Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 31 August 2023
Sec. Aging Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic New Insights on the Relationship Between Neuroplasticity, Genetic Endophenotypes, and Psychiatric Disorders Throughout Aging and in the Elderly Population View all 5 articles

Executive function deficits and medial temporal lobe atrophy in late-life depression and Alzheimer’s disease: a comparative study

Changbiao ChuChangbiao Chu1Weigang Pan,Weigang Pan2,3Yanping Ren,Yanping Ren2,3Peixian Mao,
Peixian Mao2,3*Chunlin Yang,
Chunlin Yang2,3*Chaomeng Liu,
Chaomeng Liu2,3*Yi-lang Tang,Yi-lang Tang4,5
  • 1Innovation Center for Neurological Disorders and Department of Neurology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, National Clinical Research Center for Geriatric Diseases, Beijing, China
  • 2Beijing Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders and National Center for Mental Disorders, Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 3Advanced Innovation Center for Human Brain Protection, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, United States
  • 5Mental Health Service Line, Atlanta VA Medical Center, Decatur, GA, United States

Objectives: Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and late-life depression (LLD) frequently exhibit executive function deficits (EFD) and medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA) as shared characteristics. The objective of this research was to examine the utility of the Trail Making Test (TMT) and the MTA scale in distinguishing between LLD and AD.

Methods: A study of 100 patients, 50 with AD and 50 with LLD, was conducted using a cross-sectional design. The individuals were subjected to clinical evaluations to assess their level of depression and overall cognitive abilities, which included the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). We evaluated executive function deficits (EFD) through the use of the TMT, which includes both TMT-A and TMT-B. MTA was measured using magnetic resonance imaging. To evaluate the ability of TMT and MTA scale to distinguish between the two groups, a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve was utilized. To investigate the connections between MTA and neuropsychological measures, a correlation analysis was performed.

Results: AD patients exhibited notably reduced MMSE, MoCA, and GDS scores, as well as an increased MTA total scores, time spent on TMT-A, and TMT-B compared to LLD patients (p < 0.05). TMT-A and TMT-B both exhibited excellent discriminatory power between AD and LLD, achieving area under curve (AUC) values of 92.2 and 94.2%, respectively. In AD patients, there was a negative correlation between MMSE and MoCA scores and MTA scores, while in LLD patients, there was a positive correlation between time spent on TMT-A and GDS scores and MTA scores.

Conclusion: AD patients experience more severe EFD and MTA than LLD patients. The differential diagnosis of AD and LLD can be aided by the useful tool known as TMT. It is important to acknowledge that TMT is capable of capturing only a fraction of the executive function, thus necessitating a cautious interpretation of research findings.

Introduction

Late-life depression (LLD) is a condition characterized by depressive symptoms that manifest in individuals aged 60 or older, and it was estimated to affect 4–10% of the elderly in the community (1, 2). Unlike major depressive disorder (MDD) seen in younger individuals, LLD is often associated with age-related neurodegeneration, cognitive impairment, and physical complaints (3). On the other hand, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a degenerative neurological disorder characterized by progressive and irreversible cognitive dysfunction and behavioral impairment (4). From a histological and anatomical perspective, the presence of extracellular amyloid-β (Aβ) peptide deposition forming “senile plaques” and intracellular tau protein hyperphosphorylation leading to the formation of “neurofibrillary tangles” are considered two prominent features of AD. These pathological hallmarks are significant contributors to the development of dementia and pose challenges in terms of reversibility for affected patients (5). Depressive symptoms are reported in 30–50% of AD patients and the terms “depression” and “dementia” in these patients are heterogeneous with blurred boundaries (6). Previous studies have suggested that depression may be either a risk factor or a consequence of cognitive decline, or that the two conditions may share some common mechanisms (7, 8). Differential diagnosis between LLD and AD is often challenging in clinical practice due to overlap in their symptoms and shared psychopathology (9, 10). Older individuals experiencing LLD are prone to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) compared to depressed individuals under 60 years old, particularly in memory, attention, and executive function. Additionally, individuals with LLD and AD frequently experience similar brain alterations, including microvascular disease, which may play a role in their progression (11).

Misdiagnosis is frequent in distinguishing LLD from AD, with 15–23 percent of AD patients previously diagnosed with depression and about 30% of LLD patients incorrectly diagnosed with AD (12, 13). A correct and timely diagnosis is crucial for starting appropriate treatment, preventing unnecessary diagnostic tests, and developing future interventions for AD. Improvement of depressive symptoms can lead to better cognitive function in young patients with MDD, but executive function deficits (EFD) often persist in LLD groups (14). These deficits affect impulse inhibition, cognitive flexibility, planning and organization, semantic fluency, and selective attention (15). LLD patients with EFD are at a higher risk of developing AD later on Potter et al. (16). Therefore, it is crucial to identify biomarkers of EFD in LLD patients that can differentiate them from AD patients and develop targeted interventions.

White matter hyperintensities are commonly found in both LLD and AD on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans, particularly in subcortical structures and their frontal projections (17, 18). Studies using diffusion tensor imaging have identified microstructural abnormalities in white matter tracts connecting the prefrontal cortex (PFC) with subcortical and posterior cortical regions in LLD, which are associated with executive dysfunction (19, 20). The hippocampus, which is sensitive to altered cortisol, serotonin, and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels, is also affected by both depression and AD. Additionally, early accumulation of Aβ may lead to hippocampal atrophy, which implies that depression risk may be heightened by AD pathology through hippocampal atrophy (21, 22).

MRI is a radiation-free imaging technique that provides more information compared to computed tomography (CT) and is valuable in identifying hippocampal atrophy in the elderly (23). The Schelten’s scale, also called the medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA) score, is frequently used in clinical settings to assess the shrinkage of the hippocampus (24). The evaluation of MTA through MRI is commonly included in the routine assessment of individuals experiencing cognitive decline. Various (semi-) automated segmentation tools exist to quantify the volumes of the medial temporal lobes, but their accessibility and utilization differ among radiological departments (25). Additionally, the measurement method employed can introduce bias in the determination of absolute hippocampal volumes, as manual volumetry and different automated software programs tend to delineate the anatomical structures in distinct ways (26). In regards to clinical practicality, visual assessment of MTA is still superior to volumetric measuring methods. The utilization of Schelten’s scale has exhibited comparable precision in differentiating between healthy individuals and those afflicted with AD, as automated measurement methods (27, 28).

Studies using MRI and postmortem examinations indicate a link between reduced hippocampal size and depression (2931), but more research is necessary to determine the significance of hippocampal atrophy in distinguishing LLD from AD. In addition, the Trail Making Test (TMT) holds a prominent position as one of the most frequently employed neuropsychological assessments in clinical practice, particularly within memory clinics (32). Its extensive utilization stems from its historical application in examining impairments pertaining to cognitive processing speed and executive function in individuals (33). Notably, the TMT’s ease of administration and relatively brief duration render it highly suitable for implementation in clinical assessment environments. This study had two primary aims: Firstly, to examine how executive function and MTA differ between stable LLD patients and AD patients; and secondly, to assess how well the TMT and the MTA scale can distinguish between the two disorders.

Methods

Participants

A total of 50 patients diagnosed with LLD and 50 with AD were recruited between October 1, 2020, and February 25, 2021, from Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University. LLD patients who were right-handed, between the ages of 60 and 80, and had been taking the same dose of antidepressant medication for over 4 weeks were included. It is noteworthy that within the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5 (DSM5), hopeless, sadness, and emptiness serve as subjective markers for depressive mood (DSM5, p160) (34). Nevertheless, the precise specificity of “hopeless” as an indicator for major depressive episode remains uncertain in the absence of depressed mood. In clinical settings, this modification has the potential to broaden the diagnostic range of LLD (35), given the prevalence of withdrawal, apathy, and lack of vigor among elderly individuals. Actually, the majority of psychopathological schools posited that there existed a distinction between “hopeless” and “depressed mood” in terms of their phenomenological characteristics (36, 37). Unlike the DSM5, the DSM-IV utilizes subjective markers sadness, and emptiness to identify depression, while excluding marker hopeless. Furthermore, in terms of the agreement in evaluation among clinical practitioners, the DSM-IV demonstrates a higher level of consistency compared to the DSM5 (38). Hence, the DSM-IV criteria was employed in this study. Participants were excluded if they had other major psychiatric disorder, were currently or previously dependent on drugs or alcohol, had a neurological illness such as stroke, transient ischemic attacks, or dementia, were taking medication that prevented cognitive testing, had metal implants that prevented MRI scanning, or had undergone electroconvulsive therapy within the last 6 months. The diagnosis of AD was established using the criteria for clinically probable AD from the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Diseases and the Stroke-Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association, as well as the DSM-IV criteria for Alzheimer’s type dementia. The time span from the clinical diagnosis to enrollment in the study varied from 1 month to 6 years. The institutional review board of Xuanwu Hospital of Capital Medical University examined and sanctioned the study protocols, and all participants furnished written informed consent. In cases where patients are incapable of signing the informed consent form due to cognitive impairment, their guardians will act as their replacements.

Assessment of depressive symptoms and cognitive function

To evaluate the severity of depressive symptoms and cognitive impairment, we employed various rating scales, such as the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) for depression, the Mini-Mental Status Exam (MMSE), and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) for cognition. Tailored for the elderly, the GDS comprises 30 items and adopts a yes-no/agree-disagree response format, simplifying the symptom reporting process for seniors, particularly those with cognitive, physical, or sensory limitations (39). A score between 11 and 20 suggests mild depression, whereas a score of 21 to 30 implies moderate to severe depression. We used the MMSE and MoCA for screening for cognitive impairments. It has been reported that the MMSE is limited in its ability to detect early dementia (40). The MMSE score range for normal elderly individuals overlaps with that of most individuals who meet the clinical criteria of MCI (41). Therefore, we complemented it with the MoCA (42). The MoCA assessment is a 30-point evaluation that spans multiple cognitive areas and can be completed in approximately 10 min on a single page (43). A meta-analysis conducted previously demonstrated that the MoCA exam is a more suitable screening tool for identifying MCI in patients over the age of 60 than the MMSE (44). To evaluate executive function, we employed the TMT, which comprises two sub-tests, namely TMT-A and TMT-B (45). In TMT-A, the participant is instructed to connect circles containing numbers from 1 to 25 in ascending order as fast as they can, which are arranged randomly. To complete TMT-B, the participant must switch back and forth between letters and numbers while moving in a sequential manner. TMT-A measures visual scanning, graphomotor speed, and visuomotor processing speed, while TMT-B assesses working memory and inhibition control through its performance and derived scores (46).

Image acquisition and visual assessment of MTA

To obtain high-resolution 3D T1-weighted images of the brain of each participant, we utilized the Siemens 3 T scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) and performed a turbo field echo (3D TFE) scan. 182 axial slices were scanned with a repetition time (TR) of 9.6 ms, an echo time (TE) of 4.6 ms, a flip angle of 8 degrees, a slice thickness of 1.2 mm, and an in-plane voxel size of 0.98 × 0.98 × 1.2 mm3. The participants’ heads were secured with foam pads and straps to minimize motion. According to Scheltens’ original description (27), the MTA scale was utilized to rate 3D T1-weighted MR images. Specifically, the rating was conducted on the coronal slice that was presented by MRIcron solfware in the Nii format. During this assessment, both hemispheres were evaluated and assigned a score ranging from 0 to 4 based on the following criteria: 0 indicating no atrophy, 1 indicating widening of the choroid fissure, 2 indicating additional widening of the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle and a slight decrease in hippocampal height, 3 indicating moderate loss of hippocampal volume, and 4 indicating end-stage increase of all aforementioned findings (Figure 1). The MTA rating was conducted by two proficient neuro-radiologists, who provided ratings and feedback for a total of 100 data sets. In instances where there was disagreement between the two raters regarding the highest score in each scale for the two hemispheres, the raters engaged in collaborative discussions to achieve a final consensus. Due to the minimal discrepancy of one point observed in the majority of discordant scores, primarily resulting from evaluations conducted on neighboring slices by the raters, the final consensus was often readily achieved by selecting the slice exhibiting more pronounced atrophy.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA) grading criteria. (A) Coronal T1-weighted slices at the level of the hippocampus body of five different study participants with different levels of MTA, 0 no atrophy; 1 widening of the choroid fissure; 2 additional widening of the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle and slightly decreased hippocampal height; 3 moderate loss of hippocampal volume; 4 end-stage increase of all these findings. Those were presented by MRIcron software, with the same coordinate (x = 91, y = 100, z = 121). (B) MTA visual rating is performed on a single coronal slice including hippocampus, with particular attention to the width of the choroid fissure and of the temporal horn, and to the hippocampus height.

Statistical analysis

SPSS 26.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, United States) was used for statistical analysis. Regression analysis utilizes independent descriptive factors to forecast the outcome variable. The mean ± SD was used to express age, while frequencies and percentages were used to report categorical variables such as gender. Counts and percentages were used to express sex. Continuous variables were compared using an independent t-test, while categorical variables were compared using either a chi-square test or a Mann–Whitney U test. Statistically significant results were defined as having p-values less than 0.05. To assess the ability of TMT and MTA grading to distinguish between the two groups, we created a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. The correlations of the MTA grading with neuropsychologic deficits and depressive symptoms were assessed using Pearson or Spearman correlation analysis.

Results

Comparison of demographics, neuropsychology and MTA grading between the two groups

Table 1 displays the traits of the individuals involved. The mean ages of the patients with AD and LLD were 71.20 ± 6.38 and 70.04 ± 4.62, respectively. Both groups had similar age, gender, and educational levels. Notably, the two groups displayed noteworthy disparities in illness course, GDS, MMSE, MoCA, TMT-A, TMT-B, and MTA total, with a significance level of p < 0.05.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparison of demographics, neuropsychology and MTA grading between 50 AD and 50 LLD patients.

ROC curve analysis

To evaluate the ability of TMT test and MTA grading to distinguish between the two groups, we employed ROC curve analysis and other diagnostic techniques. Improved diagnostic accuracy is indicated by a greater value of the area under the curve (AUC). Figure 2A and Table 2 showed that the AUC values for MMSE, MoCA, and GDS tests were 0.948 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.906–0.990, sensitivity 0.800, specificity 0.960), 0.904 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.847–0.961, sensitivity 0.960, specificity 0.680), and 0.738 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.636–0.840, sensitivity 0.560, specificity 0.940), respectively. Figure 2B and Table 2 showed that TMT-A, TMT-B, and MTA grading had AUC values of 0.922 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.867–0.976, sensitivity 0.900, specificity 0.840), 0.942 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.888–0.997, sensitivity 0.900, specificity 0.920), and 0.787 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.698–0.876, sensitivity 0.760, specificity 0.760), respectively, indicating a contrast in their performance.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. ROC curve analysis of TMT and MTA grading in distinguishing AD group from LLD group. The utilization of ROC curve analysis was employed to assess the discriminatory capacity of the TMT test and MTA grading in distinguishing between the two groups. A higher value of the AUC signifies enhanced diagnostic accuracy. (A) AUC of MMSE (0.948), MoCA (0.904), and GDS (0.738) in distinguishing AD group from LLD group, respectively. (B) AUC of TMT-A (0.922), TMT-B (0.942), and MTA (0.787) in distinguishing AD group from LLD group, respectively. ROC, receiver operating characteristic; AUC, area under curve; TMT, Trail Making Test; MTA, medial temporal lobe atrophy; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale; AD, Alzheimer’s disease; LLD, late-life depression.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. ROC curve analysis of MMSE, MoCA, GDS, TMT-A, TMT-B, and MTA total score in distinguishing AD patients from LLD patients.

A subgroup analysis was performed to detect the ability of TMT-A and TMT-B to distinguish AD from LLD at various stages. In patients with AD (n = 50) and mild LLD (n = 30), TMT-A and TMT-B had AUC values of 0.935 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.880–0.990, sensitivity 0.900, specificity 0.867) and 0.961 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.921–1.000, sensitivity 0.940, specificity 0.900), respectively (Figure 3A). In patients with AD (n = 50) and moderate to severe LLD (n = 20), the AUC values for TMT-A and TMT-B were 0.902 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.827–0.977, sensitivity 0.860, specificity 0.850) and 0.914 (p < 0.001; 95% CI 0.813–1.000, sensitivity 0.900, specificity 0.95), respectively (Figure 3B and Table 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. ROC curve analysis of TMT-A and TMT-B in distinguishing AD group from LLD group at various stages. (A) AUC of TMT-A (0.935) and TMT-B (0.961) in distinguishing AD group (n = 50) from group with mild LLD (n = 30), respectively. (B) AUC of TMT-A (0.902) and TMT-B (0.914) in distinguishing AD group (n = 50) from group with moderate to severe LLD (n = 20), respectively.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. ROC curve analysis of TMT-A and TMT-B in distinguishing AD patients from LLD patients at various stages.

Correlation analysis

We examined the correlations between MTA score and MMSE, MoCA, TMT-A, and TMT-B in the AD group. Similarly, we also investigated the associations between MTA score and MMSE, MoCA, TMT-A, TMT-B, and GDS in the LLD group (Figures 4, 5). Among AD patients, there was an inverse relationship observed between the MTA score and both the MMSE score (r = −0.825, p < 0.001, n = 50, Figure 4A) and the MoCA score (r = −0.769, p < 0.001, n = 50, Figure 4B). A correlation was observed in the LLD group between the MTA score and TMT-A score (r = 0.351, p = 0.013, n = 50, Figure 5C) and GDS score (r = 0.561, p < 0.001, n = 50, Figure 5E), respectively.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlation analysis between MTA score and neuropsychologic deficits in patients with AD. (A) The correlation between MTA score with MMSE score; (B) The correlation between MTA score with MoCA score; (C) The correlation between MTA score with TMT-A time spent; (D) The correlation between MTA score with TMT-B time spent.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlation analysis between MTA score and neuropsychologic deficits and depressive symptoms in patients with LLD. (A) The correlation between MTA score with MMSE score; (B) The correlation between MTA score with MoCA score; (C) The correlation between MTA score with TMT-A time spent; (D) The correlation between MTA score with TMT-B time spent; (E) The correlation between MTA score with GDS score.

Discussion

The purpose of this research was to investigate how TMT and MTA can aid in distinguishing between AD and LLD. Our findings indicate that individuals with AD exhibit greater cognitive decline, particularly in executive function, and more pronounced MTA compared to those with LLD. We also compared the MMSE and MoCA, two commonly used screening tools for cognitive measurement, with the simpler TMT, and we found similar discriminatory power. In LLD patients, we noticed a positive association between MTA classification and TMT-A and GDS, but this was not observed in the AD group.

Numerous studies have demonstrated that MoCA is more effective than MMSE in recognizing MCI due to the absence of a ceiling effect, and both assessments are precise in detecting AD (4749). Our research also revealed that MMSE had a lesser ability to differentiate AD from LLD than MoCA (0.80% vs. 0.96%), despite its AUC being marginally greater than that of MoCA (94.8% vs. 90.4%). Studies that follow patients over time have shown that the TMT test can forecast changes in clinical and functional abilities for those with AD, LLD, and MCI (16, 50, 51). Nevertheless, there are limited studies that have employed the TMT to differentiate between AD and LLD. The A and B subtests both had an AUC of over 90%, indicating excellent performance by TMT. The clinical potential of TMT-B is excellent due to its sensitivity and specificity, which are above 90%.

As per a recommended meta-analysis, LLD could be an early sign of AD (52). In comparison to AD patients, individuals with LLD are usually less affected by their everyday activities. The TMT-B evaluates cognitive flexibility and working memory in elderly individuals, which are crucial for everyday activities such as dressing and cooking. Difficulty in daily living is often experienced by older individuals with inadequate TMT-B performance (53), indicating that TMT-B could be a promising approach to differentiate between LLD and AD. The impact of depression severity on the cognitive functioning of individuals with LLD, particularly during severe depressive episodes, is evidently significant. In order to investigate the distinction between AD and LLD at various stages, a subgroup analysis was performed, focusing on the TMT-A and TMT-B. Our findings indicated that regardless of whether LLD is mild or moderate to severe, TMT-B exhibited an AUC area, sensitivity, and specificity exceeding 90%, thereby reinforcing the validity of our finding. However, some participants did not finish TMT-B within the required time, and we used the maximum time allowed to complete the test in the statistical analysis following the operational manual, which might have introduced some bias.

AD is closely related to brain atrophy, particularly in the hippocampus or MTA. In the early stages of AD (54), neurofibrillary tangles emerge in the medial temporal lobe, beginning in the transentorhinal cortex and entorhinal cortex before spreading to the hippocampal subfields and eventually reaching isocortical areas in the late stages of the disease. Despite numerous studies investigating the hippocampus in LLD (55), few have examined its subfields and associated medial temporal lobe regions (56). Our study discovered that the severity of MTA was greater in AD patients than in LLD patients, as anticipated. According to a recent meta-analysis (57), MTA has the potential to be a promising biomarker for AD progression. Additionally, we observed a negative correlation between MTA and cognitive impairment in the AD group. Nonetheless, the MTA assessment did not perform as well as the TMT in distinguishing between the two groups of patients.

Depression-executive dysfunction syndrome is a term used to describe a unique clinical presentation and inadequate response to antidepressants that may be observed in a certain group of elderly individuals with depression (58). The condition is linked to deficiencies in multiple managerial abilities, including but not limited to speaking proficiency, restraining responses, devising innovative solutions, adapting to new situations, retaining information, and/or planning physical actions (59, 60). In this investigation, we observed a favorable association between MTA and the duration of TMT-A. In LLD research, the hippocampus has been thoroughly examined, and a meta-analysis of 35 studies found that patients with LLD had significantly smaller hippocampal volumes compared to those without the condition. In LLD patients, cognitive impairment, particularly in semantic retrieval and verbal memory, is strongly associated with hippocampal atrophy (61, 62).

Despite the absence of a statistically significant distinction in age and education between the two groups in this study, it became apparent that the mean age of individuals in the AD group exceeded that of the LLD group in numerical terms, while the years of education exhibited an inverse relationship. Taylor et al. discovered that, increasing age had a greater negative correlation with medial temporal volumes in the depressed group than in the nondepressed group (mean age = 66.4 years, SD = 5.8y, range 60-86y) (56). Moreover, Tang et al. conducted a study that demonstrated a positively association between education and size variations in the CA1 and subiculum subregions of the hippocampus in a sample of non-dementia elderly individuals (63). Prior research has indicated that education plays a crucial role in enhancing cognitive resilience, which could potentially influence the outcomes of cognitive assessments, such as the MoCA and the MMSE (64, 65). Consequently, when differentiating between LLD and AD in clinical settings, it is imperative to thoroughly consider the potential confounding effects of age and education.

In addition to neurocognition and MRI, a limited number of studies have explored alternative biomarkers, including electroencephalography (EEG) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) measurement. A previous investigation demonstrated that the utilization of EEG with quantitative analysis method can effectively differentiate between patients with LLD and those with AD, exhibiting a sensitivity and specificity of approximately 92% (66). Furthermore, Liguori et al. have substantiated the efficacy of CSF measurement, as they have observed elevated levels of CSF tau proteins and diminished concentrations of CSF Aβ42 in individuals with AD compared to both those with LLD and control subjects (67). These findings can be utilized to discriminate between the two conditions. As a whole, the existing tools or approaches suggested for early differentiation between LLD and AD remain arduous and lack unanimity.

The MTA score showed a positive correlation with GDS in the LLD group but not in the AD group. MTA may indicate neuroendocrine alterations in the HPA axis, which plays a role in managing stress and mood in patients with LLD. Stress usually triggers the activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, causing the hypothalamus to release corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF), which then stimulates the pituitary gland to secrete adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) (68). ACTH then triggers the release of glucocorticoids (GCs) from the adrenal glands into the bloodstream. Physiological adaptations in response to stress are made possible by GCs, as well as epinephrine and norepinephrine from the sympathetic nervous system (69). However, in both AD and LLD patients, the HPA axis is dysregulated, leading to impaired negative feedback and increased GCs levels in the brain (70, 71). This results in reduced volumes of the hippocampal and PFC cues (72), as well as increased vulnerability of neurons to free oxygen radicals and Aβ, which are common features of both AD and LLD (73). Mood disorders and cognitive impairment, particularly EFD, have a strong correlation with these modifications.

It is important to recognize some constraints in this research. Due to the constraints of clinical diagnosis and the absence of follow-up information, it was impossible to exclude the likelihood that certain LLD patients were preclinical AD cases with depressive symptoms prior to the onset of cognitive impairments. Additionally, our findings may be limited in their generalizability and causal inference due to the relatively small sample size and cross-sectional design. Thirdly, manual evaluation of MTA was utilized, which may not be practical in typical clinical environments. Fourth, the absence of AD staging hindered the possibility of conducting subgroup analysis to delve deeper into the correlation between MTA and different stages of AD. Fifth, we did not count for potential confounders such as antidepressant use or vascular alterations in the brain that could directly or indirectly affect hippocampus volume. Sixth, the omission of AD pathological biomarkers, such as CSF tau/Aβ or amyloid-PET, which are currently integral to the in vivo diagnosis of AD, is a noteworthy aspect that warrants attention in future research. Finally, we only measured the time spent on the executive functional tests (TMT-A or TMT-B), and we did not include other indicators such as error rate pen lift reminders.

Conclusion

To summarize, our study of patients with LLD or AD using standard cognitive assessment tools and tests for executive function revealed that AD patients had more severe EFD and MTA compared to those with LLD. Moreover, TMT exhibited encouraging capability for clinical application in differentiating AD from LLD. It is important to acknowledge that TMT is capable of capturing only a fraction of the executive function, thus necessitating a cautious interpretation of research findings.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by The institutional review board of Xuanwu Hospital of Capital Medical University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

The research protocol was conceptualized and designed by CC, while PM and WP were responsible for conducting the data analyses. CY provided assistance with neuropsychological assessment and data processing, while YR and CY verified the MRI data. The manuscript underwent revision and language refinement by CL and Y-lT. All authors made contributions to the article and gave their approval for the submitted version.

Funding

The present work was supported by the National Key Research and Development Program of China (No. 2022YFC3602605) and Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission (No. Z191100006619105).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Li, H, Lin, X, Liu, L, Su, S, Zhu, X, Zheng, Y, et al. Disruption of the structural and functional connectivity of the frontoparietal network underlies symptomatic anxiety in late-life depression. Neuroimage Clin. (2020) 28:102398. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Sjöberg, L, Karlsson, B, Atti, AR, Skoog, I, Fratiglioni, L, and Wang, HX. Prevalence of depression: comparisons of different depression definitions in population-based samples of older adults. J Affect Disord. (2017) 221:123–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.06.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Manning, K, Wang, L, and Steffens, D. Recent advances in the use of imaging in psychiatry: functional magnetic resonance imaging of large-scale brain networks in late-life depression. F1000Res. (2019) 8:1366. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.17399.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Frozza, RL, Lourenco, MV, and De Felice, FG. Challenges for Alzheimer’s disease therapy: insights from novel mechanisms beyond memory defects. Front Neurosci. (2018) 12:37. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Breijyeh, Z, and Karaman, R. Comprehensive review on Alzheimer’s disease: causes and treatment. Molecules. (2020) 25:5789. doi: 10.3390/molecules25245789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Orgeta, V, Tabet, N, Nilforooshan, R, and Howard, R. Efficacy of antidepressants for depression in Alzheimer’s disease: systematic review and Meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis. (2017) 58:725–33. doi: 10.3233/JAD-161247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Invernizzi, S, Simoes Loureiro, I, Kandana Arachchige, KG, and Lefebvre, L. Late-life depression, cognitive impairment, and relationship with Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2021) 50:414–24. doi: 10.1159/000519453

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Youn, H, Hyung, WSW, Kim, J, Lee, ES, Eo, JS, Han, CE, et al. Brain amyloid accumulation possibly exacerbates concurrent mild cognitive impairment with subthreshold depression in older adults: A 1-year follow-up study. J Affect Disord. (2021) 295:93–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.08.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Emsell, L, Vanhaute, H, Vansteelandt, K, De Winter, FL, Christiaens, D, Van den Stock, J, et al. An optimized MRI and PET based clinical protocol for improving the differential diagnosis of geriatric depression and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2022) 320:111443. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2022.111443

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Downing, LJ, Caprio, TV, and Lyness, JM. Geriatric psychiatry review: differential diagnosis and treatment of the 3 D’s - delirium, dementia, and depression. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2013) 15:365. doi: 10.1007/s11920-013-0365-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Soennesyn, H, Oppedal, K, Greve, OJ, Fritze, F, Auestad, BH, Nore, SP, et al. White matter hyperintensities and the course of depressive symptoms in elderly people with mild dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. (2012) 2:97–111. doi: 10.1159/000335497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gasser, AI, Salamin, V, and Zumbach, S. Late life depression or prodromal Alzheimer’s disease: which tools for the differential diagnosis? Encéphale. (2018) 44:52–8. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2017.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ismail, Z, Smith, EE, Geda, Y, Sultzer, D, Brodaty, H, Smith, G, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms as early manifestations of emergent dementia: provisional diagnostic criteria for mild behavioral impairment. Alzheimers Dement. (2016) 12:195–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Semkovska, M, Quinlivan, L, O’Grady, T, Johnson, R, Collins, A, O’Connor, J, et al. Cognitive function following a major depressive episode: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. (2019) 6:851–61. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30291-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Pimontel, MA, Rindskopf, D, Rutherford, BR, Brown, PJ, Roose, SP, and Sneed, JR. A Meta-analysis of executive dysfunction and antidepressant treatment response in late-life depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2016) 24:31–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2015.05.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Potter, GG, Wagner, HR, Burke, JR, Plassman, BL, Welsh-Bohmer, KA, and Steffens, DC. Neuropsychological predictors of dementia in late-life major depressive disorder. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2013) 21:297–306. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2012.12.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Taylor, WD, MacFall, JR, Steffens, DC, Payne, ME, Provenzale, JM, and Krishnan, KRR. Localization of age-associated white matter hyperintensities in late-life depression. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2003) 27:539–44. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(02)00358-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Lam, S, Lipton, RB, Harvey, DJ, Zammit, AR, and Ezzati, A. White matter hyperintensities and cognition across different Alzheimer’s biomarker profiles. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2021) 69:1906–15. doi: 10.1111/jgs.17173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Alexopoulos, GS, Murphy, CF, Gunning-Dixon, FM, Glatt, CE, Latoussakis, V, Kelly, RE, et al. Serotonin transporter polymorphisms, microstructural white matter abnormalities and remission of geriatric depression. J Affect Disord. (2009) 119:132–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bae, JN, MacFall, JR, Krishnan, KRR, Payne, ME, Steffens, DC, and Taylor, WD. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex white matter alterations in late-life depression. Biol Psychiatry. (2006) 60:1356–63. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Kim, JJ, and Diamond, DM. The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2002) 3:453–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn849

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Enache, D, Cavallin, L, Lindberg, O, Farahmand, B, Kramberger, MG, Westman, E, et al. Medial temporal lobe atrophy and depressive symptoms in elderly patients with and without Alzheimer disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. (2015) 28:40–8. doi: 10.1177/0891988714541873

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Park, BY, Larivière, S, Rodríguez-Cruces, R, Royer, J, Tavakol, S, Wang, Y, et al. Topographic divergence of atypical cortical asymmetry and atrophy patterns in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain. (2022) 145:1285–98. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Teipel, S, Drzezga, A, Grothe, MJ, Barthel, H, Chételat, G, Schuff, N, et al. Multimodal imaging in Alzheimer’s disease: validity and usefulness for early detection. Lancet Neurol. (2015) 14:1037–53. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00093-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Molinder, A, Ziegelitz, D, Maier, SE, and Eckerström, C. Validity and reliability of the medial temporal lobe atrophy scale in a memory clinic population. BMC Neurol. (2021) 21:289. doi: 10.1186/s12883-021-02325-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Brinkmann, BH, Guragain, H, Kenney-Jung, D, Mandrekar, J, Watson, RE, Welker, KM, et al. Segmentation errors and intertest reliability in automated and manually traced hippocampal volumes. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. (2019) 6:1807–14. doi: 10.1002/acn3.50885

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Scheltens, P, Leys, D, Barkhof, F, Huglo, D, Weinstein, HC, Vermersch, P, et al. Atrophy of medial temporal lobes on MRI in “probable” Alzheimer’s disease and normal ageing: diagnostic value and neuropsychological correlates. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1992) 55:967–72. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.10.967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Harper, L, Fumagalli, GG, Barkhof, F, Scheltens, P, O’Brien, JT, Bouwman, F, et al. MRI visual rating scales in the diagnosis of dementia: evaluation in 184 post-mortem confirmed cases. Brain. (2016) 139:1211–25. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Steffens, DC, McQuoid, DR, Payne, ME, and Potter, GG. Change in hippocampal volume on magnetic resonance imaging and cognitive decline among older depressed and nondepressed subjects in the neurocognitive outcomes of depression in the elderly study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2011) 19:4–12. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181d6c245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Geerlings, MI, and Gerritsen, L. Late-life depression, hippocampal volumes, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal Axis regulation: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 82:339–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.12.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ballmaier, M, Narr, KL, Toga, AW, Elderkin-Thompson, V, Thompson, PM, Hamilton, L, et al. Hippocampal morphology and distinguishing late-onset from early-onset elderly depression. Am J Psychiatry. (2008) 165:229–37. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Ashendorf, L, Jefferson, AL, O’Connor, MK, Chaisson, C, Green, RC, and Stern, RA. Trail making test errors in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2008) 23:129–37. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Sánchez-Cubillo, I, Periáñez, JA, Adrover-Roig, D, Rodríguez-Sánchez, JM, Ríos-Lago, M, and Tirapu, J. Construct validity of the trail making test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2009) 15:438–50. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709090626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013).

Google Scholar

35. Sachdev, PS, Mohan, A, Taylor, L, and Jeste, DV. DSM-5 and mental disorders in older individuals: an overview. Harv Rev Psychiatry. (2015) 23:320–8. doi: 10.1097/hrp.0000000000000090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Beck, JS, and Newman, CF. Hopelessness, depression, suicidal ideation, and clinical diagnosis of depression. Suicide Life Threat Behav. (1993) 23:139–45.

Google Scholar

37. Greene, SM. The relationship between depression and hopelessness. Implications for current theories of depression. Br J Psychiatry. (1989) 154:650–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.154.5.650

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Uher, R, Payne, JL, Pavlova, B, and Perlis, RH. Major depressive disorder in DSM-5: implications for clinical practice and research of changes from DSM-IV. Depress Anxiety. (2014) 31:459–71. doi: 10.1002/da.22217

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Byrne, GJ, Pachana, NA, Goncalves, DC, Arnold, E, King, R, and Keat Khoo, S. Psychometric properties and health correlates of the geriatric anxiety inventory in Australian community-residing older women. Aging Ment Health. (2010) 14:247–54. doi: 10.1080/13607861003587628

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Creavin, ST, Wisniewski, S, Noel-Storr, AH, Trevelyan, CM, Hampton, T, Rayment, D, et al. Mini-mental state examination (MMSE) for the detection of dementia in clinically unevaluated people aged 65 and over in community and primary care populations. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2016) 2016:Cd011145. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011145.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Carlew, AR, Smith, EE, Goette, W, Lippe, B, Lacritz, L, and Rossetti, H. Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) scores in medically compromised patients: a scoping review. Health Psychol. (2021) 40:717–26. doi: 10.1037/hea0001138

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Carson, N, Leach, L, and Murphy, KJ. A re-examination of Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) cutoff scores. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2018) 33:379–88. doi: 10.1002/gps.4756

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Nasreddine, ZS, Phillips, NA, Bédirian, V, Charbonneau, S, Whitehead, V, Collin, I, et al. The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2005) 53:695–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ciesielska, N, Sokołowski, R, Mazur, E, Podhorecka, M, Polak-Szabela, A, and Kędziora-Kornatowska, K. Is the Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) test better suited than the Mini-mental state examination (MMSE) in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) detection among people aged over 60? Meta-analysis Psychiatr Pol. (2016) 50:1039–52. doi: 10.12740/PP/45368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Terada, S, Sato, S, Nagao, S, Ikeda, C, Shindo, A, Hayashi, S, et al. Trail making test B and brain perfusion imaging in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res. (2013) 213:249–55. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Llinàs-Reglà, J, Vilalta-Franch, J, López-Pousa, S, Calvó-Perxas, L, Rodas, DT, and Garre-Olmo, J. The trail making test. Assessment. (2017) 24:183–96. doi: 10.1177/1073191115602552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Jia, X, Wang, Z, Huang, F, Su, C, Du, W, Jiang, H, et al. A comparison of the Mini-mental state examination (MMSE) with the Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) for mild cognitive impairment screening in Chinese middle-aged and older population: a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry. (2021) 21:485. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03495-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Dong, Y, Lee, WY, Basri, NA, Collinson, SL, Merchant, RA, Venketasubramanian, N, et al. The Montreal cognitive assessment is superior to the Mini-mental state examination in detecting patients at higher risk of dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. (2012) 24:1749–55. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212001068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Breton, A, Casey, D, and Arnaoutoglou, NA. Cognitive tests for the detection of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), the prodromal stage of dementia: Meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2019) 34:233–42. doi: 10.1002/gps.5016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Chen, P, Ratcliff, G, Belle, SH, Cauley, JA, DeKosky, ST, and Ganguli, M. Patterns of cognitive decline in presymptomatic Alzheimer disease: a prospective community study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2001) 58:853–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.9.853

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Chapman, RM, Mapstone, M, McCrary, JW, Gardner, MN, Porsteinsson, A, Sandoval, TC, et al. Predicting conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease using neuropsychological tests and multivariate methods. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2011) 33:187–99. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2010.499356

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Stella, F, Radanovic, M, Balthazar, ML, Canineu, PR, de Souza, LC, and Forlenza, OV. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in the prodromal stages of dementia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2014) 27:230–5. doi: 10.1097/yco.0000000000000050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Bell‐McGinty, S, Podell, K, Franzen, M, Baird, AD, and Williams, MJ. Standard measures of executive function in predicting instrumental activities of daily living in older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2002) 17:828–34. doi: 10.1002/gps.646

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Jarholm, JA, Bjørnerud, A, Dalaker, TO, Akhavi, MS, Kirsebom, BE, Pålhaugen, L, et al. Medial temporal lobe atrophy in Predementia Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal multi-site study comparing staging and A/T/N in a clinical research cohort. J Alzheimers Dis. (2023) 94:259–79. doi: 10.3233/JAD-221274

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Braak, H, and Braak, E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. (1991) 82:239–59. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Taylor, WD, Deng, Y, Boyd, BD, Donahue, MJ, Albert, K, McHugo, M, et al. Medial temporal lobe volumes in late-life depression: effects of age and vascular risk factors. Brain Imaging Behav. (2020) 14:19–29. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9969-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Li, RX, Ma, YH, Tan, L, and Yu, JT. Prospective biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. (2022) 81:101699. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2022.101699

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Alexopoulos, GS. Mechanisms and treatment of late-life depression. Transl Psychiatry. (2019) 9:188. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0514-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Manning, KJ, and Steffens, DC. State of the science of neural Systems in Late-Life Depression: impact on clinical presentation and treatment outcome. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2018) 66:S17–23. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. McLennan, SN, and Mathias, JL. The depression-executive dysfunction (DED) syndrome and response to antidepressants: a meta-analytic review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2010) 25:933–44. doi: 10.1002/gps.2431

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Sawyer, K, Corsentino, E, Sachs-Ericsson, N, and Steffens, DC. Depression, hippocampal volume changes, and cognitive decline in a clinical sample of older depressed outpatients and non-depressed controls. Aging Ment Health. (2012) 16:753–62. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2012.678478

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Lim, HK, Jung, WS, Ahn, KJ, Won, WY, Hahn, C, Lee, SY, et al. Regional cortical thickness and subcortical volume changes are associated with cognitive impairments in the drug-naive patients with late-onset depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2012) 37:838–49. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Tang, X, Varma, VR, Miller, MI, and Carlson, MC. Education is associated with sub-regions of the hippocampus and the amygdala vulnerable to neuropathologies of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Struct Funct. (2017) 222:1469–79. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1287-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Garibotto, V, Borroni, B, Sorbi, S, Cappa, SF, Padovani, A, and Perani, D. Education and occupation provide reserve in both ApoE ε4 carrier and noncarrier patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Neurol Sci. (2012) 33:1037–42. doi: 10.1007/s10072-011-0889-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Sattler, C, Toro, P, Schönknecht, P, and Schröder, J. Cognitive activity, education and socioeconomic status as preventive factors for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res. (2012) 196:90–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Deslandes, A, Veiga, H, Cagy, M, Fiszman, A, Piedade, R, and Ribeiro, P. Quantitative electroencephalography (qEEG) to discriminate primary degenerative dementia from major depressive disorder (depression). Arq Neuropsiquiatr. (2004) 62:44–50. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2004000100008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Liguori, C, Pierantozzi, M, Chiaravalloti, A, Sancesario, GM, Mercuri, NB, Franchini, F, et al. When cognitive decline and depression coexist in the elderly: CSF biomarkers analysis can differentiate Alzheimer’s disease from late-life depression. Front Aging Neurosci. (2018) 10:38. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. McCann, SM, Antunes-Rodrigues, J, Franci, CR, Anselmo-Franci, JA, Karanth, S, and Rettori, V. Role of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis in the control of the response to stress and infection. Braz J Med Biol Res. (2000) 33:1121–31. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2000001000001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Leszek, J, Trypka, E, Koutsouraki, E, Michmizos, D, Yarla, NS, Tarasov, VV, et al. Late-life depression and Alzheimer disease: A potential synergy of the underlying mechanisms. Curr Med Chem. (2018) 25:5389–94. doi: 10.2174/0929867323666160902152829

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Milligan Armstrong, A, Porter, T, Quek, H, White, A, Haynes, J, Jackaman, C, et al. Chronic stress and Alzheimer’s disease: the interplay between the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, genetics and microglia. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. (2021) 96:2209–28. doi: 10.1111/brv.12750

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Kang, HJ, Bae, KY, Kim, SW, Shin, IS, Kim, HR, Shin, MG, et al. Longitudinal associations between glucocorticoid receptor methylation and late-life depression. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2018) 84:56–62. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Caraci, F, Copani, A, Nicoletti, F, and Drago, F. Depression and Alzheimer’s disease: neurobiological links and common pharmacological targets. Eur J Pharmacol. (2010) 626:64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.10.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Abrahám, I, Harkany, T, Horvath, KM, Veenema, AH, Penke, B, Nyakas, C, et al. Chronic corticosterone administration dose-dependently modulates Abeta(1-42)- and NMDA-induced neurodegeneration in rat magnocellular nucleus basalis. J Neuroendocrinol. (2000) 12:486–94. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2000.00475.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: executive function deficits, medial temporal lobe atrophy, Alzheimer’s disease, late-life depression, receiver operating characteristic

Citation: Chu C, Pan W, Ren Y, Mao P, Yang C, Liu C and Tang Y-l (2023) Executive function deficits and medial temporal lobe atrophy in late-life depression and Alzheimer’s disease: a comparative study. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1243894. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1243894

Received: 21 June 2023; Accepted: 22 August 2023;
Published: 31 August 2023.

Edited by:

Gilberto Sousa Alves, Federal University of Maranhão, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Jose Wagner Leonel Tavares, Federal University of Ceara, Brazil
Mari Nilva Maia Da Silva, University College London, United Kingdom
Ahmad Adam Khundakar, Teesside University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2023 Chu, Pan, Ren, Mao, Yang, Liu and Tang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Peixian Mao, bWFvcGVpeGlhbkBtYWlsLmNjbXUuZWR1LmNu; Chunlin Yang, eWFuZ2NodW5saW4zNjlAMTI2LmNvbQ==; Chaomeng Liu, MTEyMDE5MDAwMTg5QGNjbXUuZWR1LmNu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.