Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 16 March 2023
Sec. Computational Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic From Brain Priorities to Brain Modeling View all 6 articles

The neurobiology of functional neurological disorders characterised by impaired awareness

  • 1Institute of Life Sciences, Sant'Anna School of Advanced Studies, Pisa, Italy
  • 2Faculty of Medicine and Surgery, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
  • 3Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 4Max Planck UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 5National Hospital of Neurology and Neurosurgery (UCLH), London, United Kingdom
  • 6Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom

We review the neurobiology of Functional Neurological Disorders (FND), i.e., neurological disorders not explained by currently identifiable histopathological processes, in order to focus on those characterised by impaired awareness (functionally impaired awareness disorders, FIAD), and especially, on the paradigmatic case of Resignation Syndrome (RS). We thus provide an improved more integrated theory of FIAD, able to guide both research priorities and the diagnostic formulation of FIAD. We systematically address the diverse spectrum of clinical presentations of FND with impaired awareness, and offer a new framework for understanding FIAD. We find that unraveling the historical development of neurobiological theory of FIAD is of paramount importance for its current understanding. Then, we integrate contemporary clinical material in order to contextualise the neurobiology of FIAD within social, cultural, and psychological perspectives. We thus review neuro-computational insights in FND in general, to arrive at a more coherent account of FIAD. FIAD may be based on maladaptive predictive coding, shaped by stress, attention, uncertainty, and, ultimately, neurally encoded beliefs and their updates. We also critically appraise arguments in support of and against such Bayesian models. Finally, we discuss implications of our theoretical account and provide pointers towards an improved clinical diagnostic formulation of FIAD. We suggest directions for future research towards a more unified theory on which future interventions and management strategies could be based, as effective treatments and clinical trial evidence remain limited.

Introduction

« The self, shelter from the storm and storm itself »: Does awareness of the self and the surrounding world protect or endanger?

Functional Neurological Disorders (FND) have been regarded as a spectrum of clinical conditions difficult to disentangle. For over 50 years, they have been a no-man’s land between psychiatry and neurology, where even specialist physicians received limited training (14). Despite being debilitating conditions with a prevalence of around 50 per 100,000 people (58), and the second most common cause of a neurological outpatient visit after headache (912), FND were neglected for decades. Reasons for this include the subtle pathophysiological complexity of FND, which made theories of these disorders almost impossible to substantiate; and the historical controversy between clinical neuroscientists and psychoanalysts, which, together, put the validity of these conditions in doubt.

In recent years, however, FND have been the focus of renewed research and clinical interest (1324).

This group of clinically heterogeneous conditions has, indeed, received descriptive diagnostic criteria in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) and International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11).

In DSM-5, diagnosis of FND requires the following criteria: one or more symptoms of altered voluntary motor or sensory function; clinical evidence of incompatibility between the symptom and recognised neurological or medical conditions; symptoms or deficits are not better explained by another medical or mental disorder (25).

However, even the latest guidelines contain ambiguities that lead to confusion between clinicians, researchers, and the public (26), making it of pivotal importance to further clarify the pathophysiology of FND. Current criteria and guidelines remain rather vague and unclear, as many symptoms of FND are not even specific to neurology, but occur within rheumatological, gastroenterological, cardiological, and pain conditions (2729). The naming of these disorders remains confusing, still referring to specific yet controversial mechanisms (Conversion in DSM-5; Dissociation in ICD-11). Within the variety of FND, disturbances of awareness have been long recognised (30), but their neurobiology is even less understood than that of other functional symptoms, and would particularly benefit from clearer diagnostic classification.

By « Functionally impaired awareness disorders », or FIAD, we refer to the symptom dimension within FND characterising the nature and degree to which awareness is disturbed. We introduce the acronym FIAD to conceptually isolate and focus research on phenomenologically altered perception, e.g., glove anaesthesia, functional hemianopia. This is a clinical-descriptive conceptualisation, not a mechanistic one. We acknowledge but do not study, at this stage, the likely relationship of FIAD with disorders of higher awareness, such as awareness of the disorder itself (metacognition, insight). In FIAD, the individual’s ability to perceive the world and hence to respond is altered, not necessarily implying impaired cognition, or motor abilities.

FIAD encompass a range of subjectively altered awareness, ranging from focal sensory disturbance to profound global disorders such as trance states and functional coma. FND patients present with a large variety of clinical findings suggestive of disturbance of awareness. It is likely, but remains to be ascertained, that the neural underpinnings of distorted awareness cause distorted responsivity. The latter includes loss of reaction to sensory stimulation (including pain), loss of verbal comprehension with associated mutism (no or minimal verbal response), hypotonia, decrease or cessation of fundamental organised motor functions such as drinking and eating, and enuresis and encopresis (31, 32). Even severe FIAD such as functional coma present without laboratory abnormalities in blood and urine tests, ECG, EEG, and MRI, which are within the clinically normal range (3335).

The neurobiological theory that we will delineate nuances the rigid distinction between culture-bound and functional disorders of awareness that the major classifications demand. To furnish data for this unified theory, we turn to FIAD described worldwide (31, 34), and describe Resignation Syndrome (RS) (28, 29), Traumatic Withdrawal Syndrome (TWS), Grisi Siknis (GS), and non-epileptic seizures (NES).

Resignation syndrome

Resignation syndrome (RS), or uppgivenhetssyndrom in Swedish, was first described in the 1990s as a catatonic-like condition that induces a state of reduced consciousness (34). It has been compared to « culture-bound’ or « dissociative » disorders (3335). Affected individuals (predominantly children and adolescents in the midst of a protracted migration process) first exhibit symptoms of anxiety and depression, apathy and lethargy. They then transition to a state of severe withdrawal. Eventually, their condition may become stuporous, i.e., they stop responding, eating, talking, and become incontinent. At this stage, patients are unconscious, and tube feeding is life-sustaining. This condition can persist for months, in some cases for more than a year. Remission occurs with a gradual return to normal functioning, in many cases associated with improvement of life circumstances (36).

A case series of 46 patients by von Knorring et al. (31) addressed the background of RS, highlighting the pathogenic role of trauma. In this series, all patients had experienced or witnessed violence, rape, or killings, or threats against a close family member. Almost all children (95.6%) suffered from post-traumatic stress syndrome (PTSD) and/or a depressive episode prior to resignation syndrome. Most belonged to an ethnic or religious minority (69.6%), almost all of which were persecuted (93.5%): Uighurs, Romani, Yezidis, Armenians in Russia and Ukraine. A large proportion of the children had one (28%) or both parents (30%) suffering from a mental or severe physical disorder. Remarkably, only a minority of the children came from war zones (17.4%).

In stark contrast, others proposed that RS is a behavioural disorder « induced » by families. Sallin et al. (28) argued that separating RS patients from their parents and keeping them strictly uninformed of the asylum process, would be therapeutically beneficial. This was based on a study of 13 participants in a specialist unit: 9 of them recovered, with 8 out of these 9 subjects being separated from their parents. Furthermore, the 4 subjects who did not recover within the timeframe of the study were granted asylum. However, inferences about the nature of RS here are limited by the study methodology, and especially the treatment allocation of patients to interventions. Specifically, the separated cases came from families already assessed to have problematic parental capacity. Five of these cases were committed to compulsory social care.

Resignation syndrome patients may be suffering from catatonia (34). Both conditions are characterised by a decreased ability to initiate voluntary actions, paucity of movement, stupor and mutism. RS hence fulfils three or more diagnostic criteria for catatonia (25). DSM5 and ICD11 describe catatonia as a manifestation of another diagnosis, not a primary primary one, begging the question of whether in RS it is a manifestation of (complex) PTSD. Unlike in catatonia, increased limb tone, echolalia, echopraxia, and mannerisms are not found in RS, which is typically much longer lasting than catatonia. RS appears to respond poorly to benzodiazepine treatment (37).

Traumatic withdrawal syndrome

Traumatic withdrawal syndrome (TWS) has been observed in refugee children transferred from Australia to the Nauru Regional Processing Centre in the past decade (3841). Since 2012, 222 children, of whom at least 27 were unaccompanied, have been sent to Nauru (42). These asylum seekers were mainly from Iran, Pakistan, Sri Lanka, Pakistan, Bangladesh, low-income African countries, or stateless (43). The clinical features included pervasive social withdrawal, severe reduction or inability to walk, talk, eat, drink, self-care, and socialise. Children would actively resist or not respond to acts of care and encouragement (44). TWS was mostly documented in females aged 7–15 years, but was also found in adult males (45).

Grisi siknis

Grisi siknis (GS), prevalent amongst the Miskito people of eastern Central America, primarily affects young women between the ages of 15 and 18. It was first described in detail by the anthropologist Philip Dennis after he had come across it in the 1970 (35). Symptoms appear to be anticipated by anxiety and headaches, culminating in long periods of coma-like unconsciousness, with sudden outbreaks of violent and aggressive behaviour. Many cases were associated with gender-based violence and oppression, as the young women were pressurised into sexual relationships with older men (46). It may be argued that the symptomatology associated with the condition may avert this harassment (35). Yet, little is known about how exactly the clinical and gender dimensions of GS relate to the socio-political context in the region.

Non-epileptic seizure disorder

Non-epileptic seizure disorder (NES) remains one of the most common presentations of FND in industrialised countries (47). NES is a spectrum of paroxysmal behaviours that resemble epileptic seizures, but lacks macroscopic abnormal electrophysiological activity. They involve impairment of consciousness, flaccid or rigid collapse, and/or tremulous limb movements (48). Patients give rich and varied accounts of disturbance of awareness during seizures. NES are clinically challenging to diagnose (46); the gold standard is Video Electroencephalogram (vEEG) monitoring, where the events are monitored by continuous video recording and simultaneously co-registered with Electroencephalogram (EEG). When a patient’s habitual event is captured on vEEG and the clinicians are provided with a complete patient history, diagnosis of NES can be made with high confidence (4764).

Neurobiological theories: A historical perspective

We find a concise historical understanding of the neurobiology of FIAD to be essential to inform future development and directions in scientific research, clinical taxonomy, diagnosis and treatment.

The first attempts to integrate neuro-psychological understanding of FIAD took place in the late 19th century; yet, from the 1920s until the first decade of the new millennium, neurobiological aspects of FND and FIAD were broadly disregarded by clinicians and researchers. As a result, FIAD missed out on the revolution in biological – and indeed biopsychosocial - understanding, still standing out at the intersection between neurology and psychiatry (65, 66).

In particular, Charcot, Janet, Breuer and Freud were the main contributors to the development of theories of FIAD in the late 19th century (6769). Charcot worked with FIAD under the rubric of « hysteria »1 demonstrating cases of apparent loss of consciousness and vigorously rejecting simplistic gender-based theories and treatments (7079).

Janet’s enduring legacy to the neurobiology of FIAD was his dissociation theory (80). The latter theorised that neurobiological vulnerability, especially in traumatised patients, led to a fragmentation of psychological functions under stress, which he saw as a lesion rather than a defence. He proposed attention to play a crucial role in the pathophysiology of FIAD, and that its withdrawal was to be held responsible for the onset of symptoms (81). Janet saw functional sensory loss as a key symptom and hypothesised an abnormally high level of activity in the mechanism normally filtering out extraneous sensory input, well accounting for loss of function (64). Moreover, he maintained distraction to be an alleviating solution for functional tremor (8082). The concept of dissociation has developed since Janet, and is a major heading in ICD-11 (6066), where it describes a discontinuity in the integration of brain functions as central to FND.

In the famous « Studies on Hysteria » (1895), Breuer and Freud developed Janet’s dissociation, or splitting, of mental functions (8387). Here, excessive excitation caused by emotional events would be « converted » into somatic phenomena, leading to hysteria (88, 89). Crucially, Freud greatly emphasised the defensive function of this « conversion », which later formed the only aetiologically based category in DSM-III, and bequeathed the description of Conversion Disorder » (CD) in DSM-5. In a similar vein, « La belle indifference », defined in DSM-5 as a lack of concern about the nature or implications of the symptom » (25), is listed as a feature supporting a diagnosis of CD. The neurobiological claims of the dissociative and conversion traditions, however, give a rather ad hoc account of why many signs of FND require attention to manifest (i.e., paralysis, tremor) and why some may improve when attention is diverted (64).

Kretschmer (90, 91) described two behavioural responses to threat observed in animals, comparing these instinctive patterns to FIAD symptom patterns, such as convulsive dissociative seizures and violent tremors, and paralysis and dissociative seizures. He was also the first to hypothesise that behaviour triggered initially by a traumatic event or by a stress-induced response, through repetition, becomes increasingly habitual and automatic: a conditioned response that no longer required the presence of the inciting event (91).

With the later decline of psychoanalytic psychiatry, interest in FIAD and « hysteria » waned and its « near total disappearance » as a diagnosis was announced (92). In 1965, eminent British neurologist and psychiatrist Eliot Slater went further, maintaining that hysteria had never existed, but rather was the result of misdiagnosis (9295). In the 1980s and 1990s, most doctors disregarded these patients, who had become « almost literally invisible to medicine, and modern medicine’s untouchables» (95, 96).

Still some progress was made. In 1967, Whitlock (97) proposed a biological hypothesis for FIAD. Inhibition of afferent input at the level of the reticular formation could result in « selective depression of awareness of a bodily function » (25). Hence, attentional diversion away from the symptomatic region inhibited afferent input from that region, resulting in loss of function (98119).

The end of the 20th century and the start of the 21st (120159 ) saw a revival in the clinical and scientific interest in FIAD, due to advances in clinical neurology research and recognition that large numbers of patients were unfairly blamed and stigmatised for their disabilities.

It was indeed in this context that CD gained recognition in the clinical setting, emphasising the presence of neurological symptoms of a physical ailment with no corresponding organic cause. CD would, therefore, suggest that psychological suffering and tensions could be « converted » into physical symptoms, such as paralysis, blindness, loss of speech and/or seizures (111, 120, 136).

Overall, a growing number of neuroscience studies has been conducted since, starting to demonstrate the anatomical and functional circuitry of FND. However, our understanding is limited, and mostly about motor symptoms rather than FIAD.

Therefore, we aim to draw on recent neuro-computational principles and the attention-focused work of Edwards et al. (64), to provide a neurobiological account of FIAD integrating the « lesion » (dissociative, deficit) and « purposive » (conversion, defensive) traditions. Within a Bayesian, « active inference » framework, we propose that FIAD may be caused by maladaptive neurally encoded beliefs about the state of the world and the optimally attainable conditions to live in it. Thus, acknowledging the variety of predisposing (i.e., psychosocial adversity, gender, physical illness, exposure to symptom/illness models), precipitating (i.e., physical injury, mental health symptoms, interpersonal conflict, other stressors) and perpetuating (i.e., avoidance, illness beliefs/expectations, social isolation) factors. Finally, we lay out potential clinical implications of this account.

Better models can further inform clinicians about the genesis and the clinical trajectories of these conditions and strengthen collaborative treatments between families, patients, and professionals.

To this end, we highlight directions for future inquiry likely to yield high impact advances.

Results

We now review the current state of knowledge about FIAD, based on a comprehensive literature search. This included reviewing clinical reports, neuroimaging studies, theoretical, and computational neuroscience accounts. Details of the methods supporting this review of the literature are found in the Methods section below.

Contemporary neuroscience of functional neurological disorders with impaired awareness

More recent studies illustrate the likely neurobiological and pathophysiological pathways that may underpin the impaired awareness process in FND. They do not, however, clearly explain how a psychological stressor might be related with a particular sign or symptom, such as loss of response to sensory stimulus, loss of comprehension and social contact, or disturbance of fundamental motor activities (48, 49). Growing evidence suggests that psychological stressors (in childhood and adulthood, and whether remembered or not) are linked to biological changes (50, 51) as measured by stress biomarkers (cortisol, amylase, heart rate variability, brain activity, and epigenetic changes) (52, 53) and measurably altered neural activity may be associated with FIAD.

Specifically, the literature suggests that mechanisms underpinning attention, emotional processing and interoception could contribute to the altered perception of internal and external states (160179). Computational models explaining how the embodied brain responds to psychosocial stressors can be reconciled with recent neuroscience evidence on the above-mentioned mechanisms (180183).

Yet, the reader should notice that focusing on one mechanism at the expense of the others might result in a reductionist description of isolated components, with little regard for the complexity beyond the sum of its parts. Specifically, it is unlikely that a single direct relationship between stressful or traumatic events and FND would provide a sufficient explanation. Rather, the aetiology is more likely to be complex and entail a variety of predisposing, triggering, and perpetuating elements (184189).

In presenting the available neuroscientific evidence on possible mechanisms of FIAD, it is important to underline how, whilst the literature on FND with motor symptoms is rich, there is little available experimental evidence to account for a mechanistic hypothesis of FIAD. Here, we summarise the available evidence and connect it with theoretical work, with the aim to inform much needed development of further scientific investigations.

Altered awareness is commonly found in healthy people during highly stressful situations, and it is probably an adaptive response, such as analgesia to major injuries in battle (189196). Clinically, intense emotion, stress and trauma have often been associated with degrees of loss of awareness, but theories regarding their causal roles remain highly controversial (197200). Recent progress has led towards a more nuanced knowledge of emotion in FND, which move beyond a simple dichotomy between stress-induced or not. Numerous interdependent emotion processing activities are currently thought to be mapping onto salience and other limbic/paralimbic (e.g., ventromedial and orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex, parahippocampus, hippocampus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) circuits (202).

The « salience network » appears to recognise and respond to one’s homeostatic demands; it consists of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, anterior insula, dorsal amygdala, periaqueductal grey (PAG), and hypothalamus (203208). Enhanced amygdala and PAG bottom-up activation appears to be related with increased emotional reactivity, arousal, and protective reactions. Patients with FND had decreased amygdala habituation and higher sensitivity during processing of negative emotions. Alterations in the control of amygdala and PAG activation by the prefrontal cortex may also contribute to heightened emotional reactions (209).

Individuals with mixed active FIAD symptoms had higher baseline arousal levels than those with anxiety disorders or healthy volunteers, as measured by spontaneous changes in skin resistance and failure to achieve acclimation to repeated auditory stimulation and sonic stimulation (210223).

Yet, studies often show seemingly contradictory results. Bakvis et al. (224) reported that 19 NES patients had elevated basal diurnal cortisol levels and lower heart rate variability at baseline, indicating increased sympathetic activity (225, 226). Van der Kruijs et al. (227), instead, when employing positive outdoor images and a Stroop test found no activation differences between NES patients and healthy controls. However, stronger connectivity values between areas involved in emotion (insula), executive control (inferior frontal gyrus and parietal cortex) and movement (precentral sulcus), significantly associated with dissociation scores were retrieved in NES patients (120, 227234).

Moreover, in a single within-subject fMRI case study on CD, using a vocalisation task, Bryant and Das (235) found that inferior frontal gyrus activity was positively functionally connected to anterior cingulate and negatively to amygdala activity during speech recovery, but not during mutism. Therefore, suggesting a link between speech networks and the anterior cingulate, which controls amygdala activity (236238).

In a functional paralysis case study, Kanaan et al. (239) found increased amygdala and right inferior frontal activity, as well as decreased motor activity.

Aybek et al. (240) evaluated 12 CD patients’ responses to sad or scary vs. neutral faces (in comparison to controls matched for age, gender, IQ, and sexual trauma). Left amygdala, premotor/sensorimotor area, cingulate cortex, and PAG showed higher activity. The authors would, therefore, propose that cumulative fear sensitization in the amygdala may cause long-lasting physical reactions to stress and danger.

As PAG activation mirrors animal « freeze reactions » to scary stimuli, a potential biomarker for FND may be the absence of fear conditioning in the amygdala (120, 241245).

Everyone knows what attention is and no one knows what attention is

Over the course of time and across different disciplines, attention has been addressed and presented with many definitions (246, 247). Interestingly, it has been defined as the process that enhances representation of some kinds of information and inhibits others (248), thus privileging the former over the latter for further processing. It has long been hypothesised that « dissociative » disorders, and FIAD, in particular, involve biased attention, we researched the sources that cited original search articles to find (249255). For example, experimental participants scoring high for dissociation on a self-report scale showed reduced attention to somatosensory stimulation after they watched a trauma-related film (162, 256260). As we will describe below, predictive coding theory formulates attention as an implicit prediction about the value of sensory input. In other words, patients may gate out awareness of input, analogous to collateral discharges normally suppressing (predictably uninformative) blurring during visual saccades. Thus, predictive coding theory both emphasises and helps operationalise the role of attention in FIAD.

Neural correlates of attention characterised by disruptions in sustained and selective attention have been focus of investigation (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Neural correlates of FND involving impairment of awareness (FIAD) FIAD may include altered connectivity within and across brain circuit. This figure, adapted from Drane et al. (209), shows with thicker arrows the connections more likely to be involved in FIAD, amongst those implicated in FND in general. AMY, amygdala; SMA, supplementary motor area; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; HYP, hypothalamus; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; pgACC, perigenual anterior cingulate cortex; sgACC, subgenual anterior cingulate cortex; SM, sensorimotor area; TPJ, temporoparietal junction.

Kowloska et al. (261) demonstrated in this study that children and adolescents with acute conversion symptoms have a diminished capacity for knowledge manipulation and retention. Upon completion of the IntegNeuro neurocognitive battery by 57 participants and their matched healthy controls, FIAD patients appeared to have a diminished capacity to block and interfere with information and to inhibit responses, all of which are required for effective attention, executive function, and memory.

In this prospective case–control study by Stager et al. (262), 26 children with video-EEG-confirmed non-epileptic seizures (NES) performed a modified Stroop test. Here, NES patients showed poorer selective attention, less awareness and greater cognitive inhibition than controls.

Interestingly, the core regions of interests identified found in neuroimaging studies of FIAD are part of the attentional network and have been hypothesised to play pivotal roles in processing prediction and modulating multiple levels of the brain hierarchy.

In particular, the anterior cingulate cortex and the posterior parietal cortex have been related to a regulatory role in modulating the weighting of long-term goals, encoded by the prefrontal cortex (PFC) vs. short-term goals, encoded by lower-level areas.

The inhibition of short-term goals and prioritisation of long-term planning is also associated with the activity of interior frontal gyrus (IFG) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC), associated with top-down inhibition of the amygdala and increased connectivity between IFG and vmPFC (209).

Complementary scientific evidence therefore suggested that the inferior frontal cortex identifies conflicts in perceptual cues (sensory information) and conscious experience and translates ambiguous sensory input into precise conscious experience.

Recent evidence found the periaqueductal grey (PAG) not only involved in autonomic control, but also engaged in mechanism of cognitive control and in encoding the probability of threat, rather than fear output per se (209).

Although not directly studied in FIAD, it is worth noting that an interoceptive processes have been investigated in the context of FND and could potentially be of interest in the study of FIAD. Interoception is a bidirectional process characterised by feedback and feedforward loops that map bodily states (263268).

In the next section, we will introduce foundational concepts of the Bayesian brain hypothesis and the active inference framework, reconnecting the neurobiological plausible hypotheses to current neuroscientific evidence. We will finally show how this theoretical framework can inform future research and hypothesis testing.

FIAD and the Bayesian brain

We now turn to neural predictive coding, and specifically active inference (AI), as a useful framework to understand FIAD. It can help formulate rigorous neurobiological hypotheses, describing how changes in neural activity and connectivity could result in modulation of motor and sensory gating leading to FIAD.

Active inference processes relevant to awareness

Active inference (269275) posits that all organisms hold an implicit model of « how life should unfold », in terms of both homeostasis and development. Deviations, or expected deviations, from this scheme then motivate corrective processes, such as learning, moving or eating. Thus, active inference can be thought as a normative framework. The actions that I believe that I will take are the ones consistent with a belief that the best outcomes will be obtained. Deviations from (adaptive) predictions are surprising, so that organisms must minimise this type of surprise, furnished by their sensory information, through controlling their actions to resolve uncertainty towards belief in benign observations to come (Bayes-optimal behaviour). Technically, « how life should unfold » is called the generative model of the world and the self. Hence, the brain is fundamentally a predictive machine, performing probabilistic computations to infer the causes of sensory evidence and the likely outcomes of actions (276282).

Computationally, living beings are thought to reduce the surprise inherent in « how life is unfolding », relative to « how it should unfold », by dynamically comparing their model of the world (prior expectations, or predictions) and their observations (sensory input) (64, 269, 283) in terms of a measure of surprise called « free energy ». The comparison between model expectations and observations results in prediction error; in biological terms, the minimization of the prediction error (or surprise) is achieved by modulating synaptic activity and neuronal connections. Action and perceptual inference both contribute to free energy minimization, the former by sampling new salient sensory evidence to reduce uncertainty, the latter by updating the initial expectations (Bayesian belief update) of the agent to better account for observations (64).

Our expectations crucially depend on the true states of the world which cause our sensory observations, true states which are hidden from us and are thus only represented in the generative model as probabilistic beliefs. Their update demands probabilistic, or Bayesian, inference, which is not always objectively accurate. This is because of discrepancies between the person’s generative model, which may not correspond to the true process generating their observations, and which we postulate is important in FIAD; and to the limited resources of the human brain resulting in approximate inference.

Importantly, the communication of information within the brain is hierarchical. According to predictive coding theory, each level of the hierarchy informs the level below with expectations regarding observations, based on their most likely (hidden) causes. Different neuronal populations are responsible for encoding predictions (prediction units) and prediction error (prediction error units). Prediction errors are estimated at the lower levels and propagated to the level above, where they are used to adjust beliefs, hence minimising free energy at each consecutive level. This hierarchical structure naturally implies that higher levels encode states operating over longer time-scales than lower levels, which will become important when we reconsider the computational neuroanatomy of FIAD below.

Central to active inference is the concept of precision, which is very useful to understand FIAD. Precision, or inverse variance, can be interpreted as the confidence of the model in its prior beliefs and sensory observations, which determines their relative contributions to the updated beliefs resulting from the comparison between (top-down) expectation and (bottom-up) sensory evidence. Highly precise (prior) beliefs, or very uncertain sensory evidence, will require the brain to sample more evidence to shift its beliefs. On the other hand, great confidence in the sensory evidence (or very imprecise prior beliefs) would result in a substantial update of expectations even in the presence of sparce new evidence. In this case, the prediction error would be mostly driven by sensory information (284). It has been proposed that superficial cortical pyramidal cells encode the precision of the prediction error via synaptic gain.

Discussion

Functional disorders of awareness lacking a unifying theoretical framework, resulting in fragmented clinical understanding and management guidelines. A unifying theory of FIAD, based their functional neuroanatomy reviewed above, would be beneficial for clinicians and patients alike. Moreover, the symptomatology associated with the condition poses challenging theoretical questions in neuroscience that, if answered, would increase our understanding of the basic physiological mechanisms of awareness and elucidate its pathology (284287).

Actively inferring to be un-aware

Building on the work of Edwards et al. (64), we hypothesise that FIAD are generated by internal models of disease compatible with the clinical presentation, often shaped by adversity, which guide the gating of attention and hence, of awareness. In the case of RS, the adversity in question is severe and concerns both the index patient and their family. The models of disease consist of illness-compatible prior beliefs, which in the ill state are endowed with excessive precision by attention. Aberrant attentional processes can be caused by learning-based alterations of synaptic connectivity and/or by a primary predisposition or « preparedness » to attentional biases. In what follows, we describe how Bayesian brain theory, and specifically the active inference framework (AI), maps this attentional theory into a biologically plausible hypothesis explaining FIAD. This is likely to be helpful to direct future research, is in principle testable, and helps to de-stigmatise these conditions.

From the point of view of AI, any illness consists of inferring a state of harm (e.g., a bacterial infection) and the optimal actions (e.g., inflammation), on the basis of a generative model of the world, as we saw. This model is equipped with different types of prior beliefs (priors) which play complementary roles in modulating behaviour. Aberrant priors and their modulation can result in « unwarranted symptoms ». A key example of such priors are the beliefs over hidden states, which will cause sensory observations (the state-to-observation map is the « likelihood matrix » in AI). These are particularly relevant in neuropsychiatric conditions, for example « I experience palpitations (observation) hence a heart attack is likely (prior in panic disorder) ».

The patient’s model of themselves may contribute to symptomatology. If the purpose of the generative model is to explain the hidden (not obvious) origins of sensory input, then the patient’s own perceptions must have their underlying cause inferred. In certain situations, a state of ongoing suffering could be best explained by inferring that « I » (as the suffering entity) « am in a patient » (as in a sick) « state ». In the case of RS, « I do not experience pain » would include both « this pinprick is not disturbing » and « my sickness disturbs nociception » as components of the patient’s family’s generative model. To eliminate prediction errors caused by the expectation « I am sick » and the sensory evidence of my biological processes, the patient’s behaviour would conform to the concept of disease. This self-fulfilling prophecy would normally mediate recovery (e.g., « exhausted people rest »), but here may explain the progression of the symptoms, which are often outside conscious control.

Within the paradigm of AI, modulation of the strength of the likelihood matrix compared to the priors weighs the impact of one’s observations, and is hence regarded as attention control. Consequently, different symptomatic behaviours might emerge as Bayesian optimal for a model with aberrant priors depending on their domain and their modulation, positive or negative modulation, making them stronger or weaker. In the case of FIAD, both psychological and physical events may play important precipitating roles in predisposed individuals, namely those whose model of the world is particularly prone to extreme modulation of precision. Here, the brain’s hierarchical organisation, in which prediction at higher levels (empirical priors) infers both current happenings and most proper answers at lower levels, is crucial. If exposure to inescapable trauma, for instance, has strengthened the connection between interoceptive correlates of threat and a very precise prior belief that sensory evidence is useless (uninformative - what to do), this would make it optimal for the patient to reduce his or her sensory precision, and consequently level of awareness of sensory evidence.

Suppose, for instance, a patient with RS is endowed with highly precise prior beliefs that his current status prevents the occurrence of suffering, which would follow from responding to new information. Hypothetically, the precision of such beliefs could be learnt by an excess of aversive evidence. As common (yet anecdotal) example, RS sufferers are often the member of the family that has translated for their family in their distressing dealings with immigration authorities. Such learning could establish a precise expectation of the world’s state, and a lack of confidence (i.e., precision) over the ability of fresh sensory input to give contradictory evidence. In this setting, even random events may be classed as confirmatory signals, which would maintain inaccurate beliefs by bolstering earlier expectations. Thus, basic symptoms of RS, including weakness, social withdrawal, and cognitive disengagement, may reflect very accurate prior beliefs regarding the (lack of) usefulness of any action in reaching a desirable end. In other words, the cost of any action exceeds its utility if it does not contribute to the resolution of uncertainty (cf. « learned helplessness ») (269). In computational terms, it is possible that this process could not only involve aberrant priors’ modulation over likelihood matrix (mapping between hidden states and sensory evidence), but also over policies (actions) and transition matrix (transition between states).

At the present state of knowledge, different hypotheses, involving different « pathological » model parameters, could potentially provide different causative explanations for similar symptomatologies. The power of a computational approach to the study of FIAD is the ability to clearly formulate and explore such hypotheses with in-silico models of the disorder, which can be consequently tested with experimental paradigms. Model fitting and Bayesian model comparison are powerful tools to test contrasting hypotheses and identify the level of the hierarchy and the neuronal populations (encoding different priors and prediction errors) most likely responsible for different conditions.

Possible brain substrates for neural computations in FIAD

Reconnecting this theoretical framework to experimental evidence on FIAD, we note that one of the functions of the attentional network is to implement cognitive control over long-term (cognitively complex) goals, which we can now associate with higher precision of prediction from PFC, over immediate (less cognitive demanding) ones. The aforementioned anterior cingulate cortex and posterior parietal cortex are hypothesised to modulate the relative precision of prefrontal cortex (PFC) at the higher level of the brain hierarchy, as well as of lower-level areas. Once PFC top-down predictions are established, long-term goals are implemented by contextualising or downregulating lower-level short-term goals.

The increased connectivity between vmPFC and top-down inhibition of the amygdala, important for prioritising long-term planning (209), would also speak to top-down control, where stronger predictions from the higher levels of the hierarchy, working at longer temporal scales, would over-rule lower-level predictions, associated on faster time scales. This implies that long-term policies (associated with long-term gain or optimal behaviours), would result in maladaptive « here and now » symptoms.

In an unpredictable context, where the prediction error from sensory input cannot be easily reconciliated with one’s priors (in repeated trauma, unforeseen circumstances, lack of evidence that one’s actions lead to positive change etc), the possible conflict resolution role of the IFC might become predominant. We might also speculate that, in the presence of precise higher-level priors and imprecise (conflictual) sensory evidence, the IFC might bias posterior beliefs, and hence conscious experience, towards the upregulated priors.

Moreover, in active inference terms, the « probability of threat », encoded by the PAG, would represent the prior expectation over the hidden state « threat » causing sensory evidence. This is plausible especially once we consider the typical persistence of FIAD symptoms even when the contingent threat is not present anymore. Highly precise priors can guide behaviour even in the absence of the supporting sensory evidence, whilst a precise (prior) expectation of threat requires more contradictory sensory evidence, over a long time, to be updated from high to low probability of threat.

Finally, it is likely that interoceptive biases lead to altered awareness. As we saw, inappropriate interoceptive attention may disproportionately alter the weighting of top-down or bottom-up information streams, resulting in abnormally amplified or reduced sensory perceptions (i.e., diminished visual, auditory, skin sensitivity, or impaired consciousness) (263, 274). Within a predictive coding explanation of interoception, sensory regions transmit ascending prediction errors, which are compared with descending predictions across a hierarchy of perceptual processing (284). Interoception has been extensively discussed as a component of the predictive brain that sustains homeostasis (197). The bottom-up interoceptive prediction error would pass information about the need for actions (such as « eat ») to maintain the body homeostatic needs. In patients with RS, loss of weight and lack of active food intake, would suggest the presence of depressive modulation of the interoceptive bottom-up prediction error, overtaken by higher-level precise predictions.

Clinical implications for the diagnostic formulation of FIAD

A computational neuroscience perspective on FIAD has important preliminary implications for the clinical understanding, diagnostic formulation, and planning optimal data collection to ensure future progress. Many functional neurological conditions are diagnosed and treated relatively successfully, but this cannot be said for the more severe conditions, especially RS (28, 240). Explicitly hypothesising that RS is a form of severe FND goes further than seeing it a culture-bound syndrome, which easily underplays its neurobiological and clinical significance, and further than purely somatic characterizations such as « state », which can underplay its psychology and sociology.

First, it is imperative for the phenomenology of RS and related FIAD to be recorded as meticulously as the somatic features have been. Fragmentary accounts indicate that subjective experiences leading into and out of RS are comparable with those of other severe dissociative states, such as derealisation-depersonalisation and NES. This includes selective abolishment of aspects of awareness, and relative preservation of others, esp. auditory, dream-like states and subjective feelings of safety-seeking.

Second, it is well recognised that there are multiple paths to each functional neurological disorder, with specific predisposing factors being statistically important but by no means universal or diagnostic, so no single pathway needs to be identified. As a key example, childhood and recent trauma is a well-recognised risk factor, but it is neither necessary or sufficient (162). Health professionals treating RS and related conditions need to take trauma very seriously, without assuming that it is necessary. If substantial trauma is present, the presence of RS would signify a great vulnerability of the individual to further traumatization.

Third, a computational perspective is highly consistent with models of illness structuring FND, and particularly severe FIAD like RS. In pain and depression research, it is well established that expectations (i.e., placebo antidepressant or open-label analgesic placebo; placebo antidepressant) recruit complex neural systems substantially overlapping with the respective biological interventions. These « models of illness » may be partly innate, similar to « panic without panic » in NES, and partly learnt from the subculture and from personal experience (cf. NES in people with a family or personal history of epilepsy). It is common for severe FND to worsen with time over months and years, thanks to the vicious cycles of inference described above (cf deconditioning theory).

We propose that RS is a variant of Trance disorder of DSM-5 and ICD-11, in that it is also characterised by « a marked alteration in the individual’s state of consciousness or a loss of the individuals’ customary sense of personal identity in which the individual experiences a narrowing of awareness of immediate surroundings or unusually narrow and selective focusing on environmental stimuli ». This dovetails perfectly with the central role of the regulation of attention that the « models of illness » above entail. However, RS would be differentiated by the absence of characteristic motor patterns found in Trance disorders. Yet, a neurocomputational view of FIAD would argue against trying to precisely separate such clinical syndromes from each other, as the learnt component of FND is certain to vary the presentation. For example, our view would relativise the distinction between culturally adaptive vs. maladaptive FIAD, as the major classifications now attempt to do for trance and related disorders.

RS would then be a confluence of the dissociative, innate defensive and autonomic/interoceptive activation factors listed by Koslowska et al. (29) and summarised in Table 1 below. A key point is the overlap between the models proposed by these workers, whilst these categories provide structure for clinically assessing patients with suspected severe FIAD. Of course, serious cerebral malfunction as found in minimally conscious or persistent vegetative states due to macroscopic brain insults need to be excluded. Social, psychological, biological factors, and comorbidities should be documented and a biopsychosocial framework adopted since pathophysiology occurs in a body that cannot be considered an isolated system, as we have seen regarding the acquisition of prior beliefs. The five clinical models described by Koslowska et al. (29) constitute hypotheses for biological components of this biopsychosocial approach. Detailed clinical documentation and research need to map their features in severe FIAD, and pharmacotherapeutic trials may help distinguish dissociative from catatonia-like patients, when a difference is indeed found. Prefrontal cortical function should be decreased in the latter and increased in the former, with altered neurotransmitters of the opioid and GABA systems, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Five neurobiologically informed clinical models following the work of Kozlowska et al. (29).

Clinically, the proposed Bayesian-brain framework can also form the basis for a collaborative, psychoeducational model of illness that can be used to help families with members with severe FIAD.

This is because first, it makes it easy to accept and validate these conditions as a real, severe and brain-based, whilst at the same time encouraging agency. Second, and drawing from the best of FND care, it may replace an approach whereby clinicians seek to manipulate patients back into health, and patients’ families attempt to manage clinicians, with a truly collaborative one. Third, such collaborative care means that the family (or more generally, the social system) provides the patient with an environment within which they can actively infer their actions.

Recommendations for future research

This review aimed to provide a more coherent framework for FIAD, and Resignation Syndrome in particular. Overall, we found that research evidence in the field is still at its early stage. Whilst FND with motor symptoms have received increasing scientific interest over the last two decades, experimental research on FIAD is limited. A closer look on the available results found some evidence for the involvement of the emotional processing, attention and interoception networks in FIAD, with some conflicting results. Such conflicts may be the result of small sample numbers, variations in FIAD subtypes, methodological or task discrepancies, and indeed lack of international guidelines in identifying different conditions with overlapping symptoms. Further research is needed to advance the field with reproducible evidence. Further studies may focus on specific signs and symptoms of lack of awareness or responsivity, with attention to the differences between acute and chronic symptoms, as well as between current and remitted symptoms. Both basic neuroscience and clinical research play a fundamental role in advancing our understanding of FIAD.

Crucially, we propose a conceptual model of how the active inference framework may be used to study these conditions. It is of particular interest how different areas and networks found to play a role in FIAD naturally fit with predictive coding hypotheses, which can be computationally modelled and simulated, and finally experimentally tested. Aspects such as the role of learning via stress and trauma, attention modulation, functional and effective connectivity, and the computational role of different levels of brain hierarchy need to be further investigated. In future accounts, we envisage that the family-social system can be included meaningfully in such simulations, providing insight into the inter-personal development, persistence and purposes of shared models of disorder.

Understanding the similarities and variations between various presentation of FIAD may lead to the development of alternative treatments, management and prevention.

Concluding remarks

Although the last decade has seen an increase of material on the motor symptoms of FND, a similar systematic approach to the impaired awareness symptoms of FND is sparse. We have proposed, however, that computational approaches to the study of the brain, and Bayesian hierarchical accounts in particular, could be beneficial to inform, conceptualise and guide future research on FIAD. Consistently with previous Bayesian accounts of FND and with the available experimental evidence on FIAD, we proposed that attentional processes and top-down control mechanisms might be particularly important for the aetiopathogenesis and evolution of symptoms in FIAD. Additionally, we have maintained that psychological trauma and social stressors are important factor, although not always necessarily required to induce FIAD symptoms. In clinical practice, increasing scientific interest, data availability and structured assessments throughout the diagnostic and treatment process would be fundamental to offer clinicians a solid understanding of the patients’ symptomatology, based on individual models of the world. This could guide the development of customise treatments.

Methods

We performed a comprehensive literature search in order to ascertain neuroimaging studies, clinical reports and proposed theoretical models to contextualise the neurobiology of FIAD within social, cultural, and psychological perspectives. A PRISMA flow diagram was used to structure this search (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. PRISMA flow diagram.

Our PUBMED search covered the years 2010–2022 with the extraction of articles according to the following selection criteria: hysteria, functional neurological disorders, and conversion disorder. This choice was made to follow the narrative of the most updated review of the field published in 2012 by Edwards et al. (64).

The search was first performed with the text string ‘hysteria’ (3,469 extracted records); a second time, the text string ‘functional neurological disorders’ was used (12,442 extracted records); a third time, the text string ‘conversion disorder’ (3,000 extracted records), for a total of 18,911 selected records.

The English language filter was introduced, and the search was repeated with the same text strings as in Step 1: once with the text string ‘hysteria’ (3,221 extracted records); a second time with the text string ‘functional neurological disorders’ (12,059 extracted records); a third time with the text string ‘conversion disorder’ (2,842 extracted records), for a total of 18,122 selected records.

The Abstract filter was introduced, and the search was repeated: first, with the text string ‘hysteria’ (2,984 extracted records); second, with the text string ‘functional neurological disorders’ (11,788 extracted records); third, with the text string ‘conversion disorder’ (2,664 extracted records), for a total of 17,436 records drawn.

The Full-text filter was selected, and the search was repeated for the topics of interest: a first time with the text string ‘hysteria’ (2,914 records extracted); a second time with the text string ‘functional neurological disorders’ (11,634 records extracted); a third time with the text string ‘conversion disorder’ (2,610 records extracted), for a total of 17,158 selected records.

We then eliminated duplicate records, i.e., records common to single-text string searches, by typing the text string ‘(hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders) OR (conversion disorder)’. 11,926 non-duplicate records were extracted; thus, 5,232 duplicate articles were discarded.

An attempt was then made to exclude topics not relevant to the field of study: motor, movement, TBI/traumatic brain injury, functional vision disorder, stroke, headache, myopathy, neck/otolaryngology, intestine. To obtain this exclusion of articles, the following text string was used:

‘((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine)’.

6,480 articles were extracted from the database, which resulted in the exclusion of 5,446 articles not related to FIAD. Our choice of excluding motor functional neurological disorders (mFND) was motivated by the aim of focusing on « impaired awareness » signs. However, some relevant papers on mFND for the purpose of our review were manually retrieved.

Of all the articles found with the selection query and filters applied, those concerning the following topics were further selected:

1. lack/alteration/disorder of consciousness (124 records).

2. lack of awareness/impaired awareness (36 records).

3. new theory/theories (57 records).

4. neurobiological theories (14 records).

5. psychological theories (78 records).

6. psychosomatic (94 records).

7. biopsychosocial (20 records).

8. Bayesian (22 records).

9. Nonepileptic (206 records).

10. psychogenic seizures (196 records).

11. derealisation states (2 records).

The 11 groups of articles were extracted by typing the following text strings into the Pubmed search field:

1. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND ((Lack consciousness) OR (alteration consciousness) OR (disorders of consciousness)).

2. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND ((lack awareness) OR (impaired awareness)).

3. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND ((new theory) and (theories)).

4. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND (neurobiological theories).

5. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND (psychological theories).

6. ((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND (psychosomatic).

7. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND (biopsychosocial).

8. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND (bayesian).

9. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND (nonepileptic).

10. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND (psychogenic seizures).

11. (((hysteria) OR (functional neurological disorders)) OR (conversion disorder)) NOT (motor) NOT (movement) NOT (traumatic brain injury) NOT (TBI) NOT (functional vision disorder) NOT (stroke) NOT (headache) NOT (myopathy) NOT (neck) NOT (otolaryngology) NOT (intestine) AND (derealisation states).

Duplicated articles were eliminated, i.e., 443 papers found several times in the searched topic groups.

Although eligible, other full-text articles were excluded as not pertinent, i.e., 146 papers (autism, neurosurgery, vestibular syndrome, intestinal bowel syndrome, caregiver burden, transcriptomic, suicide).

Literature search results

A total of 260 unique articles was selected.

The selected record sets were saved in two types of files. One, in. csv format, included titles and authors’ list, whereas the other, in Pubmed format, contained the articles’ abstract.

We then generated one large. csv file, in which the individual. csv files for the searched article groups were imported and combined. An abstract column was added and abstracts from Pubmed format file were added to the unique. csv file.

The single. csv file was uploaded on Asreview, an open-source machine learning framework, that checks and helps authors’ decisions on paper inclusion for reviews (288).

Citation chaining was also performed. Both backward –we identified and examined references of articles from the original search – and forward – we researched the sources that cited original search articles to find more recent material covering the same topics (289).

The PRISMA diagram (Figure 2) summarises the structure of this systematic review, with a summary of the selected publications.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

BM: design, systematic literature search, selection and interpretation, methodology, visualisation, and writing. MM: conceptualization, methodology, supervision, validation, and writing. LC: conceptualization, design, supervision, validation, and writing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

BM is supported by Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, Pisa, as an honour course student. The Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Neuroimaging is funded by the Wellcome Trust. The Max Planck—University College London Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research is a joint initiative of the Max Planck Society and UCL. MM receives support from the NIHR UCLH Biomedical Research Centre. LC is supported by the Leverhulme Doctoral Training Programme for the Ecological Study of 27 the Brain (DS-2017-026).

Acknowledgments

We want to thank Karl Sallin, Predrag Petrovic, Elisabeth Hultcranz, Katrin Glatz Brubakk, Norman Poole, Rick Adams, Karl Friston for their comments and insights. We also thank the participants in the symposium “Is Resignation Syndrome a Functional Neurological Disorder?,” which took place on the 4th and 5th of November 2022 at the Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, UCL, London.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^The history of « hysteria » per se is often oversimplified, but outside our scope here.

References

1. Teodoro, T, Vilas Boas Garcia, S, Oliveira, R, and Oliveira, JM. No Man's land between psychiatry and neurology: functional neurological disorders and conversion disorder in emergency department settings. Psychiatr Danub. (2022) 34:109–10.

Google Scholar

2. Lehn, A, Gelauff, J, Hoeritzauer, I, Ludwig, L, McWhirter, L, Williams, S, et al. Functional neurological disorders: mechanisms and treatment. J Neurol. (2016) 263:611–20. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7893-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kanaan, RA, Armstrong, D, and Wessely, SC. Neurologists' understanding and management of conversion disorder. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2011) 82:961–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2010.233114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kanaan, RA, Armstrong, D, and Wessely, S. The role of psychiatrists in diagnosing conversion disorder: a mixed-methods analysis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2016) 11:1181–4. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S96330

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Carson, A, and Lehn, A. Epidemiology. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:47–60. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00005-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. NORD (National Organization for Rare Disorders). Functional neurological disorder [internet]. (n.d.) Available at: https://rarediseases.org/rare-diseases/fnd/ Available at: https://rarediseases.org/rare-diseases/fnd/ (Accessed April 19, 2022).

Google Scholar

7. van Beilen, M, Vogt, BA, and Leenders, KL. Increased activation in cingulate cortex in conversion disorder: what does it mean? J Neurol Sci. (2010) 289:155–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Morsy, SK, Aybek, S, Carson, A, Nicholson, TR, Stone, J, Kamal, AM, et al. The relationship between types of life events and the onset of functional neurological (conversion) disorder in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. (2022) 52:401–18. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721004669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Canna, M, and Seligman, R. Dealing with the unknown. Functional neurological disorder (FND) and the conversion of cultural meaning. Soc Sci Med. (2020) 246:112725. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rawlings, GH, Brown, I, Stone, B, and Reuber, M. Written accounts of living with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a thematic analysis. Seizure. (2017) 50:83–91. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2017.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Morgan, LA, Dvorchik, I, Williams, KL, Jarrar, RG, and Buchhalter, JR. Parental ranking of terms describing nonepileptic events. Pediatr Neurol. (2013) 48:378–82. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.12.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Kaplan, MJ, Privitera, MD, and Meziane-Tani, A. Characteristics of men with conversion disorder. Epilepsy Behav. (2021) 114:107556. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2020.107556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Pick, S, Goldstein, LH, Perez, DL, and Nicholson, TR. Emotional processing in functional neurological disorder: a review, biopsychosocial model and research agenda. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2019) 90:704–11. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2018-319201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Nemade, D, Shivkumar, V, Ferguson, P, Singh, J, and Shah, S. Psychosocial and physiologic characteristics of patients with non-epileptic events: a retrospective study. Cureus. (2020) 12:e6767. doi: 10.7759/cureus.6767

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Jimenez, XF, Aboussouan, A, and Johnson, J. Functional neurological disorder responds favorably to interdisciplinary rehabilitation models. Psychosomatics. (2019) 60:556–62. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2019.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Keatley, E, and Molton, I. A shift in approach: assessment and treatment of adults with functional neurological disorder. J Health Serv Psychol. (2022) 48:79–87. doi: 10.1007/s42843-022-00061-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Vassilopoulos, A, Mohammad, S, Dure, L, Kozlowska, K, and Fobian, AD. Treatment approaches for functional neurological disorders in children. Curr Treat Options Neurol. (2022) 24:77–97. doi: 10.1007/s11940-022-00708-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Gillan, A, and Shareef, S. Children with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Pediatr Neurol Briefs. (2019) 31:4. doi: 10.15844/pedneurbriefs-33-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Lopez, MR, and LaFrance, WC. Treatment of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. (2022) 22:467–74. doi: 10.1007/s11910-022-01209-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Baslet, G, Dworetzky, B, Perez, DL, and Oser, M. Treatment of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: updated review and findings from a mindfulness-based intervention case series. Clin EEG Neurosci. (2015) 46:54–64. doi: 10.1177/1550059414557025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Pavlova, MK, Allen, RM, and Dworetzky, BA. Sleep in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and related disorders. Clin EEG Neurosci. (2015) 46:34–41. doi: 10.1177/1550059414560565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Peterson, KT, Kosior, R, Meek, BP, Ng, M, Perez, DL, and Modirrousta, M. Right Temporoparietal junction transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a case series. Psychosomatics. (2018) 59:601–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2018.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Widdess-Walsh, P, Mostacci, B, Tinuper, P, and Devinsky, O. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Handb Clin Neurol. (2012) 107:277–95. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-52898-8.00017-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Duncan, R. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: EEG and investigation. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:305–11. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00027-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed, Washington, DC: Wash American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

26. Aybek, S, and Perez, DL. Diagnosis and management of functional neurological disorder. BMJ. (2022) 24:o64. doi: 10.1136/bmj.o64

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bransfield, RC, and Friedman, KJ. Differentiating psychosomatic, somatopsychic, multisystem illnesses, and medical uncertainty. Healthcare (Basel). (2019) 7:114. doi: 10.3390/healthcare7040114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sallin, K, Evers, K, Jarbin, H, Joelsson, L, and Petrovic, P. Separation and not residency permit restores function in resignation syndrome: a retrospective cohort study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2021). 32:75–86. doi: 10.1007/s00787-021-01833-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kozlowska, K, Scher, S, Helgeland, H, and Carrive, P. Asylum-seeking children in shutdown: neurobiological models. Dev Child Welf. (2021) 3:282–309. doi: 10.1177/25161032211036162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Hyler, SE, and Sussman, N. Somatoform disorders: before and after DSM-III. Hosp Community Psychiatry. (1984) 35:469–78. doi: 10.1176/ps.35.5.469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. von Knorring, AL, and Hultcrantz, E. Asylum-seeking children with resignation syndrome: catatonia or traumatic withdrawal syndrome? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 29:1103–9. doi: 10.1007/s00787-019-01427-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Aronsson, B, Wiberg, C, Sandstedt, P, and Hjern, A. Asylum-seeking children with severe loss of activities of daily living: clinical signs and course during rehabilitation. Acta Paediatr. (2009) 98:1977–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2009.01499.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Schieveld, JNM, and Sallin, K. Pervasive refusal syndrome revisited: a conative disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2021) 30:1–3. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01685-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Sallin, K, Lagercrantz, H, Evers, K, Engström, I, Hjern, A, and Petrovic, P. Resignation syndrome: catatonia? Culture-bound? Front Behav Neurosci. (2016) 29:7. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. O’Sullivan, S. The sleeping beauties [internet]. PANTHEON; (2021). Available at: https://www.penguinrandomhouse.com/books/606597/the-sleeping-beauties-by-suzanne-osullivan/ (Accessed September 19, 2022).

Google Scholar

36. Aviv, R. The trauma of facing deportation, vol. 27, New York, NY (2017).

Google Scholar

37. Ahmed, GK, Elbeh, K, Karim, AA, and Khedr, EM. Case report: catatonia associated with post-traumatic stress disorder. Front Psych. (2021) 7:740436. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.740436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Ahmad, O, and Ahmad, KE. Functional neurological disorders in outpatient practice: an Australian cohort. J Clin Neurosci. (2016) 28:93–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2015.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Jones, M, Gaskell, J, and Lehn, A. Differentiating psychogenic nonepileptic seizures from epileptic seizures: an observational study of patients undergoing video-electroencephalography (VEEG) in Australia. Epilepsy Behav. (2021) 114:107542. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2020.107542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Khalili-Mahani, N, Niesters, M, van Osch, MJ, Oitzl, M, Veer, I, de Rooij, M, et al. Ketamine interactions with biomarkers of stress: a randomized placebo-controlled repeated measures resting-state fMRI and PCASL pilot study in healthy men. NeuroImage. (2015) 108:396–409. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Krámská, L, Hrešková, L, Vojtěch, Z, Krámský, D, and Myers, L. Maladaptive emotional regulation in patients diagnosed with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES) compared with healthy volunteers. Seizure. (2020) 78:7–11. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2020.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Zwi, K, Sealy, L, Samir, N, Hu, N, Rostami, R, Agrawal, R, et al. Asylum seeking children and adolescents in Australian immigration detention on Nauru: a longitudinal cohort study. BMJ Paediatr Open. (2020) 4:e000615. doi: 10.1136/bmjpo-2019-000615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Australia’s man-made crisis on Nauru [Internet]. (2018). Available at: https://www.refugeecouncil.org.au/nauru-report/2/(Accessed September 19, 2022).

Google Scholar

44. Child Trauma on Nauru – The Facts [Internet]. Asylum seeker resource Centre. (2018). Available at: https://asrc.org.au/2018/08/28/child-trauma-on-nauru-the-facts/ (Accessed September 19, 2022).

Google Scholar

45. Newman, L, O'Connor, B, Reynolds, V, and Newhouse, G. Pervasive refusal syndrome in child asylum seekers on Nauru. Australas Psychiatry. (2020) 28:585–8. doi: 10.1177/1039856220956251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Venegas, MD. Grisi Siknis: a cultural idiom of gender-based violence and structural inequalities in eastern Nicaragua. Transcult Psychiatry. (2022) 59:539–50. doi: 10.1177/13634615221098310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Clinical classification of psychogenic Non-epileptic seizures based on video-EEG analysis and automatic clustering (n.d.). J Neurol Neurosurgery Psychiatry [Internet]. Available at: https://jnnp.bmj.com/content/82/9/955 (Accessed September 20, 2022).

Google Scholar

48. Brown, RJ, and Reuber, M. Towards an integrative theory of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES). Clin Psychol Rev. (2016) 47:55–70. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Arthuis, M, Micoulaud-Franchi, JA, Bartolomei, F, McGonigal, A, and Guedj, E. Resting cortical PET metabolic changes in psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES). J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2015) 86:1106–12. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-309390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Santos Nde, O, Benute, GR, Santiago, A, Marchiori, PE, and Lucia, MC. Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures and psychoanalytical treatment: results. Rev Assoc Med Bras (1992). (2014) 60:577–84. doi: 10.1590/1806-9282.60.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Krishnan, B, Faith, A, Vlachos, I, Roth, A, Williams, K, Noe, K, et al. Resetting of brain dynamics: epileptic versus psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2011) 22:S74–81. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.08.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Villagrán, A, Eldøen, G, Duncan, R, Aaberg, KM, Hofoss, D, and Lossius, MI. Incidence and prevalence of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures in a Norwegian county: a 10-year population-based study. Epilepsia. (2021) 62:1528–35. doi: 10.1111/epi.16949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Asadi-Pooya, AA. Biological underpinnings of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: directions for future research. Neurol Sci. (2016) 37:1033–8. doi: 10.1007/s10072-016-2540-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Edwards, MJ. Neurobiologic theories of functional neurologic disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. (2017) 139:131–7. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00012-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Slocum, RB. Breaking the spell: narrative medicine applications for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures (PNES). Seizure. (2021) 86:96–101. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2021.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Balhara, YP. Culture-bound syndrome: has it found its right niche? Indian J Psychol Med. (2011) 33:210–5. doi: 10.4103/0253-7176.92055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lehn, A, Watson, E, Ryan, EG, Jones, M, Cheah, V, and Dionisio, S. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures treated as epileptic seizures in the emergency department. Epilepsia. (2021) 62:2416–25. doi: 10.1111/epi.17038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Diana, G, Sainsbury, TTJ, and Meyer, MP. Bayesian inference of neuronal assemblies. PLoS Comput Biol. (2019) 15:e1007481. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Xue, W, Bowman, FD, and Kang, J. A Bayesian spatial Model to predict disease status using imaging data from various modalities. Front Neurosci. (2018) 12:184. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Wang, K, Jiang, Z, Yu, X, Shao, Y, Liu, H, Wu, S, et al. Comparative efficacy and safety of traditional Chinese patent medicine for cognitive dysfunction in diabetic cognitive dysfunction: a protocol for systematic review and Bayesian network meta-analysis. Medicine (Baltimore). (2022) 101:e28946. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000028946

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Shankle, WR, Pooley, JP, Steyvers, M, Hara, J, Mangrola, T, Reisberg, B, et al. Relating memory to functional performance in normal aging to dementia using hierarchical Bayesian cognitive processing models. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2013) 27:16–22. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31824d5668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Chen, R, and Herskovits, E. Bayesian predictive modeling based on multidimensional connectivity profiling. Neuroradiol J. (2015) 28:5–11. doi: 10.15274/NRJ-2014-10111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Jiang, X, Zhou, Y, Zhang, Y, Zhang, L, Qiao, L, and De Leone, R. Estimating high-order brain functional networks in Bayesian view for autism Spectrum disorder identification. Front Neurosci. (2022) 27:872848. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.872848

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Edwards, MJ, Adams, RA, Brown, H, Pareés, I, and Friston, KJ. A Bayesian account of 'hysteria'. Brain. (2012) 135:3495–512. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Brown, H, Adams, RA, Parees, I, Edwards, M, and Friston, K. Active inference, sensory attenuation and illusions. Cogn Process. (2013) 14:411–27. doi: 10.1007/s10339-013-0571-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Reuber, M, and Brown, RJ. Understanding psychogenic nonepileptic seizures-phenomenology, semiology and the integrative cognitive Model. Seizure. (2017) 44:199–205. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2016.10.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Bogousslavsky, J. The mysteries of hysteria: a historical perspective. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2020) 32:437–50. doi: 10.1080/09540261.2020.1772731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. van der Kruijs, SJ, Vonck, KE, Langereis, GR, Feijs, LM, Bodde, NM, Lazeron, RH, et al. Autonomic nervous system functioning associated with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: analysis of heart rate variability. Epilepsy Behav. (2016) 54:14–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Galli, S, Tatu, L, Bogousslavsky, J, and Aybek, S. Conversion, factitious disorder and malingering: a distinct pattern or a continuum? Front Neurol Neurosci. (2018) 42:72–80. doi: 10.1159/000475699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Trimble, M, and Reynolds, EH. A brief history of hysteria: from the ancient to the modern. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:3–10. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00001-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Pearce, JM. Before Charcot. Front Neurol Neurosci. (2014) 35:1–10. doi: 10.1159/000359985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Goetz, CG. Charcot, hysteria, and simulated disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:11–23. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00002-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Gray, C, Calderbank, A, Adewusi, J, Hughes, R, and Reuber, M. Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with functional neurological symptom disorder. J Psychosom Res. (2020) 129:109907. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2019.109907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Roelofs, K, and Pasman, J. Stress, childhood trauma, and cognitive functions in functional neurologic disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:139–55. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00013-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Testa, SM, Krauss, GL, Lesser, RP, and Brandt, J. Stressful life event appraisal and coping in patients with psychogenic seizures and those with epilepsy. Seizure. (2012) 21:282–7. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2012.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Tursich, M, Ros, T, Frewen, PA, Kluetsch, RC, Calhoun, VD, and Lanius, RA. Distinct intrinsic network connectivity patterns of post-traumatic stress disorder symptom clusters. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2015) 132:29–38. doi: 10.1111/acps.12387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Kanaan, RA, Kozlowska, K, and Lehn, A. The regulation of driving with non-epileptic seizures must be made clearer. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2022) 56:117–9. doi: 10.1177/00048674211009621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Gale, SD, and Hill, SW. Concurrent administration of the MMPI-2 and PAI in a sample of patients with epileptic or non-epileptic seizures: implications for an inpatient epilepsy monitoring unit. Epilepsy Behav. (2012) 25:181–4. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Heyer, GL. Atypical prodromal symptoms help to distinguish patients with psychogenic Nonsyncopal collapse among youth referred for fainting. Pediatr Neurol. (2019) 95:67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2019.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. van der Hart, O, and Horst, R. The dissociation theory of Pierre Janet. J Trauma Stress. (1989) 2:397–412.

Google Scholar

81. Janet The major symptoms of hysteria: Fifteen lectures– Google scholar [Internet]. Available at: https://scholar.google.com/scholar_lookup?&title=The%20Major%20Symptoms%20of%20Hysteria&publication_year=1907&author=Janet%2CP (Accessed September 22, 2022).

Google Scholar

82. Tasca, C, Rapetti, M, Carta, MG, and Fadda, B. Women and hysteria in the history of mental health. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. (2012) 8:110–9. doi: 10.2174/1745017901208010110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Jung, CG. The Freudian theory of Hysteria1 In:. Freud and Psychoanalysis, Abingdon, Oxfordshire: Routledge. (1961)

Google Scholar

84. Kanaan, RAA. Freud's hysteria and its legacy. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:37–44. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00004-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Pérez-Rincón, H. Pierre Janet, Sigmund Freud and Charcot's psychological and psychiatric legacy. Front Neurol Neurosci. (2011) 29:115–24. doi: 10.1159/000321781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Glucksman, ML. Freud's "project": the mind-brain connection revisited. Psychodynamic Psychiatry. (2016) 44:69–90. doi: 10.1521/pdps.2016.44.1.69

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Tran The, J, Magistretti, P, and Ansermet, F. The epistemological foundations of freud's energetics model. Front Psychol. (2018) 11:1861. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01861

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Breuer, J, and Freud, S. Studies on Hysteria. UK: Hachette (2009). 383 p.

Google Scholar

89. Bogousslavsky, J, and Dieguez, S. Sigmund Freud and hysteria: the etiology of psychoanalysis? Front Neurol Neurosci. (2014) 35:109–25. doi: 10.1159/000360244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Kretschmer, E. Hysteria, reflex, and instinct, vol. vii. Oxford, England: Philosophical Library (1960). 162 p.

Google Scholar

91. Kretschmer, E. Hysteria, reflex, and instinct. Acad Med. (1960) 35:1069.

Google Scholar

92. Slater, E. Diagnosis of “hysteria”. Br Med J. (1965) 1:1395–9. doi: 10.1136/bmj.1.5447.1395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Stone, J, Warlow, C, Carson, A, and Sharpe, M. Eliot Slater's myth of the non-existence of hysteria. J R Soc Med. (2005) 98:547–8. doi: 10.1177/014107680509801214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Egloff, N, Maecker, F, Stauber, S, Sabbioni, ME, Tunklova, L, and von Känel, R. Nondermatomal somatosensory deficits in chronic pain patients: are they really hysterical? Pain. (2012) 153:1847–51. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Brownstein, G. The road to functional neurological disorder [internet] Proto Magazine (2022). Available at: (https://protomag.com/neurology/the-road-to-functional-neurological-disorder/) (Accessed October 29, 2022).

Google Scholar

96. Scull, A, and Scull, A. Hysteria: The disturbing history. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press (2011). 240 p.

Google Scholar

97. Whitlock, FA. The aetiology of hysteria. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (1967) 43:144–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1967.tb11023.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Gordon, E, Kraiuhin, C, Kelly, P, and Meares, R. The development of hysteria as a psychiatric concept. Compr Psychiatry. (1984) 25:532–7. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(84)90053-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Tolchin, B, Dworetzky, BA, and Baslet, G. Long-term adherence with psychiatric treatment among patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia. (2018) 59:e18–22. doi: 10.1111/epi.13969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Mark, VW. Functional neurological disorder: extending the diagnosis to other disorders, and proposing an alternate disease term-Attentionally-modifiable disorder. NeuroRehabilitation. (2022) 50:179–207. doi: 10.3233/NRE-228003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Pfennig, A, Leopold, K, Martini, J, Boehme, A, Lambert, M, Stamm, T, et al. Improving early recognition and intervention in people at increased risk for the development of bipolar disorder: study protocol of a prospective-longitudinal, naturalistic cohort study (early-BipoLife). Int J Bipolar Disord. (2020) 8:22. doi: 10.1186/s40345-020-00183-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Ludwig, AM. Hysteria: a neurobiological theory. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1972) 27:771–7. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1972.01750300043007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Halliday, AM. Computing techniques in neurological diagnosis. Br Med Bull. (1968) 24:253–9. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a070646

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Behrman, J, and Levy, R. Neurophysiological studies on patients with hysterical disturbances of vision. J Psychosom Res. (1970) 14:187–94. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(70)90029-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. AM Levine, J. A controlled comparison Ludwig of five brief treatment techniques employing LSD, hypnosis, and psychotherapy. Am J Psychother. (1965) 19:417–35. doi: 10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.1965.19.3.417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Hysteria : The history of a disease/by Ilza Veith. [internet]. Wellcome collection (n.d.). Available from: https://wellcomecollection.org/works/ngrmv5xu (Accessed September 22, 2022). 431:1965.

Google Scholar

107. Loewenberger, A, Davies, K, Agrawal, N, Poole, N, and Cope, SR. What do patients prefer their functional seizures to be called, and what are their experiences of diagnosis? – a mixed methods investigation. Epilepsy Behav. (2021) 117:107817. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2021.107817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Carlson, P, and Nicholson, PK. Psychological interventions for psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: a meta-analysis. Seizure. (2017) 45:142–50. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2016.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Carson, A, Ludwig, L, and Welch, K. Psychologic theories in functional neurologic disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:105–20. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00010-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Milán-Tomás, Á, Persyko, M, Del Campo, M, Shapiro, CM, and Farcnik, K. An overview of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: etiology, diagnosis and management. Can J Neurol Sci. (2018) 45:130–6. doi: 10.1017/cjn.2017.283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Kaplan, MJ, Dwivedi, AK, Privitera, MD, Isaacs, K, Hughes, C, and Bowman, M. Comparisons of childhood trauma, alexithymia, and defensive styles in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures vs. epilepsy: implications for the etiology of conversion disorder. J Psychosom Res. (2013) 75:142–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Bodde, NM, Bartelet, DC, Ploegmakers, M, Lazeron, RH, Aldenkamp, AP, and Boon, PA. MMPI-II personality profiles of patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2011) 20:674–80. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.01.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Whitehead, K, Kandler, R, and Reuber, M. Patients' and neurologists' perception of epilepsy and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia. (2013) 54:708–17. doi: 10.1111/epi.12087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Dworetzky, BA, and Baslet, G. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures in women. Semin Neurol. (2017) 37:624–31. doi: 10.1055/s-0037-1607971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Nightscales, R, McCartney, L, Auvrez, C, Tao, G, Barnard, S, Malpas, CB, et al. Mortality in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Neurology. (2020) 95:e643–52. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000009855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Asadi-Pooya, AA, Emami, M, and Emami, Y. Gender differences in manifestations of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures in Iran. J Neurol Sci. (2013) 332:66–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2013.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Rawlings, GH, and Reuber, M. Health care practitioners' perceptions of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a systematic review of qualitative and quantitative studies. Epilepsia. (2018) 59:1109–23. doi: 10.1111/epi.14189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Yalçın, M, Tellioğlu, E, Gündüz, A, Özmen, M, Yeni, N, Özkara, Ç, et al. Orienting reaction may help recognition of patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Neurophysiol Clin. (2017) 47:231–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2017.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Raj, V, Rowe, AA, Fleisch, SB, Paranjape, SY, Arain, AM, and Nicolson, SE. Psychogenic pseudosyncope: diagnosis and management. Auton Neurosci. (2014) 184:66–72. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2014.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Voon, V, Cavanna, AE, Coburn, K, Sampson, S, Reeve, A, LaFrance, WC Jr, et al. Functional neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of functional neurological disorders (conversion disorder). J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2016) 28:168–90. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.14090217

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Linden, M, Linden, U, and Schwantes, U. Disability and functional burden of disease because of mental in comparison to somatic disorders in general practice patients. Eur Psychiatry. (2015) 30:789–92. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Dworetzky, BA, Weisholtz, DS, Perez, DL, and Baslet, G. A clinically oriented perspective on psychogenic nonepileptic seizure-related emergencies. Clin EEG Neurosci. (2015) 46:26–33. doi: 10.1177/1550059414566880

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Karakis, I, Janocko, NJ, Morton, ML, Groover, O, Teagarden, DL, Villarreal, HK, et al. Stigma in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2020) 111:107269. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2020.107269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Kirmani, BF. Importance of video-EEG monitoring in the diagnosis of epilepsy in a psychiatric patient. Case Rep Neurol Med. (2013) 2013:159842. doi: 10.1155/2013/159842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Monzoni, CM, Duncan, R, Grünewald, R, and Reuber, M. How do neurologists discuss functional symptoms with their patients: a conversation analytic study. J Psychosom Res. (2011) 71:377–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Dekker, MCJ, Urasa, SJ, Kellogg, M, and Howlett, WP. Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures among patients with functional neurological disorder: a case series from a Tanzanian referral hospital and literature review. Epilepsia Open. (2018) 3:66–72. doi: 10.1002/epi4.12096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Carter, A, Denton, A, Ladino, LD, Hassan, I, Sawchuk, T, Snyder, T, et al. Tellez-Zenteno JF; PNES Canada group. Experience of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures in the Canadian league against epilepsy: a survey describing current practices by neurologists and epileptologists. Seizure. (2018) 61:227–33. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2018.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Jafari, A, Rezaei Tavirani, M, Parvareshi Hamrah, M, Ahmadi Karvigh, S, and Bashi Zadeh Fakhar, H. Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures; a narrative review. Arch Acad Emerg Med. (2020) 8:e10. doi: 10.22037/aaem.v8i1.552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Chen, DK, Sharma, E, and LaFrance, WC Jr. Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. (2017) 17:71. doi: 10.1007/s11910-017-0781-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Brown, RJ, Bouska, JF, Frow, A, Kirkby, A, Baker, GA, Kemp, S, et al. Emotional dysregulation, alexithymia, and attachment in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2013) 29:178–83. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Asadi-Pooya, AA. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a concise review. Neurol Sci. (2017) 38:935–40. doi: 10.1007/s10072-017-2887-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Bodde, NM, van der Kruijs, SJ, Ijff, DM, Lazeron, RH, Vonck, KE, Boon, PA, et al. Subgroup classification in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2013) 26:279–89. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Scévola, L, Wolfzun, C, Sarudiansky, M, Pico, MMA, Ponieman, M, Stivala, EG, et al. Psychiatric disorders, depression and quality of life in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures and drug resistant epilepsy living in Argentina. Seizure. (2021) 92:174–81. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2021.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Rawlings, GH, and Reuber, M. What patients say about living with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a systematic synthesis of qualitative studies. Seizure. (2016) 41:100–11. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2016.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Pretorius, C, and Sparrow, M. Life after being diagnosed with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES): a south African perspective. Seizure. (2015) 30:32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2015.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Gulpek, D, Kelemence Kaplan, F, Kesebir, S, and Bora, O. Alexithymia in patients with conversion disorder. Nord J Psychiatry. (2014) 68:300–5. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2013.814711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Maa, E, Applegate, M, and Keniston, A. Auricular acupuncture for the treatment of nonepileptic seizures: a pilot study. Epilepsy Behav. (2020) 111:107329. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2020.107329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Marfeo, EE, Ni, P, Haley, SM, Jette, AM, Bogusz, K, Meterko, M, et al. Development of an instrument to measure behavioral health function for work disability: item pool construction and factor analysis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2013) 94:1670–8. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2013.03.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Pustilnik, AC. Legal evidence of subjective states: a brain-based Model of chronic pain increases accuracy and fairness in law. Harv Rev Psychiatry. (2017) 25:279–88. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Dickinson, P, and Looper, KJ. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a current overview. Epilepsia. (2012) 53:1679–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03606.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Raucher-Chéné, D, Thibaudeau, E, Sauvé, G, Lavigne, KM, and Lepage, M. Understanding others as a mediator between verbal memory and negative symptoms in schizophrenia-spectrum disorder. J Psychiatr Res. (2021) 143:429–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Deserno, L, Heinz, A, and Schlagenhauf, F. Computational approaches to schizophrenia: a perspective on negative symptoms. Schizophr Res. (2017) 186:46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Ruschil, V, Mazurak, N, Hofmann, M, Loskutova, E, Enck, P, Freilinger, T, et al. Decreased autonomic reactivity and psychiatric comorbidities in neurological patients with medically unexplained sensory symptoms: a case-control study. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:713391. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.713391

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Baslet, G, Bajestan, SN, Aybek, S, Modirrousta, M, Clin Psy JP, D, Cavanna, A, et al. Evidence-based practice for the clinical assessment of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a report from the American neuropsychiatric association committee on research. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2021) 33:27–42. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19120354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Lenio, S, Baker, S, Watson, M, Libbon, R, Sillau, S, and Strom, L. Assessing the hidden burden of psychiatric disease in patients with nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2021) 125:108382. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2021.108382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Baroni, G, Martins, WA, Piccinini, V, da Rosa, MP, de Paola, L, Paglioli, E, et al. Neuropsychiatric features of the coexistence of epilepsy and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. J Psychosom Res. (2018) 111:83–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Diprose, W, Sundram, F, and Menkes, DB. Psychiatric comorbidity in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures compared with epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. (2016) 56:123–30. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.12.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Salinsky, M, Evrard, C, Storzbach, D, and Pugh, MJ. Psychiatric comorbidity in veterans with psychogenic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2012) 25:345–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Moldovan, R, Radu, M, Băban, A, and Dumitraşcu, DL. Evolution of psychosomatic diagnosis in DSM. Historical perspectives and new development for internists. Rom J Intern Med. (2015) 53:25–30. doi: 10.1515/rjim-2015-0003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Cosci, F, and Fava, GA. The clinical inadequacy of the DSM-5 classification of somatic symptom and related disorders: an alternative trans-diagnostic model. CNS Spectr. (2016) 21:310–7. doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Porcelli, P, and Rafanelli, C. Criteria for psychosomatic research (DCPR) in the medical setting. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2010) 12:246–54. doi: 10.1007/s11920-010-0104-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Porcelli, P, and Todarello, O. Psychological factors in medical disorders assessed with the diagnostic criteria for psychosomatic research. Adv Psychosom Med. (2012) 32:108–17. doi: 10.1159/000330009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Paredes-Echeverri, S, Maggio, J, Bègue, I, Pick, S, Nicholson, TR, and Perez, DL. Autonomic, endocrine, and inflammation profiles in functional neurological disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2022) 34:30–43. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.21010025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Di Perri, C, Stender, J, Laureys, S, and Gosseries, O. Functional neuroanatomy of disorders of consciousness. Epilepsy Behav. (2014) 30:28–32. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Jaramillo-Jimenez, E, Vargas-Garcia, C, Rodriguez-Marquez, I, Sandoval-Barrios, J, Velez, MA, Alvarez, JF, et al. Psychogenic non-epileptic and epileptic seizures: clues for a differential diagnosis. Findings from a Colombian study. Rev Neurol. (2019) 69:145–51. doi: 10.33588/rn.6904.2018442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Rawlings, GH, Brown, I, and Reuber, M. Narrative analysis of written accounts about living with epileptic or psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Seizure. (2018) 62:59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2018.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Baslet, G, and Prensky, E. Initial treatment retention in psychogenic non-epileptic seizures. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2013) 25:63–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.11090223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Reinsberger, C, Perez, DL, Murphy, MM, and Dworetzky, BA. Pre- and postictal, not ictal, heart rate distinguishes complex partial and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2012) 23:68–70. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Wang, YQ, Wen, Y, Wang, MM, Zhang, YW, and Fang, ZX. Prolactin levels as a criterion to differentiate between psychogenic non-epileptic seizures and epileptic seizures: a systematic review. Epilepsy Res. (2021) 169:106508. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2020.106508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Cretton, A, Brown, RJ, LaFrance, WC Jr, and Aybek, S. What does neuroscience tell us about the conversion Model of functional neurological disorders? J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2020 Winter) 32:24–32. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19040089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Childhood trauma in patients with epileptic versus non-epileptic seizures - Yang - Epilepsia - Wiley online library [internet] (2023). Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/epi.17449?casa_token=r-cq2qMWz4QAAAAA:wn6sU7fWSvLSZA lvZCHmscw3NPuZCzFNyJ3qhcJ0nNTNknaMb5l8wnbVWfRFeSCh9mCXrsjg DBPISvs(Accessed November 1, 2022).

Google Scholar

162. Fang, Z, Li, Y, Xie, L, Cheng, M, Ma, J, Li, T, et al. Characteristics and outcomes of children with dissociative (conversion) disorders in western China: a retrospective study. BMC Psychiatry. (2021) 21:31. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03045-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Kozlowska, K, Palmer, DM, Brown, KJ, McLean, L, Scher, S, Gevirtz, R, et al. Reduction of autonomic regulation in children and adolescents with conversion disorders. Psychosom Med. (2015) 77:356–70. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Ouss, L, and Tordjman, E. Conversive disorders among children and adolescents: towards new "complementarist" paradigms? Neurophysiol Clin. (2014) 44:411–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2014.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Luthy, SK, Moss, AF, Torok, MR, McLeod, L, and Wilson, KM. Characteristics of children hospitalized for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures due to conversion disorder versus epilepsy. Hosp Pediatr. (2018) 8:321–9. doi: 10.1542/hpeds.2017-0103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Makris, G, Papageorgiou, N, Panagopoulos, D, and Brubakk, KG. A diagnostic challenge in an unresponsive refugee child improving with neurosurgery-a case report. Oxf Med Case Reports. (2021) 2021:omab020. doi: 10.1093/omcr/omab020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Sawchuk, T, Buchhalter, J, and Senft, B. Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures in children – psychophysiology & dissociative characteristics. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 294:113544. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Elbers, J, Jaradeh, S, Yeh, AM, and Golianu, B. Wired for threat: clinical features of nervous system dysregulation in 80 children. Pediatr Neurol. (2018) 89:39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2018.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Allott, K, Proffitt, TM, McGorry, PD, Pantelis, C, Wood, SJ, Cumner, M, et al. Clinical neuropsychology within adolescent and young-adult psychiatry: conceptualizing theory and practice. Appl Neuropsychol Child. (2013) 2:47–63. doi: 10.1080/08841233.2012.670566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Szaflarski, JP, and LaFrance, WC Jr. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures (PNES) as a network disorder – evidence from neuroimaging of functional (psychogenic) neurological disorders. Epilepsy Curr. (2018) 18:211–6. doi: 10.5698/1535-7597.18.4.211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Bruno, MA, Gosseries, O, Ledoux, D, Hustinx, R, and Laureys, S. Assessment of consciousness with electrophysiological and neurological imaging techniques. Curr Opin Crit Care. (2011) 17:146–51. doi: 10.1097/MCC.0b013e328343476d

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Radmanesh, M, Jalili, M, and Kozlowska, K. Activation of functional brain networks in children with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures. Front Hum Neurosci. (2020) 25:339. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00339

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Gosseries, O, Pistoia, F, Charland-Verville, V, Carolei, A, Sacco, S, and Laureys, S. The role of neuroimaging techniques in establishing diagnosis, prognosis and therapy in disorders of consciousness. TONIJ. (2016) 10:52–68. doi: 10.2174/1874440001610010052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Brühl, AB, Delsignore, A, Komossa, K, and Weidt, S. Neuroimaging in social anxiety disorder—a meta-analytic review resulting in a new neurofunctional model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 47:260–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. Qureshi, AY, and Stevens, RD. Mapping the unconscious brain: insights from advanced neuroimaging. J Clin Neurophysiol. (2022) 39:12–21. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000846

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Schiff, ND, Nauvel, T, and Victor, JD. Large-scale brain dynamics in disorders of consciousness. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2014) 25:7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

177. Schurz, M, Uddin, LQ, Kanske, P, Lamm, C, Sallet, J, Bernhardt, BC, et al. Variability in brain structure and function reflects lack of peer support. Cereb Cortex. (2021) 31:4612–27. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. Izuno, S, Yoshihara, K, and Sudo, N. Role of gut microbiota in the pathophysiology of stress-related disorders: evidence from neuroimaging studies. Ann Nutr Metab. (2021) 77:4–10. doi: 10.1159/000517420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Andreasen, NC. Linking mind and brain in the study of mental illnesses: a project for a scientific psychopathology. Science. (1997) 275:1586–93. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1586

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Reuber, M, Howlett, S, Khan, A, and Grünewald, RA. Non-epileptic seizures and other functional neurological symptoms: predisposing, precipitating, and perpetuating factors. Psychosomatics. (2007) 48:230–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.48.3.230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Plioplys, S, Doss, J, Siddarth, P, Bursch, B, Falcone, T, Forgey, M, et al. A multisite controlled study of risk factors in pediatric psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia. (2014) 55:1739–47. doi: 10.1111/epi.12773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Myers, L, Gray, C, Roberts, N, Levita, L, and Reuber, M. Shame in the treatment of patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: the elephant in the room. Seizure. (2022) 94:176–82. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2021.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

183. Andrini, HJ, Au Hoy, SL, Okhovat, AM, Lockman, J, and Goldsmith, GR. Functional seizures: the patient's perspective of a diagnostic and treatment odyssey. Epilepsy Behav Rep. (2021) 26:100509. doi: 10.1016/j.ebr.2021.100509

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

184. Srinivasan, TM. Healing altered states of consciousness. Int J Yoga. (2015) 8:87–8. doi: 10.4103/0973-6131.158468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. Riganello, F, Cortese, MD, Dolce, G, Lucca, LF, and Sannita, WG. The autonomic system functional state predicts responsiveness in disorder of consciousness. J Neurotrauma. (2015) 32:1071–7. doi: 10.1089/neu.2014.3539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

186. Pistoia, F, Sacco, S, Franceschini, M, Sarà, M, Pistarini, C, Cazzulani, B, et al. Comorbidities: a key issue in patients with disorders of consciousness. J Neurotrauma. (2015) 32:682–8. doi: 10.1089/neu.2014.3659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Pistoia, F, Sacco, S, Stewart, J, Sarà, M, and Carolei, A. Disorders of consciousness: painless or painful conditions?-evidence from neuroimaging studies. Brain Sci. (2016) 6:47. doi: 10.3390/brainsci6040047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

188. Eddy, CM, and Cavanna, AE. Video-electroencephalography investigation of ictal alterations of consciousness in epilepsy and nonepileptic attack disorder: practical considerations. Epilepsy Behav. (2014) 30:24–7. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

189. Yang, Y, He, Q, Xia, X, Dang, Y, Chen, X, He, J, et al. Long-term functional prognosis and related factors of spinal cord stimulation in patients with disorders of consciousness. CNS Neurosci Ther. (2022) 28:1249–58. doi: 10.1111/cns.13870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

190. Lee, M, Sanz, LRD, Barra, A, Wolff, A, Nieminen, JO, Boly, M, et al. Quantifying arousal and awareness in altered states of consciousness using interpretable deep learning. Nat Commun. (2022) 13:1064. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-28451-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

191. Graham, M, Doherty, CP, and Naci, L. Using neuroimaging to detect covert awareness and determine prognosis of comatose patients: informing surrogate decision makers of individual patient results. Semin Neurol. (2018) 38:555–60. doi: 10.1055/s-0038-1667385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

192. Lane, RD, Hsu, CH, Locke, DE, Ritenbaugh, C, and Stonnington, CM. Role of theory of mind in emotional awareness and alexithymia: implications for conceptualization and measurement. Conscious Cogn. (2015) 33:398–405. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

193. Yang, DY, Rosenblau, G, Keifer, C, and Pelphrey, KA. An integrative neural model of social perception, action observation, and theory of mind. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 51:263–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

194. Robison, RA, Taghva, A, Liu, CY, and Apuzzo, ML. Surgery of the mind, mood, and conscious state: an idea in evolution. World Neurosurg. (2012) 77:662–86. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2012.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

195. Naccache, L. Minimally conscious state or cortically mediated state? Brain. (2018) 141:949–60. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx324

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

196. Wardrope, A, Wong, S, McLaughlan, J, Wolfe, M, Oto, M, and Reuber, M. Peri-ictal responsiveness to the social environment is greater in psychogenic nonepileptic than epileptic seizures. Epilepsia. (2020) 61:758–65. doi: 10.1111/epi.16471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

197. Spiegel, D. Recognizing traumatic dissociation. Am J Psychiatry. (2006) 163:566–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.4.566

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

198. Pick, S, Rojas-Aguiluz, M, Butler, M, Mulrenan, H, Nicholson, TR, and Goldstein, LH. Dissociation and interoception in functional neurological disorder. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2020) 25:294–311. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2020.1791061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

199. Roberts, NA, and Reuber, M. Alterations of consciousness in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: emotion, emotion regulation and dissociation. Epilepsy Behav. (2014) 30:43–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.09.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

200. Koreki, A, Garfkinel, SN, Mula, M, Agrawal, N, Cope, S, Eilon, T, et al. Trait and state interoceptive abnormalities are associated with dissociation and seizure frequency in patients with functional seizures. Epilepsia. (2020) 61:1156–65. doi: 10.1111/epi.16532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

201. Nguyen, ML, Shapiro, MA, Demetree, JM, and White, KA. When non-epileptic seizures fool the experts. Int J Psychiatry Med. (2011) 42:331–8. doi: 10.2190/PM.42.3.g

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

202. Ray, RD, and Zald, DH. Anatomical insights into the interaction of emotion and cognition in the prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2012) 36:479–501. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

203. Seeley, WW. The salience network: a neural system for perceiving and responding to homeostatic demands. J Neurosci. (2019) 39:9878–82. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1138-17.2019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

204. Jiang, Y, Oathes, D, Hush, J, Darnall, B, Charvat, M, Mackey, S, et al. Perturbed connectivity of the amygdala and its subregions with the central executive and default mode networks in chronic pain. Pain. (2016) 157:1970–8. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

205. Aytur, SA, Ray, KL, Meier, SK, Campbell, J, Gendron, B, Waller, N, et al. Neural mechanisms of acceptance and commitment therapy for chronic pain: a network-based fMRI approach. Front Hum Neurosci. (2021) 5:587018. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.587018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

206. Farahani, FV, Karwowski, W, and Lighthall, NR. Application of graph theory for identifying connectivity patterns in human brain networks: a systematic review. Front Neurosci. (2019) 6:585. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00585

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

207. Cai, L, Wei, X, Wang, J, Yi, G, Lu, M, and Dong, Y. Characterization of network switching in disorder of consciousness at multiple time scales. J Neural Eng. (2020) 17:026024. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/ab79f5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

208. Seri, S, Brazzo, D, Thai, NJ, and Cerquiglini, A. Brain mechanisms of altered consciousness in generalised seizures. Behav Neurol. (2011) 24:43–6. doi: 10.3233/BEN-2011-0317

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

209. Drane, DL, Fani, N, Hallett, M, Khalsa, SS, Perez, DL, and Roberts, NA. A framework for understanding the pathophysiology of functional neurological disorder. CNS Spectr. (2020) 4:1–7. doi: 10.1017/S1092852920001789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

210. Lader, M, and Sartorius, N. Anxiety in patients with hysterical conversion symptoms. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1968) 31:490–5. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.31.5.490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

211. Río-Casanova, LD, González, A, Páramo, M, and Brenlla, J. Excitatory and inhibitory conversive experiences: neurobiological features involving positive and negative conversion symptoms. Rev Neurosci. (2016) 27:101–10. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2015-0022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

212. Ertan, D, Aybek, S, LaFrance, WC Jr, Kanemoto, K, Tarrada, A, Maillard, L, et al. Functional (psychogenic non-epileptic/dissociative) seizures: why and how? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2022) 93:144–57. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2021-326708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

213. Popkirov, S, Jungilligens, J, Schlegel, U, and Wellmer, J. Research on dissociative seizures: a bibliometric analysis and visualization of the scientific landscape. Epilepsy Behav. (2018) 83:162–7. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.03.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

214. Pick, S, Mellers, JDC, and Goldstein, LH. Autonomic and subjective responsivity to emotional images in people with dissociative seizures. J Neuropsychol. (2018) 12:341–55. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

215. Wincewicz, K, Sapota-Zaręba, K, and Nasierowski, T. Ganser syndrome – a dissociative disorder or a factitious disorder? A case report. Psychiatr Pol. (2022) 56:63–75. doi: 10.12740/PP/129012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

216. Schmutz, M. Dissociative seizures--a critical review and perspective. Epilepsy Behav. (2013) 29:449–56. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

217. Reuber, M, and Rawlings, GH. Nonepileptic seizures – subjective phenomena. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:283–96. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00025-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

218. Horvath, T, Friedman, J, and Meares, R. Attention in hysteria: a study of Janet's hypothesis by means of habituation and arousal measures. Am J Psychiatry. (1980) 137:217–20. doi: 10.1176/ajp.137.2.217

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

219. Chennu, S, Finoia, P, Kamau, E, Monti, MM, Allanson, J, Pickard, JD, et al. Dissociable endogenous and exogenous attention in disorders of consciousness. Neuroimage Clin. (2013) 16:450–61. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

220. Guidi, J, Fava, GA, Picardi, A, Porcelli, P, Bellomo, A, Grandi, S, et al. Subtyping depression in the medically ill by cluster analysis. J Affect Disord. (2011) 132:383–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

221. Bukh, JD, Andersen, PK, and Kessing, LV. Personality and the long-term outcome of first-episode depression: a prospective 5-year follow-up study. J Clin Psychiatry. (2016) 77:e704–10. doi: 10.4088/JCP.15m09823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

222. Asadi-Pooya, AA, Asadollahi, M, Tinker, J, Nei, M, and Sperling, MR. Post-epilepsy surgery psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia. (2016) 57:1691–6. doi: 10.1111/epi.13513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

223. Noe, KH, Grade, M, Stonnington, CM, Driver-Dunckley, E, and Locke, DE. Confirming psychogenic nonepileptic seizures with video-EEG: sex matters. Epilepsy Behav. (2012) 23:220–3. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

224. Bakvis, P, Spinhoven, P, Giltay, EJ, Kuyk, J, Edelbroek, PM, Zitman, FG, et al. Basal hypercortisolism and trauma in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia. (2010) 51:752–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2009.02394.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

225. Alyanak, B. Epilepsy, psychogenic seizure, trauma in the developmental process. Noro Psikiyatr Ars. (2013) 50:291–4. doi: 10.4274/npa.y6990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

226. Hingray, C, Maillard, L, Hubsch, C, Vignal, JP, Bourgognon, F, Laprevote, V, et al. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: characterization of two distinct patient profiles on the basis of trauma history. Epilepsy Behav. (2011) 22:532–6. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

227. van der Kruijs, SJ, Bodde, NM, Vaessen, MJ, Lazeron, RH, Vonck, K, Boon, P, et al. Functional connectivity of dissociation in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2012) 83:239–47. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2011-300776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

228. Englot, DJ, D'Haese, PF, Konrad, PE, Jacobs, ML, Gore, JC, Abou-Khalil, BW, et al. Functional connectivity disturbances of the ascending reticular activating system in temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2017) 88:925–32. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2017-315732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

229. Kamil, SH, Qureshi, M, and Patel, RS. Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) in psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES): a case report and literature review. Behav Sci (Basel). (2019) 9:15. doi: 10.3390/bs9020015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

230. Sahaya, K, Dholakia, SA, and Sahota, PK. Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: a challenging entity. J Clin Neurosci. (2011) 18:1602–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2011.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

231. Gasparini, S, Beghi, E, Ferlazzo, E, Beghi, M, Belcastro, V, Biermann, KP, et al. Management of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: a multidisciplinary approach. Eur J Neurol. (2019) 26:205–e15. doi: 10.1111/ene.13818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

232. Anzellotti, F, Dono, F, Evangelista, G, Di Pietro, M, Carrarini, C, Russo, M, et al. Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures and pseudo-refractory epilepsy, a management challenge. Front Neurol. (2020) 2:461. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00461

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

233. Zaitsev, AV, Smolensky, IV, Jorratt, P, and Ovsepian, SV. Neurobiology, functions, and relevance of excitatory amino acid transporters (EAATs) to treatment of refractory epilepsy. CNS Drugs. (2020) 34:1089–103. doi: 10.1007/s40263-020-00764-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

234. Diukova, GM, Makarov, SA, Golubev, VL, Tyutina, RR, Degterev, DA, and Danilov, AB. Psychogenic seizure imitating narcolepsy. Case Rep Neurol. (2020) 12:472–81. doi: 10.1159/000510517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

235. Bryant, RA, and Das, P. The neural circuitry of conversion disorder and its recovery. J Abnorm Psychol. (2012) 121:289–96. doi: 10.1037/a0025076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

236. Feinstein, A. Conversion disorder. Continuum (Minneap Minn). (2018) 24:861–72. doi: 10.1212/CON.0000000000000601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

237. Ali, S, Jabeen, S, Pate, RJ, Shahid, M, Chinala, S, Nathani, M, et al. Conversion disorder- mind versus body: a review. Innov Clin Neurosci. (2015) 12:27–33.

Google Scholar

238. Noll-Hussong, M, Otti, A, Wohlschlaeger, AM, Zimmer, C, Henningsen, P, Lahmann, C, et al. Neural correlates of deficits in pain-related affective meaning construction in patients with chronic pain disorder. Psychosom Med. (2013) 75:124–36. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31827e60f3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

239. Kanaan, RA, Craig, TK, Wessely, SC, and David, AS. Imaging repressed memories in motor conversion disorder. Psychosom Med. (2007) 69:202–5. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31802e4297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

240. Aybek, S, Nicholson, TR, O'Daly, O, Zelaya, F, Kanaan, RA, and David, AS. Emotion-motion interactions in conversion disorder: an FMRI study. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0123273. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0123273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

241. Classics in the History of Psychology – James Chapter 11 [Internet]. (1890). Available at: https://psychclassics.yorku.ca/James/Principles/prin11.htm (Accessed September 21, 2022).

Google Scholar

242. Asadi-Pooya, AA, Bahrami, Z, and Homayoun, M. Natural history of patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Seizure. (2019) 66:22–5. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2019.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

243. Duncan, R. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: diagnosis and initial management. Expert Rev Neurother. (2010) 10:1803–9. doi: 10.1586/ern.10.171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

244. Mostacci, B, Bisulli, F, Alvisi, L, Licchetta, L, Baruzzi, A, and Tinuper, P. Ictal characteristics of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: what we have learned from video/EEG recordings--a literature review. Epilepsy Behav. (2011) 22:144–53. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

245. Rosales, R, Dworetzky, B, and Baslet, G. Cognitive-emotion processing in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2020) 102:106639. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.106639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

246. Hommel, B, Chapman, CS, Cisek, P, Neyedli, HF, Song, JH, and Welsh, TN. No one knows what attention is. Atten Percept Psychophys. (2019) 81:2288–303. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01846-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

247. Lindsay, GW. Attention in psychology, neuroscience, and machine learning. Front Comput Neurosci. (2020) 16:29. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2020.00029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

248. Schröger, E, Marzecová, A, and SanMiguel, I. Attention and prediction in human audition: a lesson from cognitive psychophysiology. Eur J Neurosci. (2015) 41:641–64. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12816

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

249. Otasowie, J, Paraiso, A, and Bates, G. Pervasive refusal syndrome: systematic review of case reports. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2021) 30:41–53. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01536-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

250. Dhossche, Dirk, Wachtel, Lee, Goetz, Michal, and Sienaert, Pascal. (2016). Catatonia in psychiatric illnesses, in The Medical Basis of Psychiatry, 4th Ed. Springer New York. 517–535. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-2528-5_27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

251. Naviaux, RK, Naviaux, JC, Li, K, Bright, AT, Alaynick, WA, Wang, L, et al. Metabolic features of chronic fatigue syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2016) 113:E5472–80. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1607571113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

252. Schauer, M, and Elbert, T. Dissociation following traumatic stress etiology and treatment. Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-J Psychol. (2010) 218:109–27. doi: 10.1007/s13312-021-2167-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

253. Ludot, M, Merlo, M, Ibrahim, N, Piot, MA, Lefèvre, H, Carles, ME, et al. « troubles à symptomatologie somatique » chez les adolescents. Une revue systématique ["somatic symptom disorders" in adolescence. A systematic review of the recent literature]. Encéphale. (2021) 47:596–604. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2021.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

254. Kara, A, and Doğan, MT. The psychopathology, depression, and anxiety levels of children and adolescents with vasovagal syncope: a case-control study. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2021) 209:547–51. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000001334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

255. Patel, H, Blake, H, and Dunn, D. Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures in children and adolescents. Indian Pediatr. (2021) 58:259–65.

Google Scholar

256. Brown, RJ, Danquah, AN, Miles, E, Holmes, E, and Poliakoff, E. Attention to the body in nonclinical somatoform dissociation depends on emotional state. J Psychosom Res. (2010) 69:249–57. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

257. Kaplan, MJ. A psychodynamic perspective on treatment of patients with conversion and other somatoform disorders. Psychodynamic Psychiatry. (2014) 42:593–615. doi: 10.1521/pdps.2014.42.4.593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

258. Kienle, J, Rockstroh, B, Bohus, M, Fiess, J, Huffziger, S, and Steffen-Klatt, A. Somatoform dissociation and posttraumatic stress syndrome - two sides of the same medal? A comparison of symptom profiles, trauma history and altered affect regulation between patients with functional neurological symptoms and patients with PTSD. BMC Psychiatry. (2017) 17:248. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1414-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

259. Thomson, K, Randall, E, Ibeziako, P, and Bujoreanu, IS. Somatoform disorders and trauma in medically-admitted children, adolescents, and young adults: prevalence rates and psychosocial characteristics. Psychosomatics. (2014) 55:630–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2014.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

260. Gazit, S, Elkana, O, Dawidowicz, L, Yeshayahu, L, and Biran, I. Downwards vertical attention bias in conversion disorder vs controls: a pilot study. Psychosomatics. (2017) 58:633–42. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2017.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

261. Kozlowska, K, Palmer, DM, Brown, KJ, Scher, S, Chudleigh, C, Davies, F, et al. Conversion disorder in children and adolescents: a disorder of cognitive control. J Neuropsychol. (2015) 9:87–108. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

262. Stager, L, Morriss, S, McKibben, L, Grant, M, Szaflarski, JP, and Fobian, AD. Sense of control, selective attention and cognitive inhibition in pediatric functional seizures: a prospective case-control study. Seizure. (2022) 98:79–86. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2022.03.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

263. Price, CJ, and Hooven, C. Interoceptive Awareness Skills for Emotion Regulation: Theory and Approach of Mindful Awareness in Body-Oriented Therapy (MABT). Front Psychol. (2018) 28:9–798. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

264. Klein, TA, Ullsperger, M, and Danielmeier, C. Error awareness and the insula: links to neurological and psychiatric diseases. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 4:14. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

265. Koubeissi, MZ, Fernandez-Baca Vaca, G, Maciunas, R, and Stephani, C. A white matter tract mediating awareness of speech. Neurology. (2016) 86:177–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

266. Nicholson, C, Edwards, MJ, Carson, AJ, Gardiner, P, Golder, D, Hayward, K, et al. Occupational therapy consensus recommendations for functional neurological disorder. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2020) 91:1037–45. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2019-322281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

267. Baslet, G, Ehlert, A, Oser, M, and Dworetzky, BA. Mindfulness-based therapy for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2020) 103:106534. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.106534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

268. Libbon, R, Gadbaw, J, Watson, M, Rothberg, B, Sillau, S, Heru, A, et al. The feasibility of a multidisciplinary group therapy clinic for the treatment of nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2019) 98:117–23. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

269. Friston, K, FitzGerald, T, Rigoli, F, Schwartenbeck, P, and Pezzulo, G. Active inference: a process theory. Neural Comput. (2017) 29:1–49. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

270. Smith, R, Moutoussis, M, and Bilek, E. Simulating the computational mechanisms of cognitive and behavioral psychotherapeutic interventions: insights from active inference. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:10128. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-89047-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

271. Sundararajan, T, Tesar, GE, and Jimenez, XF. Biomarkers in the diagnosis and study of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a systematic review. Seizure. (2016) 35:11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2015.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

272. Kirmayer, LJ, and Gómez-Carrillo, A. Agency, embodiment and enactment in psychosomatic theory and practice. Med Humanit. (2019) 45:169–82. doi: 10.1136/medhum-2018-011618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

273. Sojka, P, Paredes-Echeverri, S, and Perez, DL. Are functional (psychogenic nonepileptic) seizures the sole expression of psychological processes? Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2022) 55:329–51. doi: 10.1007/7854_2021_225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

274. Da Costa, L, Parr, T, Sajid, N, Veselic, S, Neacsu, V, and Friston, K. Active inference on discrete state-spaces: a synthesis. J Math Psychol. (2020) 99:102447. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2020.102447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

275. Linson, A, Parr, T, and Friston, KJ. Active inference, stressors, and psychological trauma: a neuroethological model of (mal)adaptive explore-exploit dynamics in ecological context. Behav Brain Res. (2020) 17:112421. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112421

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

276. Demartini, B, Batla, A, Petrochilos, P, Fisher, L, Edwards, MJ, and Joyce, E. Multidisciplinary treatment for functional neurological symptoms: a prospective study. J Neurol. (2014) 261:2370–7. doi: 10.1007/s00415-014-7495-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

277. Godena, EJ, Perez, DL, Crain, LD, Fishel, AK, Flaherty, A, Harley, R, et al. Psychotherapy for functional neurological (conversion) disorder: a case bridging mind, brain, and body. J Clin Psychiatry. (2021) 82:21ct14246. doi: 10.4088/JCP.21ct14246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

278. Russell, L, Abbass, A, and Allder, S. A review of the treatment of functional neurological disorder with intensive short-term dynamic psychotherapy. Epilepsy Behav. (2022) 130:108657. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2022.108657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

279. Tolchin, B, Baslet, G, Martino, S, Suzuki, J, Blumenfeld, H, Hirsch, LJ, et al. Motivational interviewing techniques to improve psychotherapy adherence and outcomes for patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2020) 32:125–31. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19020045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

280. Kaplan, M. Clinical considerations regarding regression in psychotherapy with patients with conversion disorder. Psychodyn. Psychiatry. (2016) 44:367–84. doi: 10.1521/pdps.2016.44.3.367

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

281. Goldstein, LH, Mellers, JD, Landau, S, Stone, J, Carson, A, Medford, N, et al. COgnitive behavioural therapy vs standardised medical care for adults with dissociative non-epileptic seizures (CODES): a multicentre randomised controlled trial protocol. BMC Neurol. (2015) 27:98. doi: 10.1186/s12883-015-0350-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

282. Ryznar, E, and Wilcox, D. Functional coma: two case reports and a review of the literature. Psychosomatics. (2019) 60:343–19. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2019.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

283. Burke, MJ, Romanella, SM, Mencarelli, L, Greben, R, Fox, MD, Kaptchuk, TJ, et al. Placebo effects and neuromodulation for depression: a meta-analysis and evaluation of shared mechanisms. Mol Psychiatry. (2022) 27:1658–66. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01397-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

284. Seth, AK, and Friston, KJ. Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. (2016) 371:20160007. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

285. Adams, RA, Vincent, P, Benrimoh, D, Friston, KJ, and Parr, T. Everything is connected: inference and attractors in delusions. Schizophr Res. (2022) 245:5–22. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2021.07.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

286. Roberts, NA, Burleson, MH, Torres, DL, Parkhurst, DK, Garrett, R, Mitchell, LB, et al. Emotional reactivity as a vulnerability for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures? Responses while reliving specific emotions. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2020 Winter) 32:95–100. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19040084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

287. Ghaffar, O, Staines, WR, and Feinstein, A. Unexplained neurologic symptoms: an fMRI study of sensory conversion disorder. Neurology. (2006) 67:2036–8. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000247275.68402.fc

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

288. van de Schoot, R, de Bruin, J, Schram, R, Zahedi, P, de Boer, J, Weijdema, F, et al. An open source machine learning framework for efficient and transparent systematic reviews. Nat Mach Intell. (2021) 3:125–33. doi: 10.1038/s42256-020-00287-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

289. Hirt, J, Nordhausen, T, Appenzeller-Herzogn, C, and Ewald, H. Using citation tracking for systematic literature searching - study protocol for a scoping review of methodological studies and a Delphi study. F1000Res. (2020) 9:1386. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.27337.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: FND, Bayesian, theoretical neurobiology, predictive coding, brain modelling, resignation syndrome

Citation: Milano BA, Moutoussis M and Convertino L (2023) The neurobiology of functional neurological disorders characterised by impaired awareness. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1122865. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1122865

Received: 13 December 2022; Accepted: 24 February 2023;
Published: 16 March 2023.

Edited by:

Alex Kafkas, The University of Manchester, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Mirella Russo, University of Studies G. d'Annunzio Chieti and Pescara, Italy
Mario Miniati, University of Pisa, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Milano, Moutoussis and Convertino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Laura Convertino, bGF1cmEuY29udmVydGluby4xOEB1Y2wuYWMudWs=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.