Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Psychiatry, 13 February 2023
Sec. ADHD
This article is part of the Research Topic Horizons of Autism Spectrum Disorder and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Clinical Practice View all 10 articles

A randomized, sham-controlled trial of high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation on the right orbital frontal cortex in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

\nYi-chao Wang,
&#x;Yi-chao Wang1,2*Jun LiuJun Liu2Yan-chun WuYan-chun Wu2Yan WeiYan Wei2Hong-jing XieHong-jing Xie2Tao ZhangTao Zhang2Zhen ZhangZhen Zhang2
  • 1Affiliated Mental Health Center & Hangzhou Seventh People's Hospital, Zhejiang University School of Medicine, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
  • 2Zhenjiang Mental Health Center (The Fifth People's Hospital of Zhenjiang City), Zhenjiang, Jiangsu, China

Objective: This study aimed to find out the clinical and cognitive effects of high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) on the right orbital frontal cortex (OFC) in the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Methods: A total of 56 patients with ADHD were recruited as subjects and completely and randomly divided into the HD-tDCS group and the Sham group. A 1.0 mA anode current was applied to the right OFC. The HD-tDCS group received real stimulation, while the Sham group received sham stimulation in 10 sessions of treatment. ADHD symptom assessment (the SNAP-IV Rating Scale and the Perceived Stress Questionnaire) was carried out before treatment, after the 5th and 10th stimuli, and at the 6th week after the end of all stimulations, while the cognitive effect was assessed by the Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA-CPT), the Stroop Color and Word Test (Stroop), and the Tower of Hanoi (TOH). Repeated-measure ANOVA was used to find out the results of both groups before and after treatment.

Results: A total of 47 patients completed all sessions and evaluations. Their SNAP-IV score, their PSQ score, the mean visual and auditory reaction times by IVA-CPT, the interference RT of Stroop Color and Word, and the number of completed steps of TOH did not change with intervention time before and after treatment (P > 0.0031). However, the integrated visual and audiovisual commission errors and the TOH completion time results of the HD-tDCS group were significantly decreased after the 5th intervention, the 10th intervention, and the 6th week of intervention follow-up compared to the Sham group (P < 0.0031).

Conclusion: This study draws cautious conclusions that HD-tDCS does not significantly alleviate the overall symptoms of patients with ADHD but leads to significant improvements in the cognitive measures of attention maintenance. The study also attempted to fill in the gaps in research studies on HD-tDCS stimulation of the right OFC.

Clinical trial registration: ChiCTR2200062616.

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurodevelopmental disease among children and adolescents. Its core defects are mainly characterized by attention disorder, hyperactivity, impulsivity, and other clinical symptoms. Many children and adolescents with ADHD are associated with learning difficulties (1), with some having marked difficulties with emotional control (2). Neuropsychologists have found that there are performance defects in psychological processes with ADHD (3). Although ADHD is a complex and heterogeneous disorder (4), children and adolescents with ADHD often show performance impairments on tasks that measure some form of executive processes (5). ADHD is associated with deficits across a range of cognitive domains, such as arousal, executive functions, behavioral inhibition, motivation, set-shifting, and working memory (6). A recent meta-meta-analysis involved 34 meta-analyses of neurocognitive ADHD profiles (all ages) concerning 12 neurocognitive domains. Patients with ADHD have moderate impairments in multiple domains, including working memory, reaction time variability, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, intelligence/achievement, and planning/organization (7).

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a non-invasive brain stimulation therapy. Weak current (0.5–2.0 mA) affects specific brain regions through the scalp, which acts as electrodes. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (ATDCS) refers to electrical current flow from the anodic electrode to the target brain region, which increases the target brain region excitation. Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (CTDCS) refers to electrical current flowing from the cathode electrode to the target brain region, which decreases the target brain region excitation (8). This excitability change is caused by the change in the resting membrane potential in the relevant region (9). There was a prolonged effect after 30 min of the tDCS stimulation (10), and the effect even lasted several months after repeated tDCS stimulation (11). From fMRI observation, tDCS stimulation of the prefrontal lobe improved network connectivity at rest (12). The weak current of tDCS regulates cortical excitability and spontaneous neural activity by stimulating the corresponding cerebral cortex region, thus improving the functional abnormalities of the corresponding brain regions and showing good safety and tolerability (13).

Among the existing tDCS research, there are many studies on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), some of which have achieved positive results. Allenby believes that ATDCS can reduce the reaction time of Stop-Signal Tasks, which leads to the conclusion that tDCS can alleviate subject impulsivity and delayed gratification difficulties (14). Nejati suggested that the stimulation of the right DLPFC with ATDCS could improve persistent inhibition and partial interference control (15). A combined stimulation of the DLPFC and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) can reduce reaction time, improve cognitive flexibility, and improve working memory (16). According to recent research, Leffa et al. performed daily sessions of 30 min of home-based tDCS for 4 weeks on 64 participants with ADHD, totaling 28 sessions, with 2.0 mA anodal-right and cathodal-left prefrontal stimulations with 35-cm2 carbon electrodes. The efficacy results assessed by the inattentive scores of the clinician-administered versions of the Adult ADHD Self-report Scale (CASRS-I) show decreased symptoms of inattention in the active tDCS group over the three assessments compared to the sham tDCS group (17). However, according to several research studies, there is no evidence that tDCS can improve the response inhibition ability (18) and the sustained attention (19) of patients with ADHD. Some scholars suggested that the clinical efficacy and cognitive effects of tDCS in the treatment of ADHD, whether the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) or the DLPFC, still need to be further verified in future studies (20, 21).

Traditional tDCS stimulation results in the diffusion and distribution of current in a wide range of brain regions, which may not be able to display the maximum current density directly below the electrode, leading to inaccurate positioning of tDCS stimulation. In recent years, the new high-definition tDCS (HD-tDCS) has been proposed to solve the problem of traditional tDCS in affecting the stimulation target to the extra brain regions. The HD-tDCS stimulation current is limited to the region below the electrode and thus improves the accuracy. This ensures high-current density in the main target region, minimizes the stimulation of the non-target regions, and reduces the risk of side effects. This shows that the same effect can be achieved by stimulating the corresponding brain regions with less current than conventional tDCS. Researchers stimulated the right IFG of 15 subjects with ADHD aged 10–16 years with HD-tDCS of 0.5 mA and evaluated the stimulation by the N-back test and event-related potential P300/N200. The results showed that, compared with the traditional tDCS stimulation of 1.0 mA, HD-tDCS also improved working memory and inhibitory control (22). Some researchers believe that HD-tDCS should be set as a further topic to study (23).

Orbito-frontal cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical (OFCSTC) loops, also known as the impulse/force loop, are applied in the control of impulsive behavior (24). The nerve fibers of the loop originate from the OFC and extend into the inferior caudate nucleus, then travel to the thalamus, and finally return to the OFC. The inactivation of this circuit leads to impulse control difficulties and emotional processing disorders. The OFC dysfunction was significantly associated with the severity of impulsive (25) and obsessive behavior (24). Impulsive symptoms of ADHD include excessive speech, unthinking interruptions, blurting out words, and unwillingness to wait in order, all of which involve this loop. The cortical thickness of the OFC in patients with ADHD was significantly lower than that in healthy controls (26). A Structural Covariance Network (SCN) analysis shows that the volume of gray matter on the right side of the OFC of patients with ADHD decreased significantly (27). In addition, structurally, a meta-analysis of whole-brain voxel-based morphometry (VBM) showed disorder-specific gray matter volume (GMV) abnormality in the OFC in ADHD (28). The fMRI scans showed that right OFC activation significantly decreased in patients with high-risk behavioral tendencies in the Go/NO-Go tasks (29). In addition, fMRI showed that the activation of the right OFC was associated with emotion-based risk tasks in negative emergencies, reflecting that risk-taking was associated with the ability of emotion-based risk control (30). Boys with ADHD showed disorder-specific underactivation in the OFC (31). High-resolution fMRI showed that adolescent patients with ADHD display enhanced OFC signaling of future rewards and that these increased reward-related responses were correlated with the severity of hyperactivity/impulsivity (32). Decreased cognitive capacity related to hyperactivity and impulsivity was associated with reduced OFC activity during reward expectation in patients with ADHD (28).

Although OFC was not a sufficiently activated region to be underactivated in recent fMRI meta-analyses of ADHD (3335), there was evidence from the aforementioned studies for OFC underactivation, mainly in terms of rewards or emotions. Furthermore, impulse control difficulties and emotion-processing disorders based on OFCSTC could affect cognitive performance. In other words, relatively higher cognitive ability was associated with normalized OFC responses (32). Therefore, in this study, we hypothesized that HD-tDCS of the right OFC could alleviate the clinical symptoms, impulse control difficulties, and emotion processing of patients with ADHD to further improve their performance on cognitive tasks such as maintaining attention and inhibitory control and then test this hypothesis using a randomized, sham-controlled study.

Materials and methods

Research subjects

Inclusion criteria

Subjects included patients with ADHD who visited the general outpatient department and the children's outpatient department of the Zhenjiang Mental Health Center between March 2020 and November 2021. The patients were children and adolescents aged 8–18 years. They were diagnosed and reviewed by a senior associate chief physician or a chief physician of the department of psychiatry. All subjects met the diagnostic criteria of ADHD of the validated screening and diagnostic instrument: the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) and The International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10). Simultaneously, the subtypes of ADHD were also classified, such as inattentive, hyperactive-impulsive, or the combined subtype; The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV Chinese version was administered to every subject, with a total IQ ≥ 80 (36). All children were of Chinese Han origin and were right-handed.

Exclusion criteria

Contraindications for tDCS treatment include patients with metal device implants (such as the cochlear implant, the artery clamp, and the pacemaker); history of brain trauma or cerebrovascular accident, intracranial hypertension, skull defects, epilepsy, and other serious neurological, circulatory, endocrine, and other physical diseases; audio-visual impairments and color blindness, color weakness, or narrow-angle glaucoma. The abovementioned contraindications were excluded by inquiring and collecting medical history, conducting an electrocardiogram (ECG), electroencephalogram (EEG), cranial CT, and blood routine and biochemical examinations. All subjects were evaluated for no comorbidities of other mental disorders with validated screening and diagnostic instruments: DSM-5 and ICD-10, such as substance abuse/dependence, conduct disorders, personality disorders, autism, Tourette's disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Patients who had used any medication (methylphenidate, atomoxetine, etc.) in the past and recently to treat ADHD or who received other brain stimulation (transcranial magnetic stimulation, electroconvulsive shock, etc.) were also excluded.

To calculate the sample size, we used G*Power (37) with the following settings: effect size f = 0.25, α level = 0.05, power = 0.8, and correlation among repeated measures = 0.5. The minimum sample size was found to be n = 44. To prevent a potentially large number of dropouts, a total of 56 subjects were recruited, including 33 boys and 23 girls. A completely randomized experimental design was adopted, and the subjects were divided into two groups according to age through a random number table: the HD-tDCS group and the Sham group (Figure 1). A general information questionnaire was developed, including age, gender, educational years, whether the subject comes from a single-parent family, age of onset, and disease. Both the participants and their guardians were informed of this study, and signed informed consent was obtained. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Zhenjiang Mental Health Center. This trial was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) guidelines (38).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. CONSORT flow diagram (38) of this RCT from enrolment, intervention allocation, follow-up and analysis.

Methods

While the HD-tDCS group received real stimulation, the Sham group received sham stimulation. Before receiving stimulation (T0), all subjects underwent ADHD symptom assessments (SNAP-IV rating scale, Conners Parents Questionnaire) and cognitive task [Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA-CPT), Stroop, Tower of Hanoi (TOH)] assessments to collect baseline data. Then, the subjects underwent either HD-tDCS stimulation or sham stimulation. The above ADHD symptom and cognitive task assessments were repeated for all subjects after the 5th stimulus (T1), the 10th stimulus (T2), and at the 6th-week follow-up after the end of all stimuli (T3).

HD-tDCS

HD-tDCS uses a multichannel stimulator (Soterix Medical, 4 × 1-C3A, USA) that uses a constant direct current stimulator of conventional tDCS (Soterix Medical, 1 × 1 Low-Intensity Transcranial DC Stimulator, 1300A, USA), delivers, and converts it to high-definition stimulation. Conventional tDCS produces diffuse brain currents. The electrodes of HD-tDCS are arranged in a 4 × 1 ring on the skull, producing a more concentrated and precise current that is confined to the return electrode ring.

Stimulation site

Five circular Ag/AgCl electrodes with a diameter of 1 cm were placed, and one anode electrode was placed on the center: the right OFC, corresponding to the standard electrode location of the International Electroencephalogram Society 10/20 System: Fp2; four cathode electrodes (i.e., return electrodes) are placed in a square around the anode, about 5 cm away from the anode, and corresponding to Fpz, Afz, AF4, and AF8 (Figure 2). Electric field simulation was performed using the HD-Explorer software (Soterix Medical, USA). The intensity of the simulated field is indicated by the color bar, the arrow points to the direction of the current, and the length represents the current intensity (Figure 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. HD-tDCS electrode layout. An anodic electrode was placed at the center: right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), corresponding to Fp2; four cathode electrodes are placed square around the anode, corresponding to Fpz, Afz, AF4, and AF8.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. HD-tDCS electric field simulation. The intensity of the simulated field is indicated by the color bar, the arrow points to the direction of the current, and the length represents the current intensity.

Stimulation parameters

The HD-tDCS anode current intensity was 1.0 mA, and each stimulation lasted for 20 min during which there was 30 s of current increase time and 30 s of current decrease time, one time a day for five consecutive days, 2 days of rest after five consecutive days of stimulation, and a total of 10 sessions. Most of the previous HD-tDCS studies have shown effective cortical stimulation and inhibition with 2.0 mA. However, several other factors such as the age of the subject and subject-specific skull thickness could have also played a role in the differing outcomes in addition to the current intensity (39). Referring to previous studies, such as that of Breitling et al. (22), on stimulated subjects with ADHD aged 10–16 years with HD-tDCS of 0.25 and 0.5 mA, considering that the subjects are children and adolescents and the skull thickness is different from that of adults, the current intensity is selected as 1.0 mA in this study. The sham group received sham stimulation, in which the subjects of the HD-tDCS group underwent under the same electrode setting. During the stimulation, the current was increased for 30 s, and after reaching 1.0 mA, the current was reduced to 0 in the following 30 s to simulate the skin feeling during HD-tDCS and make the subjects have the same subjective feeling as the real stimulation.

Clinical symptom assessment for ADHD

Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham-IV rating scale

The Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham-IV rating scale (SNAP-IV rating scale) rating scale has good reliability and validity (40, 41). This scale is compiled according to the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for ADHD, with a total of 18 items that are summarized in two factors: attention deficit (items 1–9) and hyperactivity/impulsivity (items 10–18) were scored on a scale of 4 for symptom severity (none at all 0; A little bit is one point; Not too little is 2 points; and Very many are 3 points), selected by parents according to their children's general impression. The scores are on average.

Conners child behavior scale parent symptom questionnaire

The Conners' Parent Rating Scales (CPRS) revised in 1978 has a total of 48 items (42). Previous research has demonstrated that the Parent Symptom Questionnaire (PSQ) has good reliability in China (Cronbach's α = 0.93) and may be used to evaluate Chinese children (43). These include the Conduct factor (items 2, 8, 14, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 27, 33, 34, and 39), Learning factor (items 10, 25, 31, and 37), Physical and mental factor (items 32, 41, 43, 44, and 48), Hyperactivity-impulsivity index (items 4, 5, 11, and 13), Anxiety factor (items 12, 16, 24, and 27), and Hyperactivity index (items 4, 7, 11, 13, 14, 25, 31, 33, 37, and 38). Each item was rated on a scale of 4; 0 to 3 by parents based on observation. The score was on average.

Cognitive tasks

Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test

The IVA + CPT is a valuable tool for assessing ADHD (44, 45). The first part of the test is the Visual Attention Test. The visual stimulus numbers 0–9 were presented on the computer monitor screen, and each time, the 10 numbers were randomly arranged on the screen for about 2 s (the time was not fixed to exclude the interference of the subjects' preparatory actions); the subjects were required to find out the specific number (such as “3”) then and click the left mouse button to confirm. The test cycle was 12 min. The second part was the Auditory Attention Test: auditory stimulus numbers 0–9 were played by a computer speaker and a random number was played each time with an interval of about 2 s. The subjects were asked to identify whether the number they heard was a specific number (such as “5”) and then click the left mouse button to confirm. The test was repeated several times and lasted for 12 min. The third part was the Combination of the Audiovisual Attention Tests: whenever a random number 0–9 is displayed on the computer display screen, random numbers 0–9 were displayed on the computer speaker, and when the number displayed on the screen matches the number played by the speaker, the subject was required to confirm by clicking the left mouse button. The test interval was about 2 s and the cycle lasted for 12 min. The evaluation indices included the correct response numbers, the commission errors (false response numbers), the omissions (missed report numbers), and the average reaction time (ms) of the combination of visual, auditory, and audiovisual indices.

Stroop Color and Word Test

This test is a measurement paradigm of interference suppression (46), which is divided into the basic reading part: the Word Test and the Color Test; interference with the reading part: the Word Meaning Interference Test and the Color Meaning Interference Test. In the word test, the subjects are required to read out different characters on the card, including 30 Chinese characters “red, green, blue, and yellow” printed in black on a white background in 3 rows × 10 columns. In the color test, the subjects are required to read out the colors of different color blocks. There are 30 color blocks randomly arranged in 3 rows × 10 columns of “red, green, blue, and yellow.” The time (s) of completing the Word and Color Tests are recorded. The Word and Color Tests are automated processes that assess short-term attention and reading speed. In the interference with the reading part, 12 rows × 9 columns are randomly arranged in four colors: red, green, blue, and yellow, showing four kinds of Chinese characters: “red, green, blue, and yellow.” A total of 50% of the characters have the same meaning and color and 50% of the characters do not. In the Word Meaning Interference Test, if there is a color word with inconsistent meaning and color, the subjects are required to read the color instead of the Chinese character (for example, “red” is printed in green and “green” is read instead of “red”) and name the color. In the Color Meaning Interference test, the subjects are required to read the color words with inconsistent meanings and the color according to the meaning of the word and eliminate the color interference. In the Test order, after the Word Test, namely the establishment of the word dominance response, the Word Meaning Interference Test was conducted. In the same way, after the completion of the Color Test, the color dominance response was established before the Color Meaning Interference Test. The subjects were required to complete the above test quickly within the specified time (2 min). Finally, the RT (reaction time) of the interference effect of Word and Color was calculated: the RT incongruent of color and word—RT congruent of color and word.

The Tower of Hanoi

The mission consists of three vertical wooden poles and a fixed number of disks of different sizes (four disks in this study) with holes in them such that they can be placed on the poles. The goal is to move the disks from a starting position to a target position and arrange them in a pyramid form on the target position (47). Constraint conditions: only one disk can be moved at a time and a larger disk cannot be on top of a smaller disk to complete the task in the process. The disk must either be in the process of moving or on the pole. The image of the disk was displayed on a screen and the subject could move the disk by pressing the corresponding key on a keyboard. The evaluation included the total completion time(s) and the steps taken between the first and last moves.

The cognitive tasks above were completed on the computer with software from Nanjing Vishee Medical Technology Co., Ltd.

Statistical analysis

SPSS 28.0 statistical software was used for data analyses. The measurement scale-data satisfying the normal distribution and homogeneity of variance were expressed as mean ± standard deviation (M ± SD). The nominal-data measurements were expressed as number ± percentage [N (%)]. The independent sample T-test/Chi-square test and the repeated-measures ANOVA were adopted. Then, Mauchly's sphericity test was used to evaluate the sphericity of the data before implementing the repeated measures ANOVA. If a P-value was > 0.05, it indicates that Mauchly's sphericity test was violated and therefore the Greenhouse–Geisser test was performed. If a P-value was < 0.05, it indicates that Mauchly's sphericity test was accepted and therefore Roy's largest root exact test was performed. To test the effects of HD-tDCS, repeated measures ANOVA was performed for the within-subject factor of TIME (T0, T1, T2, and T3), the between-subject factor of CONDITION (the real stimulation and the sham stimulation), and the interaction factor of TIME × CONDITION and the Bonferroni correction test as well as the Bonferroni post-hoc test was used. Because ANOVA has 16 variables, after Bonferroni's correction test, test statistics with a P-value of <0.0031 indicate significant results. It means that if the P value is less than alpha 0.31%, then we reject the null hypothesis and consider the result to be statistically significant. GraphPad Prism 9 was used for the diagram.

Results

General demographic information

During the implementation of the experiment, nine cases were lost due to “inconvenient medical treatment, busy study, troublesome treatment and evaluation process, uncomfortable stimulation of the head, and no treatment effect,” including four cases in the HD-tDCS group and five cases in the Sham group, which were not included in the statistics. Before the experiment, the subjects were grouped and divided. Finally, 47 subjects completed the experiment and entered the stage of result analysis (Figure 1). There were 24 subjects in the HD-tDCS group, including 14 boys and 10 girls, and their average age was (11.29 ± 2.51) years. There were 23 subjects in the Sham group, including 13 boys and 10 girls; the average age was (11.74 ± 2.59) years. There were no significant differences between the two groups in terms of age, gender, total IQ, educational years, whether they come from a single-parent family, age of onset, course of disease, and ADHD type between the two groups (P > 0.05). In addition, SNAP-IV and PSQ were taken as baseline clinical manifestations, and there was no significant difference between the two groups (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Subject's demographic characteristics, intelligence, and clinical manifestations.

Clinical symptom assessment results from SNAP-IV and PSQ

Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the SNAP-IV and PSQ scores of both groups. The results showed no statistical significance (P > 0.05) in terms of TIME, CONDITION, and interaction TIME×CONDITION, suggesting that the Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity factor scores of SNAP-IV, Conduct factor, Learning factor, Physical and mental factor, Hyperactivity-impulsivity index, Anxiety factor, and the Hyperactivity index scores of PSQ did not change with the intervention time in the HD-tDCS group and the Sham tDCS group.

Comparison of cognitive task results

Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the correct response number of visual IVA-CPT. There were no statistically significant main effects in terms of TIME (F(3,43) = 4.916, P = 0.005), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 0.546, P = 0.464), and interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(3,43) = 0.006, P = 0.083). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the commission errors of visual IVA-CPT. There were statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(3,43) =11.191, P < 0.001), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 11.512, P = 0.001), and interaction effect for TIME×CONDITION (F(3,43) = 6.635, P < 0.001). A Bonferroni correction test (post-hoc) showed the false response number decreased under the HD-tDCS condition when compared with T0 and T1 (P < 0.001), T2 (P < 0.001), and T3 (P < 0.001). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the omission of visual IVA-CPT. There were statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(3,43) = 6.486, P = 0.001), but no statistically significant main effect for the CONDITION effect (F(1,45) = 0.628, P = 0.432) and the interaction effect for TIME×CONDITION (F(3,43) = 0.702, P = 0.556; Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Comparison of IVA-CPT visual between the two groups at different time points. *The commission errors of IVA-CPT visual: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.516, P < 0.001 TIME (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 11.191, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0 438), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 11.512, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.204), TIME × CONDITION(Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 6.635, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.316). **The correct response number of IVA-CPT visual: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.685, P = 0.005. TIME (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 4.916, P = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.255), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 0.546, P = 0.464, ηp2 = 0.012), TIME × CONDITION (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 0.006, P = 0.083, ηp2 = 0.006). ***The omission of IVA-CPT visual: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.700, P = 0.008. TIME (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 6,486, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.312), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 0.628, P = 0.432, ηp2 = 0.014), TIME × CONDITION (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 0.702, P = 0.556, ηp2 = 0.047).

Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the correct response number of auditory IVA-CPT. There were statistically significant main effects in terms of TIME (F(2.635,118.573) = 11.204, P < 0.001), but no statistically significant effect for CONDITION (F(1,45) = 1.930, P = 0.172) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(2.635,118.573) = 0.269, P = 0.822). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the commission errors of auditory IVA-CPT. There were statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(3,43) = 7.360, P < 0.001) and CONDITION (F(1,45) = 14.210, P < 0.001), but no statistically significant interaction effect for TIME×CONDITION (F(3,43) = 3.974, P = 0.014). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the omission of auditory IVA-CPT. There were statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(3,43) = 11.242, P < 0.001), but no statistically significant main effects for CONDITION (F(1,45) = 3.007, P = 0.090) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(3,43) = 0.956, P = 0.422; Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Comparison of IVA-CPT auditory between the two groups at different time points. *The commission errors of IVA-CPT auditory: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.697, P = 0.008. TIME (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43)= 7.360, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.339), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 14.210, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.240), TIME × CONDITION (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 3.974, P = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.217). **The correct response number of IVA-CPT auditory: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.785, P = 0.061. TIME (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.635,118.573) = 11.204, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.199), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 1.930, P = 0.172, ηp2 = 0.041), TIME × CONDITION (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.635,118.573) = 0.269, P = 0.822, ηp2 = 0.006). ***The omission of IVA-CPT auditory: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.730, P = 0.017. TIME (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 11.242, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.440), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 3.007, P = 0.090, ηp2 = 0.063), TIME × CONDITION (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 0.956, P = 0.422, ηp2 = 0.063).

Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the correct response number of the audiovisual combination of IVA-CPT. There were statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(2.891,130.078) =7.092, P < 0.001), but no statistically significant main effect for CONDITION (F(1,45) =3.744, P = 0.059) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(2.891,130.078) = 0.010, P = 0.998). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the commission errors of the audiovisual combination IVA-CPT. There were statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(3,43) =12.467, P < 0.001), CONDITION (F(1,45) =15.457, P < 0.001) and the interaction effect for TIME×CONDITION (F(3,43) =5.469, P = 0.003). A Bonferroni correction test (post hoc) showed a decrease in the false response number in the HD-tDCS condition compared with T0 and T1 (P < 0.001), T2 (P < 0.001), and T3 (P < 0.001). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the omission of the audiovisual combination IVA-CPT. There were statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(2.865,128.912) = 12.314, P < 0.001), but no statistically significant main effect for CONDITION (F(1,45) = 1.879, P = 0.177) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(2.865,128.912) = 0.440, P = 0.716; Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Comparison of IVA-CPT audiovisual combination between the two groups at different time points. *The commission errors of IVA-CPT audiovisual combination: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.683, P = 0.005. TIME (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43)= 12.467, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.465), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 15.457, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.256), TIME × CONDITION (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 5.469, P < 0.003, ηp2 = 0.276). **The correct response number of IVA-CPT audiovisual combination: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.940, P = 0.747. TIME (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.891,130.078) = 7.092, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.136), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 3.744, P = 0.059, ηp2 = 0.014), TIME × CONDITION (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.891,130.078) = 0.010, P = 0.998, ηp2 = 0.002). ***The omission of IVA-CPT audiovisual combination: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.926, P = 0.643. TIME (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.865,128.912) = 12.314, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.215), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 1.879, P = 0.177, ηp2 = 0.040), TIME × CONDITION (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.865,128.912) = 0.440, P = 0.716, ηp2 = 0.010).

Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the mean reaction time of visual IVA-CPT. There were no statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(2.877,129.444) =1.739, P = 0.162), CONDITION (F(1,45) =0.471, P = 0.496) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(2.877,129.444) = 0.220, P = 0.875). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the mean reaction time of auditory IVA-CPT. There were no statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(2.878,129.494) = 2.002, P = 0.117), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 0.080, P = 0.778) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(2.878,129.494) = 0.574, P = 0.626). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the mean reaction time of the audiovisual combination IVA-CPT. There were statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(2.681,120.643) = 10.156, P < 0.001), but no statistically significant main effect for CONDITION (F(1,45) = 0.165, P = 0.687) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(2.681,120.643) = 0.500, P = 0.679; Figure 7).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Comparison of IVA-CPT mean reaction time between the two groups at different time points. *The mean reaction time of IVA-CPT audiovisual combination: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.824, P = 0.133. TIMF (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.681,120.643) = 10.156, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.184), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 0.165, P = 0.687, np2 = 0.004), TIME × CONDITION (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.681,120.643) = 0.500, P = 0.679, ηp2 = 0.011).

These results suggested that the correct response number of the visual, the mean visual, and the auditory reaction time of both groups did not change with the intervention time, the correct response number of auditory and audiovisual combination, the commission errors of auditory reaction time, the omission of the visual, auditory, and audiovisual combination, and the average reaction time for the audiovisual combination that gradually increased or decreased with the intervention time in both groups, but there was no significant increase or decrease in the HD-tDCS group than in the Sham tDCS group and the commission errors of visual and audiovisual combination in both groups decreased gradually with the intervention time. Furthermore, the HD-tDCS group compared with the Sham tDCS group was more significantly decreased after the 5th intervention, the 10th intervention, and the 6th-week follow-up.

Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the Interference RT of Word. There was no statistically significant main effect for TIME (F(2.593,116,707) = 3.376, P = 0.026) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(2.593,116.707) = 2.350, P = 0.085). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the Interference RT of Color. There were no statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(3,43) = 3.454, P = 0.025) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(3,43) = 3.429, P = 0.025). Figure 8 indicates that the Interference RT of Color and Word in both groups did not change with the intervention time.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Comparison of Stoop Interference effect RT of Word and Color between the two group at different time. *The interference effect of word: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.785, P = 0.060. TIME (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.593,116.707) = 3.376, P = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.070), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 12.790, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.221), TIME × CONDITION (Greenhouse-Geisser test: F(2.593,116.707) =2.350, P = 0.085, ηp2 = 0.050). **The interference effect of color: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.689, P = 0.006. TIME (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 3.454, P = 0.025, ηp2 = 0.194), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 9.532, P < 0.003, ηp2 = 0.175), TIME × CONDITION (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 3.429, P = 0.025, ηp2 = 0.194).

Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the total completion time of TOH. There were statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(3,43) =13.237, P < 0.001) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(3,43) =6.733, P < 0.001). A Bonferroni correction test (post hoc) showed that the total completion time reduced in the HD-tDCS condition compared to T0 and T1 (P < 0.001), T2 (P < 0.001), and T3 (P < 0.001). Repeated measurement ANOVA was performed for the total completion steps of TOH. There were no statistically significant main effects for TIME (F(3,43) = 5.194, P = 0.004), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 9.410, P = 0.004) and the interaction effect for TIME × CONDITION (F(3,43) = 2.639, P = 0.062). It suggests that the total completion steps of TOH in both groups did not change with the intervention time, but the total completion time of TOH in both groups decreased gradually with the intervention time. Furthermore, the HD-tDCS group compared with the Sham tDCS group was more significantly reduced after the 5th intervention, the 10th intervention, and the 6th-week follow-up (Figures 9, 10).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Comparison of TOH completion time between the two groups at different time. *The total completion time oh TOH: Mauchly sphericity test: W = 0.548, P < 0.001. TIME (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 13.237, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.480), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 6.672, P = 0.013, ηp2 = 0.129), TIME × CONDITION (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 6.733, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.320.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Comparison of TOH completion steps between the two groups at different time. *The total completion steps of TOH: Mauehly sphericity test: W = 0.369, P < 0.001. TIME (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 5.194, P = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.266), CONDITION (F(1,45) = 9.410, P = 0.004, ηp2 =0.173), TIME × CONDITION (Roy's largest root exact test: F(3,43) = 2.639, P = 0.062, ηp2 = 0.156).

Discussion

This study observed some positive effects. The results showed that the commission errors of the visual and audiovisual combination of IVA-CPT tasks changed significantly in the two groups. Furthermore, comparing real stimulation with sham tDCS, there was a significant improvement in the commission errors after real HD-tDCS intervention, and this effect was even reflected in the follow-up 6 weeks later. The IVA-CPT tasks were not only attention-maintaining tasks but also inhibition-control tasks. The tasks were intended to be mildly boring to produce the omission (i.e., inattention) and commission errors (i.e., impulsivity) through a series of trial sets requiring responding and not responding, respectively. The commission errors of IVA-CPT reflected the inhibition ability of impulse. The subjects had to suppress impulses instead of making mistakes when they received the visual and audiovisual combination stimuli (44, 45, 48). Therefore, it showed that real HD-tDCS can improve the inhibitory control of the subjects in addition to improving attention maintenance. This was similar to previous studies that showed that tDCS tended to improve significantly only in interference control and inhibition, but not in working memory or reaction time variability in the analyses of neuropsychological performance measures (49). This effect is also supported by studies on the mechanism of tDCS, which regulates the concentration levels of the excitatory neurotransmitter glutamate and the inhibitory neurotransmitter γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA). A magnetic resonance spectrum study found that GABA concentration increased after ATDCS stimulation, while glutamate concentration decreased after CTDCS stimulation (50). Some researchers believe that part of the mechanism of tDCS is to regulate the excitatory and inhibitory balance of the cortex (E/I) (51). Some studies suggested that, although the improvement of inhibitory control is assumed to be caused by the enhanced activity of the stimulating region, many experiments do not stimulate the target region alone. For example, the use of large electrodes (35 cm2 surface area) will cause extensive changes in cortical excitability, which may lead to changes in the overall arousal level of the brain (52). Therefore, it cannot be simply identified as the therapeutic effect produced by a certain area. Sotnikova et al. used ATDCS to stimulate the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and analyzed the functional connectivity of the brain through functional magnetic resonance after stimulation. The results showed that, in the N-Back Task, whether the left DLPFC is under the action of the electrode or the left premotor cortex, the left auxiliary motor cortex, and the precuneus are not under the action of the electrode, tDCS-induced activity in these regions, suggesting that anodal tDCS can lead to increased neuronal activation and connectivity, which stimulates not only the brain regions below the electrode but also the possibly other brain regions further away (53). These studies indicate that HD-tDCS is the future direction of research. As HD-tDCS solves the problem of traditional tDCS affecting the stimulation target and the outer brain region, the stimulation current is limited to the region below the electrode, thus improving the accuracy and excluding the interference of non-target region stimulation. However, there are few studies on HD-tDCS and no studies on the right OFC. In the past, only Breitling et al. (22) studied the effect of HD-tDCS on the right IFG in 33 adolescent ADHD subjects for five consecutive days. In this study, HD-tDCS was applied to OFC, and the neuropsychological measures showed that it had a positive effect on attention maintenance and inhibitory control. It was consistent with the role of OFC in the OFCSTC loop, and it also confirmed our hypothesis that tDCS activated OFC to improve impulse control difficulties and emotional processing, thereby further improving the performance of cognitive tasks, such as attention maintenance and inhibitory control.

Another interesting result of this study was that, after the stimulation of real tDCS, the TOH completion time decreases, while the number of TOH completion steps did not change, which seemed to contradict the results of the aforementioned improved inhibitory control. The TOH is a problem that cannot be solved in one step. Subjects need to plan a reasonable sequence of steps to follow the rules and use as few steps as possible. The functions measured by TOH include cognitive planning, problem-solving, attention shifting, and attention maintenance. Inhibitory control also involved solving the TOH problem, in which subjects had to temporarily shift the smaller disk away from where it should end up to place the larger disk in the desired position. Working memory also participates in the whole process of TOH problem-solving, which is inseparable from the spatial memory of the location of the disk, which is undoubtedly a kind of working memory activity to remember the location of a specific disk while moving the disk. If impulsivity control or working memory had been improved in the TOH task, the number of TOH completed steps should have been reduced, but this was not the case. The TOH only increased the time of accomplishment, but not the number of steps required for accomplishment. The possible explanation is that real tDCS improves attention maintenance, and subjects need to increase sustained attention to complete tasks without distraction. In this study, although the time of completing TOH was significantly shortened after real HD-tDCS intervention, there were no significant differences between the two groups in the reaction time, the omission of the IVA-CPT task, and the Interference RT of Stroop Color and Word. The interference effect of Stroop Word and Color reflects attention duration, alertness, and cognitive processing speed. Meanwhile, the subjects need to suppress the automatic processing response to the word meaning or color meaning itself, eliminate the interference of the dominant stimulus attribute, respond to the inferior attribute of the stimulus, and evaluate the inhibition control ability of the subjects. This could be interpreted as that the TOH has higher and more complex difficulties than IVA-CPT and Stroop, requiring more executive function mobilization, which is consistent with previous studies. Gill et al. (54) found that ATDCS stimulation is more effective with a higher working memory load. Another possible explanation, similar to the absence of significant improvement in ADHD symptoms, is that our study may have done too few sessions to observe the effect of the Stoop effect.

In terms of clinical symptoms of ADHD, the results of this study show that there were no significant changes in the SNAP-IV and PSQ scores, and each factor of the two groups before and after the intervention indicates that HD-tDCS, despite real stimulus or sham stimulus, had no obvious immediate effect on the overall symptoms of ADHD. Most of the previous tDCS studies focused on neuropsychological changes, and only a few studies focused on clinical symptoms. Some researchers suggested that there is a dissociation between neuropsychological deficits and clinical symptoms of ADHD, which means that even if there is improvement in the neuropsychological deficits after or during tDCS, such as improvement in inhibitory control and WM, it does not mean that the clinical symptoms have improved as well (55). Meta-analyses of tDCS studies targeting mostly the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex show small effects on cognitive improvements with only two out of three studies showing clinical improvements (21). The systematic retrieval and meta-analysis of tDCS studies showed that most anode tDCS of the left dlPFC had only a very limited trend-level effect in improving inhibition and processing speed, and there was no evidence of alleviation in attention and other clinical symptoms (20). However, other researchers have come up with different conclusions. Brauer et al., by meta-analyzing 13 studies, including 20 study arms, showed that tDCS had an immediate effect on overall symptom severity, inattention, and impulsivity, but not on hyperactivity. The results were significant in children and adolescents. The follow-up data (3 days−4 weeks after stimulation) suggested an ongoing beneficial effect regarding overall symptom severity and a delayed effect on hyperactivity (49). They came to this conclusion on the basis that, although most of these studies did not provide a clinical outcome replacement for assessing the effect of tDCS on cognitive functioning in ADHD, there are several studies that report high correlations between different executive dysfunctions and ADHD core symptoms (56). Soff et al. observed that tDCS could improve the subjects' working memory and memory consolidation ability, thereby alleviating symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity, by following five consecutive anodal tDCS sessions. By the 7th day after the treatment, a long-lasting tDCS effect was implied when applying for repeated sessions (57). Previous studies showed that the tDCS physiological effects might depend on the stimulation duration and current intensity with the potential for long-lasting neuroplastic changes after multiple sessions, likely due to the changes in the synaptic strength induced by long-term potentiation (LTP)-like response and metaplasticity mechanisms (11, 58). However, this delayed effect was not observed for the 10 repeated sessions in this study and the effect of HD-tDCS after 6 weeks of follow-up was also insignificant. The likely explanation was that most of the previous studies tested five sessions and only two studies tested larger numbers of sessions. Westwood et al. (59) found no improvement after 15 sessions and Leffa et al. (17) found an improvement after 28 sessions but not after 14 sessions. This suggested that we need more sessions. Our study may have done too few sessions to observe the effect of the clinical symptoms.

In terms of other neuropsychological indicators, the results of this study showed that, compared with sham stimulus, real HD-tDCS had no significant changes in the correct response number of auditory and audiovisual combination, the commission errors of auditory, the omission of visual, auditory, and audiovisual combination, and the average reaction time of audiovisual combination. The average reaction time of IVA-CPT reflects alertness, cognitive processing speed, and hand-eye-ear coordination. The number of missing reports in IVA-CPT reflects the subjects' attention deficits, that is the intensity and stability of attention. This is partly similar to previous studies. Ouellet et al. evaluated the executive function of healthy subjects by the Iowa Gambling Task, the Stroop Task, the Visual Simulation Scale, the Continuous Work Task, and the Stop Signal Task, among others, after receiving 1.5 mA ATDCS in the left or right OFC. The results showed that subjects receiving ATDCS stimulation of the OFC had more favorable decision-making ability, but tDCS had no effect on attention level (60). The findings of the use of tDCS to improve ADHD cognition were mixed, with some positive results on improving cognition. However, the effect value observed in the meta-analysis is very small. Although the comparability of the results was hampered by the large heterogeneity of the study designs and methods, outcome measures, stimulation parameters, and the sites of anodal and cathodal stimulation (21), there also was heterogeneity in cognitive dysfunction in ADHD (61). Based on current evidence, most of the cognitive effects that have been demonstrated are small and insignificant (20, 21). The results of this study can also be interpreted as the learning effect and the repetition effect of tasks. However, tDCS stimulation might also enhance the learning effect. Sham stimuli that were immediately followed by effective stimuli showed better task performance than expected (62). Jacoby and Lavidor (19) also found that the continuous performance task was not affected by tDCS stimulation, and they believed that the learning effect and the repetition effect of the CPT task itself might have an impact on hyperactivity.

The OFC has also been the target region of tDCS research in recent years, although some results have not been particularly promising. For example, some researchers suggested that tDCS reduces resting blood perfusion in the OFC, which is negatively correlated to risky task behavior (63). The tDCS stimulates the OFC, although it has no effect on the impulsivity, exploration of novel things, and risk-taking behaviors of patients with ADHD. However, it may benefit the resistance to new things and avoidance behaviors of OCD patients (64). Recently, a strictly double-blind, randomized, sham-controlled trial was conducted to treat 50 boys with ADHD with right frontal hypothalamus (rIFC) anode tDCS (near the OFC stimulation site) for 15 working days, and combined with cognitive training, the results showed no clinical or cognitive improvement. The findings suggested that rIFC stimulation may not be indicated as a neurotherapy for cognitive or clinical remediation for ADHD (59). However, conclusive evidence from previous tDCS studies in ADHD is mixed by remarkable heterogeneity with respect to stimulus protocol, sample, ADHD symptoms, and cognitive outcome measures. Therefore, this study draws cautious conclusions that, although HD-tDCS does not significantly improve the overall symptoms of patients with ADHD, it can significantly improve their attention maintenance and other neuropsychological deficits. The results further speculated the effect of HD-tDCS in ADHD, indicating that decision-making and impulse control (cognitive and motor control) are complex and interrelated processes and they depend on neural networks containing multiple cortical and subcortical regions, among which the OFC is particularly important. This study also fills the gap in the research of HD-tDCS stimulation of the right OFC.

Conclusion

This rigorous randomized, sham-controlled trial that had 10 sessions of HD-tDCS was conducted over the right OFC in 47 children and adolescents with ADHD. Although tDCS cannot be recommended as an alternative neurotherapy for ADHD yet, this study draws cautious conclusions that HD-tDCS does not significantly improve the overall symptoms of ADHD patients but leads to significant improvements in cognitive measures of attention maintenance. This study also fills in the research blank of HD-tDCS stimulation of the right OFC.

Limitations

This study has some limitations, for example, the sample size is relatively small. When patients with ADHD and their parents gave informed consent in the clinic, although experimenters have given full information and explanation, they are still sensitive to words like “electrical stimulation” and even mistake them for “electric shock.” Therefore, they often refused to join the group, which also added to the difficulty of sample collection. A small sample size poses risks that uncontrollable and confounding variables may be unevenly distributed between groups and, thus, potentially affect the results of the experiment. Another example is the higher depigmentation rate. The depigmentation follow-up was analyzed and it was found that, although they only report mild and transient side effects such as tingling or itching of the scalp, mostly not too good stimulation experience is given, which prompted them to depigmentation experiments. This was also reflected in the fact that some of the subjects who completed the experiment showed impatience when the stimulus was administered. Some subjects even reject repetitive and boring cognitive tasks. They think that the whole experiment process of nearly 2 h is tedious, which is also a big factor for the subjects for whom getting medical treatment is difficult. Finally, the interference items were not excluded as much as possible, and the differences in the age of the subjects (age stratification was not achieved) were not distinguished in the grouping. Among the recruited subjects, there were fewer different ADHD subtypes. For example, in this study, there were more subjects with the combined subtype, but fewer subjects with inattention and hyperactive-impulsive subtypes. Therefore, it was not possible to distinguish the differences among the treatment effects of different subtypes. The learning effect and repetition effect were not excluded, and no crossover experimental design was carried out. All of these factors may overestimate or underestimate the effect of the stimulus. The cortical activity of each subject is also different from their own cognitive level, and these confounding factors are also important reasons for the effect of HD-tDCS. Future studies should systematically evaluate the role of interindividual factors (i.e., ADHD subtype, types of the deficit) and stimulation parameters (i.e., site, polarity, intensity, duration, and repetition rate) on tDCS efficacy in the ADHD population (55).

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Zhenjiang Mental Health Center. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

Y-cWa and JL conceived, designed this study protocol, and wrote this manuscript. Y-cWa, JL, Y-cWu, YW, H-jX, TZ, and ZZ performed this study. Y-cWa, Y-cWu, and ZZ discussed and conducted data analysis. All authors contributed to data analysis, drafting or revising the article, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

This study was funded by the Zhenjiang Science and Technology Plan (social development guidance) project of the Jiangsu Province of China (No. FZ2020059).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL, Crowell EW. Learning disabilities and ADHD: overlapping spectrum disorders. J Learn Disabil. (2000) 33:417–24. doi: 10.1177/002221940003300502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2014) 171:276–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Faraone SV, Banaschewski T, Coghill D, Zheng Y, Biederman J, Bellgrove MA, et al. The World Federation of ADHD International Consensus Statement: 208 evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 128:789–818. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Nigg JT, Sibley MH, Thapar A, Karalunas SL. Development of ADHD: etiology, heterogeneity, and early life course. Annu Rev Dev Psychol. (2020) 2:559–83. doi: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-060320-093413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Matthews M, Nigg JT, Fair DA. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2014) 16:235–66. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-45758-0_249

CrossRef Full Text

6. Posner J, Polanczyk GV, Sonuga-Barke E. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. (2020) 395:450–62. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)33004-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Pievsky MA, McGrath RE. The neurocognitive profile of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review of meta-analyses. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2018) 33:143–57. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acx055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. (2000) 527(Pt 3):633–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Stagg CJ, Nitsche MA. Physiological basis of transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroscientist. (2011) 17:37–53. doi: 10.1177/1073858410386614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology. (2001) 57:1899–901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Cohen Kadosh R, Soskic S, Iuculano T, Kanai R, Walsh V. Modulating neuronal activity produces specific and long-lasting changes in numerical competence. Curr Biol. (2010) 20:2016–20. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Keeser D, Meindl T, Bor J, Palm U, Pogarell O, Mulert C, et al. Prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation changes connectivity of resting-state networks during fMRI. J Neurosci. (2011) 31:15284–93. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0542-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Buchanan DM, Bogdanowicz T, Khanna N, Lockman-Dufour G, Robaey P, D'Angiulli A. A systematic review on the safety and tolerability of transcranial direct current stimulation in children and adolescents. Brain Sci. (2021) 11:212. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11020212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Allenby C, Falcone M, Bernardo L, Wileyto EP, Rostain A, Ramsay JR, et al. Transcranial direct current brain stimulation decreases impulsivity in ADHD. Brain Stimul. (2018) 11:974–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Nejati V, Movahed Alavi M, Nitsche MA. The impact of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder symptom severity on the effectiveness of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on inhibitory control. Neuroscience. (2021) 466:248–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Nejati V, Salehinejad MA, Nitsche MA, Najian A, Javadi A-H. Transcranial direct current stimulation improves executive dysfunctions in ADHD: implications for inhibitory control, interference control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. J Atten Disord. (2020) 24:1928–43. doi: 10.1177/1087054717730611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Leffa DT, Grevet EH, Bau CHD, Schneider M, Ferrazza CP, da Silva RF, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation vs sham for the treatment of inattention in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the TUNED randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry. (2022) 79:847–56. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.2055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Cosmo C, Baptista AF, de Araújo AN, do Rosário RS, Miranda JG, Montoya P, et al. A randomized, double-blind, sham-controlled trial of transcranial direct current stimulation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0135371. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Jacoby N, Lavidor M. Null tDCS Effects in a sustained attention task: the modulating role of learning. Front Psychol. (2018) 6:9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Westwood SJ, Radua J, Rubia K. Noninvasive brain stimulation in children and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2021) 46:E14–33. doi: 10.1503/jpn.190179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Rubia K, Westwood S, Aggensteiner PM, Brandeis D. Neurotherapeutics for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a review. Cells. (2021) 10:2156. doi: 10.3390/cells10082156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Breitling C, Zaehle T, Dannhauer M, Tegelbeckers J, Flechtner H-H, Krauel K, et al. Comparison between conventional and HD-tDCS of the right inferior frontal gyrus in children and adolescents with ADHD. Clin Neurophysiol. (2020) 131:1146–54. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.12.412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Karstens MI, Cohen Kadosh R. Targeting neuronal correlates of executive function in ADHD using brain stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. (2020) 131:1142–3. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Wood J, Ahmari SE. A framework for understanding the emerging role of corticolimbic-ventral striatal networks in OCD-associated repetitive behaviors. Front Syst Neurosci. (2015) 9:171. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Nanda P, Tandon N, Mathew IT, Padmanabhan JL, Clementz BA, Pearlson GD, et al. Impulsivity across the psychosis spectrum: correlates of cortical volume, suicidal history, and social and global function. Schizophr Res. (2016) 170:80–6. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.11.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Fernández-Jaén A, López-Martín S, Albert J, Fernández-Mayoralas DM, Fernández-Perrone AL, Tapia DQ, et al. Cortical thinning of temporal pole and orbitofrontal cortex in medication-naïve children and adolescents with ADHD. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 224:8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Li X, Cao Q, Pu F, Li D, Fan Y, An L, et al. Abnormalities of structural covariance networks in drug-naive boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2015) 231:273–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Gao X, Zhang M, Yang Z, Wen M, Huang H, Zheng R, et al. Structural and functional brain abnormalities in internet gaming disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a comparative meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:679437. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.679437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Brown MR, Benoit JR, Juhás M, Lebel RM, MacKay M, Dametto E, et al. Neural correlates of igh-risk behavior tendencies and impulsivity in an emotional Go/NoGo fMRI task. Front Syst Neurosci. (2015) 9:24. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Cyders MA, Dzemidzic M, Eiler WJ, Coskunpinar A, Karyadi KA, Kareken DA, et al. Negative urgency mediates the relationship between amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex activation to negative emotional stimuli and general risktaking. Cerebral Cortex. (2015) 25:4094–102. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Chantiluke K, Barrett N, Giampietro V, Santosh P, Brammer M, Simmons A, et al. Inverse fluoxetine effects on inhibitory brain activation in non-comorbid boys with ADHD and with ASD. Psychopharmacology. (2015) 232:2071–82. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3837-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Tegelbeckers J, Kanowski M, Krauel K, Haynes J-D, Breitling C, Flechtner H-H, et al. Orbitofrontal signaling of future reward is associated with hyperactivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Neurosci. (2018) 38:6779–86. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0411-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hart H, Radua J, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K. Meta-analysis of fMRI studies of timing in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2012) 36:2248–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Lukito S, Norman L, Carlisi C, Radua J, Hart H, Simonoff E, et al. Comparative meta-analyses of brain structural and functional abnormalities during cognitive control in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Psychol Med. (2020) 50:894–919. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720000574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Samea F, Soluki S, Nejati V, Zarei M, Cortese S, Eickhoff SB, et al. Brain alterations in children/adolescents with ADHD revisited: a neuroimaging meta-analysis of 96 structural and functional studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 100:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Zhang HC. The revision of WISC-IV Chinese version. Psychol Sci. (2009) 32:1177–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

37. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Moher D, Hopewell S, Schulz KF, Montori V, Gøtzsche PC, Devereaux PJ, et al. CONSORT 2010 explanation and elaboration: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. Int J Surg 10. (2012) 28–55. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2011.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Parlikar R, Vanteemar SS, Shivakumar V, Narayanaswamy CJ, Rao PN, Ganesan V. High definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS): a systematic review on the treatment of neuropsychiatric disorders. Asian J Psychiatr. (2021) 56:102542. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Gau SS, Shang CY, Liu SK, Lin CH, Swanson JM, Liu YC, et al. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham, version IV scale - parent form. Int J Method Psychiatr Res. (2008) 17:35–44. doi: 10.1002/mpr.237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Hall CL, Guo B, Valentine AZ, Groom MJ, Daley D, Sayal K, et al. The validity of the SNAP-IV in children displaying ADHD symptoms. Assessment. (2020) 27:1258–71. doi: 10.1177/1073191119842255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Gianarris WJ, Golden CJ, Greene L. The Conners' Parent Rating Scales: a critical review of the literature. Clin Psychol Rev. (2001) 21:1061–93. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(00)00085-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fan J, Du YS, Wang LW. The norm and reliability of the Conners Parent Symptom Questionnaire in Chinese urban children. Shanghai Arch Psychiat. (2005) 17:321–3.

Google Scholar

44. Kim J, Lee Y, Han D, Min K, Kim D, Lee C, et al. The utility of quantitative electroencephalography and integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test as auxiliary tools for the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder diagnosis. Clin Neurophysiol. (2015) 126:532–40. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.06.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Gamvrouli M, Gamvrouli I, Triantafyllou C. Evaluation tools of the characteristics of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents: a literature review. Psychiatriki. (2021). doi: 10.22365/jpsych.2021.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Homack S, Riccio CAA. meta-analysis of the sensitivity and specificity of the Stroop Color and Word Test with children. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2004) 19:725–43. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2003.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kopecky H, Chang HT, Klorman R, Thatcher JE, Borgstedt AD. Performance and private speech of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder while taking the tower of Hanoi test: effects of depth of search, diagnostic subtype, and methylphenidate. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2005) 33:625–38. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-6742-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Tinius TP. The intermediate visual and auditory continuous performance test as a neuropsychological measure. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2003) 18:199–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(01)00196-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Brauer H, Breitling-Ziegler C, Moliadze V, Galling B, Prehn-Kristensen A. A transcranial direct current stimulation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of clinical efficacy outcomes. Prog Brain Res. (2021) 264:91–116. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2021.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Clark VP, Coffman BA, Trumbo MC, Gasparovic C. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) produces localized and specific alterations in neurochemistry: a 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Neurosci Lett. (2011) 500:67–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.05.244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Krause B, Márquez-Ruiz J, Cohen Kadosh R. The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation: a role for cortical excitation/inhibition balance? Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:602. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. McIntire LK, McKinley RA, Goodyear C, Nelson J. A comparison of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation and caffeine on vigilance and cognitive performance during extended wakefulness. Brain Stimul. (2014) 7:499–507. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Sotnikova A, Soff C, Tagliazucchi E, Becker K, Siniatchkin M. Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates neuronal networks in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Brain Topogr. (2017) 30:656–72. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0552-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Gill J, Shah-Basak PP, Hamilton R. It's the thought that counts: examining the task-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive function. Brain Stimul. (2015) 8:253–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Salehinejad MA, Wischnewski M, Nejati V, Vicario CM, Nitsche MA. Transcranial direct current stimulation in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0215095. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0215095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Silverstein MJ, Faraone SV, Leon TL, Biederman J, Spencer TJ, Adler LA, et al. The relationship between executive function deficits and DSM-5-defined ADHD symptoms. J Atten Disord. (2020) 24:41–51. doi: 10.1177/1087054718804347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Soff C, Sotnikova A, Christiansen H, Becker K, Siniatchkin M. Transcranial direct current stimulation improves clinical symptoms in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Neural Transm. (2017) 124:133–44. doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1646-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Cirillo G, Di Pino G, Capone F, Ranieri F, Florio L, Todisco V, et al. Neurobiological after-effects of non-invasive brain stimulation. Brain Stimul. (2017) 10:1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Westwood SJ, Criaud M, Lam SL, Lukito S, Wallace-Hanlon S, Kowalczyk OS, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) combined with cognitive training in adolescent boys with ADHD: a double-blind, randomised, sham-controlled trial. Psychol Med. (2021) 1−16. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721001859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Ouellet J, McGirr A, Van den Eynde F, Jollant F, Lepage M, Berlim MT, et al. Enhancing decision-making and cognitive impulse control with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) applied over the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC): a randomized and sham-controlled exploratory study. J Psychiatr Res. (2015) 69:27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Roberts BA, Martel MM, Nigg JT. Are there executive dysfunction subtypes within ADHD? J Atten Disord. (2017) 21:284–93. doi: 10.1177/1087054713510349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Breitling C, Zaehle T, Dannhauer M, Bonath B, Tegelbeckers J, Flechtner H-H, et al. Improving interference control in ADHD patients with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Front Cell Neurosci. (2016) 10:72. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Weber MJ, Messing SB, Rao H, Detre JA, Thompson-Schill SL. Prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation alters activation and connectivity in cortical and subcortical reward systems: a tDCS-fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. (2014) 35:3673–86. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Zhu Y, Yang D, Ji W, Huang T, Xue L, Jiang X, et al. The relationship between neurocircuitry dysfunctions and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a review. Biomed Res Int. (2016) 2016:3821579. doi: 10.1155/2016/3821579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), orbital frontal cortex (OFC), high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS), executive function (EF), IVA-CPT

Citation: Wang Y-c, Liu J, Wu Y-c, Wei Y, Xie H-j, Zhang T and Zhang Z (2023) A randomized, sham-controlled trial of high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation on the right orbital frontal cortex in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Front. Psychiatry 14:987093. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.987093

Received: 05 July 2022; Accepted: 13 January 2023;
Published: 13 February 2023.

Edited by:

Tingyu Li, Chongqing Medical University, China

Reviewed by:

Weizhao Lu, Shandong First Medical University, China
Katya Rubia, King's College London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2023 Wang, Liu, Wu, Wei, Xie, Zhang and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yi-chao Wang, yes 12118263@zju.edu.cn

ORCID: Yi-chao Wang orcid.org/0000-0002-0811-1288

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.