Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 26 April 2024
Sec. Mood Disorders

Environmental stress and emotional reactivity: an exploratory experience sampling method study

  • 1School of Public Health, LKS Faculty of Medicine, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, School of Clinical Medicine, LKS Faculty of Medicine, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
  • 3Lee Kong Chian School of Medicine, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
  • 4Department of Psychosis, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore, Singapore
  • 5State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Background: Previous studies have shown a relationship between environments and mental health. However, limited studies have investigated the impact of environment stress (ES) on emotional reactivity. Our study aimed to fill this gap by examining how daily ES affects momentary emotional reactivity using experience sampling method (ESM).

Methods: Participants were randomly recruited from a prospective cohort study in Hong Kong to participate in a 7-day ESM study. The participants received eight electronic signals daily assessing their ES, positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA). Participants were categorized into depressed group or control group based on Revised Clinical Interview Schedule. Psychometric properties of the ESM assessment were evaluated. Multilevel linear regression analyzes were conducted to examine the association of ES with PA, NA and the group status of the participants (cases versus controls).

Results: A total of 15 participants with depression and 15 healthy controls were recruited, and 1307 momentary assessments were completed with a compliance rate of 77.8%. The depressed group demonstrated a significant increase in NA in response to ES, while the control group showed a decrease in PA. In addition, the depressed group reported a lower perception of control and interaction with their environment compared to the control group.

Conclusion: Using ESM, a valid, reliable, and easy-to-use self-reporting tool, our findings provided valuable insights on the potential mechanisms underlying emotional responses to stressful environments.

Introduction

With a large number of people living in cities, urban environments have become increasingly prevalent globally. Earlier studies have consistently highlighted the relationship between the environment and mood disorders (1). However, there is limited evidence on how the environment affects our everyday emotions. Emotions can fluctuate moment to moment in response to the stress from the surrounding environment, also known as environmental stress (ES) (24).

Understanding the connection between ES and emotional reactions is essential for developing effective interventions and promoting population mental health. Stress has been conceptualized as the individual’s subjective evaluation of the perceived stress of specific events and minor disturbances that occur as part of their daily routine (5). These daily stressors typically refer to the challenges of day-to-day living, ranging from common familial issues to workplace conflict and obstacles from the physical environment (6). ES is the perceived environmental-related stress in daily life, while immediate emotional reactions, such as positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA), represent the response to these daily stressors (7). This complex interplay of emotional reactivity to ES has been explored using the experience sampling method (ESM), a structured self-reported technique designed to examine subjective momentary experiences in everyday life (810). In the early stages, ESM primarily used traditional pen-and-paper diaries or questionnaires. However, as technology advanced, electronic devices and smartphone applications have been integrated into momentary assessment (911). Using electronic devices, participants are prompted at random intervals to fill out questionnaires capturing their current emotions, environment and activities. ESM offers the advantage of collecting data that concerns both the context (e.g., location and situation) and structure (e.g., the associations between environment and emotion) (12). These daily techniques provide a reliable means to study various aspects of a people’s life throughout the day (13). Utilizing ESM to collect real-time responses at multiple time points reduces recall bias and assessment error, improving ecological validity and reliability (14). It is widely used in studies on people with depression, psychotic disorder and substance abuse (1517).

The impact of environment on emotional responses fluctuates among individuals. Studies have shown that stress-related emotions, in particular, sadness-depression, commonly arise from individual-environment interactions (18). Therefore, studying emotional reactions to environmental stress in people suffering from depression is important for understanding this dynamic. A recent systematic review found that the ESM, when deployed through electronic devices, effectively measures psychological outcomes such as mood and stress in adult patients with various physical conditions (11). Momentary affect has been largely correlated with the physical and social context of the environment. For instance, NA has been associated with being alone and at the hospital, whereas PA is often linked to environments and public locations crowded with people (19). Despite this, the application of smartphone-based ESM for studying urban stress and its psychometric properties is limited (20). Enhancing our understanding of environmental stressors and the dynamic change in emotional responses is particularly important considering the established relationship between the environment and mental health. By employing ESM, we can gain valuable insights into how people with depression respond to environmental stressors.

This exploratory study was designed to investigate the momentary emotional responses to daily ES using ESM. By collecting real-time data in naturalistic settings, we aimed to compare the effects of ES on individuals with and without depression. We hypothesized that ES would increase NA and decrease PA. As compared to healthy individuals, we hypothesized that participants with depression would exhibit greater emotional reactivity (both PA and NA), interact less with their environment, and perceive a lower sense of personal control over their surroundings.

Materials and methods

Study design and participants

This study used data from a 3-year longitudinal follow-up study of the Hong Kong Mental Morbidity Survey (HKMMS). The baseline HKMMS was a population-representative survey consisting of 5719 Chinese adults aged between 16 and 75 living in Hong Kong (21, 22). After 3 years, a follow-up study was conducted to investigate the long-term mental health outcomes. In this exploratory case-control study, invitations were randomly sent to a total of 45 participants (23 cases and 22 control subjects), out of which 33 of them (17 cases and 16 control subjects) agreed to participate in the study. One particular case was unable to be matched with control subjects due to the challenges encountered in finding a suitable match. A comparison between those who agreed to participate and those who declined was conducted, revealing no significant differences between the two groups in terms of age, gender, education level, marital status and employment status (p > 0.05). Participants with a score of 12 or higher on the Revised Clinical Interview Schedule (CIS-R) (23) were recruited as cases. Depression diagnosis was further ascertained using the Chinese-bilingual version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Axis I, patient version (SCID) (24, 25). Control subjects were age-, gender- and education-matched to the cases, and had no lifetime history of mental disorders as defined by the SCID. Exclusion criteria included intellectual disability, dementia or significant cognitive impairment, lifetime history of psychotic disorders, major visual problem that affected perception of the surrounding environment, and inability to use a smartphone-based program. The study was carried out in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical approval was obtained from the Institutional Review Board of the University of Hong Kong/Hospital Authority Hong Kong West Cluster (Reference No. UW15-304). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants at the time of recruitment.

ESM procedure and assessments

Potential participants were contacted by phone to explain the aim and objective of this study. After obtaining participants’ consent, face-to-face interviews were conducted at their homes for an initial assessment and detailed explanation of the study procedures. Participants were given a smartphone (Samsung Galaxy J5) with the ESM application installed. The device was programmed to emit random electronic signals eight times a day between 10:00 am and 10:00 pm for seven consecutive days. At least one signal was scheduled within each 90-minute span using a random number table. The timing of the signals was designed to balance the participant burden while ensuring the collection of valid data at random timepoints throughout the day. On receiving each electronic signal, participants were prompted to complete a self-reported questionnaire within a 15-minute window. Any data submitted beyond this time were discarded. The same set of questionnaires was used throughout the study period, with each taking approximately 2 minutes to complete. The ESM procedure was explained to participants during a briefing session, and a practice trial was administrated prior to the initial data collection to ensure that the participants understood the instructions. Previous studies showed that the application of ESM was feasible and valid among both the general population (26) and people with depression (27).

At each signal, participants were first instructed to take a photograph of their current location, which was used to verify the surrounding environment. They were then asked to respond to a list of 20 ESM items (Table 1), most of which were rated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not at all; 7 = very much). Additional context-based questions regarding their location and social environment were answered by selecting from a list of options. Emotional states were assessed with four PA (items 2, 4, 6 and 9: happy, relaxed, satisfied and excited) and four NA (items 3, 5, 7 and 8: anxious, lonely, irritated and sad). PA and NA scores were derived from the average of their respective items. With reference to previous ESM studies on measuring activity- or event-related stress (5, 28, 29), we assessed ES by asking the extent to which participants currently experience discomfort with the environment. ES was considered any of the three self-reported items measuring arousal (item 10: I feel uncomfortable my current environment), frustration (item 11: I am frustrated with my current environment) and stress perceived from the surrounding environment (item 12: I would like to leave this place). ES score was calculated as the average of these three items. Interaction, personal control and social engagement in the environment were measured with items 13 to 18. A general statement of ‘my mood is affected by the surrounding environment right now’ was asked at the end of assessment. In addition to the ESM procedure, the CIS-R total scores were retrieved from the follow-up study in assessing psychological distress. The score was derived from 14 sections of non-psychotic symptoms, with higher scores indicating greater psychological distress (23, 30).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 List of ESM items.

Data processing

During the 7-day assessment, each participant generated eight time-point entries per day, resulting in a maximum of 56 observations. Assuming full compliant from 30 participants, the dataset would comprise a total of 1680 observations. Data processing and cleaning were performed according to the data management guide for experience-sampling studies (31). Before data analysis, the ESM data were checked to identify any problematic entries. Specifically, we classified entries that took less than 10 seconds to complete (less than 0.5 seconds per item) as non-compliant responses, indicating the participant tapped the answers without considering the options. Furthermore, trials with over 90% of identical entries were considered as invalid and excluded from the analysis (32).

Statistical analysis

The psychometric properties of the ESM assessment were first examined. The four PA items, four NA items and three ES items were assessed for internal consistency using Cronbach’s alpha coefficient. The convergent validity of the affect items was evaluated using correlational analysis of CIS-R total scores with PA and NA scores. The ESM data followed a hierarchical structure in which multiple momentary observations are nested within participants. Due to the highly correlated observations within each individual, multilinear regression modeling was employed to analyze ESM data variability at individual and group levels simultaneously (33). Prior research has proposed compliance rate cut-offs between 30% and 60% of prompts (28, 3436). Given the limited exploration of momentary environmental stress, our study employed a higher compliance rate of 70% to effectively capture a comprehensive picture of participants’ daily experiences in this understudied area. Participants who completed less than 70% of the data points (fewer than 40 self-reports out of 56) were excluded from the analysis. Prior to analysis, the individual-level covariates (e.g., the ES) at different data points were centred around the individuals means (37). Multilevel linear regression analysis was first conducted with PA and NA as the dependent variables and ES as independent variables in the model. These models were then further adjusted for age, gender and the CIS-R total score. Additionally, in the separate ES-sensitivity models, ES, group status (0 = control, 1 = depressed) and their interaction term (ES × GROUP) were included as independent variables to examine whether the group status (healthy controls versus depressed participants) moderated emotional reactivity to the daily environment. The analysis of interaction and personal control with the environment between groups were conducted using same procedure. All statistical analyzes were carried out using SPSS Statistics version 20.0 (38).

Results

Participant characteristics

Seventeen participants with depression and sixteen healthy matched control subjects were recruited. Two individuals from the depression group and one from the control group failed to complete at least 40 entries (compliance rate ranging from 11% to 39%) and were excluded from the analyzes. The final sample consisted of 15 depressed cases and 15 control subjects. Women comprised two thirds of the sample. The mean age of the sample was 49.4 years (SD 8.9 years; range 31 to 66 years), and 80% of the participants had a secondary education or above. There were no significant differences between two groups regarding marital status (p = 0.256) or employment status (p = 0.143). The median CIS-R scores for the control subjects and depressed cases were 0 (range 0 to 7) and 23 (range 12 to 33), respectively. The sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the sample are depicted in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the sample.

Table 3 presents the ESM characteristics of the sample. Of the 1680 signals sent, we received 1307 completed momentary assessments (675 from the control group and 635 from the depressed group). All of the participants completed at least 70% of the entries. The overall compliance rate was 77.8% (range 71.4% to 89.3%). The control group had a higher compliance rate (78.6%) than the depressed group (75%) (p = 0.008). Compared to the control group, the depressed group displayed a lower PA (p < 0.001), higher NA (p < 0.001) and increased ES (p < 0.001). The frequency distribution of the responses on PA, NA and ES are presented in Supplementary Figures 1.1, 1.2 and 1.3, respectively. A comprehensive view of ESM responses on PA for 15 cases and 15 control subjects is shown in Supplementary Figure 2. Participants with depression reported spending more alone time (p < 0.001), had more time at home (p < 0.001) and were less engaged in social events (p < 0.001) than the control participants. They also indicated less impact of the environment on their mood (p < 0.001).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 ESM characteristics of the sample.

Psychometric properties of ESM assessment

The Cronbach’s alpha for momentary assessments was 0.96 for PA, 0.80 for NA and 0.98 for ES, indicating good internal consistency among the items in the three subscales (39). The correlation coefficient between PA and NA was moderately high (-0.64), suggesting a significant negative correlation between the two opposite affect items. Furthermore, a correlation of -0.68 of PA and a correlation of 0.51 of NA with the CIS-R total score suggested moderate to good convergent validity of the affect items.

Association between ES and affects

Multilevel linear regression analysis revealed significant associations between ES and PA (B = -1.21; 95% CI -1.64 to -0.78; p < 0.001) and between ES and NA (B = 0.45; 95% CI 0.26 to 0.63; p < 0.001) in the full sample of 30 participants. These associations persisted after adjustment for age, gender and the CIS-R total score (Table 4). A significant interaction effect of ES × GROUP was found for both PA (B = 0.71; 95% CI 0.35 to 1.08; p < 0.001) and NA (B = -0.34; 95% CI -0.54 to -0.13; p = 0.003), indicating that the group modified the positive and negative emotional reactivity toward ES (Table 5). When stratified by group, ES was associated with a decreased PA for the control group (B = -1.77; 95% CI -2.39 to -1.16; p < 0.001), while no association was observed for depressed group (B = -0.12; 95% CI -0.80 to 0.57; p = 0.723). On the other hand, ES was associated with an increased NA for the depressed group (B = 0.35; 95% CI 0.03 to 0.68; p = 0.031), but this was not seen in the control group (B = 0.08; 95% CI -0.18 to 0.34; p = 0.527) (Supplementary Table 1). Moreover, participants with depression reported less interaction with the environment (B = -1.20; 95% CI -2.29 to -0.11; p = 0.032) and perceived less personal control over their environment compared to those in the control group (B = -1.37; 95% CI -2.52 to -0.22; p = 0.021) (Supplementary Table 2).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Multilevel model estimates for PA and NA with ES as predictor (n=30).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 Multilevel model estimates for PA and NA with ES, GROUP and ES x GROUP as predictors (n=30).

Discussion

To our knowledge, our study is among the very few studies that employed ESM to investigate emotional responses to daily environmental stress. We recruited 15 participants with depression and 15 matched healthy controls, and 1307 momentary assessments were completed with a satisfactory compliance rate of 77.8%. The diary technique of ESM captures momentary emotions, events and environmental conditions through intensive and repeated self-report measures. By assessing participants’ perceptions of daily life conditions, ESM offers insights into underlying psychological processes (9, 40, 41). We specifically examined environment-related stress experience and emotional reactivity concurrently. The collection of real-time momentary experiences minimized retrospective bias, and therefore are able to reflect a more accurate relationship between the environment, stress and affective states at any given moment. Furthermore, our findings demonstrated excellent internal consistency for affect and ES assessments. The emotional states obtained through the ESM also correlated well with CIS-R measures, indicating good convergent validity of the affect items. In sum, ESM is a feasible, valid and reliable tool in assessing environmental factors and emotion in daily life.

Environmental stress and emotional reactivity

Our study identified a significant relationship between ES and emotional reactivity. We noted that stress from the environment was associated with lower PA and higher NA, regardless of participants’ demographic characteristics or levels of psychological distress (42). However, emotional responses involving changes in various response systems such as feelings, perceptions, and behavior, may vary between healthy individuals and those with depression (43). We therefore examined whether participants’ health status (healthy versus depressed) altered emotional reactions to daily environment factors. We observed a notable increase in NA towards environmental stressors in the depressed group, while no such increase was found in control group. These results aligned with previous studies that individuals with depression tend to experience negative emotional reactions to daily stress across different contexts, such as events, activities and social interactions (28, 44, 45). For instance, Myin-Germeys et al. (28) examined emotional reactivity in different patient groups with severe mental illnesses and found that the group with major depressive disorder displayed greater stress-induced NA compared to healthy control subjects. In another ESM study of 279 twin pairs, Wichers et al. (46) suggested that the tendency to respond to daily life stressors with NA could be considered as a risk of depression. Moderately stressful events induced minimal effect to healthy people but greatly triggered NA in those with depression or a strong familial predisposition to depression. Our study built upon previous findings and provided further evidence that daily life stressors are not limited to specific events or activities, but also encompass the influence of physical environment.

Environmental stress in depressed individuals

Negative emotional reactions to daily ES are a potential marker for depression, as supported by other observational studies linking stress induced by adverse environment to an increased depression risk (47, 48). This mechanism could be explained by stress sensitivity, where individuals with higher sensitivity to stress are more prone to developing affective symptoms (49). People with history of depression exhibit greater sensitivity to daily stresses, and even modest stressors can trigger NA. Our findings suggest that daily hassles in environment could heighten stress sensitivity in those with depression, thereby intensifying their negative reactions. Despite facing more ES, the depressed participants perceived their mood to be less affected by the environment. These findings suggest that people with depression may encounter stressful experience with limited awareness of how their mood is influenced by the environment.

Environmental stress in healthy individuals

Notably, healthy control subjects in our study showed no changes in NA but exhibited a significant decrease in PA when exposed to environmental stressors. However, we could not observe this trend in depressed participants, which contradicts our hypothesis. Several factors could explain this discrepancy. First, the depressed group consistently had low PA levels throughout the study period, reflecting the pathological state of depression and made it difficult to detect emotional reactivity (28). Second, previous studies have shown that naturalistic acute stressors reduce hedonic capacity or the ability to experience pleasure in healthy subjects (50, 51). Perceived stress manifests as decreased PA rather than increased NA in non-clinical populations (52). Third, the healthy participants may have been more conscious of the surrounding environment than the depressed subjects. Many of them expressed concerns about the environment and its potential impact, which might have increased their sensitivity to emotional changes associated with their surroundings. Lastly, the effect of ES on affect may differ among individuals with different personality traits like agreeableness, neuroticism and extraversion (53, 54). Future studies should account for these factors while examining the affective response to ES.

Environment and stress vulnerability

Our findings indicate a difference in emotional response to ES between people with depression and those without. Depressed individuals tend to manifest stress through NA, while healthy individuals exhibit stress through PA. These results suggest that the psychopathological pathway of depression may be associated with stress in urban environments. Ecological factors contribute to depression and may be influenced by other psychosocial mediators such as perceived control and interactions with physical environment. Individuals with a lower sense of control over their environment are less receptive to positive experiences and more sensitive to daily environmental stressors, may make them more susceptible to depression. In addition, our findings suggest that underlying vulnerability modifies how individuals respond to stressors in different situations. Intervention strategies should consider the environmental contextual factors and the psychosocial mechanism connecting everyday stressful environments to symptomatology.

Strengths and limitations

This study has several strengths worthy of note. First, the methodological design using ESM offered not only strong ecological validity, but also good reliability, improved accuracy and reduced retrospective bias. By collecting real-time data on daily affect, environmental stress and other relevant information for a week, we assessed moment-to-moment experiences in participants’ everyday environment. To ensure a comprehensive understanding of these experiences, we placed great emphasis on achieving a high compliance rate among participants, ensuring a robust and diverse set of information for analysis. The ESM technique proves particularly beneficial for people with depression, who are frequently reported to have a negative retrospective bias (55, 56). Second, we focused on the stressful experiences the participants perceived from their environment instead of assessing the quality of the environment. This approach allowed us to unambiguously capture the adverse psychological effects (i.e., emotional reactivity) caused by suboptimal environments (57). Third, we incorporated multiple information sources, which enhanced the accuracy of the ESM measures. For example, participants were asked to provide photographs for location verification and CIS-R scores were retrieved from interview for between-subject measures. Finally, the use of semi-structured SCID interviews ensured adequate diagnostic verification and confirmation of symptom severity to support the participants’ eligibility.

Several methodological limitations must be addressed. Despite employing an advanced experience sampling method, this study was limited by its modest sample size and potentially limited generalizability of the findings. Further investigation with a larger sample size is warranted. In addition, a practice effect was commonly reported in ESM studies, especially among patients with major depressive disorder (27, 58). Although we did not observe a decrease in the response latencies over the course of the study (practice effect not supported), the depressed participants had a lower compliance rate compared to the controls, which could potentially contribute to a reduced statistical power. Nevertheless, the compliance rate of 75% in the depressed group was comparable with, and somewhat higher than the rates reported in previous studies (59, 60). Besides, it is important to note that while negatively appraised environments are not necessarily the same as stressful environments, there is often an overlap in daily life. Participants who are predisposed to high NA might report elevated ES. Similarly, the more intense an environment is, it tends to be perceived as negative (61). The response bias should be acknowledged. Another limitation was the self-reported nature of the ESM, which predominantly assessed the subjective affective states. Future studies should consider incorporating objective physiological measures, such as heart rate or cortisol level, to gain a more comprehensive understanding of the stress response to the environment. Finally, the concept of environmental stress and its associated contextual factors like location or social situation, have yet to be fully explored. Future research should explore the impact of ES on emotions in different contexts.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this study highlighted the importance role of environment-related stress in regulating emotional responses. By utilizing ESM, a reliable and valid self-report tool, we observed significant variability of emotional reactions between depressed and healthy individuals. Importantly, exposure to environmental stressors led to a rise in NA for individuals with depression and a reduction in PA for the healthy participants. Furthermore, people with depression perceived less control over their environment and had less interaction with it. This study extended beyond previous studies by shedding light on the potential mechanisms underlying emotional reactions to stressful environments. The role of daily-environment stress-sensitivity fostered our understanding of the psychological mechanisms contributing to depression. Future investigation into this mechanism would help to develop ecological momentary interventions targeting resilience building towards daily environmental stressors and promoting effective self-management of emotions.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Institutional Review Board of the University of Hong Kong/Hospital Authority Hong Kong West Cluster (Reference No. UW15-304). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

CW: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. WC: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. KL: Visualization, Writing – review & editing. EC: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. LL: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Health and Health Services Research Fund, Food and Health Bureau of the Government of the Hong Kong SAR (Grant No. 09101601) and Commissioned Research on Mental Health Policy and Services, Food and Health Bureau, Hong Kong SAR Government (Grant No. SMH-48).

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants from the Hong Kong Mental Morbidity Survey (HKMMS) who contributed their invaluable time and perspectives to this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1375735/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Galea S. The urban brain: new directions in research exploring the relation between cities and mood-anxiety disorders. Depress Anxiety. (2011) 28:857–62. doi: 10.1002/da.20868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Brown LH, Strauman T, Barrantes-Vidal N, Silvia PJ, Kwapil TR. An experience-sampling study of depressive symptoms and their social context. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2011) 199:403–9. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31821cd24b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. aan het Rot M, Hogenelst K, Schoevers RA. Mood disorders in everyday life: a systematic review of experience sampling and ecological momentary assessment studies. Clin Psychol Rev. (2012) 32:510–23. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Höhn P, Menne-Lothmann C, Peeters F, Nicolson NA, Jacobs N, Derom C, et al. Moment-to-moment transfer of positive emotions in daily life predicts future course of depression in both general population and patient samples. PLoS One. (2013) 8:e75655. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Myin-Germeys I, van Os J, Schwartz JE, Stone AA, Delespaul PA. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2001) 58:1137–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.12.1137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Caspi A, Bolger N, Eckenrode J. Linking person and context in the daily stress process. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1987) 52:184–95. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.1.184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Myin-Germeys I, van Os J. Stress-reactivity in psychosis: evidence for an affective pathway to psychosis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2007) 27:409–24. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R. Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. J Nerv Ment Dis. (1987) 175:526–36. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198709000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2008) 4:1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Myin-Germeys I, Kasanova Z, Vaessen T, Vachon H, Kirtley O, Viechtbauer W, et al. Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: new insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry. (2018) 17:123–32. doi: 10.1002/wps.20513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Yang YS, Ryu GW, Choi M. Methodological strategies for ecological momentary assessment to evaluate mood and stress in adult patients using mobile phones: systematic review. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. (2019) 7:e11215. doi: 10.2196/11215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hurlburt RT. Randomly sampling thinking in the natural environment. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1997) 65:941–9. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.65.6.941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hektner JM, Schmidt JA, Csikszentmihalyi M. Experience sampling method: measuring the quality of everyday life. Sage Publications, Inc. (2007).

Google Scholar

14. Augustine AA, RJ. L, Mehl MR, C. T eds. Handbook of Research Methods for Studying Daily Life. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2012) p. 497–510.

Google Scholar

15. Myin-Germeys I, Birchwood M, Kwapil T. From environment to therapy in psychosis: a real-world momentary assessment approach. Schizophr Bull. (2011) 37:244–7. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Kramer I, Simons CJP, Hartmann JA, Menne-Lothmann C, Viechtbauer W, Peeters F, et al. A therapeutic application of the experience sampling method in the treatment of depression: a randomized controlled trial. World Psychiatry. (2014) 13:68–77. doi: 10.1002/wps.20090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Fatseas M, Serre F, Alexandre J-M, Debrabant R, Auriacombe M, Swendsen J. Craving and substance use among patients with alcohol, tobacco, cannabis or heroin addiction: a comparison of substance- and person-specific cues. Addiction. (2015) 110:1035–42. doi: 10.1111/add.12882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Lazarus RS, Cohen JB. “Environmental stress”. In: Altman I, Wohlwill JF, editors. Human behavior and environment. Boston, MA: Springer (1977). p. 89–127.

Google Scholar

19. Yang YS, Ryu GW, Park CG, Yeom I, Shim KW, Choi M. Mood and stress evaluation of adult patients with Moyamoya disease in Korea: ecological momentary assessment method using a mobile phone app. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. (2020) 8:e17034. doi: 10.2196/17034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bergou N, Hammoud R, Smythe M, Gibbons J, Davidson N, Tognin S, et al. The mental health benefits of visiting canals and rivers: An ecological momentary assessment study. PloS One. (2022) 17:e0271306. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0271306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lam LC, Chan WC, Wong CS, Chen EY, Ng RM, Lee EH, et al. The Hong Kong mental morbidity survey: background and study design. East Asian Arch Psychiatry. (2014) 24:30–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

22. Lam LC, Wong CS, Wang MJ, Chan WC, Chen EY, Ng RM, et al. Prevalence, psychosocial correlates and service utilization of depressive and anxiety disorders in Hong Kong: the Hong Kong Mental Morbidity Survey (HKMMS). Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2015) 50:1379–88. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1014-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Chan WC, Wong CS, Chen EY, Ng RM, Hung SF, Cheung EF, et al. Validation of the Chinese version of the revised clinical interview schedule: findings from Hong Kong mental morbidity survey. East Asian Arch Psychiatry. (2017) 27:3–10.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

24. So E, Kam I, Leung C, Chung D, Liu Z, Fong S. The Chinese-bilingual SCID-I/P project: stage 1-reliability for mood disorders and schizophrenia. Hong Kong J Psychiatry. (2003) 13:7–18.

Google Scholar

25. So E, Kam I, Leung C, Pang A, Lam L. The Chinese bilingual SCID-I/P project: stage 2-reliability for anxiety disorders, adjustment disorders and ‘no diagnosis’. Hong Kong J Psychiatry. (2003) 13:19–25.

Google Scholar

26. van Os J, Verhagen S, Marsman A, Peeters F, Bak M, Marcelis M, et al. The experience sampling method as an mHealth tool to support self-monitoring, self-insight, and personalized health care in clinical practice. Depress Anxiety. (2017) 34:481–93. doi: 10.1002/da.22647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Telford C, McCarthy-Jones S, Corcoran R, Rowse G. Experience sampling methodology studies of depression: the state of the art. Psychol Med. (2012) 42:1119–29. doi: 10.1017/s0033291711002200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Myin-Germeys I, Peeters F, Havermans R, Nicolson NA, DeVries MW, Delespaul P, et al. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis and affective disorder: an experience sampling study. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2003) 107:124–31. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.02025.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Booij SH, Snippe E, Jeronimus BF, Wichers M, Wigman JTW. Affective reactivity to daily life stress: Relationship to positive psychotic and depressive symptoms in a general population sample. J Affect Disord. (2018) 225:474–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Lewis G, Pelosi AJ, Araya R, Dunn G. Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: a standardized assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychol Med. (1992) 22:465–86. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700030415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. McCabe KO, Mack L, Fleeson W. “A guide for data cleaning in experience sampling studies”. In: Mehl MR, Conner TS, editors. Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2012). p. 321–38.

Google Scholar

32. Christensen TC, Barrett LF, Bliss-Moreau E, Lebo K, Kaschub C. A practical guide to experience-sampling procedures. J Happiness Stud. (2003) 4:53–78. doi: 10.1023/A:1023609306024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fleeson W. “Using experience sampling and multilevel modeling to study person-situation interactionist approaches to positive psychology”. In: Ong AD, Van Dulmen MHM, editors. Oxford Handbook of Methods in Positive Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2007). p. 501–14.

Google Scholar

34. Jacobs N, Myin-Germeys I, Derom C, Delespaul P, van Os J, Nicolson NA. A momentary assessment study of the relationship between affective and adrenocortical stress responses in daily life. Biol Psychol. (2007) 74:60–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Block VJ, Meyer AH, Miché M, Mikoteit T, Hoyer J, Imboden C, et al. The effect of anticipatory stress and openness and engagement on subsequently perceived sleep quality-An Experience Sampling Method study. J Sleep Res. (2020) 29:e12957. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Das-Friebel A, Lenneis A, Realo A, Sanborn A, Tang NKY, Wolke D, et al. Bedtime social media use, sleep, and affective wellbeing in young adults: an experience sampling study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2020) 61:1138–49. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kreft IG, de Leeuw J, Aiken LS. The effect of different forms of centering in hierarchical linear models. Multivariate Behav Res. (1995) 30:1–21. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3001_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp (2011).

Google Scholar

39. Gliem JA, Gliem RR. “Calculating, interpreting, and reporting Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient for Likert-type scales”. In: Midwest Research to Practice Conference in Adult, Continuing and Community Education, Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, USA (2003) 1:82–7.

Google Scholar

40. Trull TJ, Ebner-Priemer UW. Using experience sampling methods/ecological momentary assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical assessment and clinical research: introduction to the special section. Psychol Assess. (2009) 21:457–62. doi: 10.1037/a0017653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Iida M, Shrout PE, Laurenceau J-P, Bolger N. “Using diary methods in psychological research”. In: Cooper H, editor. APA handbook of research methods in psychology, Vol. 1: foundations, planning, measures, and psychometrics. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (2012) p. 277–305.

Google Scholar

42. Larson R. “The experience sampling method”. In: Csikszentmihalyi M, editor. Flow and the foundations of positive psychology. Dordrecht: Springer (2014). p. 21–34.

Google Scholar

43. Bylsma LM, Morris BH, Rottenberg J. A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. (2008) 28:676–91. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Barge-Schaapveld DQ, Nicolson NA, Berkhof J, deVries MW. Quality of life in depression: daily life determinants and variability. Psychiatry Res. (1999) 88:173–89. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(99)00081-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Wichers M, Schrijvers D, Geschwind N, Jacobs N, Myin-Germeys I, Thiery E, et al. Mechanisms of gene-environment interactions in depression: evidence that genes potentiate multiple sources of adversity. Psychol Med. (2009) 39:1077–86. doi: 10.1017/s0033291708004388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Wichers M, Myin-Germeys I, Jacobs N, Peeters F, Kenis G, Derom C, et al. Genetic risk of depression and stress-induced negative affect in daily life. Br J Psychiatry. (2007) 191:218–23. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.032201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Latkin CA, Curry AD. Stressful neighborhoods and depression: a prospective study of the impact of neighborhood disorder. J Health Soc Behav. (2003) 44:34–44. doi: 10.2307/1519814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Cutrona CE, Wallace G, Wesner KA. Neighborhood characteristics and depression: an examination of stress processes. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2006) 15:188–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00433.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Wichers M, Geschwind N, Jacobs N, Kenis G, Peeters F, Derom C, et al. Transition from stress sensitivity to a depressive state: longitudinal twin study. Br J Psychiatry. (2009) 195:498–503. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.056853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Bogdan R, Pizzagalli DA. Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: implications for depression. Biol Psychiatry. (2006) 60:1147–54. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Pizzagalli DA, Bogdan R, Ratner KG, Jahn AL. Increased perceived stress is associated with blunted hedonic capacity: potential implications for depression research. Behav Res Ther. (2007) 45:2742–53. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Berenbaum H, Connelly J. The effect of stress on hedonic capacity. J Abnorm Psychol. (1993) 102:474–81. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.102.3.474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Miller DJ, Vachon DD, Lynam DR. Neuroticism, negative affect, and negative affect instability: establishing convergent and discriminant validity using ecological momentary assessment. Pers Individ Dif. (2009) 47:873–7. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Sandstrom GM, Lathia N, Mascolo C, Rentfrow PJ. Putting mood in context: using smartphones to examine how people feel in different locations. J Res Pers. (2016) 69:96–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ben-Zeev D, Young MA, Madsen JW. Retrospective recall of affect in clinically depressed individuals and controls. Cognit Emot. (2009) 23:1021–40. doi: 10.1080/02699930802607937

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Ben-Zeev D, Young MA. Accuracy of hospitalized depressed patients' and healthy controls' retrospective symptom reports: an experience sampling study. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2010) 198:280–5. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181d6141f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Watson D. Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive and negative affect: their relation to health complaints, perceived stress, and daily activities. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1988) 54:1020–30. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Husky MM, Gindre C, Mazure CM, Brebant C, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Sanacora G, et al. Computerized ambulatory monitoring in mood disorders: feasibility, compliance, and reactivity. Psychiatry Res. (2010) 178:440–2. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Bylsma LM, Taylor-Clift A, Rottenberg J. Emotional reactivity to daily events in major and minor depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (2011) 120:155–67. doi: 10.1037/a0021662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Palmier-Claus JE, Myin-Germeys I, Barkus E, Bentley L, UdaChina A, Delespaul PA, et al. Experience sampling research in individuals with mental illness: reflections and guidance. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2011) 123:12–20. doi: 10.1111/acps.2010.123.issue-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Koolhaas JM, Bartolomucci A, Buwalda B, de Boer SF, Flügge G, Korte SM, et al. Stress revisited: a critical evaluation of the stress concept. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:1291–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: environmental stress, affect, depression, momentary assessment, experience sampling

Citation: Wong CSM, Chan WC, Lo KWY, Chen EYH and Lam LCW (2024) Environmental stress and emotional reactivity: an exploratory experience sampling method study. Front. Psychiatry 15:1375735. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1375735

Received: 24 January 2024; Accepted: 04 April 2024;
Published: 26 April 2024.

Edited by:

Trine Vik Lagerberg, Oslo University Hospital, Norway

Reviewed by:

Kyle Minor, Purdue University Indianapolis, United States
Manthar Ali Mallah, University of Nebraska Medical Center, United States

Copyright © 2024 Wong, Chan, Lo, Chen and Lam. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Corine Sau Man Wong, wongcsm@hku.hk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.