12). This process adds an affective dimension and a sense of ownership to the bodily self-representation.

In the early stages of development, affective touch forms part of an integrated system of multisensory and motor interactions. This includes neonatal imitation (16) and affective attunement (17). These experiences contribute to a multisensory integration that supports the development of the affective representation of the bodily self (18, 19).

In affective attunement, the caregiver enhances the sensorimotor rhythm of affiliative touch by integrating touch with other sensory modalities, thereby preventing adaptation and maintaining the child’s pleasurable interest (17). The affective motivation for multisensory integration stems from the pleasure of bonding with the caregiver. The temporal rhythm of actions within the relational space acts as a unifying force and guiding principle for multisensory integration (2022). This process supports the development of a coherent multisensory representation of the bodily self and the surrounding environment (2022).

The affiliative touch and the motor system interact from intrauterine origins. During the fetal stage of development, the movement of both the mother and the fetus produces oscillations in the amniotic fluid, which stimulate the fetal lanugo, which is equipped with tactile C-receptors. This stimulation increases the production of oxytocin and endorphins in the limbic system, thereby generating pleasure (23). According to this well-established hypothesis, affective touch motivates fetal movement, thereby facilitating multisensory connections during the third trimester of pregnancy.

The pleasure derived from affiliative touch also influences the formation of the boundaries of the bodily self by delineating the distinction between the self and the external world (8, 9).

Federn (24), a seminal figure in psychoanalytic psychotherapy, postulated that ego boundaries are invested with affect (libido) and that ego fragility in psychotic patients is attributable to an inadequate affective investment in ego boundaries.

The self, the self-object, and embodied simulation

The simultaneous stimulation of exteroceptive and interoceptive receptors provided by affective touch (9) confers a particular significance to the caregiver. Indeed, the caress is perceived by the receiver as both an external stimulus and as emanating from an internal organ of the body (13).

This, in addition to enhancing the effect of affective touch in the attachment process, could facilitate the formation of self-objects (25) and transitional objects (26, 27), which represent both the self and an entity separated from the self. The empirical evidence on child development indicates that the sense of self develops in two distinct but complementary ways. 1. as a discrete entity and 2. through the process of mirroring with others (28). These two modes of self-experience tend to integrate during development, resulting in a unified sense of self that is consciously perceived as separate from others while maintaining an unconscious symbiosis with them (28). From a phenomenological and neurobiological perspective, the dialectic between similarity and otherness is manifested in embodied simulation, which is based on two simultaneous principles: first, similarity, whereby the other is implicitly perceived as an extension of the self; and second, distinction between self and other (2932). The mirror neuron system is posited to underlie processes of similarity, which are analogous to the psychoanalytic concept of projective identification (33, 34). Conversely, multisensory integration, which is facilitated by the motor system and the experience of affective touch, is essential for perceiving the bodily self as distinct and separate from the other (35).

Affiliative touch and psychosis

The various functions described, in which the affiliative touch is involved, are frequently impaired in individuals diagnosed with psychosis (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Affiliative touch functions altered in psychosis.

Attachment disorders and traumatic alterations of the stress axis

A substantial body of evidence indicates a correlation between attachment disorders and the onset of psychosis in adults (36, 37).

Research suggests that early exposure to adverse childhood experiences, including neglect (characterized by a lack of emotional and physical care by the caregiver), sexual abuse and bullying, is associated with an increased risk of psychotic symptoms (3841), by impairing the development of secure attachment (36, 37).This risk is associated with permanent alterations in the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (42) and the impairment of oxytocin’s ability to regulate stress hormones such as cortisol (43).

The likelihood of developing psychosis in individuals who have experienced traumatic events during childhood is estimated to be three to fifteen times higher than in those who have not, depending on the nature and frequency of the trauma experienced (4446).

Psychotic alterations of the sense of self

Alterations of the bodily self

From a phenomenological perspective, disturbances in the sense of self are underpinned by alterations in the bodily self. This is the primary feature of the psychopathology of schizophrenia (47, 48).

In the psychoanalytic field, disturbances in the sense of self have also been observed in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia (49, 50). It has been hypothesized that disturbances in self representation and self-awareness are the result of disturbances in multisensory integration (51).

Cognitive neuroscience has recently provided ample empirical evidence for alterations in the multisensory integration processes underlying bodily self-disorders, which would profoundly alter the complex structure of the self (32).

Alterations of the boundaries of the self and ownership

The boundaries of the self are often disrupted in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia, as observed by pioneers of psychoanalytic treatment of psychosis such as Tausk (52) and Federn (24).

The neuroscientific literature has identified the significance of multisensory integration of disparate body signals in the formation of a sense of body ownership and the differentiation between one’s own body and that of others (5356). This process involves the posterior cortex of the insula (57).

Individuals diagnosed with first-episode schizophrenia demonstrate alterations in the distinction between the self and the other, as well as changes in the activity of the posterior insula cortex, when observing stimulation based on affective touch. This form of touch integrates internal and external sensory signals and contributes to multisensory integration, the definition of self-boundaries, and the experience of ownership (20, 32).

Patients diagnosed with schizophrenia exhibit reduced interoceptive accuracy (58), a measure of body awareness and insula function (57). Furthermore, in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia, anatomical and functional changes have been observed in the insular cortex (5961), a key region in the interoceptive processing of the sense of self that is directly linked to affective tactile C receptors (1012).

Embodied simulation disorders

Embodied simulation offers a basis for experimental examination of the balance between similarity and differentiation in intersubjectivity (2932). As previously stated, the mirror neuron system plays a pivotal role in the perception of similarity (34). Conversely, multisensory integration, which is supported by the motor system and affective touch, is essential for the differentiation of the bodily self from others (35).

In recent years, dysfunctions of embodied simulation have been documented in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia. These studies have revealed impairments in mirror neuron maps, which influence the perception of similarity (6265), and in sensorimotor circuits, which are associated with self/other differentiation (59, 61, 66, 67).

Discussion

Affiliative touch has the potential to be an effective therapeutic intervention for psychosis for a number of reasons. Firstly, it has been shown to foster attachment and modulate the traumatic stress response (58). Secondly, it promotes multisensory integration, which contributes to the formation of the bodily self and a sense of ownership (1820). Thirdly, it enhances the exchange of interoceptive and exteroceptive sensory information, which reinforces the boundaries of the self (8, 9). Finally, it stimulates the insula and interoception, facilitating bodily awareness (1012). Moreover, the activation of both exteroceptive and interoceptive pathways by affiliative touch (8, 9) may facilitate a balanced relationship between otherness and similarity in embodied simulation, ultimately enhancing the quality of intersubjective relationships.

The therapeutic use of affiliative touch may be constrained by certain limitations, particularly in the case of patients who have experienced childhood trauma. A study conducted by Maier et al. (2020) (68) demonstrated that adults who have undergone childhood maltreatment are prone to experiencing discomfort in the presence of affiliative touch. It is therefore necessary to exercise caution in this regard.

The pharmacological use of oxytocin through exogenous intranasal administration acts on a broad range of unspecified brain areas, resulting in the inability to achieve the same therapeutic effects as affiliative touch. The latter produces elevated oxytocin levels at targeted nerve endings and receptors (69).

Given the documented effects of affiliative touch on various functions that are disrupted in individuals with psychotic disorders, there is a clear need for research to investigate the potential therapeutic benefits of affiliative touch. Despite this, and despite the considerable amount of basic research that has been conducted on affiliative touch, a recent systematic review (70) found that there has been minimal investigation into the clinical applications of affiliative touch in psychosis, with only a single study providing some evidence (71).

In this study, researchers developed a novel sensorimotor intervention for patients with psychosis, called Amniotic Therapy. This intervention integrates elements of affiliative touch and early parent-child interaction movements. The warm water environment of amniotic therapy facilitates the seamless combination of affective touch and movement (13, 72, 73). The results of this new treatment are promising, showing improvements in symptoms, social relationships and employment (72).

In one particular case, interoceptive accuracy was measured by observing an increase in its values after three years of therapy, indicative of an increase in interoception concomitant with a strengthening of the boundaries of the self (71, 73). However, the small sample size limits the generalizability of the results.

The objective of this paper is to stimulate further research and clinical applications of affective touch in psychosis, specifically in relation to the sense of self. Although this area is under-researched, it has significant therapeutic potential without harmful side effects. This contribution to the discussion of affective touch and its relationship to the sense of self aims to stimulate further research and encourage its clinical application.

Author contributions

MP: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Dunbar M. The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2010) 34(2):260 –8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bowlby J. A Secure Base: Parent-Child Attachment and Healthy Human Development. (1988) London, Routledge.

Google Scholar

3. Ainsworth MDS. Attachments beyond infancy. Am Psychol. (1989) 44:709–16. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Beebe B, Jaffe J, Markese S, Buck K, Chen H, Cohen P, et al. The origins of 12-month attachment: a microanalysis of 4-month mother-infant interaction Attach. Hum Dev. (2010) 12:3–141. doi: 10.1080/14616730903338985

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Walker SC, Trotter PD, Swaney WT, Marshall A, McGlone FP. C-tactile afferents: Cutaneous mediators of oxytocin release during affiliative tactile interactions? Neuropeptides. (2017) 64:27–38. doi: 10.1016/j.npep.2017.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Feldman R. Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Hormones Behav. (2012) 61:380–91. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Walker SC, McGlone FP. The social brain: neurobiological basis of affiliative behaviours and psychological well-being. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2013) 14:190–205. doi: 10.1016/j.npep.2013.10.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Feldman R, Gordon I, Schneiderman I, Weisman O, Zagoory-Sharon O. Natural variations in maternal and paternal care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent-infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology;. (2010) 35:1133e41. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.01.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Crucianelli L, Filippetti ML. Developmental perspectives on interpersonal affective touch. Topoi. (2020) 39:575–86. doi: 10.1007/s11245-018-9565-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. McGlone F, Wessberg J, Olausson H. Discriminative and affective touch: Sensing and feeling. Neuron. (2014) 82:737–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Olausson H, Lamarre Y, Backlund H, Morin C, Wallin BG, Starck G, et al. Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal touch and project to insular cortex. Nat Neurosci. (2002) 5:900–4. doi: 10.1038/nn896

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Craig AD. How do you feel-now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Peciccia M, Buratta L, Ardizzi M, Germani A, Ayala G, Ferroni F, et al. Sense of self and psychosis, part 1: Identification, differentiation and the body; A theoretical basis for amniotic therapy. Int Forum Psychoanalysis. (2022) 31:226–36. doi: 10.1080/0803706X.2021.1990401

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Triscoli C, Croy I, Steudte-Schmiedgen S, Olausson H, Sailer U. Heart rate variability is enhanced by long-lasting pleasant touch at CT-optimized velocity. Biol Psychol. (2017) 128:71–81. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.07.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Fairhurst MT, Löken L, Grossmann T. Physiological and behavioral responses reveal 9-month-old infants’ sensitivity to pleasant touch. Psychol Sci. (2014) 25:1124–31. doi: 10.1177/0956797614527114

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science;. (1977) 198:75–8. doi: 10.1126/science.198.4312.75

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Stern DN, Hofer L, Haft W, Dore J. Affect attunement: The sharing of feeling states between mother and infant by means of inter-modal fluency. In: Field  TM, Fox NA, editors. Social perception in infants. Ablex, Norwood, NJ (1985). p. 249–68.

Google Scholar

18. Bremner AJ. Developing body representations in early life: combining somatosensation and vision to perceive the interface between the body and the world. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2016) 58:12–6. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Meltzoff AN, Saby JN, Marshall PJ. Neural representations of the body in 60- day-old human infants. Dev Sci. (2019) 22:e12698. doi: 10.1111/desc.12698

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Gallese V, Ardizzi M, Ferroni F. Schizophrenia and the bodily self. Schizophr Res. (2024) 269:152–62. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2024.05.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gallese V, Sinigaglia C. The bodily self as power for action. Neuropsychologia. (2010) 48:746–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gallese V, Sinigaglia C. How the body in action shapes the self. J Conscious. Stud. (2011) 18:117–43.

Google Scholar

23. Bystrova K. Novel mechanism of human fetal growth regulation: a potential role of lanugo, vernix caseosa and a second tactile system of unmyelinated low-threshold C-afferents. Med Hypotheses. (2009) 72:143–6. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2008.09.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Federn P. Narcissism in the structure of the ego. Int J Psycho-Anal. (1928) 9:401–19.

Google Scholar

25. Kohut H. The Analysis of the Self: A Systematic Approach to the Psychoanalytic Treatment of Narcissistic Personality Disorders. Chicago: University of Chicago Press (1971).

Google Scholar

26. Winnicott DW. Transitional objects and transitional phenomena. Int J Psycho-Analysis. (1953) 34:89–97.

Google Scholar

27. Winnicott DW. Playing and Reality. London: Tavistock Publications (1971).

Google Scholar

28. Stern DN. The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and development psychology. New York: Basic Books (1985).

Google Scholar

29. Gallese V. The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology. (2003) 36:171–80. doi: 10.1159/000072786

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Gallese V. Embodied simulation: from neurons to phenomenal experience. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. (2005) 4:23–48. doi: 10.1007/s11097-005-4737-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gallese V. Before and below “theory of mind”: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Philos Trans R Soc B. (2007) 362:659–69. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.2002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Gallese V. Bodily selves in relation: embodied simulation as second- person perspective on intersubjectivity. Philos Trans R Soc B. (2014) 369:20130177. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0177

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Olds DD. Identification: psychoanalytic and biological perspectives. J Am Psychoanal Assoc. (2006) 54:17–46. doi: 10.1177/00030651060540012001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gallese V. Mirror neurons and intentional attunement: commentary on olds. J Am Psychoanal Assoc. (2006) 54:47–57. doi: 10.1177/00030651060540011101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ebisch SJ, Ferri F, Perrucci MG, DʼAmico L, Romani GL, Gallese V. Differential involvement of somatosensory and interoceptive cortices during the observation of affective touch. J Cogn Neurosci. (2012) 23:1808–22. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21551

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Berry K, Barrowclough C, Wearden A. A review of the role of adult attachment style in psychosis: unexplored issues and questions for further research. Clin Psychol Rev. (2007) 27:458–75. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Zeanah CH, Gleason MM. Annual Research Review: Attachment disorders in early childhood— clinical presentation, causes, correlates, and treatment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2015) 56:207–22. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.2015.56.issue-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Perry BD. Childhood experience and the expression of genetic potential: what childhood neglect tells us about nature and nurture. Brain Mind. (2002) 3:79–100. doi: 10.1023/A:1016557824657

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Read J, Fosse R, Moskowitz A, Perry BD. The traumagenic neurodevelopmental model of psychosis revisited. Neuropsychiatry. (2014) 4:65–79. doi: 10.2217/npy.13.89

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, Lieverse R, Lataster T, Viechtbauer W, et al. Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: A meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and crosssectional cohort studies. Schizophr Bull. (2012) 38:661–71. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Morgan C, Gayer-Anderson C. Childhood adversities and psychosis: Evidence, challenges, implications. World Psychiatry. (2016) 15:93–102. doi: 10.1002/wps.20330

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Aas M, Pizzagalli DA, Laskemoen JF, Reponen EJ, Ueland T, Melle I, et al. Elevated hair cortisol is associated with childhood maltreatment and cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and in bipolar disorders. Schizophr Res. (2019) 213:65–71. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.01.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Heinrichs M, Domes G. Neuropeptides and social behavior: Effects of oxytocin and vasopressin in humans. Prog Brain Res. (2008) 170:337–50. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00428-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bebbington P, Jonas S, Kuipers E, King M, Cooper C, Brugha T, et al. Childhood sexual abuse and psychosis: data from a cross-sectional national psychiatric survey in England. Br J Psychiatry. (2011) 199:29–37. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.083642

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bechdolf A, Thompson A, Nelson B, Cotton S, Simmons MB, Amminger GP, et al. Experience of trauma and conversion to psychosis in an ultra-high-risk (prodromal) group. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. (2010) 121:377–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01542.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. van Winkel R, Stefanis NC, Myin-Germeys I. Psychosocial stress and psychosis. A review of the neurobiological mechanisms and the evidence for gene-stress interaction. Schizophr Bull. (2008) 34:1095–105. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Sass LA, Parnas J. Schizophrenia, consciousness, and the self. Schizophr. Bull. (2003) 29:427–44. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Hur J-W, Kwon JS, Lee TY, Park S. The crisis of minimal self-awareness in schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Schizophr Res. (2014) 152:58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bion WR. Second Thoughts: Selected Papers on Psychoanalysis. London: William Heinemann (1967).

Google Scholar

50. Benedetti G. Psychotherapy of Schizophrenia. New York: New York University Press (1987).

Google Scholar

51. Peciccia M, Benedetti G. The integration of sensorial channels through progressive mirror drawing in the psychotherapy of schizophrenic patients with disturbances in verbal language. J Am Acad Psychoanalysis Dynamic Psychiatry. (1998) 26:109–22. doi: 10.1521/jaap.1.1998.26.1.109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Tausk V. The psychoanalytic treatment of schizophrenia. Int J Psycho-Analysis. (1933) 14:169–87.

Google Scholar

53. Blanke O. Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2012) 13:556–571. doi: 10.1038/nrn3292

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ehrsson HH. Multisensory processes in body ownership. In: Multisensory perception (2020). London, Routledge p. 179–200.

Google Scholar

55. Serino A, Alsmith A, Costantini M, Mandrigin A, Tajadura-Jimenez A, Lopez C. Bodily ownership and self-location: components of bodily self-consciousness. Conscious. Cogn. (2013) 22:1239–52. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Tsakiris M. The multisensory basis of the self: from body to identity to others. Q J Exp Psychol. (2017) 70:597–609. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1181768

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Tsakiris M, Jiménez A--, Costantini M. Just a heartbeat away from one’s body: interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations. Proc R Soc B: Biol Sci. (2011) 278:2470–6. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2547

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Ardizzi M, Ambrosecchia M, Buratta L, Ferri F, Peciccia M, Donnari S, et al. Interoception and positive symptoms in schizophrenia. Front Hum Neurosci. (2016) 10:379. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00379

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Ebisch SJH, Salone A, Ferri F, De Berardis D, Romani GL, Ferro FM, et al. Out of touch with reality? Social perception in first-episode schizophrenia. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2013) 8:394–403. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Wylie KP, Tregellas JR. The role of the insula in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2010) 123:93–104. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.08.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Ebisch SJH, Mantini D, Northoff G, Salone A, De Berardis D, Ferri F, et al. Altered brain long-range functional interactions underlying the link between aberrant self-experience and self-other relationship in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2014) 40:1072–82. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Thakkar KN, Peterman JS, Park S. Altered brain activation during action imitation and observation in schizophrenia: a translational approach to investigating social dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (2014) 171:539–48. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13040498

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Mehta UM, Thirthalli J, Aneelraj PJ, Gangadhar BN, Keshavan MS. Mirror neuron dysfunction in schizophrenia and its functional implications: A systematic review. Schizophr Res. (2014) 160:9–19. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Valizadeh A, Mbwogge M, Rasouli Yazdi A, Hedayati Amlashi N, Haadi A, Shayestefar M, et al. The mirror mechanism in schizophrenia: a systematic review and qualitative meta-analysis. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:884828. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.884828

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Mohring N, Shen C, Hahn E, Ta TMT, Dettling M, Neuhaus AH. Mirror neuron deficit in schizophrenia: evidence from repetition suppression. Schizophr. Res. (2015) 168:174–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.07.035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Gallese V, Ferri F. Jaspers, the body, and schizophrenia: the bodily self. Psychopathology. (2013) 46:330–6. doi: 10.1159/000353258

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Gallese V, Ferri F. Psychopathology of the bodily self and the brain: the case of schizophrenia. Psychopathology. (2014) 47:357–64. doi: 10.1159/000365638

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Maier A, Gieling C, Heinen-Ludwig L, Stefan V, Schultz J, Güntürkün O, et al. Association of childhood maltreatment with interpersonal distance and social touch preferences in adulthood. AJP. (2020) 177:37–46. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19020212

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Glone F MC, Moberg KU, Norholt H, Eggart M, Mueller-Oherlinghausen B. Touch medicine: bridging the gap between recent insights from touch research and clinical medicine and its special significance for the treatment of affective disorders. Front Psychiatry. (2024) 15:1390673. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1390673

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Papi M, Decandia D, Laricchiuta D, Cutuli D, Buratta L, Mazzeschi C. The role of affective touch in mental illness: a systematic review of CT fibers as a potential therapeutic tool and its clinical implications. Front Psychopathol. (2024).

Google Scholar

71. Peciccia M, Germani A, Ardizzi M, Buratta L, Ferroni F, Mazzeschi C, et al. Sense of self and psychosis, part 2: A single case study on amniotic therapy. Int Forum Psychoanalysis. (2022) 31:237–48. doi: 10.1080/0803706X.2021.1990402

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Peciccia M, Mazzeschi C, Donnari S, Buratta L. A sensory-motor approach for patients with a diagnosis of psychosis. Some data from an empirical investigation on amniotic therapy. Psychosis. (2015) 7:141–51. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2014.926560

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Germani A, Ambrosecchia M, Buratta L, Peciccia M, Mazzeschi C, Gallese V. Constructing the sense of self in psychosis using the amniotic therapy: a single case study. Psychosis. (2019) 11:277–81. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2019.1618381

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: affective touch, sense of self, attachment, trauma, stress regulation, border of the self, psychosis

Citation: Peciccia M (2024) Affiliative touch, sense of self and psychosis. Front. Psychiatry 15:1497724. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1497724

Received: 17 September 2024; Accepted: 16 October 2024;
Published: 13 November 2024.

Edited by:

Giulia Ballarotto, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Paolo Meneguzzo, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Peciccia. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maurizio Peciccia, bWF1cml6aW8ucGVjaWNjaWFAdW5pcGcuaXQ=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.