Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 14 February 2024
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Biological and Non-Pharmacological Treatments of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder and Related Disorders View all 8 articles

Exploring the relationship between context and obsessions in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms: a narrative review

  • Chair of Addiction Research, Institute of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

Obsessions in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) have long been proposed to differ from intrusive thoughts in unaffected individuals based on appraisal of the thoughts. However, more recent research indicates that cognitive processes behind obsessions may differ significantly from those in healthy individuals concerning their contextual relationship. This narrative literature review summarizes current evidence for the role of context-relatedness for obsessions in OCD and intrusive thoughts in affected and unaffected individuals. The review encompasses a total of five studies, two of which include individuals diagnosed with OCD (one study also includes a group of unaffected control individuals), while the other three studies investigate the relationship between OCD symptoms and context in unaffected individuals. As assessed by mainly self-reports, the review examines the connection between thoughts and their context, shedding light on how the repetition and automaticity of thoughts, as well as their detachment from context over time contribute to defining obsessions in contrast to intrusive thoughts. However, the link with context depends on the content of the obsessions. We propose the term “decontextualization of thoughts” to describe the phenomenon that obsessions gradually lose their connection with external context during the development of OCD. Future research should investigate whether this hypothesis can be supported by experimental evidence and identify whether this shift might be more likely a cause or a consequence of the disorder.

1 Introduction

Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a serious and common mental disorder with a lifetime prevalence of about 2.3% (1). Despite recent increases in prevalence due to the COVID-19 pandemic (2, 3), OCD is considered a “neglected mental disorder” that attracts comparatively little public attention (4) and receives less than 1% of the total amount of mental health research funding (5). The disorder is characterized by obsessions, which are defined as recurrent intrusive thoughts, impulses or images. Obsessions lead to subsequent compulsions, which are actions taken to alleviate the distressing thoughts or to prevent negative outcomes associated with them (6). Obsessions and compulsions as the core symptoms of OCD have been well-studied and frequent themes include contamination, checking and symmetry (79). Appraisal of intrusive thoughts has been found to significantly predict OCD severity (10) and is an important aspect of cognitive models of OCD. Although there is some overlap in the content between intrusive thoughts in healthy participants and obsessions in OCD (11, 12), obsessions are often ego-dystonic, unwanted and persist over time (6). However, there is limited understanding of the specific triggers or context-related factors that contribute to the development of obsessions. It has been proposed that differences may exist in the contextual factors that influence the occurrence of intrusive thoughts and obsessions, particularly in terms of their proximity to the present situation or context (1214).

Early research suggests that intrusive thoughts in healthy individuals usually have identifiable external or internal triggers, but obsessions in OCD are just as likely to be internally or externally triggered as they occur with no discernible trigger at all (11). As an example, the obsession of harming a child could be triggered by external stimuli, like the sight of a child or objects that could potentially be used to harm a child (11). Thus, intrusive thoughts and obsessions can either occur spontaneously or be externally triggered, with the former category usually causing more distress (15). In line, Lee and Kwon (16) proposed a distinction between two types of obsessions, each with distinct characteristics. Autogenous obsessions occur spontaneously and include sexual, aggressive and immoral thoughts or impulses. In contrast, reactive obsessions are triggered by specific stimuli within the context and include thoughts about contamination, mistake, accident, asymmetry, and loss. The weaker link between obsessions in OCD and their context as based on early findings (11) has been addressed in a more recent study. This study showed a greater tendency for obsessions to be indirectly rather than directly linked to context as a characteristic feature of OCD, distinguishing it from healthy control participants (12). Additionally, it has been demonstrated that nonclinical individuals with a higher frequency of context-unrelated thoughts tend to exhibit increased OCD symptoms (13). These results suggest that the cognitive processes underlying obsessions may significantly deviate from those in healthy individuals, particularly in terms of their contextual relationship (13). Obsessions in OCD appear to exhibit a weaker connection to the context, and this relationship may further diminish as the disease progresses.

Context is defined as the sum of circumstances that characterizes the experience of the current situation (17). It serves to characterize the situation and synthesizes all internal (such as physiological states, cognitive, emotional, biographical history) and external (including environmental and social) aspects of a situation (17, 18). In terms of the regulation of thoughts, it is adaptive to align them with the environment or external context. For example, thinking about last nights dinner is more useful when trying to recreate the same meal, than when trying to steer the car out of a tight parking lot.

The alignment of obsessions in OCD with their context of occurrence can thus be examined at two different levels: the internal and the external context (13, 19). The internal context originates from within the person, while the external context can be perceived and assessed through the senses, including sight, hearing, smell, taste, and touch (13, 19). If an obsession matches the internal context, it matches the individuals ongoing thought processes, emotional state or the biographical history (13). An obsession that matches with external context is one in which its content is directly linkable to an event that is monitored with the senses (12). Certainly, both are often interconnected. In addition, certain obsessions, such as the thought of harming ones own child, may have connections to the external context but strongly contradict the individuals internal beliefs and values. Following this, greater adaptability arises when thoughts harmonize with the context, facilitating a more integrated mode of action. Conversely, context incongruent thoughts, which are unexpected or even in conflict with internal beliefs and values, generate larger prediction errors and surprise signals, which may contribute to the overinterpretation of thoughts, thereby exacerbating OCD. Furthermore, incongruent thoughts likely distract from the task at hand and lead to decreased performance. The primary objective of this narrative review is to examine and summarize the available research concerning the role of context and context-relatedness of obsessions in OCD. The literature search was conducted with the search terms (“obsessions” or “OCD”) and context in the search engine Google Scholar. Results were manually screened for relevance and literature references in relevant articles were further screened for relevant articles.

2 Obsessions and their relationship with context

In this section, we will present studies that specifically explore the role of context in the manifestation of obsessions. Our review encompasses a total of five studies that investigate the interplay between thoughts and their context in OCD symptomatology. Of these, two studies were conducted in individuals diagnosed with OCD (12, 14), one of them also including a group of unaffected control individuals, while three studies focused on OCD symptoms in unaffected participants (13, 19, 20). Recognizing the heightened significance of the two clinical studies in elucidating the role of context in OCD, we will initiate our discussion by presenting these. Short summaries of the included studies can be found in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Information on the five main studies described.

The study by Julien and colleagues (12) examined contextual aspects of intrusive thoughts in a group of individuals diagnosed with OCD (N = 33) and compared them to a control group of healthy individuals (N = 99). Participants were instructed to identify their three most disturbing intrusive thoughts and then indicate the degree to which the thought is related to the external context (direct, indirect, no link) after having received training to understand these categories. In order to explore whether the context link depends on the specific content of the intrusive thought, the authors additionally categorized them into the domains symmetry, checking, washing, impulse phobia, hoarding, rumination and schizotypy. The relationship of a thought to external context could either be direct or indirect or there is no link the external context. To exemplify the three different categories used (direct, indirect and no link), we will use the thought “I may not have extinguished my cigarette correctly” based on Julien and colleagues (12). This thought in a situation, where there is observable evidence of a cigarette still emitting smoke would be classified as having a direct link (12). A thought with an indirect link is characterized by the obsessions theme not being directly supported by clear and accurate information from the context, but still being loosely associated with it (12). For instance, the same thought in a situation when the cigarette has been extinguished and there is no information indicating that it in fact has not been extinguished well (12). In this case, a link between theme of the intrusive thought (cigarette still burning) and the context in which it occurs (seeing the cigarette) exists, but the link is indirect. Finally, no link between the content of an intrusive thought and the context of occurrence means that the intrusive thought above is detached from the external context (e.g. occurs while taking a shower) and is rather triggered by an internal narrative unrelated to the current context (34). Importantly, this three-level classification refers only to external context. When it comes to internal context, distinguishing between direct and indirect evidence can be challenging due to limited introspection and a lack of clear criteria for defining links between intrusive thoughts and internal contextual events, such as a feeling. Of note, intrusive thoughts instead of obsessions were assessed to ensure comparability between individuals with OCD and non-clinical control participants in the study. Furthermore, the study was based on the assumption that nonclinical intrusive thoughts and obsessions are similar in content and differ only with regard to their appraisal (35, 36). This study demonstrated that the link between context and intrusive thoughts differed between individuals diagnosed with OCD and unaffected control participants (12). Intrusive thoughts of non-clinical control participants were mostly directly linked to context, whereas intrusive thoughts of participants diagnosed with OCD were only indirectly linked to context. Importantly, the strength of context link differed depending on the content of the intrusive thought; for example, thoughts related to symmetry were more likely to be directly linked to a trigger within the context compared to thoughts about washing. Interestingly, on an individual level, the link between intrusive thoughts and context tended to remain consistent across all three reported intrusive thoughts (12), suggesting that the connection between context and thoughts is a stable characteristic within a person. In summary, these findings suggest that the relationship between intrusive thoughts and the external context is diminished in individuals with OCD.

More recently, Llorens-Aguilar and colleagues (14) examined whether the link to context differed between intrusive thoughts and obsessions and whether this link changed over the course of the disorder. During an interview procedure, 68 individuals with OCD were asked to report their most upsetting obsession and intrusive thought, detailing how these thoughts occurred at different time points (14). Then, these descriptions were categorized by experts according to the link with external context using the three levels described above (direct, indirect, no link). It was found that during the first occurrence, both obsessions and intrusive thoughts were most commonly indirectly linked to the context. This typically occurred when individuals experienced negative mood and increased stress levels compared to their usual state. Moreover, the connection between thought and external context diminished over time for obsessions but not intrusive thoughts. Consequently, at the time point of the last experience, obsessions were equally likely to occur with indirect as they were with no link to context. In contrast, the link between intrusive thoughts and context remained stable over time. The authors argue that in the course of OCD, obsessions become more automatic and detached from context, which might be based on deficient learning (14). Compared to intrusive thoughts, obsessions are characterized by the repetition and increased automaticity of thoughts, coupled with an increasing detachment from context over time, which is not a defining feature of intrusive thoughts. However, the results of the two so far reported studies indicate that this detachment from context is already apparent in intrusive thoughts, which were found to have an indirect connection to the context in OCD patients (14), while being directly linked to the context in healthy individuals (12). Furthermore, the study examined the context links with regard to thought content. As proposed by Lee and Kwon (16), thoughts with sexual, aggressive, or immoral contents were classified as autogenous obsessions and thoughts on contamination, mistakes, accidents, asymmetry or loss are considered as reactive obsessions. Importantly, Lee and Kwon (16) suggested that autogenous thoughts or obsessions are less likely to depend on external evoking stimuli than reactive thoughts or obsessions. However, Llorens-Aguilar and colleagues (14) found that intrusive thoughts and obsessions that would be classified as autogenous according to Lee and Kwon (16), i.e. involving sexual and aggressive themes, occur with indirect evidence, while reactive thoughts and obsessions occur with no link to the context (14). Contrary to the exact classification of themes with context links in the initial proposal by Lee and Kwon (16), this partly aligns with Julien and colleagues (12). In sum, this research indicates that the thematic nature of intrusive thoughts and obsessions may play a crucial role in determining the connection between thoughts and context.

In a non-clinical study, Audet and colleagues (19) asked N = 248 non-clinical participants to report an intrusive thought they experienced and describe the context in which it occurred. Clinicians rated these intrusive thoughts as either directly linked to context or not linked to context. While intrusive thoughts with no link to context predicted OCD symptoms as measured by the Vancouver Obsessional Compulsive Inventory (VOCI, 24), intrusive thoughts that were linked to the context did not (19). Additionally emphasizing the altered role of context in OCD, intrusive thoughts with no link to context correlated with increased scores on inferential confusion measures (19). Inferential confusion is a metacognitive reasoning process, in which an imagined possibility is mixed up with an existing probability (37). The correlation indicates that conclusions may be reached without adequate consideration of contextual evidence (19), potentially connected with increased out-of-context thoughts. Interestingly, ego-dystonicity of a thought, which is characteristic for OCD, was not able to predict OCD symptoms (19). Ego-dystonic thoughts contradict the persons moral values or intentions, and are therefore perceived as invasive and irrational, and not linked to the internal context of a person (19, 38). This suggests that for OCD symptoms, intrusive thoughts might be independent of external context rather than internal context (ego-dystonicity), suggesting a smaller role for ego-dystonicity than traditionally thought (19). However, the authors note that the absence of identifiable triggers for the intrusive thoughts might alternatively reflect impaired introspection and access to mental states as part of the disorder (19, 39) instead of a decoupling of context and OCD-related thoughts. Because of the link with inferential confusion, the importance of therapeutically addressing subjective reasoning processes (metacognitive processes) that might be biased in OCD is emphasized (19).

In three other non-clinical studies, Fradkin and Huppert (13) examined non-clinical samples (N = 599) by means of the “Out-of-Context Thoughts Questionnaire (OCTQ)” which was developed by the authors (13). As a result, increased thoughts with no link to context were significantly associated with OCD symptoms on the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised, OCI-R (13, 26). The OCTQ examines the decoupling of thoughts from context along four domains: Cognitive context, where thoughts are unexpected relative to previous ones; Affective context, thoughts unexpected relative to current emotions; Self context, thoughts unexpected relative to the own self-concept; Resisted thoughts, thoughts unexpected despite the efforts to control them (13). While the total score on the OCI-R and the obsessions, checking and neutralizing subscales were consistently significantly associated with increased thoughts with no link to context, findings for the hoarding and ordering subscale of the OCI-R were mixed in the two studies assessing this. Interestingly, the washing subscale of the OCI-R was not associated with increased thoughts with no link to context. Generally, the correlations of the subscales that are associated with reactive obsessions with OCTQ were lower than for the autogenous obsessions subscale. This is consistent with the theory of Lee and Kwon (16) claiming that washing as a reactive obsession possesses a higher likelihood of being triggered directly, and is in line with Julien and colleagues (12) who found that reactive obsessions (in their study hoarding and symmetry) occur closely linked to context. However, it remains unclear why the checking subscale, which is also considered a reactive obsession, significantly correlated with OCTQ in the two studies. Importantly, as the authors note, the subscales of the OCI-R that are associated with reactive obsessions do not measure reactive obsessions as such, but instead compulsive behaviors connected to these, which further explains the differential association with thoughts not linked to context (13). Additionally, the authors suggest that the decoupling of thoughts from the current context makes obsessions unpredictable and thus highly aversive and uncontrollable which increases the probability that the thoughts reappear explaining the vicious circle of obsessions (13). However, generalizability of results is limited since only unaffected individuals were included and self-reports were used to assess thoughts with no link to context.

As the first study investigating the role of context in OCD symptoms experimentally instead of self-reports (20), utilized the contextual vignettes task (CVT) in unaffected participants (N = 557). In the task, different scenarios of various OCD themes are described, such as aggression or contamination. The scenarios in the stories are either directly linked to the context (i.e., provide direct evidence for the reality of a subsequent thought) or unlinked to the context (lack direct evidence for the reality of the thought). An exemplary scenario of the aggression theme without link to context is a story about the reader watching a movie about a psychopath who kills his family, but appeared normal to others before. During the movie, the readers family arrives home and a suggested potential intrusive thought is that the reader might harm his/her family. After reading the stories, participants were shown potential intrusive thoughts and selected the ones that could occur to them in this situation, the likelihood of these and the corresponding distress and unwantedness. As a result, corresponding with other studies (10, 13), it was demonstrated that individuals with an increased likelihood of reporting intrusive thoughts with no link to context showed higher OCD symptom scores (VOCI, 24). These individuals additionally showed increased levels of ego-dystonicity and judged their intrusive thoughts as more repugnant (20). Furthermore, similar to Audet et al. (19), inferential confusion predicted likelihood, distress and unwantedness of intrusive thoughts (20), emphasizing the strong association of an altered role of context with other OCD phenomena.

Overall, the described studies suggest that OCD symptoms (pathological obsessions) are less connected to both, external (12, 14, 19, 20) and internal (13) context. Importantly, the degree of thought-context decoupling distinguishes intrusive thoughts in unaffected individuals from obsessions in OCD, where there are reduced relations to context in the latter group (12). In line, studies in unaffected individuals demonstrated that intrusive thoughts with reduced relations to context predict OCD symptoms (13, 19, 20), while intrusive thoughts linked to context do not (19, 20). However, due to mixed evidence, the degree to which thoughts in OCD relate to context is still unclear i.e. whether obsessions are more likely to be indirectly linked or not linked with context at all (12, 14). It is possible that this depends on the stage of the disorder with a more advanced stage resulting in a reduced coupling of thoughts with context as a function of increased automaticity of obsessive thoughts. This interpretation is supported by evidence from Llorens-Aguilar et al. (14) who showed that obsessions increasingly decouple from the current context. Furthermore, evidence indicates that the degree to which obsessions in OCD are linked to context relates to the content of obsessions, i.e. different topics of obsessions are not affected by decontextualization equally (12, 14). Overall, there is strong evidence for alterations in the thought-context coupling in OCD.

3 Discussion

The literature presented suggests a diminished link of obsessions to context in OCD, particularly in the sense that both intrusive and obsessive thoughts seem to have weaker connections to external and internal contextual factors (1214, 19, 20). Generally, obsessions tend to have indirect or no links with context, and this tendency appears to intensify with the progression of symptoms (12, 14). The degree of thought-context decoupling in OCD seems to depend on the specific theme of obsessions (12, 14). Differences in contextual links have already been considered in the distinction between autogenous and reactive obsessions (16). Reactive obsessions were assumed to be related to the external context, whereas autogenous obsessions were thought to manifest spontaneously without any direct links to the external context. However, these relationships did not replicate in the studies investigated here. For example, Julien et al. (12) found that in line with the assumption of Lee and Kwon (16), symmetry was more directly linked to context. Surprisingly, contamination, which was proposed as a reactive obsession and might be anticipated to have a similar direct link to context, was found to be more indirectly linked. Moreover, Llorens-Aguilar et al. (14) demonstrated that autogenous obsessions are more related to the external context than reactive ones. In sum, while some themes occur less linked with context than others, more research is needed to figure out which contents should be grouped together to represent thoughts more closely related to context and those less related to context. Potentially, new labels other than autogenous and reactive should be considered to prevent confusion with the old categories.

Obsessive thoughts that occur unrelated to the context play a significant role in OCD, but it is not clear how these thoughts contribute to OCD. One perspective suggests that when these obsessions arise unexpectedly and out-of-context, they may be perceived as threatening (13). This idea is supported by research indicating that negative thoughts are perceived as more aversive when they occur out-of-context (13). This perception of heightened threat may add to the aversiveness of the thought itself and then further increase the tendency to engage in efforts to control their thoughts (13, 40, 41). Paradoxically, these efforts to control thoughts could increase the salience and significance attributed to out-of-context thoughts, further characterizing them as obsessions (13).

In addition, Fradkin and colleagues (13) have suggested that individuals with OCD have altered meta-cognitive beliefs about the predictability of normal thoughts, leading them to magnify the significance of out-of-context thoughts. This in turn, prompts increased attempts to control these thoughts and eventually increase their salience (13). However, it is unlikely that altered meta-cognitive beliefs are the only cause of out-of-context-thoughts. Clinicians have identified thought-context decoupling (19), experimental evidence has also established a link between out-of-context thoughts and OCD (20).

We propose the term “decontextualization of thoughts” to describe the phenomenon that obsessions lose their connection with context. Evidence suggests that decontextualization of thoughts occurs gradually during the development of OCD disorder (14), which is potentially contributing to the severity of symptoms. This process may by driven by increased automaticity of thoughts due to alterations in learning in OCD. Furthermore, a reduced sense of Agency (SoA) has been observed in OCD (42). It has been shown that decontextualization is associated with a decreased SoA over thoughts, which is defined as the feeling that ones own thoughts originate from oneself (43) as well as increased distrust of the senses (19, 44). More specifically, individuals with OCD experience themselves as less autonomous and more passive in the thought-generation process (43). As a result, they may not experience context as salient and clear as unaffected participants, making it challenging to adequately associate thoughts with context. If individual with OCD perceive the context less clearly, it may lead to a perception of infinite possibilities for actions in a situation and therefore thoughts might seem less predictable. Another perspective suggests that alterations in the ability to process contexts (“context processing”), could explain the detachment of obsessions from their context. Similar alterations have been observed for mental disorders like schizophrenia and post-traumatic stress disorder (4549). Context processing is one part of working memory that encompasses all processes that allow for on-line maintenance and manipulation of contextual information (such as environmental stimuli, instructions or goals) (45). This enables adaptive responses to stimuli in the presence of distraction (4547, 50, 51). Alterations in context processing may represent a transdiagnostic phenotype affecting psychopathology, such as OCD. Studies in OCD have shown an intact ability to detect context changes, yet these changes are often disregarded in subsequent actions (52). Fradkin and colleagues (53) on the other hand found deficits in recognizing shifts in cue-outcome contingencies (i.e. context changes) in individuals with OCD. Hence, it remains unclear whether individuals with OCD have difficulties in perceiving and recognizing context changes or whether they are only impaired in applying this contextual knowledge to guide appropriate behavior.

3.1 Limitations

Despite a significant body of evidence demonstrating an association between decontextualization and OCD in various studies, it is essential to acknowledge that the majority of studies have utilized non-clinical participants (13, 19, 20). In order to generalize findings to OCD, it is crucial to incorporate studies involving individuals diagnosed with OCD, such as those conducted by Julien and colleagues (12) and Llorens-Aguilar et al. (14). Another limitation of prior studies is that the majority relied on self-reports, often with a retrospective element (1214, 19). Even studies that relied on expert ratings were based on information provided by participants (14, 19). However, participants may inadvertently omit information indicating a close link with context, either due to a lack of awareness of its relevance or constraints on their introspection. Another limitation to consider is the broad and multifaceted nature of the term context compassing various aspects such as mood, life events experienced or the current action (14). Therefore, establishing a hierarchy to determine the significance of each aspect when linking it to a thought is crucial. For instance, deciding how to classify the link between a thought and context, when the current activity is unrelated, but the mood matches the thought. Without such a hierarchy, it is challenging to compare context links across participants and studies. It is important to note that the presented evidence regarding the altered link between obsessions and context is based on single studies rather than meta-analyses, and therefore has to be interpreted with caution. Nevertheless, albeit variations in methodologies, such as self-report based vs. experimental manipulation, these studies generally arrive at similar conclusions.

3.2 Decontextualization of behavior

Elevated decontextualization in OCD is not restricted to obsessive thoughts, but may also extend to compulsive behaviors. OCD is characterized by an increased reliance on habitual behaviors, which remain resistant to changes in context, including changes in situations or personal goals (54, 55). Thereby, habitual behavior in OCD is context-independent and persists despite changes in context. In fact, (symptomatic) compulsive behaviors in OCD rely on the habitual system, rendering them highly automatic (56), rigid, inflexible and difficult to stop (55, 57). Notably, habitual behavior is often strongly cue-driven, while the context and other aspects of the situation are neglected. In support, Gillan and colleagues (55) demonstrated an increased tendency for habitual behaviors as opposed to goal-directed behavior in OCD, and this tendency is correlated with OCD symptom severity. Furthermore, Gillan and colleagues (57) subsequently showed that individuals with OCD encounter difficulties in unlearning avoidance habits that were established based on a conditioned warning stimulus that had previously prevented them from harm. This evidence indicates an excessive focus on cues in OCD, often at the expense of not responding to (changes in) context. In accordance, previous studies have shown that control behavior in OCD is less influenced by external error signals (58) and the adjustment of behavior in OCD is less influenced by external feedback (59). In line with these findings, a classic Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer task (PIT), which investigates the effect of Pavlovian conditioned stimuli on instrumental behavior, revealed that individuals with OCD are less able to use contextual information from the PIT cues to adapt their behavior (60). Collectively, these studies suggest that contextual information play a less significant role in behavior selection in OCD, resulting in decontextualized behavior.

As cognitive flexibility is required for goal-directed behavior (61), over-reliance on habitual, context-independent behavior can further be assessed by means of cognitive flexibility tasks. Cognitive flexibility is a fundamental executive function characterized by the capacity to change behavior or cognitive strategies when faced with new challenges, rules, environments or priorities (i.e. contexts) (61, 62). This ability can be evaluated through cognitive flexibility tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (63), reversal learning paradigms (59) and set shifting tasks (6466). Impairments in both cognitive flexibility (i.e. cognitive inflexibility) and behavioral flexibility are typically observed in OCD (6770). As impaired cognitive flexibility has also been found in unaffected first-degree relatives, it has been considered a risk marker or endophenotype of OCD (71). In line, a meta-analysis revealed that cognitive flexibility is strongly affected in OCD (Cohens d = .517) (72). Interestingly, there are also examples where cognitive flexibility in OCD is superior to that of nonclinical control participants specifically when a previously inhibited mental set becomes relevant again (73). This seemingly paradoxical finding has been interpreted as a deficit in adapting to novel task rules, in contrast to better performance when dealing with familiar task rules (73). More generally, recent agent-based simulations have demonstrated that increased habitual tendencies (“strong habit learner”), which also overlap with the clinical picture of OCD (74), compromise the ability to recognize changes in contexts on a reversal learning task (75). In addition, alterations of economic decision making have been observed in OCD and it would also be interesting to investigate the effects of context on formal parameters of economic decision-making frameworks (e.g. 7679).

3.3 Neural correlates of context processing

At the neural level, the hippocampus plays an important role in representing contextual information and is necessary for contextual learning (80, 81). Alterations in hippocampal areas and hippocampal-prefrontal-thalamic circuitries are suggested to underlie the ability to derive contextual inferences, playing a central role in the psychopathology of PTSD (48). Based on accumulating evidence for alterations in context processing in OCD, it is likely that changes in the network may also be evident in OCD, and contribute to the clinical picture of such patients. Indeed, there is evidence that hippocampal volume may be altered in OCD (8285). Interestingly, reduced hippocampal volume has been shown in individuals who score high on ordering and checking symptoms (82). In summary, there are only few studies investigating hippocampal changes and it would be interesting to examine how hippocampal structure and function in OCD relate to context processing.

Overall, laboratory-based studies indicate a pivotal role of impaired cognitive flexibility (6872, 86), increased habitual behavior (55, 57) and altered decision-making in OCD (7679). In general, there is evidence for decontextualization in OCD, affecting obsessive thoughts and compulsive behavior. This is evident in the limited ability of individuals with OCD to adapt their behavior to changing contextual information (57, 69), leading to a reduction in adaptive goal-directed behavior. Alterations in context processing in OCD may be related to changes in hippocampus volume (8285). However, while the link between decontextualization and habitual behavior as well as cognitive inflexibility in OCD is plausible, so far, no studies directly explored the role of context in compulsive behavior. As a result, the following discussion of the implications will focus on decontextualization of obsessions.

3.4 Future directions

Given the finding that obsessions in OCD tend to decontextualize, potentially contributing to the disorder (14), an important question arises: Can this progress be counteracted, perhaps through therapeutic interventions? A secondary consultation would be to explore whether such interventions might lead to an improvement in symptoms. In particular, already in the early stages of the disorder, potential overgeneralization of obsessions should be addressed, i.e., it should be examined whether a particular obsession is justified in a particular context, based on the strength of the context link (direct, indirect or no link) with the original trigger of the obsession. The crucial question would be how to increase salience of context, both internal and external, in a way that minimizes the occurrence of decontextualized thoughts by reducing them proactively. Of note, such an approach would require an understanding of other predisposing factors that could indicate a potential progression towards an OCD-like pathological state, such as increased distress, which has been associated with the emergence of decontextualized thoughts in OCD (12).

An avenue for future research lies in understanding why seemingly similar intrusive thoughts and obsessions diverge progressively over time with regard to their link to context and how this development can be predicted (14). Secondly, it should be investigated whether this development is related to OCD severity. Additionally, it should be deciphered how the distinct contents of obsessions relate to decontextualization. Reliable insights from context measures, such as out-of-context thoughts (13), could potentially serve as predictive indicators for the development of OCD and inform the creation of new treatment approaches. Furthermore, additional experimental clinical studies are needed to rule out meta-cognitive beliefs as a contributing factor to decontextualization in OCD.

4 Conclusion

Taken together, we suggest that obsessions and potentially compulsions in OCD are decontextualized (12, 14, 19, 20, 57). The reduced reliance on contextual information to guide thoughts and behaviors in OCD may be associated with the reduced adaptability and flexibility often observed in the condition (6870, 87). Decontextualized thoughts and behaviors in OCD might lead to an exaggerated emphasis on the significance of cues related to symptoms (88) and result in overgeneralization across contexts.

Author contributions

FW: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KS: Writing – review & editing. TE: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Ruscio AM, Stein DJ, Chiu WT, Kessler RC. The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Mol Psychiatry. (2010) 15:53–63. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.94

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Guzick AG, Candelari A, Wiese AD, Schneider SC, Goodman WK, Storch EA. Obsessive-compulsive disorder during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2021) 23:71. doi: 10.1007/s11920-021-01284-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Zheng Y, Xiao L, Xie Y, Wang H, Wang G. Prevalence and characteristics of obsessive-compulsive disorder among urban residents in wuhan during the stage of regular control of coronavirus disease-19 epidemic. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:594167. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.594167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hauser TU. On the development of OCD. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2021) 49:17–30. doi: 10.1007/7854_2020_195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Woelbert E, Kirtley A, Balmer N, Dix S. How much is spent on mental health research: developing a system for categorising grant funding in the UK. Lancet Psychiatry. (2019) 6:445–52. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30033-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Abramowitz JS, Franklin ME, Schwartz SA, Furr JM. Symptom presentation and outcome of cognitive-behavioral therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2003) 71:1049–57. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.6.1049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Garcia-Soriano G, Belloch A, Morillo C, Clark DA. Symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: from normal cognitive intrusions to clinical obsessions. J Anxiety Disord. (2011) 25:474–82. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Stewart SE, Rosario MC, Baer L, Carter AS, Brown TA, Scharf JM, et al. Four-factor structure of obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms in children, adolescents, and adults. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2008) 47:763–72. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318172ef1e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Barrera TL, Norton PJ. The appraisal of intrusive thoughts in relation to obsessional-compulsive symptoms. Cognit Behav Ther. (2011) 40:98–110. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2010.545072

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Rachman S, de Silva P. Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behav Res Ther. (1978) 16:233–48. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(78)90022-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Julien D, OConnor KP, Aardema F. Intrusions related to obsessive-compulsive disorder: a question of content or context? J Clin Psychol. (2009) 65:709–22. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20578.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Fradkin I, Huppert JD. When our train of thought goes off track: The different facets of out-of-context thoughts in obsessive compulsive disorder. J Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disord. (2018) 18:31–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2018.06.001.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Llorens-Aguilar S, Garcia-Soriano G, Arnaez S, Aardema F, OConnor K. Is context a crucial factor in distinguishing between intrusions and obsessions in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder? J Clin Psychol. (2021) 77:804–17. doi: 10.1002/jclp.23060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Parkinson L, Rachman S. Part II. The nature of intrusive thoughts. Adv Behav Res Ther. (1981) 3:101–10. doi: 10.1016/0146-6402(81)90008-4.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lee HJ, Kwon SM. Two different types of obsession: autogenous obsessions and reactive obsessions. Behav Res Ther. (2003) 41:11–29. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00101-2.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Maren S, Phan KL, Liberzon I. The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2013) 14:417–28. doi: 10.1038/nrn3492.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Jacoby RJ, Abramowitz JS. Inhibitory learning approaches to exposure therapy: A critical review and translation to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. (2016) 49:28–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.07.001.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Audet J-S, Aardema F, Moulding R. Contextual determinants of intrusions and obsessions: The role of ego-dystonicity and the reality of obsessional thoughts. J Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disord. (2016) 9:96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2016.04.003.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Audet J-S, Wong SF, Radomsky AS, Aardema F. Not all intrusions are created equal: The role of context, feared-self perceptions and inferential confusion in the occurrence of abnormal intrusions. J Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disord. (2020) 26:100537. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2020.100537.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Thordarson DS, Radomsky AS, Rachman S, Shafran R, Sawchuk CN, Ralph Hakstian A. The vancouver obsessional compulsive inventory (VOCI). Behav Res Ther. (2004) 42:1289–314. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Aardema F, Moulding R, Radomsky AS, Doron G, Allamby J, Souki E. Fear of self and obsessionality: Development and validation of the Fear of Self Questionnaire. J Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disord. (2013) 2:306–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2013.05.005.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Purdon C, Cripps E, Faull M, Joseph S, Rowa K. Development of a measure of egodystonicity. J Cogn Psychotherapy. (2007) 21:198–216. doi: 10.1891/088983907781494537.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Moulding R, Anglim J, Nedeljkovic M, Doron G, Kyrios M, Ayalon A. The Obsessive Beliefs Questionnaire (OBQ): examination in nonclinical samples and development of a short version. Assessment. (2011) 18:357–74. doi: 10.1177/1073191110376490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Rachman S. (2007). Treating Religious, Sexual, and Aggressive Obsessions. In Antony M. M., Purdon C., Summerfeldt L. J. (Eds.), Psychological treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder: Fundamentals and beyond (pp. 209–229). American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/11543-009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Foa EB, Huppert JD, Leiberg S, Langner R, Kichic R, Hajcak G, et al. The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychol Assess. (2002) 14:485–96. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.14.4.485.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Henry JD, Crawford JR. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. (2005) 44:227–39. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Rammstedt B, John OP. Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J Res Personality. (2007) 41:203–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Carriere JSA, Seli P, Smilek D. Wandering in both mind and body: individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Can J Exp Psychol. (2013) 67:19–31. doi: 10.1037/a0031438.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther. (1995) 33:335–43. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Aardema F, Wu KD, Careau Y, OConnor K, Julien D, Dennie S. The expanded version of the Inferential Confusion Questionnaire: further development and validation in clinical and non-clinical samples. J Psychopathol Behav Assessment. (2010) 32:448–62. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9157-x.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. GroupT OCCW. Psychometric validation of the Obsessive Beliefs Questionnaire and Interpretation of Intrusions Inventory—Part 2: Factor analysis and testing of a brief version. Behav Res Ther. (2005) 43:1527–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Garcia-Soriano G. Obsessional intrusive thoughts inventory. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2008). doi: 10.1037/t23131-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Julien D, OConnor KP, Aardema F. Intrusive thoughts, obsessions, and appraisals in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2007) 27:366–83. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.12.004.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Rachman S. A cognitive theory of obsessions: elaborations. Behav Res Ther. (1998) 36:385–401. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10041-9.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Salkovskis PM. Cognitive-behavioural factors and the persistence of intrusive thoughts in obsessional problems. Behav Res Ther. (1989) 27:677–82. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(89)90152-6.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Aardema F, OConnor KP, Emmelkamp PM, Marchand A, Todorov C. Inferential confusion in obsessive-compulsive disorder: the inferential confusion questionnaire. Behav Res Ther. (2005) 43:293–308. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.003.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Strauss AY, Fradkin I, McNally RJ, Linkovski O, Anholt GE, Huppert JD. Why check? A meta-analysis of checking in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Threat vs. distrust senses. Clin Psychol Rev. (2020) 75:101807. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101807.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Aardema F, Johansson P, Hall L, Paradisis S-M, Zidani M, Roberts S. Choice blindness, confabulatory introspection, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: A new area of investigation. Int J Cogn Ther. (2014) 7:83–102. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2014.7.1.83.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Amir N, Cashman L, Foa EB. Strategies of thought control in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav Res Ther. (1997) 35:775–7. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00030-2.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Moulding R, Kyrios M. Anxiety disorders and control related beliefs: the exemplar of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD). Clin Psychol Rev. (2006) 26:573–83. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.009.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Gentsch A, Schutz-Bosbach S, Endrass T, Kathmann N. Dysfunctional forward model mechanisms and aberrant sense of agency in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2012) 71:652–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.022.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fradkin I, Eitam B, Strauss AY, Huppert JD. Thoughts as unexpected intruders: context, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, and the sense of agency over thoughts. Clin psychol Science. (2019) 7:162–80. doi: 10.1177/2167702618797102.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Wong SF, Williams AD, Grisham JR. Distrust of the senses and its association with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2017) 57:189–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2017.06.002.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Barch DM, Carter CS, MacDonald AW 3rd, Braver TS, Cohen JD. Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: diagnostic specificity, 4-week course, and relationships to clinical symptoms. J Abnorm Psychol. (2003) 112:132–43. doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.112.1.132.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Cohen JD, Barch DM, Carter C, Servan-Schreiber D. Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks. J Abnorm Psychol. (1999) 108:120–33. doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.108.1.120.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Cohen JD, Servan-Schreiber D. Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychol Rev. (1992) 99:45–77. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.99.1.45.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Liberzon I, Abelson JL. Context processing and the neurobiology of post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuron. (2016) 92:14–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.039.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Zabag R, Deri O, Gilboa-Schechtman E, Richter-Levin G, Levy-Gigi E. Cognitive flexibility in PTSD individuals following nature adventure intervention: is it really that good? Stress. (2020) 23:97–104. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2019.1645113.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Blackman RK, MacDonald AW, Chafee MV. Effects of ketamine on context-processing performance in monkeys: a new animal model of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2013) 38:2090–100. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.118.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. MacDonald AW III, Goghari VM, Hicks BM, Flory JD, Carter CS, Manuck SBA. convergent-divergent approach to context processing, general intellectual functioning, and the genetic liability to schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. (2005) 19:814. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.6.814.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Vaghi MM, Luyckx F, Sule A, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, De Martino B. Compulsivity reveals a novel dissociation between action and confidence. Neuron. (2017) 96:348–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.09.006.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Fradkin I, Adams RA, Parr T, Roiser JP, Huppert JD. Searching for an anchor in an unpredictable world: A computational model of obsessive compulsive disorder. psychol review. (2020) 127:672. doi: 10.1037/rev0000188.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Gillan CM, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ, van den Heuvel OA, van Wingen G. The role of habit in compulsivity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2016) 26:828–40. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.12.033.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Gillan CM, Papmeyer M, Morein-Zamir S, Sahakian BJ, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, et al. Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2011) 168:718–26. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Starcevic V, Berle D, Brakoulias V, Sammut P, Moses K, Milicevic D, et al. Functions of compulsions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2011) 45:449–57. doi: 10.3109/00048674.2011.567243.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Gillan CM, Apergis-Schoute AM, Morein-Zamir S, Urcelay GP, Sule A, Fineberg NA, et al. Functional neuroimaging of avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2015) 172:284–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040525.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Rotge JY, Langbour N, Dilharreguy B, Bordessoulles M, Guehl D, Bioulac B, et al. Contextual and behavioral influences on uncertainty in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Cortex. (2015) 62:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.010.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Endrass T, Koehne S, Riesel A, Kathmann N. Neural correlates of feedback processing in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. (2013) 122:387–96. doi: 10.1037/a0031496.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Peng Z, He L, Wen R, Verguts T, Seger CA, Chen Q. Obsessive-compulsive disorder is characterized by decreased Pavlovian influence on instrumental behavior. PloS Comput Biol. (2022) 18:e1009945. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Zhou X, Meng Y, Schmitt HS, Montag C, Kendrick KM, Becker B. Cognitive flexibility mediates the association between early life stress and habitual behavior. Pers Individ Differences. (2020) 167:110231. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110231.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. (2013) 64:135–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Miles S, Howlett CA, Berryman C, Nedeljkovic M, Moseley GL, Phillipou A. Considerations for using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to assess cognitive flexibility. Behav Res Methods. (2021) 53:2083–91. doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01551-3.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, Fineberg NA, del Campo N, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. (2008) 321:421–2. doi: 10.1126/science.1154433.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Gottwald J, de Wit S, Apergis-Schoute AM, Morein-Zamir S, Kaser M, Cormack F, et al. Impaired cognitive plasticity and goal-directed control in adolescent obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med. (2018) 48:1900–8. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717003464.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Stemme A, Deco G, Busch A. The neuronal dynamics underlying cognitive flexibility in set shifting tasks. J Comput Neurosci. (2007) 23:313–31. doi: 10.1007/s10827-007-0034-x.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Watkins LH, Sahakian BJ, Robertson MM, Veale DM, Rogers RD, Pickard KM, et al. Executive function in Tourettes syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med. (2005) 35:571–82. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003691.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Gruner P, Pittenger C. Cognitive inflexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuroscience. (2017) 345:243–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.07.030.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Ferreira S, Moreira P, Magalhaes R, Coelho A, Marques P, Portugal-Nunes C, et al. Frontoparietal hyperconnectivity during cognitive regulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder followed by reward valuation inflexibility. J Psychiatr Res. (2021) 137:657–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.11.008.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Snyder HR, Kaiser RH, Warren SL, Heller W. Obsessive-compulsive disorder is associated with broad impairments in executive function: A meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Sci. (2015) 3:301–30. doi: 10.1177/2167702614534210.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2007) 164:335–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.335.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Abramovitch A, Abramowitz JS, Mittelman A. The neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2013) 33:1163–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.09.004.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Wolff N, Giller F, Buse J, Roessner V, Beste C. When repetitive mental sets increase cognitive flexibility in adolescent obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2018) 59:1024–32. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12901.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Voon V, Derbyshire K, Ruck C, Irvine MA, Worbe Y, Enander J, et al. Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Mol Psychiatry. (2015) 20:345–52. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.44.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Schwobel S, Marković D, Smolka MN, Kiebel SJ. Balancing control: a Bayesian interpretation of habitual and goal-directed behavior. J Math Psychol. (2021) 100:102472. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2020.102472.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Sip KE, Gonzalez R, Taylor SF, Stern ER. Increased loss aversion in unmedicated patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2017) 8:309. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Sip KE, Muratore AF, Stern ER. Effects of context on risk taking and decision times in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Psychiatr Res. (2016) 75:82–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.12.002.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Aranovich GJ, Cavagnaro DR, Pitt MA, Myung JI, Mathews CA. A model-based analysis of decision making under risk in obsessive-compulsive and hoarding disorders. J Psychiatr Res. (2017) 90:126–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.02.017.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. George SA, Sheynin J, Gonzalez R, Liberzon I, Abelson JL. Diminished value discrimination in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A prospect theory model of decision-making under risk. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:469. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00469.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Pennartz CM, Ito R, Verschure PF, Battaglia FP, Robbins TW. The hippocampal-striatal axis in learning, prediction and goal-directed behavior. Trends Neurosci. (2011) 34:548–59. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.08.001.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Geva-Sagiv M, Dimsdale-Zucker HR, Williams AB, Ranganath C. Proximity to boundaries reveals spatial context representation in human hippocampal CA1. Neuropsychologia. (2023) 189:108656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108656.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Reess TJ, Rus OG, Gursel DA, Schmitz-Koep B, Wagner G, Berberich G, et al. Association between hippocampus volume and symptom profiles in obsessive-compulsive disorder. NeuroImage Clin. (2018) 17:474–80. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.11.006.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Jiang L, Qiao K, Li Q, Hu Y, Zhang X, Wang J, et al. Categorical and dimensional deficits in hippocampal subfields among schizophrenia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cognit Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2023) 8:91–101. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.06.011.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Vattimo EFQ, Dos Santos AC, Hoexter MQ, Frudit P, Miguel EC, Shavitt RG, et al. Higher volumes of hippocampal subfields in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2021) 307:111200. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2020.111200.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Fouche JP, Groenewold NA, Sevenoaks T, Heany S, Lochner C, Alonso P, et al. Shape analysis of subcortical structures in obsessive-compulsive disorder and the relationship with comorbid anxiety, depression, and medication use: A meta-analysis by the OCD Brain Imaging Consortium. Brain Behav. (2022) 12:e2755. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Sternheim L, van der Burgh M, Berkhout LJ, Dekker MR, Ruiter C. Poor cognitive flexibility, and the experience thereof, in a subclinical sample of female students with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Scand J Psychol. (2014) 55:573–7. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12163.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Kim T, Kim M, Jung WH, Kwak YB, Moon SY, Kyungjin Lho S, et al. Unbalanced fronto-pallidal neurocircuit underlying set shifting in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain. (2022) 145:979–90. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab483.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Albertella L, Le Pelley ME, Chamberlain SR, Westbrook F, Fontenelle LF, Segrave R, et al. Reward-related attentional capture is associated with severity of addictive and obsessive-compulsive behaviors. Psychol Addict Behav. (2019) 33:495–502. doi: 10.1037/adb0000484.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: obsessive-compulsive disorder, self-report, obsessive behavior, cognition, mental health

Citation: Weiss F, Schwarz K and Endrass T (2024) Exploring the relationship between context and obsessions in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms: a narrative review. Front. Psychiatry 15:1353962. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1353962

Received: 11 December 2023; Accepted: 31 January 2024;
Published: 14 February 2024.

Edited by:

Mohammadreza Shalbafan, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Iran

Reviewed by:

Atefeh Zandifar, Alborz University of Medical Science, Iran
Jony Sheynin, Texas A&M Health Science Center, United States
Najmeh Shahini, Golestan University of Medical Sciences, Iran

Copyright © 2024 Weiss, Schwarz and Endrass. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Franziska Weiss, RnJhbnppc2thLndlaXNzMUB0dS1kcmVzZGVuLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.