Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 06 April 2023
Sec. Autism

Examine sex differences in autism spectrum disorder in school-aged children and adolescents with fluent language

Yan Ji&#x;Yan JiYue Ji&#x;Yue JiHui-lin ZhuHui-lin ZhuSan-mei ChengSan-mei ChengXiao-bing Zou
&#x;Xiao-bing Zou*Feng-lei Zhu
&#x;Feng-lei Zhu*
  • Child Developmental and Behavioral Center, The Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China

There are noteworthy sex disparities in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders (ASD), while findings regarding the sex differences in core symptoms are inconsistent. There are few relevant studies on sex differences in mainland China. This study was dedicated to a deeper understanding of the impact of sex differences on the clinical presentation of ASD with fluent language. We retrospectively studied 301 children with ASD (58 females) and utilized raw scores from the ADI-R and ADOS and the intelligence quotient (IQ) to measure symptomatology. Based on the Full-Scale IQ (FS-IQ), a binary split of average, above-average IQ (high-IQ), and below-average IQ (low IQ) occurs at 85. Across the entire sample, males and females are comparable in the FS-IQ, while males scored higher in the Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI) (F = 7.812, p = 0.006). ADI-R did not find any statistically significant sex differences in the diagnostic cutoff score satisfaction or the raw domain scores. While a significant effect of sex on ADOS social affect domain scores was found in the total sample [λ = 0.970, partial η2 = 0.030, F (3,295) = 3.019, p = 0.030]. Tests of between-subjects effects revealed that males scored higher than females mainly in the ADOS reciprocal social interaction subcategory (partial η2 = 0.022, F = 6.563, p = 0.011). Stratified analysis revealed that the effect of sex on ADOS reciprocal social interaction subcategory scores only significant in the low-IQ children with ASD (partial η2 = 0.092, F = 10.088, p = 0.002). In general, overall cognitive functioning is similar across males and females with ASD, while males have a higher perceptual reasoning ability. Females with ASD are more likely to have comorbid intellectual impairment than males, and they could require additional intervention support. Autistic children with low IQs are more likely to exhibit sex differences in their core symptoms than children with high IQs. Intelligence plays a key role in sex-based differences in the core symptoms of ASD.

1. Introduction

The predominant features of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental disorder that appears in the early stages, are persistent challenges with social interaction and communication, as well as restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRB), interests, and activities (1). Although the exact cause of ASD is still unknown, it is widely acknowledged that gene–environment interactions play a role in its development. The prevalence of ASD has considerably increased in recent years. The latest estimations of the prevalence of ASD among children aged 8 in the United States showed that it was one in 44, with a male-to-female sex ratio of 4.2:1, and the prevalence of ASD in both males and females is substantially affected by their cognitive performance (2). Individualized care and treatment for children with ASD are increasingly advocated by the international community since they impose a substantial burden on the affected people and their families (3).

Gender, or sex, is a critical concept in autism research and clinical practice. Gender is a socio-cultural construct, whereas sex is a biological or physiological attribute (4). This study used the term “sex” to define how males and females differ from one another. The child’s development history, symptom observation, and clinical confirmation are the rudimentary building blocks of the ASD diagnostic process. For a precise diagnosis and a personalized intervention, it is essential to consider the sex differences in the clinical presentation of children with ASD. As the gold standard for autism diagnosis, the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) (5) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) (6) (both abbreviated as “Double A”) were both used to evaluate the severity of autism symptoms. It is challenging for research workers to compare the symptom severity of children with ASD who belong to different age groups using cross-module raw scores due to obstacles that have been put in place to account for the developmental trajectories of age as well as the varied testing tasks in each module of the ADOS. The calibrated severity score (CSS), a standardized 10-point severity metric that delivers a quantitative assessment of symptom severity, was developed as a solution to this problem (7). Perhaps, it would prompt concerns about whether CSS will somehow weaken the ability of this diagnostic tool to distinguish sex differences in ASD symptoms.

It is well recognized that there are significant sex disparities in the prevalence of ASD, while the sex differences at the core phenotypic manifestations of ASD are inconsistent. A large European multi-site sample study employing gold-standard diagnostic methods found that females and males with ASD experienced comparable levels of social interaction and communication difficulties, while early childhood restricted and repetitive behaviors were less prevalent in females than in males (8). Not only that, but a recent meta-analysis that included 11 original studies also found no evidence of sex differences in social and communication function in either ASD or ADHD children (9). Additionally, it has been demonstrated that boys and girls did not differ in terms of scores on commonly used screening instruments (Modified-Checklist for Autism in Toddlers-Revised with Follow-up and Social Communication Questionnaire) (10). Since the sex difference among ASD children is not adequately captured by standard measures, there are divergent opinions on this aspect. Both the disproportionately biased “male lens” and lack of knowledge about the ASD symptoms unique to females hampered eventual referral and diagnosis (11, 12). Compared to males, females received ASD diagnoses later (13). Female individuals with ASD had significantly better social interaction and social communication skills, according to the comprehensive conclusions of a systematic review and meta-analysis of sex/gender differences, while a similar sex/gender profile has been found in non-autistic people (14). Following a previous review, autistic females may display female-gender-typical narrow interests, higher social attention, linguistic abilities, motivation for friendship, and more camouflage than autistic males (15). In addition, severity trajectory studies also establish sex differences at varying levels. Symptom severity decreases were more common during early childhood, and girls experienced greater symptom severity decreases and fewer symptom severity increases than boys (16, 17). The variability between investigations may have been partially explained by the heterogeneity of the measures or tools utilized. Additionally, studies have established that cultural issues and an individual’s age play a significant role in sex differences, which can lead to erratic and inconsistent results (10, 14).

Sex differences in ASD have gained growing research attention, yet there is a paucity of studies exploring sex differences in low-resource settings, although the majority of autistic people reside in low- and middle-income countries rather than high-income ones (18). Studies comparing male and female ASD patients had some limitations, such as small female sample sizes, a wide age range, the use of cross-version testing tools, and some studies did not consider participants’ cognitive and developmental abilities, as well as language proficiency (19–21). When it comes to measuring cognitive ability, results may vary depending on the participant’s age and the type of cognitive test employed (22, 23). Only a few studies in mainland China evaluated sexual differences in the clinical profiles of children with ASD. The clinical phenotypes of preschool children with ASD differ between boys and girls, according to research conducted previously by colleagues at our center. They discovered that girls with ASD exhibit more socio-emotional reciprocity and fewer RRBs than boys, and the types of RRBs that girls exhibit are different from those of boys (24). It has been proposed, however, that person-environment fit is a key determinant of functioning for autistic people in general, where environmental factors such as excessive social communication demands or bullying will increase disability, whereas a tolerant social environment or a suitable workplace will instead facilitate abilities (25). Due to this, findings may be inconsistent between preschool-age and school-age children. To better understand the impact of sex differences on the core symptoms of ASD, a single-center retrospective case–control study was designed to minimize the confounding effects on the outcome. In this study, the core symptoms of school-aged children and adolescents with ASD were evaluated for sex differences utilizing raw scores from the ADOS and ADI-R assessments.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants

We retrospectively studied 301 children with ASD, investigating their core symptoms and looking for any sex differences. All the participants were diagnosed with ASD at the Child Developmental and Behavioral Center in the Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University, from January 2019 to June 2022. Participants, which consist of 243 male and 58 female patients, were selected based on their age and language ability. In essence, the sample selected for this study is a convenience sample. We collected all eligible cases diagnosed at our center in recent years.

2.2. Inclusion and exclusion

2.2.1. Inclusion criteria

A. The chronological age ranged from 6 to 16 years old.

B. Had the first diagnosis of ASD according to the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (1).

C. Verbally fluent: children who can be spontaneous and flexibly use sentences with multiple clauses that describe logical connections between sentences, and can talk about objects or events not immediately present (6).

D. The ADI-R, WISC-IV, and ADOS (Module 3) assessments were all completed at the same time.

E. Parents signed an “informed consent” at the time of the hospital visit, giving their authorization for the use of their child’s assessment data for scientific analysis.

2.2.2. Exclusion criteria

Schizophrenia, co-occurring visual impairment, and hearing impairment were excluded.

2.3. Measures

A battery of tests, including the gold-standard ASD diagnostic assessments (ADI-R and ADOS) and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (Chinese Version) (WISC-IV) (26), were to be leveraged to measure symptomatology.

2.3.1. The autism diagnostic observation schedule

The ADOS consists of four modules that are appropriate to different levels of expressive language and is a semi-structured, standardized evaluation of social interaction, communication, and restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRB) for people who are suspected to have ASD (6). Module 3 contains 13 tasks and 28 rating items, but not all ratings are used as the algorithm items of the diagnostic reference score. This module requires that participants have fluent verbal skills and the ability to talk about things that are not immediately present. Each rating item was graded from severe to moderate to typical, with severe being scored as 2, indicating “definite evidence of autism,” moderate being rated as 1, suggesting “partially but not entirely evidence of autism,” and typical being rated as 0, denoting “no evidence of abnormality related to autism.” These tasks and rating items constitute three ADOS subcategories: the communication subcategory (domain A, with a diagnostic cutoff of 3 points for autism and 2 points for ASD), the reciprocal social interaction subcategory (domain B, with a diagnostic cutoff score of 6 points for autism and 4 points for ASD), and the restricted and repetitive behavior subcategory (domain C, without the cutoff score). Domain A and Domain B constitute the social affect domain (domain A and B), and the diagnostic cutoff score is 10 points for autism and 7 points for ASD. The subject’s scores on the three ADOS domains all need to be greater than or equal to the relevant cutoff scores for autism/ASD to be diagnosed. For statistical analysis, the raw scores of the full scale and each subdomain and their relation to the cutoff scores were evaluated. The full-scale score of ADOS is the result of adding up the raw scores of each domain. The diagnostic compliance rate represents the percentage of individuals in the group whose domain scores satisfy the cutoff scores of the autism or ASD diagnostic criteria.

2.3.2. The autism diagnostic interview-revised

ADI-R is a semi-structured, investigator-based interview for caregivers of children suspected of having a diagnosis of autism or pervasive developmental disorders, appropriate for children of mental ages ranging from approximately 18 months into adulthood (5). The primary clinical interview questions are divided into five sections: the opening questions, the communication questions, the social development and play questions, the repetitive and restricted behavior questions, and the general behavior problems. The caregivers’ responses are then encoded using two predominant scoring algorithms. Based on the coding methods, a score of 0 indicates “no definite behavior of the type specified,” a score of 1 represents “behavior of the type specified probably present but defining criteria not fully met,” a score of 2 indicates “definite abnormal behavior of the type described in the definition,” and a score of 3 was used to describe the extreme severity, where the highest scores indicate the highest degree of deficiency. Moreover, severity scores of 2 and 3 are both coded as 2. Under the algorithm for diagnosis, 16 items were established to measure reciprocal social interaction (domain B′), 13 items to measure communication (domain A′), and 8 items to measure RRB (domain C′). The diagnostic cutoff score for verbal subjects is 8 points in domain A′, 10 points in domain B′, and 3 points in domain C′. To be diagnosed with autism, a child must meet the diagnostic cutoff score in each of the three domains and exhibit specific aberrant behaviors that were present at or before the age of 36 months (domain D′), as reported by the caregiver or determined by the interviewer. Raw scores of the full scale or each domain and their relation to the diagnostic cutoff scores were assessed for statistical analysis. The full-scale score of ADI-R is the result of adding up the raw scores of each domain.

2.3.3. The Wechsler intelligence scale for children-fourth edition-Chinese version

The Chinese version of the WISC-IV, which was revised by H. C. Zhang and his team in 2007, has the same functional properties as the original version of the WISC-IV and is good for clinical application (26, 27). The WISC-IV will output four sub-scale indices: the Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), the Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI), the Working Memory Index (WMI), and the Processing Speed Index (PSI), as well as a full-scale IQ (FS-IQ). All individuals were measured with the Chinese version of the WISC-IV, and their FS-IQ would be used as a basis for stratifying. Children’s capability on some scales may be too low to reliably measure, hence the FS-IQ is an estimate due to “the floor effects” in intelligence tests. Average and above-average IQs (≥85) were defined as high IQs, while below-average IQs (<85) were defined as low IQs. We IQ-stratified our analysis by separately examining the two groups. Two medical staff members reviewed the research dataset after it was extracted from the hospital’s electronic case management system.

2.4. Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS statistical software (version 20.0). The age difference between males and females was tested with the t-test. The difference in domain scores for satisfying cutoff scores of the autism or ASD diagnostic criteria was tested with the Chi-square test. IQ, ADI-R domain scores, and ADOS scores in the domain of social affect obeyed or nearly obeyed the normal distribution and satisfied the homogeneity of variance in Levene’s test. Sex differences were tested using multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), with sex as the fixed factor and age and FS-IQ as covariates. ADOS RRB scores were skewed, and non-parametric tests (Mann–Whitney U tests) were applied to compare groups. At p < 0.05, the difference was statistically significant.

3. Results

3.1. Individual general information and sex differences in IQ

The sample as a whole consisted of 301 participants, with 58 females, giving the male/female sex ratio of 4.19:1. In the total sample, the average age was 114.23 (30.85) months, and the average FS-IQ was 92.17 (19.24). The age difference between males and females was not statistically significant (t = −0.997, p = 0.320). Multivariate tests revealed a significant effect of sex on IQ [λ = 0.941, partial η2 = 0.059, F (5,295) = 3.713, p = 0.003]. Tests of between-subjects effects revealed that males and females were roughly comparable in FS-IQ, VCI, WMI, and PSI (Table 1). While males scored higher than females in the PRI (F = 7.812, p = 0.006).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Test of the between-subjects effect of sex on the intelligence of children with ASD.

3.2. Individual features of children with ASD in different IQ groups

There were 104 children with ASD (female = 24) in the group of low IQs and 197 (female = 34) in the high-IQ group (Table 2). Children in the high-IQ group were older than those in the group of low IQs (t = 2.160, p = 0.032), and the FS-IQ of the former was significantly higher (t = 20.695, p < 0.001). The male/female sex ratio was slightly higher for children in the high-IQ group, but the difference was not significant (x2 = 1.481, p = 0.224). In the stratified analysis, males and females from the high-IQ group were roughly comparable in FS-IQ (t = −0.818, p = 0.414) and age (t = −0.964, p = 0.336), as were males and females from the low-IQ group in age (t = −0.013, p = 0.990). While males had a higher FS-IQ than females in the low-IQ group (t = 2.856, p = 0.005).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Individual characteristics of children with ASD in different IQ groups.

3.3. Sex differences at the autism diagnosis compliance rate of ADI-R

Both in the total sample and the high- or low-IQ group of children with ASD, we did not find any statistically significant sex differences in the diagnosis compliance rates (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparisons of the autism diagnosis compliance rate in the subdomains of ADI-R between males and females with ASD.

3.4. Sex differences in the autism and ASD diagnosis compliance rate of ADOS

Based on the autism or ASD cutoff scores in ADOS, we found no statistically significant sex differences in the diagnostic compliance rates both in the total sample and in the high- or low-IQ group of children (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Autism or ASD diagnosis compliance rate in ADOS.

3.5. Autism diagnosis compliance rate in both ADI-R and ADOS assessment

In the total sample, the difference in autism diagnosis compliance rate between males and females was not statistically significant. According to results from Table 5, it can be calculated that, for the total sample, the vast majority of males (84.36%) and females (82.76%) met the autism diagnostic condition in at least one of the diagnostic schedules. In a similar vein, the sex difference in the autism diagnosis compliance rate was insignificant in both the high-IQ and low-IQ groups.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Autism diagnosis compliance rate in both ADI-R and ADOS assessment.

3.6. Sex differences among children with ASD in the domain scores of ADI-R and ADOS

In the total sample, MANCOVA (with age and FS-IQ as covariates) revealed that there was no discernible difference between males and females on the ADI-R domain scores [λ = 0.984, partial η2 = 0.016, F (4,294) = 1.157, p = 0.330] (Table 6). Besides, no significant sex differences were found either in high-IQ children with ASD [λ = 0.984, partial η2 = 0.016, F (4,190) = 0.770, p = 0.546] or in low-IQ children with ASD [λ = 0.959, partial η2 = 0.041, F (4,98) = 1.041, p = 0.390]. In the ADOS RRB domain, the Mann–Whitney U tests did not find any significant statistical difference between males and females in the total sample (Z = −1.902, p = 0.057), or in the high-IQ (Z = −1.653, p = 0.098) or low-IQ (Z = −1.424, p = 0.154) children with ASD (Table 7). In the ADOS social affect domain, there were significant sex differences in the total sample [λ = 0.970, partial η2 = 0.030, F (3,295) = 3.019, p = 0.030] and in the low-IQ children with ASD [λ = 0.878, partial η2 = 0.122, F (3,98) = 4.555, p = 0.005]. While no significant effect of sex on ADOS scores was found in the high-IQ children with ASD [λ = 0.984, partial η2 = 0.016, F (3,191) = 1.018, p = 0.386] (Table 7). In the total sample, tests of between-subjects effects revealed that males scored higher than females in the ADOS reciprocal social interaction subcategory (partial η2 = 0.022, F = 6.563, p = 0.011) and in the ADOS full-scale scores (partial η2 = 0.023, F = 7.007, p = 0.009). In the low-IQ children with ASD, tests of between-subjects effects revealed that males scored higher than females in the ADOS reciprocal social interaction subcategory (partial η2 = 0.092, F = 10.088, p = 0.002) and in the ADOS full-scale scores (partial η2 = 0.088, F = 9.624, p = 0.002).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Sex differences among children with ASD in the domain scores of ADI-R.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Sex differences among children with ASD in the domain scores of ADOS.

4. Discussion

This study looks into possible sex differences in the core symptoms of children with ASD, as reported by parents and observed by testers. One point worth noting is that males with ASD have higher perceptual reasoning ability than females, despite having similar levels of overall intelligence. ADI-R did not find any statistically significant sex differences in the core symptoms of ASD. Significant effects of sex on ADOS were found in the reciprocal social interaction subcategory, especially among low-IQ children with ASD. Autistic children with low IQs are more likely to exhibit sex differences in their core symptoms than children with high IQs.

As is common knowledge, IQ and social interaction skills are strongly influenced by age and are highly variable and unstable in autistic preschoolers (28–30). Before the age of six, children’s brains are more plastic, and intervention is far more effective (31, 32). Indeed, decreases in autism symptom severity are more frequent in early childhood (17). Although early diagnosis and early intervention are significant themes within the field of autism research, the reality is that there is a widespread delay in the diagnosis of autism (33). The preschool years are critical for early intervention and early diagnosis, making them the primary focus of autism research. Because autism intervention is a long procedure, studying the clinical features of children with autism after the age of 6 years should not be overlooked. An in-depth investigation into the differences in core symptoms between boys and girls with autism will help to increase the diagnostic accuracy in clinical decision-making and individualization in the treatment and intervention processes. The majority of the various clinical assessments of autistic children will involve language development. Additionally, a child’s performance or score on an assessment in other domains will be impacted by their language proficiency. As a result, Wechsler IQ tests and other clinical assessments usually underestimate the abilities of autistic children who are nonverbal or have limited language skills, which makes it less likely that such individuals would measure reliably or validly (22, 28, 34). Standardized assessment tools, Wechsler IV, ADI-R, and ADOS-M3, were used for this study to examine the sex differences in clinical symptoms, allowing us to gain a deeper understanding of the clinical phenotypic traits of autistic children. The study’s focus was on school-age children and adolescents who were linguistically fluent.

Only in the area of perceptual reasoning did we find sex differences in the intellectual characteristics of linguistically fluent children with ASD. In line with earlier research, the Griffith evaluation revealed that boys with ASD had higher reasoning developmental quotients than girls (35). In addition, boys without ASD share the same perceptual reasoning advantage as boys with ASD (36). The variation in brain structure between males and females (37, 38) may aid in explicating the sex difference on the Perceptual Reasoning Index observed during our study. However, the association between perceptual reasoning ability and specific brain structures has yet to be confirmed by further research. The FS-IQ of children within the H-IQ group was considerably greater than that of the L-IQ group, and children within the H-IQ group were older than those in the L-IQ group. This result backs up the significant association between age and intelligence in children with ASD. The sex ratio (M/F) decreased among children with low IQ, and males had a higher FS-IQ than females in the low-IQ group, implying that female children with ASD are more likely to have comorbid intellectual impairment. Coinciding with a prior relevant study’s findings, boys are more likely than girls to have an IQ that is average or higher (39). This suggests that autistic girls could require additional interventional support addressing intellectual impairments.

The ADI-R assessment did not detect any statistically significant sex differences in all subcategory domain scores or diagnosis compliance rates. The conclusions remained consistent during the IQ-stratified analysis. In line with the results of earlier relevant investigations, the ADI-R and ADOS revealed no sex differences in the severity of ASD (8, 40). However, our finding was at odds with the result that indicated early-childhood RRB was lower in females (8). One possible reason for differences in results may be that our sample composition obscured any language- or intelligence-dependent sex differences in RRB in individuals with ASD. The small percentage of females in our sample is another explanation for the present results.

According to the findings of the ADOS test, sex differences in the core symptoms were found among the total sample, particularly in low-IQ ASD children. Females with low IQ were less severe than males in the reciprocal social interaction domain. Our results were not fully consistent with recent findings that ADOS-2 has no widespread systematic measurement bias based on race or sex (41). One possible and important reason may be that Kalb LG’s research did not consider the effect of intelligence on the results of ADOS. We observed sex differences in the core symptoms of ASD in children with low IQ but failed to find the difference in children with high IQ. This may be explained by the “camouflage mechanism.” Camouflaging was defined as the utilization of specific behavioral and cognitive strategies by autistic people to adapt to the predominately non-autistic social world (42–45). Emerging evidence suggests that autistic females demonstrate higher levels of camouflage than autistic males, and higher compensatory camouflage levels are linked to higher IQ (46, 47). Moreover, autistic males tend to exhibit more externalizing behaviors, while autistic females usually present more internalizing issues (48). As a result, it is more difficult for testers or clinicians to identify the behavioral manifestations of female autistic patients as autistic during the ADOS assessment. It is worth pondering that sex disparities in the core symptoms of autistic children may therefore be even more obvious if the study participants were further divided into subtle groups based on intelligence.

The ADI-R is a diagnostic instrument that evaluates children indirectly by using the descriptions of their parents or other primary caregivers. It is relatively subjective and usually subject to recall bias. Even with the aforementioned flaws or weaknesses, the ADI-R is a more comprehensive overview of the most aberrant states that the kid has experienced throughout his or her life and at the age of 4–5 years to reflect the traits of autistic children. ADOS ratings are based on a child’s immediate performance during testing, and evaluation results may be a little shaky or biased. ADI-R and ADOS can make up for each other’s deficiencies. At the overall level, the diagnostic compliance rate was analyzed for all subjects according to the diagnostic criteria for autism both in ADI-R and ADOS. There are three possible outcomes: the patients were identified as having “autism” by both ADI-R and ADOS, the patients were recognized as having “autism” based on only one of the two “gold standards,” and neither ADI-R nor ADOS were satisfied with the “autism” diagnosis. There were no sex differences in the composition ratios of the three conditions. Most individuals, whether males or females, met the autism diagnostic criteria in at least one of the diagnostic tools. While less than half of the children with fluent languages could satisfy the diagnostic criteria for both tests, what’s more, 15.64–17.24% of school-age children and adolescents with fluent languages will be underdiagnosed by both the ADI-R and the ADOS. It is worth noting that, even if an individual does not meet the diagnostic criteria for autism on either of the double A tests, it does not rule out the possibility that he or she has autism. Furthermore, a recent study reveals that developmental-behavioral pediatricians could diagnose ASD in young children without routinely using ADOS (49). When we performed stratified analysis according to cognitive level, we identified minor variations in core symptoms between boys and girls, but it was still difficult to discern differences in the performance of core symptoms between boys and girls with autism by double A alone. Children with average intelligence may use their cognitive abilities to cover up or make up for their social deficiencies. However, children in the low-IQ group may not notice much of a benefit from this compensating mechanism. This reinforces the hypothesis that sex differences might be more pronounced in autistic kids who have cognitive impairments. To thoroughly examine this assumption, a larger sample size is required as the majority of autistic children are moderate and do not have coexisting intellectual deficiencies.

Our study has several limitations that need to be addressed. Since the sample for this study was obtained from a single center, the research subjects might have been biased in their selection. Under the presumption of guaranteeing diagnostic homogeneity, multi-center collaboration could be employed in the future to enhance the sample’s representativeness. The study excluded nonverbal subjects, preschoolers, and individuals whose first language is not fluent, and the number of females and children with low IQ was small. Therefore, the findings from this study only reflect the tip of the iceberg for the autism spectrum community. In the future, in-depth analysis based on variables such as whether patients received intervention or not, the type of intervention, and the duration of the intervention will be necessary because the impact of intervention parameters on clinical symptoms was not taken into account in this study. The impacts of comorbidity on the clinical symptoms of autism were not taken into consideration in this research. Future research needs to be explored separately depending on whether the psychiatric comorbidity or the common comorbidity is present or absent in children with ASD, the type and severity of the comorbidity, and how they are managed or treated. In this study, one intelligence test tool and two reference standard diagnostic assessment tools were employed to measure sex differences in the symptoms of autism, and all these instruments were other-assessment tools. To better understand the sex differences of children with ASD, complementary information provided by self-assessment tests was necessary.

5. Conclusion

In general terms, overall cognitive functioning is similar across males and females with ASD; however, males have a higher perceptual reasoning ability. Female ASD is more likely to have comorbid intellectual impairment and could require additional intervention support. School-aged autistic children and adolescents with fluent language but low IQs are more likely to exhibit sex differences in their core symptoms than those with high IQs. Intelligence plays a key role in sex-based differences in the core symptoms of ASD. More representative populations and a thorough analysis of the underlying factors that may influence the clinical presentations are needed to deepen understanding of the differences between males and females with ASD.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

YaJ and YuJ: conceptualization, methodology, and writing—original draft. H-lZ and S-mC: data acquisition and curation, validation, and data analysis. X-bZ and F-lZ: conceptualization, project administration, supervision, and writing—review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 81873801).

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all colleagues for assistance in data extraction and measurement administration. We sincerely thank the caregivers and children who participated in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5™. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc (2013).

Google Scholar

2.Maenner, MJ, Shaw, KA, Bakian, AV, Bilder, DA, Durkin, MS, Esler, A, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of autism Spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2018. MMWR Surveill Summ. (2021) 70:1–16. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss7011a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3.Lord, C, Charman, T, Havdahl, A, Carbone, P, Anagnostou, E, Boyd, B, et al. The lancet commission on the future of care and clinical research in autism. Lancet. (2022) 399:271–334. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(21)01541-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4.Strang, J, van der Miesen, A, Caplan, R, Hughes, C, da Vanport, S, and Lai, M. Both sex- and gender-related factors should be considered in autism research and clinical practice. Autism. (2020) 24:539–43. doi: 10.1177/1362361320913192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5.Lord, C, Rutter, M, and Le Couteur, A. Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (1994) 24:659–85. doi: 10.1007/bf02172145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6.Lord, C, Risi, S, Lambrecht, L, Cook, EHJr, Leventhal, BL, DiLavore, PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2000) 30:205–23. doi: 10.1023/a:1005592401947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7.Gotham, K, Pickles, A, and Lord, C. Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2009) 39:693–705. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8.Tillmann, J, Ashwood, K, Absoud, M, Bölte, S, Bonnet-Brilhault, F, Buitelaar, J, et al. Evaluating sex and age differences in ADI-R and ADOS scores in a large European multi-site sample of individuals with autism Spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2018) 48:2490–505. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3510-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9.Mahendiran, T, Brian, J, Dupuis, A, Muhe, N, Wong, PY, Iaboni, A, et al. Meta-analysis of sex differences in social and communication function in children with autism Spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Front Psych. (2019) 10:804. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10.Ros-Demarize, R, Bradley, C, Kanne, SM, Warren, Z, Boan, A, Lajonchere, C, et al. ASD symptoms in toddlers and preschoolers: an examination of sex differences. Autism Res. (2020) 13:157–66. doi: 10.1002/aur.2241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11.Mo, K, Sadoway, T, Bonato, S, Ameis, SH, Anagnostou, E, Lerch, JP, et al. Sex/gender differences in the human autistic brains: a systematic review of 20 years of neuroimaging research. Neuroimage Clin. (2021) 32:102811. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12.Osório, JMA, Rodríguez-Herreros, B, Richetin, S, Junod, V, Romascano, D, Pittet, V, et al. Sex differences in sensory processing in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2021) 14:2412–23. doi: 10.1002/aur.2580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13.Kavanaugh, BC, Schremp, CA, Jones, RN, Best, CR, Sheinkopf, SJ, and Morrow, EM. Moderators of age of diagnosis in > 20,000 females with autism in two large US studies. J Autism Dev Disord. (2021) 53:864–9. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05026-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14.Wood-Downie, H, Wong, B, Kovshoff, H, Cortese, S, and Hadwin, JA. Research review: a systematic review and meta-analysis of sex/gender differences in social interaction and communication in autistic and nonautistic children and adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2021) 62:922–36. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15.Lai, M, and Szatmari, P. Sex and gender impacts on the behavioural presentation and recognition of autism. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2020) 33:117–23. doi: 10.1097/yco.0000000000000575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16.Waizbard-Bartov, E, Ferrer, E, Heath, B, Rogers, S, Nordahl, C, Solomon, M, et al. Identifying autism symptom severity trajectories across childhood. Autism Res. (2022) 15:687–701. doi: 10.1002/aur.2674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17.Waizbard-Bartov, E, Ferrer, E, Young, G, Heath, B, Rogers, S, Wu Nordahl, C, et al. Trajectories of autism symptom severity change during early childhood. J Autism Dev Disord. (2021) 51:227–42. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04526-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18.Hahler, E-M, and Elsabbagh, M. Autism: a global perspective. Curr Dev Disord Rep. (2015) 2:58–64. doi: 10.1007/s40474-014-0033-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19.Lombardo, MV, Lai, MC, and Baron-Cohen, S. Big data approaches to decomposing heterogeneity across the autism spectrum. Mol Psychiatry. (2019) 24:1435–50. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0321-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20.Napolitano, A, Schiavi, S, La Rosa, P, Rossi-Espagnet, MC, Petrillo, S, Bottino, F, et al. Sex differences in autism Spectrum disorder: diagnostic, neurobiological, and behavioral features. Front Psych. (2022) 13:889636. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.889636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21.Sara, C. Sex/gender differences in children with autism spectrum disorder: a brief overview on epidemiology, symptom profile, and neuroanatomy. J Neurosci Res. (2022). doi: 10.1002/jnr.25000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22.Nader, AM, Courchesne, V, Dawson, M, and Soulières, I. Does WISC-IV underestimate the intelligence of autistic children? J Autism Dev Disord. (2016) 46:1582–9. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2270-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23.Duvall, SW, Huang-Storms, L, Presmanes Hill, A, Myers, J, and Fombonne, E. No sex differences in cognitive ability in Young children with autism Spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50:1770–85. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03933-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24.Wang, S, Deng, H, You, C, Chen, K, Li, J, Tang, C, et al. Sex differences in diagnosis and clinical phenotypes of Chinese children with autism Spectrum disorder. Neurosci Bull. (2017) 33:153–60. doi: 10.1007/s12264-017-0102-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25.Lai, MC, Lombardo, MV, Auyeung, B, Chakrabarti, B, and Baron-Cohen, S. Sex/gender differences and autism: setting the scene for future research. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2015) 54:11–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26.Zhang, H. The revision of WISC-IV Chinese version. Psychol Sci. (2009) 32:1177–9. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2009.05.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27.Kaufman, AS, Flanagan, DP, Alfonso, VC, and Mascolo, JT. Test review: Wechsler intelligence scale for children, fourth edition (WISC-IV). J Psychoeduc Assess. (2006) 24:278–95. doi: 10.1177/0734282906288389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28.Courchesne, V, Girard, D, Jacques, C, and Soulières, I. Assessing intelligence at autism diagnosis: mission impossible? Testability and cognitive profile of autistic preschoolers. J Autism Dev Disord. (2019) 49:845–56. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3786-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29.Sigman, M, and McGovern, CW. Improvement in cognitive and language skills from preschool to adolescence in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2005) 35:15–23. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1027-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30.Turner, LM, Stone, WL, Pozdol, SL, and Coonrod, EE. Follow-up of children with autism spectrum disorders from age 2 to age 9. Autism. (2006) 10:243–65. doi: 10.1177/1362361306063296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31.Fuller, E, and Kaiser, A. The effects of early intervention on social communication outcomes for children with autism Spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50:1683–700. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03927-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32.Solomon, M, Iosif, A, Reinhardt, V, Libero, L, Nordahl, C, Ozonoff, S, et al. What will my child's future hold? Phenotypes of intellectual development in 2-8-year-olds with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2018) 11:121–32. doi: 10.1002/aur.1884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33.Mitroulaki, S, Serdari, A, Tripsianis, G, Gundelfinger, R, Arvaniti, A, Vorvolakos, T, et al. First alarm and time of diagnosis in autism Spectrum disorders. Compr Child Adolesc Nurs. (2020) 45:75–91. doi: 10.1080/24694193.2020.1834013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34.Tager-Flusberg, H, and Kasari, C. Minimally verbal school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder: the neglected end of the spectrum. Autism Res. (2013) 6:468–78. doi: 10.1002/aur.1329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35.Li, H, Wen, H, Yang, L, Wu, W, Wang, C, and Jia, F. Differences in the autistic severity and developmental level of children with autism spectrum disorder of different gender and age. Chin J Behav Med Brain Sci. (2021) 30:27–32. doi: 10.3760/cma.j.cn371468-20200722-01584

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36.Li, C, Zhu, N, Zeng, L, Dang, S, Zhou, J, Kang, Y, et al. Sex differences in the intellectual functioning of early school-aged children in rural China. BMC Public Health. (2016) 16:288. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-2956-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37.Bajaj, S, Raikes, A, Smith, R, Dailey, NS, Alkozei, A, Vanuk, JR, et al. The relationship between general intelligence and cortical structure in healthy individuals. Neuroscience. (2018) 388:36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38.Schmithorst, VJ. Developmental sex differences in the relation of neuroanatomical connectivity to intelligence. Intelligence. (2009) 37:164–73. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39.Katusic, MZ, Myers, SM, Weaver, AL, and Voigt, RG. IQ in autism Spectrum disorder: a population-based birth cohort study. Pediatrics. (2021) 148:e2020049899. doi: 10.1542/peds.2020-049899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40.Margherita, P, Marco, T, Silvia, G, Eleonora, N, Raffaella, T, Roberta, I, et al. Sex differences in autism Spectrum disorder: an investigation on Core symptoms and psychiatric comorbidity in preschoolers. Front Integr Neurosci. (2021) 14:594082. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2020.594082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41.Kalb, LG, Singh, V, Hong, JS, Holingue, C, Ludwig, NN, Pfeiffer, D, et al. Analysis of race and sex bias in the autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS-2). JAMA Netw Open. (2022) 5:e229498. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.9498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42.Cook, J, Hull, L, Crane, L, and Mandy, W. Camouflaging in autism: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2021) 89:102080. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43.Hull, L, Petrides, K, Allison, C, Smith, P, Baron-Cohen, S, Lai, M, et al. "putting on my Best Normal": social camouflaging in adults with autism Spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord. (2017) 47:2519–34. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44.Lawson, WB. Adaptive morphing and coping with social threat in autism: an autistic perspective. J Intellect Disabil Diagn Treatment. (2020) 8:519–26. doi: 10.6000/2292-2598.2020.08.03.29

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45.Pearson, A, and Rose, K. A conceptual analysis of autistic masking: Understanding the narrative of stigma and the illusion of choice. Autism Adulthood. (2020) 3:52–60. doi: 10.31219/osf.io/6rwa5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46.Wood-Downie, H, Wong, B, Kovshoff, H, Mandy, W, Hull, L, and Hadwin, JA. Sex/gender differences in camouflaging in children and adolescents with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2021) 51:1353–64. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04615-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47.Hull, L, Lai, M, Baron-Cohen, S, Allison, C, Smith, P, Petrides, K, et al. Gender differences in self-reported camouflaging in autistic and non-autistic adults. Autism. (2020) 24:352–63. doi: 10.1177/1362361319864804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48.Lundin, K, Mahdi, S, Isaksson, J, and Bölte, S. Functional gender differences in autism: an international, multidisciplinary expert survey using the international classification of functioning, disability, and health model. Autism. (2021) 25:1020–35. doi: 10.1177/1362361320975311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49.Barbaresi, W, Cacia, J, Friedman, S, Fussell, J, Hansen, R, Hofer, J, et al. Clinician diagnostic certainty and the role of the autism diagnostic observation schedule in autism Spectrum disorder diagnosis in Young children. JAMA Pediatr. (2022) 176:1233–41. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2022.3605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorders, sex difference, intelligence quotient, core symptoms, autism diagnostic observation schedule, autism diagnostic interview-revised, Wechsler intelligence scale for children-fourth edition (Chinese version)

Citation: Ji Y, Ji Y, Zhu H-l, Cheng S-m, Zou X-b and Zhu F-l (2023) Examine sex differences in autism spectrum disorder in school-aged children and adolescents with fluent language. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1151596. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1151596

Received: 26 January 2023; Accepted: 13 March 2023;
Published: 06 April 2023.

Edited by:

Kleanthes K. Grohmann, University of Cyprus, Cyprus

Reviewed by:

Elena Grigorenko, University of Houston, United States
Wen-Xiong Chen, Guangzhou Medical University, China

Copyright © 2023 Ji, Ji, Zhu, Cheng, Zou and Zhu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiao-bing Zou, em91eGJAbWFpbC5zeXN1LmVkdS5jbg==; Feng-lei Zhu, emh1ZmxlaTNAbWFpbC5zeXN1LmVkdS5jbg==

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share last authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.