- 1Foreign Languages School, Jinggangshan University, Ji’an, Jiangxi, China
- 2Foreign Languages College, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, Jiangxi, China
This research delves into unexplored territories of Foreign Language Anxiety (FLA), going beyond the traditional focus on language proficiency. We examined the nuanced roles of language exposure and individual differences in cognitive control abilities in shaping FLA. By engaging 46 English learners in a comprehensive assessment, our analysis uncovered significant yet distinct contributions of these factors to various aspects of FLA. Notably, proficiency predicted communication and overall anxiety, exposure influenced evaluation anxiety, while inhibition and mental set shifting abilities significantly predicted communication and test anxiety respectively. These findings illuminate the complexity of FLA, revealing that it stems from a multifaceted interplay of language proficiency, exposure, and cognitive control. This holistic understanding offers valuable insights for educators and learners alike, emphasizing the importance of tailored strategies that address not just language proficiency but also exposure opportunities and cognitive strengths.
Introduction
Foreign language anxiety (FLA) has garnered significant attention as an affective factor in second/foreign language acquisition. Prior research has consistently linked FLA to language proficiency, highlighting a negative correlation (1–6). While much emphasis has been placed on external sources of FLA, such as teacher-student dynamics and classroom environments (7), the investigation into the roles of language learning experiences and individual cognitive differences has been relatively scarce. Despite isolated studies exploring the relationships between language use experiences (8) and cognitive control abilities (9, 10) with anxiety in broader contexts, the intricate interplay between these factors and FLA remains underexplored. The present study endeavors to bridge this gap by delving into the specific contributions of language proficiency, language exposure, and cognitive control abilities to FLA, offering a nuanced perspective on the multifaceted nature of this affective variable.
Anxiety and FLA
Spielberger et al. (11) articulated anxiety as a subjective experience encompassing tension, apprehension, nervousness, and worry, triggered by autonomic nervous system arousal. This emotion assumes two distinct forms: trait anxiety, a stable personality characteristic pervading various situations, and state anxiety, a fleeting emotional state contingent upon specific contexts (12). MacIntyre and Gardner (13) introduced the concept of foreign language classroom anxiety (FLCA), a particular manifestation rooted in the unique stressors of language learning environments, distinguishing it from generalized anxiety scenarios (3, 14, 15).
FLCA, as defined by Horwitz et al. (15), embodies a distinct blend of self-perceptions, beliefs, emotions, and behaviors that stem from the singular challenges of foreign language classrooms. This anxiety is multifaceted, transcending the mere sum of its components: communication apprehension, test anxiety, and evaluation anxiety. Communication apprehension encompasses the fear of engaging in linguistic interactions, whether it be speaking or listening. Test anxiety reflects the performance-related stress of failing linguistic assessments. Evaluation anxiety, meanwhile, arises from the anticipation of negative judgments from proficient teachers or peers, leading to an aversion towards evaluative situations. Recognizing the intricacies of FLCA, Horwitz et al. (15) devised the Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (FLCAS), a robust instrument with an alpha coefficient of 0.93, indicative of high internal reliability. Widely adopted and validated (16), the FLCAS comprises 33 questions, meticulously crafted to capture the nuanced dimensions of anxiety within foreign language classrooms. Given that language learning often unfolds primarily within classroom settings, the current study operationally defines foreign language anxiety (FLA) as synonymous with FLCA, focusing on the general experiences of learners within these environments.
L2 proficiency and FLA
Language anxiety has emerged as a pivotal affective factor, consistently ranking among the foremost predictors of students’ linguistic proficiency (17). A vast majority of research endeavors have zeroed in on the intricate interplay between foreign language achievement and anxiety, yielding remarkably consistent results that underscore a moderately inverse relationship between the two (18). Azher et al. (19), for instance, surveyed 149 undergraduates using the FLCA questionnaire and correlated their language achievement, as indicated by GPA, with anxiety levels. Their analysis underscored a statistically significant negative correlation (r=-.273, p<.001). Similarly, a comprehensive meta-analysis by Teimouri et al. (6), spanning 105 samples across 23 countries and encompassing 19,933 participants, reaffirmed this negative link, with a stronger correlation coefficient of r=-.36 (p<.001).
Notably, the impact of language achievement extends across the entire foreign language learning continuum, from input to processing to output, all of which are inversely related to FLA, as evidenced by Onwuegbuzie et al. (20). While a substantial body of research has examined the overarching relationship between language proficiency and FLA, there is also a growing interest in exploring its nuanced connections with specific components of FLA. Su (21), for example, isolated the Communication Apprehension Subscale from the FLCAS to assess its relationship with English proficiency. Their findings mirrored a negative correlation (r=-.119, p<.001), indicating that higher self-perceived English proficiency is associated with lower communication anxiety. Likewise, Hu et al. (22), in a study involving 631 primary school students, uncovered a significant negative relationship between language achievement and all individual components of FLA (ps<.01), further corroborating the multifaceted nature of this connection. In conclusion, language proficiency stands as a pivotal factor influencing FLA, with its relationship to various FLA components warranting further investigation to deepen our understanding of this complex interplay.
Language exposure and FLA
Language exposure is paramount to igniting and fostering linguistic prowess in early stages, as emphasized by Mayberry et al. (23). Acting as a pivotal predictor of vocabulary richness and grammatical mastery, the quantum of L2 exposure intimately correlates with the trajectory of bilingual development, fostering proficiency through dual language immersion (24). Conversely, inadequate L2 exposure hinders language acquisition progress, fostering anxiety and unease in learners during language production, thereby potentially exacerbating foreign language anxiety (FLA) (25). The influence of language exposure on FLA manifests in diverse contexts, including overseas experiences, immersive classrooms, and the age of acquisition (AoA). Overseas sojourns not only accelerate linguistic proficiency but also bolster mental resilience, with prior encounters with target cultures inversely predicting FLA (20, 26b). This underscores that EFL learners immersed in the target language environment exhibit diminished FLA. Similarly, immersion students, who acknowledge and manage their FLA more effectively than their non-immersion peers (27), report lower communication anxiety, with extracurricular language usage further mitigating FLA (28).
AoA also plays a crucial role, as early L2 exposure tends to diminish FLA (28, 29). Research on Chinese EFL learners reveals that earlier L2 exposure is inversely associated with FLA across linguistic contexts, with pre-school exposure leading to significantly lower anxiety levels (29). Similarly, American-born bilinguals, who acquired English at a young age, exhibit lower FLA compared to their counterparts residing outside the U.S.-Mexico border (30). In French immersion programs, heightened language interaction with advancing grades (7-9) correlates with declining FLA levels (31).
While some scholars posit an indirect link, where increased L2 proficiency stemming from exposure subsequently mitigates FLA (32), direct evidence elucidating the explicit impact of L2 exposure on FLA remains elusive. Further research is warranted to unravel the intricate interplay between these variables and its implications for language learning outcomes.
Cognitive control and FLA
Cognitive control, an umbrella term encompassing processes like inhibition, mental set shifting, and working memory updating (33, 34), is instrumental in goal-directed behaviors. Language learning, inherently a cognitive endeavor involving encoding, storage, and retrieval (35), has recently sparked interest in its interplay with cognitive control and anxiety. Research underscores a bidirectional relationship between cognitive control and anxiety. High anxiety levels have been associated with cognitive impairments, including deficits in cognitive control (36), while impairments in cognitive control, particularly inflexibility, can precipitate anxiety (10, 37–39). Notably, various facets of cognitive control negatively correlate with anxiety (40–42), highlighting the potential for a vicious cycle where cognitive difficulties fuel anxiety, and vice versa.
In the context of language learning, particularly foreign language acquisition, cognitive control takes on heightened significance. When two languages are simultaneously activated, the ability to inhibit the non-target language, especially the dominant one, becomes crucial for effective communication (43). Furthermore, cognitive control facilitates context monitoring and seamless language switching (44), suggesting that enhanced cognitive control abilities may foster a more relaxed language learning experience, thereby mitigating foreign language anxiety (FLA). While prior studies have delved into the relationship between cognitive control and anxiety in general settings, their specific role in the foreign language learning context remains underexplored. We posit that robust cognitive control abilities expedite foreign language processing, fostering emotional adaptability and ultimately mitigating FLA.
The current study
Based on the above review, the current study seeks to bridge a significant gap in understanding the intricate relationship between second language (L2) proficiency, exposure to the L2 environment, and cognitive control abilities, and their differential contributions to foreign language anxiety (FLA) among learners of English as a Foreign Language (EFL). The study aims to investigate three distinct research questions (RQs).
RQ1: To what extent does second language (L2) proficiency serve as a predictor of foreign language anxiety (FLA)?
RQ2: Is second language (L2) exposure a potential indicator of foreign language anxiety (FLA)?
RQ3: Does cognitive control act as a predictor of foreign language anxiety (FLA)?
Our central hypothesis posits that while L2 proficiency, exposure, and cognitive control abilities are all vital factors that contribute to FLA, they each exert their influence in distinct and nuanced ways. L2 proficiency, often seen as a cornerstone of language learning, may mitigate FLA by enhancing learners’ confidence and reducing their fear of making mistakes. L2 exposure, which refers to the amount and quality of interaction with the target language, can either exacerbate or alleviate FLA depending on the learner’s perception of their language environment and their level of preparedness to engage in such interactions. Cognitive control abilities, encompassing executive functions such as inhibition and mental set shifting, play a pivotal role in modulating FLA. They enable learners to effectively manage distractions, prioritize tasks, and switch their mental sets during language learning activities. By strengthening these abilities, learners may be better equipped to cope with anxiety-provoking situations and persist in their language learning endeavors. Therefore, the current study aims to make a significant contribution to the field of language learning and anxiety research by providing a nuanced understanding of the differential contributions of L2 proficiency, exposure, and cognitive control abilities to FLA. By integrating these multifaceted factors and offering practical insights, our research endeavors to pave the way for more effective strategies to alleviate FLA and foster successful language learning experiences for EFL learners worldwide.
Materials and methods
Procedure
All participants were required to complete a series of tests in a single session within a quiet laboratory setting. Initially, they undertook two cognitive control tasks: the Flanker Task and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Subsequently, they completed the Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Questionnaire. Following this, they reported their language background, specifically their experiences and proficiency in learning English as a foreign language, using the Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire. Finally, they took an objective L2 (English) proficiency test, the Semantic Category Verbal Fluency Test. The entire testing process took approximately one hour, with the sequence of tests being counter-balanced to control for order effects.
Participants
Forty-six (44 females, 2 males) English major postgraduate students from Jiangxi Normal University participated in the current study. All of them voluntarily participated in the study for course credit with informed consent, and their rights were protected by the protocols of the Academic Committee of the university. The participants were native speakers of Chinese and took English as a foreign language, which is mandatory in most schools in China. They started English learning at about 10 years old in schools. They were English major students in their first year of the master’s program. In their previous college years, they studied a series of English courses, such as Listening, Speaking, Reading, Writing, British Literature, American Literature, Linguistics, Language Teaching, and Translation Studies.
Materials
Linguistic background
The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q) was used to gather participants’ linguistic backgrounds, including age, language learning history, education, language proficiency and language exposure (45, 46). LEAP-Q encompasses four fundamental language skills: listening, speaking, reading, and writing, assessed separately for both L1 and L2. Each skill is rated on a scale from 1 to 10, where “1” signifies minimal proficiency in the language and “10” represents native-level competence. Language exposure is calculated as a percentage, based on the fact that participants utilized either their first language (L1) or their second language (L2), with the sum of L1 and L2 equaling 100%.
Objective L2 proficiency
In order to objectively assess L2 proficiency, we employed the Semantic Category Verbal Fluency Test, a widely recognized measure of vocabulary size (44) and a commonly utilized proxy for overall proficiency in L2 acquisition (47–49). In comparison to subjective self-assessments, objective tests are considered more reliable, despite their high correlation (46). During the test, participants were instructed to generate as many words as possible within 60 seconds for three specific categories: job titles, furniture items, and fruits. The total number of words reported by each participant was tallied and used as their test score. It is worth noting that we did not assess participants’ verbal fluency in Chinese (L1), as it is their native and primary language. Furthermore, all participants had received higher education, which typically requires a high level of proficiency in Chinese. Consequently, their first language (L1) proficiency was deemed uniformly high, despite potential individual variations.
Foreign language anxiety
We adopted the Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (15) as the operational measure of foreign language anxiety (FLA) since our participants were students in college and they used English primarily in English classes. As previously stated, the scale has gained widespread recognition within the field for its reliable assessment of various dimensions of foreign language anxiety. This assessment is comprised of 33 questions, each utilizing a 5-point Likert scale that ranges from 1, indicating “strongly disagree,” to 5, representing “strongly agree.” Of the 33 questions, the items that reflect lack of anxiety include 2, 5, 8, 11, 14, 18, 22, 28, and 32, whereas all the other 24 items reflect anxiety. Consequently, the nine items that indicate a deficiency in anxiety should have their scoring inverted, ensuring that elevated scores correspond to heightened levels of foreign language anxiety. Overall, the questions were categorized into four aspects: communication apprehension, test anxiety, negative evaluation, and general learning. Communication apprehension includes 12 items: 1, 3, 4, 9, 13, 14, 18, 20, 24, 27, 29, and 33. Test anxiety includes 3 items: 8, 10, and 21. Fear of negative evaluation includes 6 items: 2, 7, 15, 19, 23, and 31. The remaining items are labeled as general anxiety.
Cognitive control
The Flanker Task (50) is a typical and well-established behavioral task used to measure cognitive control, particularly for inhibition. Inhibition reflects the ability to suppress responses that are inappropriate in a given situation (33). In the task, participants are required to respond to the direction of the target stimuli by pressing designated keys on the computer. The target stimulus is a red arrow flanked by three types of shapes. In the first type (congruent condition), the target arrow is flanked by arrows of the same direction; in the second type (neutral condition), the target arrow is flanked by diamond symbols; in the third type (incongruent condition), the target arrow is flanked by arrows of opposite direction. In the E-prime designed computerized procedure following previous literature (51–53), the task was designed in two blocks: a practice block and a formal experimental block. For each trial, a fixation of “+” appeared for 250 ms, and then a randomly presented stimulus appeared for 2000 ms, within which participants were to decide the direction of each target arrow as fast and accurately as possible by pressing designated keys on the computer keyboard. The participants could not start the formal experimental block (108 trials) unless they performed with an accuracy rate above 80% in the practice block.
The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is a well-recognized behavioral task used to measure mental set shifting (53–55). Mental set shifting is the ability to switch back and forth between multiple tasks, operations, dimensions, or mental sets (33, 56). In this test, participants are asked to classify each response card according to four stimulus cards (one red triangle, two green stars, three yellow crosses, and four blue circles). Each card is a mixture of numbers, colors, and shapes. In the computerized version of WCST via E-prime 2.0, there were 12 practice trials and 128 formal experimental trials. In each trial, a fixation “+” appeared for 1000 ms. Then the four stimulus cards and a response card appeared simultaneously. Each participant was required to classify the response card according to any of the stimulus cards by pressing designated keys (within 3000 ms), followed by feedback for 1000 ms before the next trial. The implied sorting rule would change after a few trials (5-9 trials) without the participants’ awareness.
Results
All the data and data analysis syntax are accessible at https://osf.io/vpzrd/(DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/VPZRD). In our study’s data analyses, we begin by presenting the descriptive statistics for all participants’ background information. Subsequently, we detail the descriptive data regarding participants’ foreign language anxiety levels and their cognitive control performance metrics. Lastly, we unveil the outcomes of our correlation and regression analyses, aimed at identifying which variables are predictive of the various facets of foreign language anxiety.
Linguistic backgrounds
The descriptive data presented in Table 1 indicated that the average age of the participants (N=46) was 23.57 years (SD=1.36). They had acquired English as a foreign language for an average duration of 13.50 years (SD=1.82). For the entire group, the exposure to English amounted to 32.17% (SD=13.48) of their linguistic experience. Consequently, exposure to Chinese constituted 67.83% for our participants. The mean English verbal fluency across the three categories (job, furniture, fruits) was 31.09 (SD=5.99), serving as an objective measure of vocabulary size and, by extension, language proficiency. Self-reported language proficiency ratings revealed an average score of 21.41 (SD=2.17) for English and 35.17 (SD=2.63) for Chinese. While the participants demonstrated proficiency in both languages, these data suggest that Chinese is their dominant language.
Foreign language anxiety
As mentioned in the method section, the Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (15) assesses general anxiety, communication apprehension, test anxiety, and evaluation anxiety, with further item breakdowns shown in Table 2. As evident from Table 2, the participants collectively exhibited an overall anxiety level of 88.04 (SD=8.54) out of a possible 165 points, suggesting a moderately high level of foreign language anxiety. More specifically, the level of communication apprehension is 32.43 out of 60 (54%), fear of negative evaluation 17.02 out of 30 (57%), test anxiety 9.24 out of 15 (62%), and general anxiety 29.35 out of 60 (49%). Upon examining the comprehensive data, it shows that, comparatively, test anxiety exhibits the highest degree of anxiety across the four categories, succeeded by fear of negative evaluation, communication apprehension, and general anxiety.
Cognitive control tasks
In the Flanker task, two participants were excluded as a result of operation failure. In data trimming, for the response times (from the Flanker task and the WCST), outliers and deviations from 3SDs for each participant on each condition were excluded, accounting for about 2.1% of all data sets. The Flanker task was employed to assess inhibition, typically quantified by the difference in response times (RTs) between incongruent and congruent trials (57–59). In the present study, however, inhibition is computed using the formula “(incongruent - congruent)/congruent,” which represents the relative disparity in RTs between incongruent and congruent trials against the backdrop of baseline congruent RTs. This approach is beneficial in accounting for individual variances in overall processing speed. More modest outcomes indicate superior inhibition. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) was employed to assess mental set shifting (55). This is gauged by the quantity of categories successfully completed, with one category indicating that the participant has correctly completed at least five consecutive trials. A higher number of completed categories suggests a superior capacity for mental set shifting. The descriptive results of the two tasks are presented in Table 3.
As Table 3 shows, participants responded more quickly in the congruent condition than the neutral condition (M= 510 ms, SD=75 vs. M= 525 ms, SD=76), and the neutral condition was faster than the incongruent condition (M= 525 ms, SD=76 vs. M= 562 ms, SD=85). The indicator of inhibition was M=.10 (SD=.06). Repeated measures of variance analyses revealed that the factor of condition (congruent, neutral, and incongruent) was significant, F (2, 86) = 65.231, p <.001, η2 =.603, reflecting that participant responded fastest in the congruent condition where there is a facilitation of the Flankers (<<<<<), whereas slowest in the incongruent condition where the Flankers conflict the direction of the target arrow (<<><<). In the WCST, participants correctly completed 6.90 categories (SD=2.57). All these completed categories reveal how participants were able to switch mental sets when different rules were implied.
Correlation analyses
In order to find out the relationship between participants’ linguistic background, foreign language proficiency, cognitive control, and foreign language anxiety, we conducted correlation analyses. The results of correlations between linguistic background, language proficiency, and foreign language anxiety are presented in Table 4. The results showed that age and foreign language learning history were not correlated with any aspect of foreign language anxiety (ps>.05). However, language exposure was negatively correlated with evaluation anxiety (r=-.374, p=.011) and test anxiety (r=-.326, p=.027), which indicates that the more foreign language exposure the learners have, the less anxiety of evaluation and test they will experience. Finally, L2 proficiency (indicated by objective verbal fluency test scores) was negatively correlated with communication anxiety (r=-.433, p=.003) and overall anxiety (r=-.357, p=.015). These results suggest that the higher L2 (foreign language) proficiency learners have, the less anxious they will be in general, particularly when communicating with others.
Secondly, we conducted correlation analyses between cognitive control performances and foreign language anxiety. Cognitive control performances included inhibition from the Flanker task and completed categories from the WCST. The results are presented in Table 5. As evident from the table, the inhibition [(RT differences between incongruent and congruent trials)/congruent trials] of the Flanker task exhibited a negative correlation with general anxiety, albeit with marginal significance (r=-.292, p=.054), which suggests that weaker ability of inhibition (higher score) is associated with lower anxiety. Conversely, the mental set shifting (completed categories) of the WCST demonstrated a significantly positive correlation with test anxiety (r=.409, p=.005), which indicates that better shifting ability is related to higher test anxiety.
Multiple regression analyses
Based on the correlations, we conducted stepwise multiple regressions, to explore what factors can significantly predict Foreign Language Anxiety when all the significant variables of the correlation analyses were entered. Although less theory-driven, stepwise multiple regression is a beneficial statistical method used to identify the best subset of predictor variables to include in a regression model for predicting a dependent variable. It’s an iterative process that involves both adding or removing variables based on their statistical significance (60, 61). For our analysis, L2 proficiency, L2 exposure, inhibition, and mental set shifting are taken as independent variables, and general anxiety, communication anxiety, evaluation anxiety, test anxiety, and overall anxiety are taken as dependent variables. In each regression analysis of each dependent variable, the model begins with no predictor variables. Then, enter the most significant variable; the variable with the strongest correlation is added to the model. Next, the algorithm continues by iterative adding and removing. In the adding process, it considers the remaining predictor variables and adds the one that improves the model’s fit the most while remaining statistically significant. In the removing process, if a variable in the model no longer significantly contributes to the prediction, it is removed. The process continues until all significant variables are included and all non-significant variables are removed (Criteria: Probability-of-F-to-enter <= .050, Probability-of-F-to-remove >= .100).
The overall results of multiple regression analyses are presented in Table 6. The outcomes of the multiple regression analysis conducted on general anxiety revealed that no predictors were retained in the model. This suggests that none of the variables, including L2 proficiency, L2 exposure, and cognitive control, were able to predict general anxiety levels. The results of multiple regressions on communication anxiety showed that both L2 proficiency and inhibition significantly remained in the model, R=.516, R2=.266, adjusted R2=.230, F (2, 41) =7.425, p=.002, whereas L2 exposure and mental set shifting were excluded (ps>.05). According to the model, communication anxiety can be predicted by L2 proficiency and inhibition, with communication anxiety = 42.781– 0.412*L2 proficiency + 24.310*Inhibition + ϵ.
The results of multiple regressions on evaluation anxiety showed that in the model only L2 exposure predicted the level of evaluation anxiety, R=.358, R2=.128, adjusted R2=.108, F (1, 42) =6.192, p=.017, whereas L2 proficiency, inhibition and mental set shifting were excluded (ps>.05). Therefore, the model indicates that evaluation anxiety = 19.380 – 0.073 * L2 exposure + ϵ.
The results of multiple regressions on test anxiety showed that only the variable of completed categories significantly predicted the level of test anxiety in the model, R=.411, R2=.169, adjusted R2=.149, F (1, 42) =8.545, p=.006, while L2 proficiency, L2 exposure and inhibition were excluded (ps>.05). Therefore, according to model, it indicates that test anxiety = 7.426 + 0.264* completed categories + ϵ.
The results of multiple regressions on overall anxiety scores showed that only L2 proficiency significantly predicted the total score of anxiety in the model, R=.368, R2=.135, adjusted R2=.115, F (1, 42) =6.564, p=.014, with L2 exposure, inhibition and mental set shifting excluded (ps>.05). The model shows that Overall Anxiety = 104.925 – 0.538 * L2 proficiency + ϵ.
In summary, our research findings indicate that various factors distinctly influence different dimensions of foreign language anxiety. More specifically, second language proficiency was a significant predictor of both communication anxiety and overall anxiety. Exposure to the second language significantly predicted evaluation anxiety. Additionally, inhibition was a significant predictor of communication anxiety, and mental set shifting was a significant predictor of test anxiety.
Discussion
The present study delved into the multifaceted roles of L2 proficiency, L2 exposure, and cognitive control differences in shaping Foreign Language Anxiety (FLA). The findings revealed that these three factors uniquely contribute to distinct aspects of FLA, aligning with our premise that FLA is a complex phenomenon predictable by a diverse array of influences, encompassing linguistic proficiency, language exposure, and cognitive control abilities that transcend linguistic barriers. These findings provide implications in devising targeted interventions that aim to alleviate FLA and enhance language learning outcomes.
Firstly, our findings indicate that higher L2 proficiency mitigates foreign language anxiety, which is consistent with prior research (6, 19, 21, 22, 62). The results of stepwise multiple regressions analyses reveal that L2 proficiency is a pivotal predictor of communication and overall anxiety, with higher L2 proficiency contributing to lower level in the aspects of communication and overall anxiety, suggesting its targeted influence on specific anxiety domains. Notably, our study uncovers that L2 proficiency does not uniformly impact all facets of FLA (e.g., general, evaluation, and test anxiety), particularly distinguishing itself from the communicative and overarching anxiety aspects.
Given that communication anxiety constitutes a significant portion of overall FLA, the precise nature of this relationship remains ambiguous—whether it stems solely from communication anxiety or a confluence of factors. Notably, previous studies, relying primarily on the FLCA to gauge overall anxiety, have overlooked the intricate interplay between L2 proficiency and the discrete components of FLA, namely, general, communication, evaluation, and test anxiety. In addition, a distinguishing characteristic of our study lies in the utilization of the category verbal fluency test as a measure of L2 proficiency, which necessitates not just linguistic proficiency but also language regulation skills (34, 63). This nuanced approach implies that heightened linguistic skills coupled with adept language regulation strategies collectively contribute to a reduction in anxiety levels, particularly in the communication and overall anxiety, providing a fresh perspective on the multifaceted nature of L2 proficiency and its anxiety-mitigating effects.
Secondly, our investigation identifies the pivotal role of language exposure in shaping foreign language anxiety. Our discovery that L2 exposure significantly forecasts evaluation anxiety aligns with prior research, specifying that higher L2 exposure contributes to lower evaluation anxiety. Masangya’s (8) study, for instance, explored the intricate relationship between FLA, L2 exposure, and writing performance among university students in Manila, employing the LEAP-Q, FLCAS, and a writing task. Its findings echoed ours, highlighting the significance of L2 exposure in predicting anxiety, particularly evaluation anxiety.
Literature attests to the beneficial effects of multilingual exposure on communication proficiency (64), suggesting that heightened language exposure is likely to alleviate language anxiety. In our review, we noted that language exposure manifests in diverse forms, ranging from overseas experiences, immersion classes, to early age of acquisition (27, 29). While our participants lacked these immersive experiences, their modest L2 exposure—averaging 32.17% across listening, speaking, reading, and writing activities—still emerged as a notable predictor of FLA, particularly of evaluation anxiety. This finding highlights the importance of even modest L2 exposure in mitigating anxiety, particularly in evaluative context. It implies that as L2 exposure increases, learners are likely to experience reduced anxiety related to others’ evaluations, fostering a more confident approach to language learning situations and lessening the fear of negative assessments.
Thirdly, our findings underscore the intricate interplay between cognitive control mechanisms and anxiety levels, particularly for the components of inhibition and mental set shifting. Specifically, we observed that inhibition significantly predicted communication anxiety, whereas mental set shifting notably predicted test anxiety. This finding aligns with previous research (40–42), highlighting the close nexus between cognitive control and anxiety.
However, the intricacies of this relationship remain somewhat controversial. Firstly, our study revealed that higher inhibition score measured by the Flanker (lower inhibitory control ability) predicted higher level of communication anxiety, which is consistent with previous findings that stronger ability of inhibition reduces anxiety (41, 42). The distinction is that our research uncovered the impact of inhibition on communication anxiety whereas their studies identified the link between inhibition and overall anxiety. However, other investigations, such as Cardinale et al. (65), have reported contrasting results, with individuals exhibiting higher anxiety displaying enhanced inhibitory control capabilities in cognitive tasks, suggesting that elevated anxiety may, paradoxically, coincide with improved inhibitory control, which is actually consistent with our correlation result between inhibition and general anxiety. The distinct outcomes indicating that inhibition is associated with general anxiety and communication anxiety in contrasting manners underscore the significance of discerning the mechanisms underlying various facets of anxiety.
Secondly, in contrast to our hypothesis, increased mental set shifting ability, as indicated by more completed categories of WCST, significantly predicted heightened test anxiety, indicating that better ability of mental set shifting contributes to higher test anxiety. This finding diverges from some previous studies (41, 42), which reported direct links between cognitive control deficits and increased anxiety. Kertz et al. (66), for instance, traced the developmental trajectory of anxiety from early childhood, linking cognitive control deficits to heightened anxiety through school years. Our results, however, indicate that a more proficient mental set shifting ability is associated with heightened anxiety. These inconsistent findings challenge our initial hypotheses and underscore the multifaceted nature of cognitive control and anxiety, particularly in the context of language learning and testing, where learners’ attitudes and strategies may influence their cognitive performance. Rezazadeh and Zarrinabadi (67) propose that, in language learning scenarios, a strong inhibition coupled with a heightened need for closure might hinder learners’ engagement, whereas in testing situations, superior attention control instead of shifting might be crucial for task focus.
Furthermore, recent studies in diagnosed PD patients (68) and neural measures (69, 70) have underscored the elevated risk of cognitive impairment among anxious individuals. However, the directionality of this relationship remains elusive, with debates raging over whether cognitive control influences anxiety or vice versa (71–73). Even so, our findings contribute to the ongoing discourse on the complex interplay between cognitive control and anxiety, emphasizing the need for nuanced examinations of these relationships within specific contexts and considering the diverse dimensions of both constructs. Future research should delve deeper into these nuances, elucidating the precise mechanisms underlying this intricate relationship.
Finally, the findings provide some implications regarding the multifaceted roles of L2 proficiency, L2 exposure, and cognitive control in shaping Foreign Language Anxiety (FLA). By identifying specific aspects of FLA that are influenced by these factors, educators can design targeted interventions tailored to the individual needs of students. Those with lower L2 proficiency may benefit from additional language practice and exposure. The role of L2 exposure in influencing FLA highlights the importance of providing learners with rich and meaningful language immersion experiences. This could involve increased opportunities for interaction with native speakers, immersion programs, or the use of technology to simulate real-life language contexts. By fostering a supportive and immersive learning environment, educators can help reduce anxiety and enhance language acquisition. Recognizing the impact of cognitive control on FLA underscores the need to incorporate cognitive training exercises into language curricula. This could include activities that improve attention, working memory, and inhibitory control—all of which are critical for effective language processing. By strengthening these underlying cognitive abilities, learners may be better equipped to manage anxiety and perform at a higher level in language tasks.
Limitations and suggestions for future research
The current study presents compelling evidence that foreign language anxiety (FLA) is a multifaceted phenomenon influenced not solely by language proficiency but also by language exposure and cognitive control, each contributing uniquely to distinct aspects of anxiety. However, it is crucial to acknowledge the limitations of this research. Firstly, we are aware of the limitation of stepwise procedures as it is less theory driven than other methods (74). Moreover, the causal link between cognitive control and FLA remains inconclusive and merits further experimental scrutiny in subsequent studies. Longitudinal experiments are essential to unravel the directionality and dynamics of this relationship. Secondly, the current sample size, while informative, could have benefited from an even larger cohort to strengthen the robustness of the multiple regression analyses and enable more nuanced insights. This highlights the need for future investigations to expand their scope and sample diversity to ensure comprehensive understanding. In light of these limitations, future research endeavors must delve deeper into the intricate interplay between cognitive control and FLA. Longitudinal designs should be prioritized to clarify the causal chain and examine how cognitive control evolves and influences FLA over time. Furthermore, a nuanced examination of the various components of cognitive control (e.g., attention, working memory, inhibitory control) and their specific relationships to different facets of FLA is warranted. This should be conducted in tandem with an exploration of additional factors, such as learners’ attitudes and emotions towards language information processing, to provide a more holistic understanding of FLA and its mitigation strategies.
Conclusion
In conclusion, the present study aimed to unravel the intricate web of factors that contribute to foreign language anxiety, focusing specifically on learners’ language proficiency, language exposure, and cognitive control abilities. Our findings underscore that the multifaceted nature of FLA transcends mere language proficiency; it is intricately linked to the extent of language exposure and individual variations in cognitive control capabilities. These insights are pivotal for comprehending the underlying mechanisms and origins of FLA, offering valuable guidance to language educators and learners alike.
Data availability statement
The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://osf.io/vpzrd/doi.10.17605/OSF.IO/VPZRD.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by The Academic Committee of Jiangxi Normal University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
YX: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. ZX: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The study receives funds from The Research Project of Basic Education in Jiangxi Province (SZUJGYY2024-1151).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Aida Y. Examination of Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope’s construct of foreign language anxiety: The case of students of Japanese. Modern Lang J. (1994) 78:155–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.1994.tb02026.x
2. Gardner RC, Smythe PC, Clément R, Gliksman L. Second-language learning: A social psychological perspective. Can Modern Lang Rev. (1976) 32:198–213. doi: 10.3138/cmlr.32.3.198
3. Horwitz EK. On the misreading of Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope (1986) and the need to balance anxiety research and the experiences of anxious language learners. In: Gkonou MDC, Dewaele JM, editors. New insights into language anxiety: Theory, research and educational implications. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters (2017). p. 31–50.
4. MacIntyre PD. An overview of language anxiety research and trends in its development. In: Gkonou C, Daubney M, Dewaele J-M, editors. New insights into language anxiety : theory, research and educational implications. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters (2017). p. 11–30.
5. MacIntyre PD, Gardner RC. The subtle effects of language anxiety on cognitive processing in the second language. Lang Learn. (1994) 44:283–305. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1994.tb01103.x
6. Teimouri Y, Goetze J, Plonsky L. Second language anxiety and achievement. Stud Second Lang Acquisition. (2019) 41:363–87. doi: 10.1017/s0272263118000311
7. Young DJ. Creating a low-anxiety classroom environment: what does language anxiety research suggest? Modern Lang J. (1992) 75:426–39. doi: 10.2307/329492
8. Masangya EM. The language anxiety and language exposure of ESL students’ on their writing proficiency. Advanced Sci Lett. (2018) 24:2328–32. doi: 10.1166/asl.2018.10946
9. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. (2007) 7:336–53. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336
10. Spinhoven P, van der Veen DC, Voshaar RCO, Comijs HC. Worry and cognitive control predict course trajectories of anxiety in older adults with late-life depression. Eur Psychiatry. (2017) 44:134–40. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.05.002
11. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. (1983) Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (Form Y). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.
12. Cattell RB, Scheier IH. (1961) The meaning and measurement of neuroticism and anxiety. New York: The Ronald Press Company (1961).
13. MacIntyre PD, Gardner RC. Language anxiety: Its relationship to other anxieties and to processing in native and second languages. Lang Learn. (1991) 41:513–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1991.tb00691.x
14. Gardner MP. Mood states and consumer behavior: A critical review. J Consumer Res. (1985) 12:281–300. doi: 10.1086/jcr.1985.12.issue-3
15. Horwitz EK, Horwitz MB, Cope J. Foreign language classroom anxiety. Modern Lang J. (1986) 70:125–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.1986.tb05256.x
16. Guo Y, Xu J, Liu X. English language learners’use of self-regulatory strategies for foreign language anxiety in China. System. (2018) 76:49–61. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2018.05.001
17. Liu M, Huang W. An exploration of foreign language anxiety and English learning motivation. Educ Res Int. (2011) 1:1–8. doi: 10.1155/2011/493167
18. Horwitz E. Language anxiety and achievement. Annu Rev Appl Linguistics. (2001) 21:112–27. doi: 10.1017/S0267190501000071
19. Azher M, Anwar MN, Naz A. An investigation of foreign language classroom anxiety and its relationship with students achievement. J Coll Teach Learn (TLC). (2010) 7:33–40.
20. Onwuegbuzie A, Bailey P, Daley C. Factors associated with foreign language anxiety. Appl Psycholinguistics. (1999) 20:217–39. doi: 10.1017/S0142716499002039
21. Su YC. College students’ Oral communication strategy use, self-perceived english proficiency and confidence, and communication anxiety in Taiwan’s EFL learning. Educ Stud. (2021) 57(6):650–69. doi: 10.1080/00131946.2021.1919677
22. Hu X, Zhang X, McGeown S. Foreign language anxiety and achievement: A study of primary school students learning English in China. Lang Teach Res. (2024) 28:1594–615. doi: 10.1177/13621688211032332
23. Mayberry RI, Lock E, Kazmi H. Linguistic ability and early language exposure. Nature. (2002) 417:38–8. doi: 10.1038/417038a
24. Place S, Hoff E. Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Dev. (2011) 82:1834–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01660.x
25. Jennifer JM, Ponniah RJ. Investigating the levels, types and causes of second language writing anxiety among Indian freshmen. J Asia TEFL. (2017) 14:557–63. doi: 10.18823/asiatefl.2017.14.3.13.557
26. Matsuda S, Gobel P. Anxiety and predictors of performance in the foreign language classroom. System. (2004) 32:21–36. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2003.08.002
27. Baker SC, MacIntyre PD. The role of gender and immersion in communication and second language orientations. Lang Learn. (2003) 53:65–96. doi: 10.1111/0023-8333.00224
28. Dewaele J-M, Petrides KV, Furnham A. Effects of trait emotional intelligence and sociobiographical variables on communicative anxiety and foreign language anxiety among adult multilinguals: A review and empirical investigation. Lang Learn. (2008) 58:911–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2008.00482.x
29. Jin Y, Dewaele J-M. The effect of positive orientation and perceived social support on foreign language classroom anxiety. System. (2018) 74:149–57. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2018.01.002
30. Haley M, Romero Marin M, Gelgand JC. Language anxiety and counseling self-efficacy. J Multicultural Couns Dev. (2015) 43:162–72. doi: 10.1002/jmcd.12012
31. MacIntyre PD, Baker SC, Clément R, Donovan LA. Sex and age effects on willingness to communicate, anxiety, perceived competence, and L2 motivation among junior high school French immersion students. Lang Learn. (2003) 53:137–66. doi: 10.1111/1467-9922.00226
32. Luo H. Foreign language speaking anxiety: A study of Chinese language learners. J Natl Council Less Commonly Taught Languages. (2014) 15:99–117.
33. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe. tasks: latent variable Anal Cognit Psychol. (2000) 41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
34. Xie Z, Antolovic K. Differential impacts of natural L2 immersion and intensive classroom L2 training on cognitive control. Q J Exp Psychol. (2022) 75:550–62. doi: 10.1177/17470218211040813
35. MacIntyre PD. How does anxiety affect second language learning? A reply to Sparks and Ganschow. Modern Lang J. (1995) 79:90–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.1995.tb05418.x
36. Shields GS, Moons WG, Tewell CA, Yonelinas AP. The effect of negative affect on cognition: Anxiety, not anger, impairs executive function. Emotion. (2016) 16:792–7. doi: 10.1037/emo0000151
37. Beaudreau SA, O’Hara R. Late-life anxiety and cognitive impairment: a review. Am J Geriatric Psychiatry. (2008) 16:790–803. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31817945c3
38. Hallion LS, Tolin DF, Assaf M, Goethe J, Diefenbach GJ. Cognitive control in generalized anxiety disorder: relation of inhibition impairments to worry and anxiety severity. Cogn Ther Res. (2017) 41:610–8. doi: 10.1007/s10608-017-9832-2
39. O’Rourke EJ, Halpern LF, Vaysman R. Examining the relations among emerging adult coping, executive function, and anxiety. Emerging Adulthood. (2020) 8:209–25. doi: 10.1177/2167696818797531
40. Beaudreau SA, MacKay-Brandt A, Reynolds J. Application of a cognitive neuroscience perspective of cognitive control to late-life anxiety. J Anxiety Disord. (2013) 27:559–66. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.03.006
41. Beaudreau SA, Petkus AJ, Hantke NC, Gould CE. Anxiety and cognitive functioning. In: Byrne G, Pachana N, editors. Anxiety in older people: clinical and research perspectives. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press (2021).
42. Butters MA, Bhalla RK, Andreescu C, Wetherell JL, Mantella R, Begley AE, et al. Changes in neuropsychological functioning following treatment for late-life generalised anxiety disorder. Br J Psychiatry. (2011) 199:211–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.090217
43. Green D. Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Lang Cogn. (1998) 1:67–81. doi: 10.1017/S1366728998000133
44. Bialystok E, Craik FI, Green DW, Gollan TH. Bilingual minds. psychol Sci Public Interest. (2009) 10:89–129. doi: 10.1177/1529100610387084
45. Marian V, Blumenfeld HK, Kaushanskaya M. The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q) Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. J Speech Language Hearing Res. (2007) 50:940–67. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)
46. Prior A, Gollan TH. Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2011) 17:682–91. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000580
47. Alderson JC. Diagnosing foreign language proficiency: The interface between learning and assessment. London: Continuum. (2005).
48. Masrai A. Lexical knowledge and L2 general language proficiency: collocational competence and vocabulary size as determinants of lexical knowledge. Cogn Process. (2023) 24:289–300. doi: 10.1007/s10339-022-01120-2
49. Miralpeix I, Muñoz C. Receptive vocabulary size and its relationship to EFL language skills. Int Rev Appl Linguistics Lang Teach. (2018) 56:1–24. doi: 10.1515/iral-2017-0016
50. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Percept Psychophysics. (1974) 16:143–9. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267
51. Dong Y, Xie Z. Contributions of second language proficiency and interpreting experience to cognitive control differences among young adult bilinguals. J Cogn Psychol. (2014) 26:506–19. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.924951
52. Luk G, Anderson JA, Craik FI, Grady C, Bialystok E. Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain Cogn. (2010) 74:347–57. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.09.004
53. Xie Z, Dong Y. Contributions of bilingualism and public speaking training to cognitive control differences among young adults. Bilingualism: Lang Cogn. (2017) 20:55–68. doi: 10.1017/s1366728915000474
54. Barceló F, Knight RT. Both random and perseverative errors underlie WCST deficits in prefrontal patients. Neuropsychologia. (2002) 40:349–56. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00110-5
55. Yudes C, Macizo P, Bajo T. The influence of expertise in simultaneous interpreting on non-verbal executive processes. Front Psychol. (2011) 2:309. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00309
56. Monsell S. Control of mental processes. In: Bruce V, editor. Unsolved mysteries of the mind: Tutorial essays in cognition. Taylor & Francis, Erlbaum (1996).
57. Bialystok E, Craik FI, Klein R, Viswanathan M. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychol Aging. (2004) 19:290–303. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290
58. Costa A, Hernandez M, Costa-Faidella J, Sebastian-Galles N. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition. (2009) 113:135–49. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001
59. Paap KR, Greenberg ZI. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognit Psychol. (2013) 66:232–58. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
60. Anderson DR, Sweeney DJ, Williams TA, Freeman J, Shoesmith E. Statistics for business and economics. Boston: Cengage Learning, Inc (2014).
61. Henderson DA, Denison DR. Stepwise regression in social and psychological research. psychol Rep. (1989) 64:251–7. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1989.64.1.251
62. Al-Shboul MM, Ahmad IS, Nordin MS, Rahman ZA. Foreign language anxiety and achievement: systematic review. Int J English Linguistics. (2013) 3:32. doi: 10.5539/ijel.v3n2p32
63. Zirnstein M, van Hell JG, Kroll JF. Cognitive control ability mediates prediction costs in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognition. (2018) 176:87–106. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.001
64. Liberman Z, Woodward AL, Keysar B, Kinzler KD. Exposure to multiple languages enhances communication skills in infancy. Dev Sci. (2017) 20(1):e12420. doi: 10.1111/desc.12420
65. Cardinale EM, Subar AR, Brotman MA, Leibenluft E, Kircanski K, Pine DS. Inhibitory control and emotion dysregulation: A framework for research on anxiety. Dev Psychopathol. (2019) 31:859–69. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000300
66. Kertz SJ, Belden AC, Tillman R, Luby J. Cognitive control deficits in shifting and inhibition in preschool age children are associated with increased depression and anxiety over 7.5 years of development. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2016) 44:1185–96. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0101-0
67. Rezazadeh M, Zarrinabadi N. Examining need for closure and need for cognition as predictors of foreign language anxiety and enjoyment. J Multilingual Multicultural Dev. (2023) 44:83–95. doi: 10.1080/01434632.2020.1798972
68. Dissanayaka NNW, Lawson RA, Yarnall AJ, Duncan GW, Breen DP, Khoo TK. Anxiety is associated with cognitive impairment in newly-diagnosed Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2017) 36:63–8. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.01.001
69. Cavanagh JF, Shackman AJ. Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: meta-analytic evidence. J Physiol Paris. (2015) 109:3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003
70. Jeong H, Sugiura M, Suzuki W, Sassa Y, Hashizume H, Kawashima R. Neural correlates of second-language communication and the effect of language anxiety. Neuropsychologia. (2015) 66:182–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.02.012
71. Arlt J, Yiu A, Eneva K, Taylor Dryman M, Heimberg RG, Chen EY. Contributions of cognitive inflexibility to eating disorder and social anxiety symptoms. Eat Behav. (2016) 21:30–2. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.12.008
72. Grant DM, White EJ. Influence of anxiety on cognitive control processes. In: Braddick O, editor. Oxford research encyclopedia of psychology. New York: Oxford University Press (2016). doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.74
73. Robinson OJ, Krimsky M, Grillon C. The impact of induced anxiety on response inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:69. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00069
74. Lewis-Beck MS. Stepwise regression: A caution. Political Method. (1978) 5(2):213–40. Available online at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/25791533
Keywords: L2 proficiency, L2 exposure, cognitive control, FLA, English learners
Citation: Xu Y and Xie Z (2024) Exploring the predictors of foreign language anxiety: the roles of language proficiency, language exposure, and cognitive control. Front. Psychiatry 15:1492701. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1492701
Received: 07 September 2024; Accepted: 14 November 2024;
Published: 28 November 2024.
Edited by:
Federico Gallo, UiT-The Arctic University of Norway, NorwayCopyright © 2024 Xu and Xie. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Zhilong Xie, Y29sbGludGhhbmtAMTYzLmNvbQ==