Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychiatry, 25 March 2024
Sec. Psychopharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychotropic Overprescribing to Youth: Scope of the Problem, Causes, and Possible Solutions View all 6 articles

Commentary: Perspectives on ADHD in children and adolescents as a social construct amidst rising prevalence of diagnosis and medication use

  • 1Accare Child Study Center, Groningen, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center Groningen (UMCG), Groningen, Netherlands
  • 3Specialists in Youth and Family Care, Levvel, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 5Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Amsterdam University Medical Centers (AUMC), Amsterdam, Netherlands

A Commentary on
Perspectives on ADHD in children and adolescents as a social construct amidst rising prevalence of diagnosis and medication use

By Banaschewski T, Häge A, Hohmann S and Mechler K (2024) Front. Psychiatry. 14:1289157. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1289157

1 Introduction

The recent perspective article by Banaschewski et al. (1) in Frontiers in Psychiatry speculates about a potential “transformative paradigm shift” in the field of ADHD. This proposed paradigm shift centralizes around the notion that ADHD would not reflect a natural entity, but instead is best described as a social or cultural construct. I wholeheartedly agree with this statement, and in this commentary, I advocate for the necessity of this paradigm shift. It will yield significant benefits for individuals with ADHD in at least two crucial aspects: (1) by mitigating many of the negative consequences of receiving an ADHD classification, among which is an overreliance on medication, and (2) by improving the quality of our support.

2 Negative consequences of the current ADHD paradigm

The ADHD paradigm of the last decades, in which ADHD is mostly regarded as a natural entity, may have led to several unintended but potentially harmful consequences. Although everyone agrees that ADHD is the result of a complex interplay of biological and contextual factors (2), the current scientific and societal narrative predominantly focuses on biological factors such as genetics and brain functioning (37). Contextual factors like poverty (8), parental psychopathology (9), trauma (10), screentime (11), early deprivation (12), and being youngest in class (13) receive substantially less attention. The risk of this predominant emphasis on biological factors is that it may create a deterministic, individualized, and decontextualized view on ADHD. Decontextualization refers to the belief by children, their parents, teachers and clinicians that children themselves, or their brains, are primarily responsible for their symptoms. Decontextualization can have many negative consequences. Here, I will briefly discuss three: prognostic pessimism, stigma, and overreliance on medication.

2.1 Prognostic pessimism

First, decontextualization could lead to prognostic pessimism (i.e., having less hope for and lower expectations about the future). This is plausible as biological, decontextualized explanations of problem behaviors may imply that these problems are persistent and incurable (1417). This phenomenon is firmly established for internalizing disorders: The more people attribute their problems to biology, the longer they expect them to last (18, 19), the poorer they perceive their own coping skills (20), and the more negative they rate their prognosis (21). A single study on ADHD mirrored this trend: experimentally induced individualized, decontextualized beliefs about ADHD led to pessimistic expectations about the child’s potential (22). This resembles with other work demonstrating that academic expectations of children with ADHD are disproportionately low (23), as also mentioned by Banaschewski and colleagues. This is crucial because expectations often become self-fulfilling prophecies (i.e., Pygmalion vs Golem effects; 24).

2.2 Stigma

Second, decontextualization could lead to stigmatization. Although not directly studied for ADHD, people are more reluctant to interact with people with mental health problems and perceive them as more dangerous, when they assume these mental health problems are caused by biological factors (17, 25, 26). Strikingly, this applies to clinicians as well: Clinicians linking their clients’ problems to biology display less empathy than clinicians attributing problems to non-biological factors (27).

2.3 Overreliance on medication

Third, decontextualization could lead to an overreliance on medication. When the starting point is that ADHD is a (neuro)biological disorder, clinical decision making may guide towards brain-focused solutions like medication (28, 29). Indeed, the more clinicians explain problems by biological factors, the higher they estimate the effectiveness of medication (3033). Similarly, greater emphasis on biological problem explanations also correlates with reduced confidence in the effectiveness of non-pharmacological, often context-focused, treatments (19, 27, 29).

3 A paradigm shift to inform better support for children with ADHD

A shift towards a paradigm that more explicitly acknowledges the influence of contextual factors on ADHD will inform better support for children with ADHD. Clinical guidelines recommend both behavioral parent training and medication as first-choice interventions but in practice, many more children with ADHD receive medication than behavioral parent training (34). An important reason for this discrepancy is that a decontextualized view on ADHD guides children, parents, teachers and also clinicians towards medication before initiating other treatments. Instead, the notion of a socially constructed concept of ADHD, as proposed by Banaschewski and colleagues, automatically puts more emphasis on contextual factors. The logical consequence of this paradigm shift will hopefully be an increase in the implementation of context-focused behavioral interventions for children in ADHD.

Of these interventions, the evidence-base for behavioral parent training is most compelling (for meta-analyses, see 3537). Treatment sequencing studies demonstrate that initiating treatment with such behavioral interventions yields superior outcomes compared to initiating treatment with medication, both in terms of effectiveness and costs (38, 39). Crucially, initiating treatment with behavioral interventions substantially reduces the need for medication later (40). By implementing such a stepped-care approach, overtreatment with medication is prevented while maintaining pharmacological treatment for those children who really need it.

4 Discussion

The upcoming paradigm shift in the field of ADHD will indeed be transformative, as anticipated by Banaschewski and colleagues. While it will undoubtedly come with many challenges, I see at least two clear gains: it will mitigate several unintended but harmful consequences of the current paradigm and it will pave the way to more balanced stepped-care recommendations, ultimately benefiting many children with ADHD and their families.

Author contributions

TD: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Conceptualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Anouk van Dijk, Barbara van den Hoofdakker, Branko van Hulst, Sanne te Meerman and Yehuda Pollak for many inspiring conversations that have fueled my thoughts as outlined in this commentary. Parts of this commentary are adapted from a grant proposal I submitted to the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development in January, 2024.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Banaschewski T, Häge A, Hohmann S, Melcher K. Perspectives on ADHD in children and adolescents as a social construct amidst rising prevalence of diagnosis and medication use. Front Psychiatry. (2024) 14:1289157. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1289157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Faraone SV, Banaschewski T, Coghill D, Zheng Y, Biederman J, Bellgrove MA, et al. The world federation of ADHD international consensus statement: 208 evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 128:789–818. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Mitchell J, Read J. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, drug companies and the internet. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2012) 17:121–39. doi: 10.1177/1359104511410151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Bourdaa M, Konsman JP, Sécail C, Venturini T, Veyrat-Masson I, Gonon F. Does television reflect the evolution of scientific knowledge? The case of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder coverage on French television. Public Underst Sci. (2015) 24:200–9. doi: 10.1177/0963662514525419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Freedman JE. An analysis of the discourses on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in US special education textbooks, with implications for inclusive education. Int J Inclusive Educ. (2016) 20:32–51. doi: 10.1080/13603116.2015.1026624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Batstra L, Foget L, van Haeringen C, Te Meerman S, Thoutenhoofd ED. What children and young people learn about ADHD from youth information books: A text analysis of nine books on ADHD available in Dutch. Scand J Child Adolesc Psychiatr Psychol. (2020) 8:1–9. doi: 10.21307/sjcapp-2020-001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. van Langen MJ, Szőke R, Rijkelijkhuizen DN, Durston S, van Hulst BM. Lost in explanation: internal conflicts in the discourse of ADHD psychoeducation. BMC Psychiatry. (2022) 22:1–9. doi: 10.1186/s12888-022-03955-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Keilow M, Wu C, Obel C. Cumulative social disadvantage and risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Results from a nationwide cohort study. SSM Population Health. (2020) 10:100548. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Björkenstam E, Björkenstam C, Jablonska B, Kosidou K. Cumulative exposure to childhood adversity, and treated attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a cohort study of 543 650 adolescents and young adults in Sweden. Psychol Med. (2018) 48:498–507. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Ouyang L, Fang X, Mercy J, Perou R, Grosse SD. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and child maltreatment: a population-based study. J Pediatr. (2008) 153:851–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.05.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Landhuis CE, Poulton R, Welch D, Hancox RJ. Does childhood television viewing lead to attention problems in adolescence? Results prospective longitudinal study Pediatr. (2007) 120:532–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-3004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Østergaard SD, Larsen JT, Dalsgaard S, Wilens TE, Mortensen PB, Agerbo E, et al. Predicting ADHD by assessment of Rutter’s indicators of adversity in infancy. PloS One. (2016) 11:e0157352. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Caye A, Petresco S, de Barros AJD, Bressan RA, Gadelha A, Goncalves H, et al. Relative age and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Data from three epidemiological cohorts and a meta-analysis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 59:990–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.07.905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Phelan JC. Geneticization of deviant behavior and consequences for stigma: the case of mental illness. J Health Soc Behav. (2005) 46:307–22. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bennett L, Thirlaway K, Murray AJ. The stigmatising implications of presenting schizophrenia as a genetic disease. J Genet Counsel. (2008) 17:550–9. doi: 10.1007/s10897-008-9177-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lebowitz MS, Rosenthal JE, Ahn WK. Effects of biological versus psychosocial explanations on stigmatization of children with ADHD. J Atten Disord. (2016) 20:240–50. doi: 10.1177/1087054713489208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lebowitz MS, Appelbaum PS. Biomedical explanations of psychopathology and their implications for attitudes and beliefs about mental disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2019) 15:555–77. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Lebowitz MS, Ahn W, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Fixable or fate? Perceptions of the biology of depression. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2013) 81:518–27. doi: 10.1037/a0031637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kemp JJ, Lickel JJ, Deacon BJ. Effects of a chemical imbalance causal explanation on individuals’ perceptions of their depressive symptoms. Behav Res Ther. (2014) 56:47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Lebowitz MS, Ahn W. Blue genes? Understanding and mitigating negative consequences of personalized information about genetic risk for depression. J Genet Counsel. (2018) 27:204–16. doi: 10.1007/s10897-017-0162-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lebowitz MS, Pyun JJ, Ahn WK. Biological explanations of generalized anxiety disorder: effects on beliefs about prognosis and responsibility. Psychiatr Serv. (2014) 65:498–503. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201300101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hennig T, Reininger KM, Schütt ML, Doll J, Ricken G. Essentialist beliefs about attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): an empirical study with preservice teachers. Emot Behav Difficulties. (2023) 28:52–67. doi: 10.1080/13632752.2022.1973074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Eisenberg D, Schneider H. Perceptions of academic skills of children diagnosed with ADHD. J Atten Disord. (2007) 10:390–7. doi: 10.1177/1087054706292105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Rosenthal R, Jacobson L. Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher Expectation and pupils’ Intellectual Development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston (1968).

Google Scholar

25. Haslam N. Genetic essentialism, neuroessentialism, and stigma: commentary on Dar-Nimrod and Heine. Psychol Bull. (2011) 137:819–24. doi: 10.1037/a0021860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kvaale EP, Haslam N, Gottdiener WH. The ‘side effects’ of medicalization: A meta-analytic review of how biogenetic explanations affect stigma. Clin Psychol Rev. (2013) 33:782–94. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lebowitz MS, Ahn WK. Effects of biological explanations for mental disorders on clinicians’ empathy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2014) 111:17786–90. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414058111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Iselin MG, Addis ME. Effects of etiology on perceived helpfulness of treatments for depression. Cognit Ther Res. (2003) 27:205–22. doi: 10.1023/A:1023931510273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lebowitz MS, Appelbaum PS. Beneficial and detrimental effects of genetic explanations for addiction. Int J Soc Psychiatry. (2017) 63:717–23. doi: 10.1177/0020764017715113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Ahn W, Proctor CC, Flanagan EH. Mental health clinicians’ beliefs about the biological, psychological, and environmental bases of mental disorders. Cognit Sci. (2009) 33:147–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01010.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Lüllmann E, Berendes S, Rief W, Lincoln TM. Benefits and harms of providing biological causal models in the treatment of psychosis—an experimental study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2011) 42:447–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Carter L, Read J, Pyle M, Morrison AP. The impact of causal explanations on outcome in people experiencing psychosis: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2017) 24:332–47. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Gershkovich M, Deacon BJ, Wheaton MG. Biomedical causal attributions for obsessive–compulsive disorder: associations with patient perceptions of prognosis and treatment expectancy. J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord. (2018) 18:81–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2018.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Dekkers TJ, Groenman AP, Wessels L, Kovshoff H, Hoekstra PJ, van den Hoofdakker BJ. Which factors determine clinicians’ policy and attitudes towards medication and parent training for children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2022) 31:483–93. doi: 10.1007/s00787-021-01735-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Dekkers TJ, Hornstra R, van der Oord S, Luman M, Hoekstra PJ, Groenman AP, et al. Meta-analysis: Which components of parent training work for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2022) 61:478–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Doffer DPA, Dekkers TJ, Hornstra R, van der Oord S, Luman M, Leijten P, et al. Sustained improvements by behavioral parent training for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis into longer-term child and parental outcomes. JCPP Advances. (2023) e12196. doi: 10.1002/jcv2.12196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hornstra R, Groenman AP, van der Oord S, Luman M, Dekkers TJ, van der Veen-Mulders L, et al. Review: Which components of behavioral parent and teacher training work for children with ADHD? A meta-regression analysis on child behavioral outcomes. Child Adolesc Ment Health. (2023) 2:258–68. doi: 10.1111/camh.12561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Page TF, Pelham WE, Fabiano GA, Greiner AR, Gnagy EM, Hart KC, et al. Comparative cost analysis of sequential, adaptive, behavioral, pharmacological, and combined treatments for childhood ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2016) 45:416–27. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.910791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Pelham WE Jr., Fabiano GA, Waxmonsky JG, Greiner AR, Gnagy EM, Pelham WE, et al. Treatment sequencing for childhood ADHD: A multiple-randomization study of adaptive medication and behavioral interventions. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2016) 45:396–415. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1046179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Coles EK, Pelham WE, Fabiano GA, Gnagy EM, Burrows-MacLean L, Wymbs BT, et al. Randomized trial of first-line behavioral intervention to reduce need for medication in children with ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2020) 49:673–87. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2019.1673426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ADHD, decontextualization, stigma, medication, prognostic pessimism, paradigm

Citation: Dekkers TJ (2024) Commentary: Perspectives on ADHD in children and adolescents as a social construct amidst rising prevalence of diagnosis and medication use. Front. Psychiatry 15:1383492. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1383492

Received: 07 February 2024; Accepted: 14 March 2024;
Published: 25 March 2024.

Edited by:

Julie M. Zito, University of Maryland, United States

Reviewed by:

Jörg Michael Fegert, University of Ulm, Germany

Copyright © 2024 Dekkers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tycho J. Dekkers, dC5kZWtrZXJzQGFjY2FyZS5ubA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.