Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 17 March 2023
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Bodily Self and Its Alterations: Psychopathological, Neural and Theoretical Aspects View all 6 articles

Functional Neurologic Disorders, disorders to be managed by neurologists, or are neurologists wandering in a dangerous field with inadequate resources?

Marco Onofrj,
Marco Onofrj1,2*Paola Ajdinaj,Paola Ajdinaj1,2Anna Digiovanni,Anna Digiovanni1,2Naveed MalekNaveed Malek3Giovanni Martinotti,Giovanni Martinotti1,4Filippo Maria FerroFilippo Maria Ferro1Mirella Russo,Mirella Russo1,2Astrid Thomas,Astrid Thomas1,2Stefano Luca Sensi,Stefano Luca Sensi1,2
  • 1Department of Neuroscience, Imaging, and Clinical Sciences, “G. D'Annunzio University” of Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy
  • 2Center for Advanced Studies and Technology (CAST), “G. D'Annunzio University” of Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy
  • 3Barking, Havering, and Redbridge University Hospitals NHS Trust, London, United Kingdom
  • 4Department of Clinical, Pharmaceutical and Biological Sciences, University of Hertfordshire, Hertfordshire, United Kingdom

In recent years, some neurologists reconsidered their approach to Medically Unexplained Symptoms and proposed Functional Neurologic Disorders (FND) as a new entity, claiming that neurology could offer alternative treatment options to the psychotherapies provided in psychiatry settings. FNDs, for this purpose, should include only the disorders listed as Conversion from the Somatic Symptom and Related Disorders (SSRD) group. The present review analyzes the rationale of this position and challenges the arguments provided for its support. The review also discusses the systematization of these disorders as provided by public health systems. It outlines risks stemming from economic support and public funding uncertainty, given their negligible epidemiological dimensions resulting from the parcellation of SSRD. The review underlines the unresolved issue of Factitious Disorders, which are in the same SSRD category of the international classification but are, nonetheless, overlooked by the theoretical proponents of the FND entity. Comorbidity with other psychiatric disorders is also analyzed. We propose a model that supports the continuum between different SSRD conditions, including Factitious Disorders. The model is based on the emergence of feigned death reflex and deception from frontal lobe dysfunction. Finally, the paper summarizes the wealth of historical psychiatric and psychodynamic approaches and critical reviews. The study also puts in context the categorization and interpretation efforts provided by the most eminent researchers of the past century.

Introduction

Functional neurologic disorders (FND) came back into the clinical repertoire of neurologists only in the last 20 years, after decades of neglect and misconceptions, according to editorials and position papers published in several Neurology journals (19). A new scientific society, the Functional Neurological Disorders Society (FNDS), was founded in 2017 to improve the quality of care for FND patients. Several widely high-impact scientific papers have been published in the last two decades. These publications addressed the issues of the identification of positive features (1, 2), and social stigma linked to previously used definitions like “Hysteria” or “psychogenic” or “psychosomatic” disorders, referring to FND (3, 4). They also focused on treatment opportunities offered by physiotherapy (5, 6), as well as speculated by pathophysiology theories mainly centered on the representations of the body, movement, and volition (agency) rather than on psychodynamic mechanisms (7).

Editorials and commentaries in various neurology journals highlighted the impact of FND (and Somatic Symptoms Disorders) on neurological practice, a phenomenon so pervasive that some authors described it at “epidemic” levels (8, 9).

The 2013 edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 5th edition (DSM-5) (10) accepted the term FND as an equivalent to Conversion disorder. FND were also placed inside the category of Somatic Symptom and Related Disorders (SSRD), together with somatic symptom disorder-Briquet Syndrome (SSD), Illness anxiety disorder-hypochondria, psychological symptoms occurring during other medical conditions, and Factitious Disorders (Table 1). The DSM-5 (10) also describes, for each subcategory, epidemiological and associated features, culture-related factors, comorbidity, and prognosis, providing essential clues on procedures and discussions that were the background for the classification (duly prefaced, as for other DSM versions, by the explanation that DSM is a guide to shared nomenclature, not an easy alternative to training in psychiatry).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Somatic symptom and related disorders—adapted from DSM-5.

Theoretical models and laboratory evidence of abnormal network connectivity and neurotransmitter unbalance in FND patients helped conceptualize FND as a unique disorder, separable from the somatoform (1113) and somatic symptom (10) disorder categories, even if it has been placed in the category of SSRD. This framing represents, however, a challenge to the conceptualization of FND as a unique entity, as DSM-5 implies the five entities of SSRD to be within the spectrum of FND. Moreover, the DSM-5 highlighted associations with other psychiatric disorders, with the most challenging concept being the inclusion of Factitious Disorder inside the category. Factitious Disorders are based on the deception enacted by the patient, that is, the pretense of having a medical or psychiatric disorder with the patient genuinely unaware of enacting such deception (14). This is truly a major challenge, as it introduces concepts of self-deception in SSD and the disavowal of intention (15). Some authors, coalescing in 2001 to present a book on contemporary approaches to Hysteria, proposed a continuum encompassing Hysteria, Conversion, and Factitious Disorder, as reformulated in recent publications (1618) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The presence of a medical condition can often mesh with deception and somatization, supporting the hypothesis of an hystero-malingering continuum. Reproduced, with modifications, from Feldman and Yates, 2018.

Our paper addresses the topic of FND and SSD in detail. It also calls to action to encourage a multidisciplinary approach that bridges the gap between the disciplines of neurology and psychiatry. The paper also underlines the need to reconsider the seminal psychopathological approach (19).

We also summarize the long-standing history of psychodynamic interpretations of FND, frame it into the broader category of SSRD, analyze the unresolved issue of Factitious Disorders as well as Malingering, discuss the evidence for various treatment options, and suggest a model for the disorder.

The arguments in favor of a neurologic reclassification of FND

The prevalence of patients with FND or Medically Unexplained Symptoms (MUS, including fibromyalgia, chronic fatigue syndrome, and reflex sympathetic algodystrophy/causalgia) in neurology clinics is estimated to be around 16–30% (2). Several authors have described such a high prevalence as representing a crisis, or a silent epidemic (8), invoking calls for newer approaches to reaching out to help patients with FND. As an extreme example of the attitude, an authoritative editorial wrote that “the patients do not want to hear that they have a psychiatric disorder and they go from doctor to doctor, psychiatrists do not seem interested anyway, and the prognosis is terrible” (19), and the concept was plainly reported in a very recent paper (20).

Several reviews and position papers (1, 21) have detailed the main reasons to separate FND into a category that can cover a well-demarcated group of patients acceptable in much wider neurology practice settings including out-patient and in-patient settings. Citing evidence and views of the authors from these papers (1, 21), the main reasons included the following issues:

1. The pattern of motor presentations was consistent through time. Foot dragging, knee buckling, give-away loss of muscle tone, arm fall without pronation, or distractible motor disorders like dystonia, tremor, or myoclonus, which are the most common motor presentations of FND in patients accessing neurology clinics in the last decade (2), are the same features in patients previously dubbed as hysterics in scientific reports from the early 20th century (22). The above-mentioned features, together with the variants of Hoover’s sign (23), could be considered examples of “positive” features for the diagnosis of FND, as opposed to diagnoses of FND based on the exclusion of medical causes.

2. Treatments based on physiotherapy provide better results than psychotherapy (24). Earlier studies suggested that a trial of physiotherapy was better suited for patients than psychotherapy, as providing a “saving face” solution (18). More recent studies have focused on the possible effect of motor retraining (2527) yet were less dismissive of psychotherapeutic or other approaches.

3. Several studies have underlined that FND, if untreated, persists for a longer time during follow-up (28, 29). A few years ago, a systematic review showed that up to 40% of patients with FND report similar or worse outcomes at 7-year follow-ups (28).

4. Some studies using imaging techniques in FND showed hypoactivation of the contralateral primary motor cortex, decreased activity in the parietal lobe, aberrant activation of the amygdala, increased temporo-parietal junction activity, and hyperactivation of insular regions. Functional connectivity shows aberrant connections between the amygdala and motor areas, temporoparietal junctions, and the insula (7, 30, 31). The unintentional, unconscious, production of motor or sensory symptoms was framed into models of top-down processing of sensory experiences, recapitulating the concept of “priors” i.e., the unconscious memory of prior experiences predicting the outcome of the present experience (32, 33). The models suggest that a distortion of the preparatory motor output is the mechanism of motor FND (30), akin to the hypothesized model of dystonia (31) and the model mechanism of its treatment with botulinum toxin (34, 35).

5. Stressful life events during childhood or adulthood are often quoted among the predisposing or precipitating factors for FND (36).

6. Overlaps between FND and neurological disorders like Parkinson’s disease (3742), Dementia with Lewy bodies (37, 38, 42), epilepsy (43), or multiple sclerosis (44) have been previously described, showing that FND could precede or accompany the onset of these conditions (45, 46).

7. In several discussions, the model underlines the absolute absence of access to consciousness of FND symptoms (15, 4749), thereby suggesting a difference from Factitious Disorders and Malingering, the latter two being generated by deception, thus a supposedly volitional act (50). Several reviews quote the DSM-5 categorization (10), in which factitious disorders are placed in a separate category of somatic symptoms disorders (which, nonetheless, include FND). Many discussions on FND compare this disorder to Malingering (16, 17), overlooking or ignoring the complex question posed by the imposing presence of Factitious Disorders.

Counterarguments and objections

1. The phenomenological descriptions of various motor FND may be simply due to physical limitations rather than represent a pattern of a specific disorder. For instance, due to the mechanical limitations of muscle-joint ranges of motion, patterns like foot-dragging or knee buckling seem the only options available to enact a loss of function of muscles around a joint in a limb. Moreover, specific patterns are only seen in some ethno-psychiatric disorders, i.e., disorders present only in some geographical areas affect people of some ethnic backgrounds. For example, the sensation of shrinking of the penis, termed Koro (51, 52), is only described in Asian and African populations, occasionally with epidemic presentations. The explosive bursts of violence or aggressive agitation are only described with the Amok of South-East Asia (52, 53). The hyper-motor choreic and myoclonic manifestations of the Jumping Frenchman of Maine (54) or Latah (52, 55), were geographically and temporally limited, in episodes that were finally interpreted as Mass Hysteria (53, 56), same as the dancing disease that hit Strasbourg (57) at the end of the 16th century (58). These examples are evidence of a culturally influenced manifestation of conversion/FND symptoms in limited geographical areas. Recent puzzling evidence of cultural influence was provided by the epidemy of TikTok Tourette, which found room on social and general media (59, 60).

2. Reports of the efficacy of physiotherapy in FND patients suffer from the same selection biases burdening many studies on the effect of physiotherapy and other therapies organized in complex settings, i.e., need for sufficiently blinded study designs (5). The number of selected patients is often small, matching of controls is absent, and cross-overs or inferiority designs have never been attempted. There are no studies comparing psychotherapy with physiotherapy on the long-term outcomes in FND nor with other techniques claimed to be efficacious in FND, like hypnosis, transcranial direct current or magnetic stimulation, mindfulness training, narrative exposure treatment, or approaches through alternative medicine (6163).

3. The long-term persistence of FND symptoms is probably biased by patients’ cohort selection. Cases presenting late with other severe diseases, medical or neurologic, do show that Conversion, fibromyalgia, and reflex sympathetic dystrophy may disappear once that new clinical entity supersedes (7, 40, 64). The only paper describing follow-up functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) assessments of FND shows a reduction of symptoms (65). In contrast to recent position papers on FND (66), prior DSM versions (11, 13) reported that Conversion is time-limited. That FND and SSD could be time-limited conditions was also underlined by several psychiatric and psychoanalytic studies (16, 18), as shown by the puzzling question posed at the early times marked by claims on end or crisis of psychoanalysis, i.e., “where did all the hysterics end up?” (16, 18). A preposterous example related to where the hysterics disappeared can be found in the investigations on this disappearance. After the seminal demonstrations by Charcot and colleagues on the phenomenology of Hysteria (67), based on the presentation of the affected patient to the audience, the topic faded from general attention. However, some curious researchers investigated the follow-up of the patients who had served as eminent examples during the seminars in the clinic. Not surprisingly, some of the demonstrative cases were later found to be still available for practical demonstrations of Hysteria, given the provision of a modest fee (68).

4. The idea that Conversion may be determined by an underlying brain disorder dates back to the first descriptions of the condition. Charcot believed that eredo-degeneration was the subtending mechanism (67, 69), Freud was aligned with the hypothesis by his mentor (70, 71), several psychiatrists re-proposed the concept (72, 73), by suggesting explanations that were based on neurophysiological knowledge of the time, i.e., cortical hyperexcitability and enhanced response to reticular system afferents was the hypothesized mechanism when the reticular system was the ultimate discovery of neurosciences (74). Studies on fMRI activation of cortical or subcortical areas during voluntary or involuntary movements (movement analysis protocols) are scarce (30, 7577), which is in contrast with the many studies on resting state connectivity (77). Any resting state (fMRI) connectivity study can only provide inference on putative networks, as it does not record what happens during a motor behavior but only shows the set of networks statistically evidenced during rest. With task studies performed during a voluntary movement, fMRI shows activation of multiple areas, including the motor cortex, the premotor cortex, primary and secondary sensory cortices, supplementary motor area and contralateral cerebellum, invariably also the frontal lobes, parietal lobes, and temporal lobes in the majority of volunteers for these studies (Figure 2). Activations of the same areas were observed during a triggered movement in a patient with putative involuntary movements of the alien hand, anterior type, and in 36 patients affected by Tourette syndrome, during the putatively involuntary but suppressible tics (35, 78, 79). Moreover, there are no studies on involuntary movement prototypes, like chorea, ballism, rubral tremors, and L-dopa-induced dyskinesias. This is because the studies are technically complex, there is no back-averaging program software for fMRI, and the time resolution of fMRI is limited to 0.5 s or more, far more than the time resolution of electrophysiology and the time of reflex circuit activations, thus making it impossible to catch the temporal sequence of recruited areas. Therefore, the variability of voluntary activations during movements performed upon control conditions makes the results of studies in putative, involuntary movements less than contentious. Moreover, recent reviews had to admit powerful limitations, “many different techniques, tasks, and heterogeneous clinical samples were used, rendering any attempt to do a meta-analysis difficult. Most studies had several limitations, among which small sample size and confounding factors were the most frequent” (80). As a concluding criticism, we must also underline that in the few fMRI studies attempting a comparison with FND patients, the matched group was made of healthy controls instructed to feign a paresis (8183). This selection introduces the relevant bias constituted by access to consciousness of the aberrant motor behavior, with the consequential question about what the difference is showing. Are we seeing differences in the network subtending the motor act or in allowing or impeding access to consciousness? If the latter is likely, how would the disavowal, i.e., the psychopathological denial, of access to consciousness, and its effect on volitional networks, be accounted for? Not secondary, all SSD, including FND, are burdened by frequent association with other psychiatric disorders, as discussed in the next section. Accurate matching procedures should provide control groups burdened by the same or similar comorbid patterns (personality disorders, obsessive–compulsive disorders, or even patients affected by SSD who did not show Conversion/FND features). With this matching, the comparison would rest on more solid grounds and provide an observation on the different effects of suggestibility, which was considered the core feature of Hysteria by Babinski (84, 85).

5. The frequency of trauma and stressful life events was only modestly different between patients with FND and controls or patients with hand dystonia (86). But the hypothesis on childhood trauma as the predisposing etiology of FND is challenged by the century-old observations, which were the seminal findings of psychoanalysis. Sigmund Freud only in his early case descriptions, identified traumatic experiences (i.e., sexual harassment during childhood) as the predisposing or precipitating factor for conversion disorders (87). However, shortly after his first few studies, when he felt challenged by the inconsistency of recalled histories, he developed his seminal theories on the fantasy of trauma, interpreted as a psychodynamic mechanism structured unconsciously in defense and denial of the intrapsychic conflict of ideas and affects (87). Several earlier studies on Hysteria, Conversion, Somatoform and Factitious disorders did not find any significant prevalence of trauma in early life (18). In psychotherapy, a technique recently emerged, the Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR), which seemed to be specifically suited for the treatment of trauma (88), but no studies of its effect on FND/SSD have been attempted.

6. FND appear frequently in patients with parkinsonism (up to 59%) before or after the onset of motor symptoms (89), and pseudo-seizures are frequently coexistent with epileptic disorders (43). FND overlaps, or pseudo-relapses, are a consistent problem in Multiple Sclerosis management (44). Studies in patients with parkinsonism have uniformly shown that FND can predict the occurrence of cognitive decline and coexist, often, with hallucinations (89, 90). Often FND remit when dopaminergic treatments are initiated but recur when cognitive decline supersedes (40). However, these studies were based on a different hypothesis rather than simply providing evidence that FND may be the expression of a brain disorder. That evidence had already been provided by studies on post-encephalitic parkinsonism (9193). It was not casually coincident with the birth of psychodynamic theories. The purpose of the studies on parkinsonism was to understand whether the development of FND was linked to the same dysfunction which leads to the occurrence of hallucinations. In other terms, to conceptualize FND as an expression of the weakening of frontoparietal control networks and subsequent loss of consensual reality and emergence, disinhibition, of the internal narrative generator, the posterior cingulate gyrus (89, 90, 94). It might be argued that FND appearing in the presence of cognitive decline and psychosis are different from FND appearing in the absence of both. It is also debated whether the findings in neurologic disorders should constitute the model, to be dissected in “formes frustes,” in the quest for understanding mechanisms and treatment. With these comparisons, it will be possible to elucidate whether FND/SSD are a form of psychosis rather than a separate disorder.

7. Nevertheless, the main flaw of the explanatory theory relies in the simplified demarcating line separating unconscious from willed behaviors and considering Factitious Disorders as akin to Malingering (37), which is not a factitious disorder, and was, in some studies, used as a comparator for FND. Malingering is in fact not a medical term and is not listed as a diagnosis in DSM-5 (10). In malingering, the motivation (gain) is external such as receiving money (95) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Activated areas during voluntary movements revealed by fMRI studies (A) vs. activated areas during involuntary movements revealed by fMRI studies in patients with alien limb and other movement disorders (B). BG: Basal Ganglia; Cer: Cerebellum; CMA: Cingulate Motor Area; In: Insula; IPL: Inferior Parietal Lobe; M1 primary motor cortex; MFG: Middle Frontal Gyrus; Op: Operculum; PreM: Premotor cortex; RN: Red Nucleus; S1: primary sensory cortex; (pre-)SMA: Supplementary Motor Area; SMG: SupraMarginal Gyrus; SP: Striato-Pallidal complex; SPC: Superior Parietal Cortex; STC: Superior Temporal Cortex; (v)Th: (ventral) Thalamus. http://smart.servier.com.

The main criticism: The unsolved and overlooked problem of factitious disorder

Factitious Disorder obtains a definite identity only in DSM-III (11). Therefore related interpretations are likely biased by the prior insufficient separation from other SSD and from Malingering. Before DSM-III, Factitious Disorder and malingering were considered primarily present in the military (in conscripted subjects) and the criminal world (96). For some authors, a catalyst of Factitious Disorders and Malingering was the creation of social welfare states with access to financial compensations or unnecessary care (18).

In the DSM-IV (12), the Factitious Disorder was also termed “Munchausen Syndrome,” according to the early presentation of the disorder by R. Asher (1951) (97), but DSM-5 disfavors its use. The reasons for opting for other terms are discussed in detail by Feldman and Yates, 2018 (98), where attention is focused on the forensic categorization of the disorder and the need to highlight the abusive behavior in legal terms.

The core definitions of Factitious Disorders are that these “are conditions in which a patient intentionally produces or feigns physical or psychological symptoms…without obvious secondary gain” (ICD-10 definition) (14). DSM-5 states that “the motivation for the behavior is to assume the sick role” but, despite stating that deception and feigning are the core element allowing a diagnosis, it also says that “assessment of conscious intention is unreliable” and that the chance to conclude for a diagnosis is linked to the chance to incur into evidence of feigning (10). Therefore, guidelines (10) state that deception and the absence of external incentives for the behavior are diagnostic criteria, yet they mention that the intention of feigning cannot be reliably assessed. Any assessment should be capable of identifying deception and consciousness of deceiving by separating the elements of voluntary activity. However, assessments of deception and feigning are far from being accomplished, far from a resolutive operating technique, and surrendered to legal, judicial, rather than medical matters. Because pathological lying (pseudologia fantastica) is a critical component of factitious illness, it is argued that the clinician should actively seek its identification. Pathological lying is distinguished from “normal” lying by several characteristics, including the recurrent, enduring, and compulsive presentations, the fantastic, self-aggrandizing content, the possible ego-dystonic structure with maladaptive or destructive outcomes for the quality of life of the person involved (98).

Psychodynamic interpretations describe in primary gain (i.e., the solution of an intrapsychic conflict) the origin of Factitious Disorder, at difference with secondary gains, which are the practical or economic benefits resulting from the enactment of a distinct behavior (1). Primary gain was described as keeping an internal conflict or need out of awareness, and secondary gain as avoiding a particular activity that is noxious and getting support from the environment that otherwise might not be forthcoming. However, the primary gain is also the origin of Conversion, to the point that several studies depict a continuum from Conversion to factitious to malingering (98) (Figure 1).

Psychodynamic studies highlight that “factitious disorders are famously difficult to treat medically and are highly refractory to psychotherapy” (36). Only a few reports could describe the interaction between a therapist and a patient affected by Factitious Disorder. Many authors resorted to writing that communication was burdened by deception and opposition. Some authors (99) describe confrontational management once that deception was documented. Many authors underline the oppositive defiant response to attempts to rationalize and explain the behavior, constantly leading to the concluding words “what if that’s true?,” a comment underlining the scarce access to consciousness of the behavior.

The most interesting report from a psychotherapy session was from one of the few collaborative patients, who was describing herself as “desperate to try and get help”: she wrote “I despise myself for all the things I have done and have continually tried to stop what is like an addiction.” Similar, descriptions can be found in the recent book by Feldman and Yates (98).

The unconscious origin of the feigning behavior is interpreted as a drive to be in control of medical conditions which were previously experienced as painful, i.e., that these disorders are repetitive compulsions motivated by the desire for mastery (e.g., taking forced control over the medical personnel providing care) (36). Feigning, or inflicting damages to a proxy in a state of blurred consciousness, is therefore interpreted as the expression of an unconscious wish to enact a personal drama and to reinforce the strength of a relationship with medical professionals who figure in the fantasy lives of those affected by the disorder (100). In the 1978 draft of the DSM-III (11), the motive subtending Factitious Disorders was described as the compulsion to act out a sadomasochistic relationship with physicians regarded as parental figures.

Most psychodynamic interpretations were produced before DSM-III set a category for Factitious Disorders, and close reevaluation of the described cases often unveils a mixture of SSRD (Conversion or FND) and factitious disorders in the same patients (101104).

Psychiatric and psychodynamic approaches

Classifications and medical disciplines involved in FND management

The International Classification of Diseases (ICD), in its ICD-10 version (14), classified FND only under the category of psychiatric disorders, and the ICD-11 version (101) listed only functional tremor, functional parkinsonism, and pseudo-seizures among disorders classified as neurologic disorders.

Most national health services do not list FND among their Diagnosis Related Groups (DRG) (102), which benefit from cost coverages for hospitalizations from neurology clinics. Among the European nations, only the United Kingdom (UK) National Health Service (NHS) considers reimbursements to neurologists via their NHS Trusts under the coding category of “neuropsychiatric disorders.” It must also be underlined that, in UK NHS, there is a peculiar disproportion, as compared with other countries, between the number of psychiatrists and neurologists. The ratio is 1 to 8, at difference from Italy, where ratios are 2 to 1. Further differences must also be underlined in the different targets of the two disciplines in different countries. In UK and Germany, psychiatrists manage dementia cases, which are dealt with by neurologists in other countries. It is, therefore, possible that the urge to frame the category of FND into neurology was dependent on the contingent organization of UK’s NHS, while in other countries, the discipline of psychiatry has far wider access to long-term management, social access, and approaches to legal issues than the discipline of neurology.

The national health care systems of countries providing free health care or even insurance-based health care are commonly based on coding or the DRG (102) assessment of disorders to evaluate whether access to health care can be economically supported and justified. The deployment of DRG coverages varies remarkably among countries, as indicated previously. In the USA, the Insurance based coverage is subject to posttreatment screening, with the risk of direct charges to the patient.

In Italy (102), Switzerland, France, Belgium, the Netherlands, and Germany, all DRG related to Conversion/FND and including the category of somatoform/somatic symptoms disorders are recognized only for the discipline of psychiatry.

As a final comment, it should be underlined that, recently, several editorials and analyses appeared in the US scientific literature, underlining the need for better psychiatric training for the residents in neurology. The studies highlighted the inadequate training of neurologists and their troubles when dealing with conditions overlapping with psychiatry, suggesting several projects to improve knowledge, as opposed to the current 4 weeks-training (103).

Comorbidity or associations

The association with anxiety and depressive disorders as well as the association of one SSRD with the others, is quoted for all categories. While the association with obsessive–compulsive disorder is uniquely quoted for illness anxiety disorder, the association with dissociative disorders is quoted in DSM-5 (10) for conversion/FND, and in DSM-IV-TR with histrionic, antisocial, borderline, and dependent personality Disorders. The association between Factitious Disorder and borderline personality disorder is quoted in DSM-IV-TR (13). Factitious Disorder was removed from the DSM-IV (12) category “Dissociative Disorders” (i.e., multiple personality disorder, dissociative amnesia) to the DSM-5’s SSRD group. Factitious Disorders may appear in association with other mental disorders. DSM-5 (10) quotes the association between the other four SSRD and dissociative disorders. DSM IV-TR (13) included histrionic, antisocial, borderline, and dependent personality disorders. DSM-5 (10) is not shying away from defining that “some aspects of factitious disorders might represent criminal behavior.”

Adoption studies of somatization disorders (100, 104106), analyzing the occurrence of somatization in adopted patients affected by somatization disorders, and personality traits of natural parents, showed clearly that somatization disorder was associated with heritable personality traits such as a predisposition to antisocial behavior and substance abuse (18).

Interpretations of FND and SSD

When investigating Hysteria and teaching his conclusions to Freud, Charcot explained that hysteric symptoms were the expression of the loss of “function” and were associated with hereditary neurodegeneration. This was to pursue the disclosure of the “physical” background of disorders (18, 67, 107, 108).

Babinski offered an edge-cutting definition of Hysteria, by writing that “Hysteria is a disorder caused by suggestion, treatable by persuasion” (84, 85, 109), but this definition did not survive, as the underlying optimism was eventually challenged by a massive amount of evidence.

Also, Freud (18) was aiming at a “physical” explanation of disorders and framed his explanations within the confines of the dominant scientific knowledge of the time, thus energy, entropy, and displacement of energy were the pillars of his metapsychology.

Several studies of the second half of the last century (18), hypothesized that conversion disorders could be different from other psychosomatic symptoms, and linked to cortical hyperexcitability and insufficient inhibition of afferents from the reticular system, the structure which was the ultimate discovery at the time (107, 108).

The organic, physical, mechanistic hypotheses thus appear embedded in the history of neuropsychiatry, the use of the term “functional” is far from new, and references to the dominant knowledge-based theories of the time are recurrent, as in all other aspects of scientific endeavor (Kuhn, Feyerabend) (110, 111).

With the development of psychoanalysis and psychodynamic theories, the interpretation of somatic symptoms disorders was, however, mostly ascribed to mental functioning, defense mechanisms, and coping styles (18, 67).

After his early, retracted, description of trauma as a predisposing factor, Freud (18) reconsidered his interpretation and developed his ideas of Conversion as the representation of the unconscious attempt to compromise between drive (pulsions) and repression (defense mechanisms). Freud identified as “primary gain” the result of this unconscious coping strategy, i.e., gaining advantageous stability against the emergence of a hostile drive and the psychic cost of denying its existence. He identified, subsequently, “secondary gains” in the relational advantages (including economic gains) which could be derived from the enacted behavior (87).

This seminal theory is still the bedrock of psychodynamic interpretations of the expression of mental disorders: defense mechanisms are analyzed in DSM versions and coded as narcissistic, immature, neurotic, and mature to explain the underlying psychodynamic mechanisms of mental disorders. Accordingly, conversion symptoms and hypochondriasis are listed among the immature defense mechanisms (112).

The interpretation of the mechanisms of somatic symptoms disorders has been multifaceted, hedging on different, often divergent theories produced by psychoanalytic schools (18).

Jung ascribed Hysteria to intense, “exaggerated,” manifestations of the conflict with proximal (relational or familiar) figures, introducing in this veiled interpretation, the concept of hostile attitude and production of the somatic symptom to deny and hide hostility (113). Jung agrees with Freud that hysterical symptoms are the return of repressed memories in the patient’s personal background.

This concept reemerges in several psychodynamic studies on somatization (18), going as far as suggesting that hypochondria underlines an evil personality and that somatic symptoms are produced to constrain the unconscious emergence of hostile attitudes, or that somatic symptoms are produced as an atonement of hostile feelings to obtain the primary gain, with denial, and a secondary gain by playing the sick role, thus obtaining a legitimate way to get dependency needs met (112).

A similar interpretation by K. Leonhard suggested that “hysteria has a purpose,” contrary to hypochondria, which is the expression of angst.

The nosology effort made by K. Leonhard [114–117]deserves further attention. This author, leading, at his times, the neuropsychiatric services of the Berlin Charité Hospital, third heir to the psychiatry school of Wernicke, and Kleist and to the gigantic German psychiatry schools (114, 115) of Kraepelin (116, 117), Bleuler (118) and others, produced outstanding disease classification systems, which were often anticipating concepts that reappeared only lately in literature and classification systems.

K. Leonhard described Hysteria (119122), with other SSD, as a symptom of different mental disorders rather than considering it a disease. This concept, with which we agree, anticipates the DSM-IV TR classification methods, based on multiple axes of disease categorization, which highlights the coexistence of multiple expressions of mental disorders. The multiaxial categorization was abandoned by the DSM-5 manual, which was instead attempting core definitions of the different disorders.

Janet agreed with Freud that fixed subconscious ideas were at the core of Conversion and suggested that “a narrowing of the field of consciousness” was responsible for symptoms, linking somatic symptom disorders to dissociative disorders (18).

Several psychoanalysts (18) continued to interpret Hysteria along the original Freudian lines, in terms of psychosexual fixations, referring back to pre-genital stages of development (i.e., anal or oral) (123125). Others shifted the paradigm in terms of object fixations as the drivers of the primary gain mechanisms (126128).

Lacan (129, 130) produced new theories focusing on the fragmented infantile experience of the ego (mirror stage), which is painfully constructed through experience, and on re-emergence of fragmentation in psychosis, of which he attempted to decipher the language. His studies led to further development of psychoanalysis as a theory of meaning and communication, rather than causal sequences. Hysteria was interpreted as the paradigm of the symptom as a symbol or metaphor of unconscious needs or drives. Lacan described the fragmented experience of the body as the original self-representation, which evolves through comparisons in the mirror stage, to form an integrated body experience when separating from the mother’s body.

Hysteria in Lacan’s writings is one of the four “Discourses” constituting the essential models of communication between a self and the other (130). The four “discourses” are Master, University, Analyst, and Hysteria, representing the modalities of communication, addressed to a personification of the agent (an unexpected precursor of the recent emphasis on agency), to knowledge, to search for truth, and to the attempt to reach the “other.” In this theory, Hysteria is not just the product of imaginary anatomy. It is a main structure of communication, and several Lacanian psychoanalysts, as Lacan himself, use the term hysteric to highlight a modality of communication attempting to build a social bond with the other, i.e., Hysteria is not simply the somatic symptom but a modality of communication attempting to link the “other” to questions posed by the self. In some over-simplifications Lacan writes that the introduction of Hysteria as a structure of communication can lead to the paradoxical conclusion that “we are all hysterics.” Beyond the structural placement, many definitions of Hysteria in Lacanian literature are outstanding: Hysteria as a communication “which cannot be mastered by knowledge and therefore remains outside of history,” Hysteria as imago of the fragmented body, “hysteria is a chimaera, bringing to the mind the myth of the sphinx,” i.e., acting as the sphinx posing a riddle to man, where the riddle is a recurrent request for recognition of identity through the answer of the questioned man; the hysterics start out with “I am what you say” and end with “all of what I am you cannot say” i.e., the hysteric is asking for recognition, but the recognition is always insufficient and prompts a renewed question.

Later studies focused on general models of personality (18), including dimensions of temperament and character, structured in a temperament and character Inventory, which provided strong reliability, regardless of cultural background, gender, or age. The four dimensions of temperament (131) are listed as harm avoidance (serotoninergic), novelty seeking, reward dependency (dopaminergic), and persistence, and the three dimensions of character (131) are listed as self-directedness, cooperative and self-transcendent. Studies of patients with conversion and somatization disorders, conducted based on the personality assessment, revealed a link with the multidimensional structure of personality, consisting of high harm avoidance and high novelty seeking, with low self-directedness, and commented on the frequent association with borderline and avoidant or obsessive personality disorders. These personality studies (131, 132) underlined the multidimensional background of patients with somatic symptom disorders, suggesting that understanding the prevalent traits would be crucial for treatment planning and management, including the choice of psychotropic drugs and psychotherapy techniques (133, 134).

In philosophy, then in psychology, three main elements of voluntary (willed) actions (135) were considered as possibly representing separate processes: (a) volition, or will, defined as the power that the mind has to order the forbearing of an idea; (b) agency, as the perception of being the one who chooses or enacts the action; (c) intention, as a conscious choice of who is the agent of the action. For example, in organic weakness or paresis, there is intention, volition, and agency of the effort to perform an act but no motor effect; in utilization and imitation behaviors of frontal lobe diseases there is no intention nor volition, while agency, i.e., perception of being the one who performs the act, is preserved (although possibly blurred by the frontal lobe lesions that induce the stimulus bound behaviors); in sensory deafferentation, or posterior alien hand symptom, there is no agency, i.e., the moving arm is not perceived as part of the body.

In FND/Conversion, the definition of the missing elements is less clear; originally the focus was on volition, “they say, I cannot; it looks like I will not; but it is I cannot will” (136), therefore FND was interpreted as dependent on disordered will. The implication of conscious or unconscious mechanisms, however, blurred the definition. The psychodynamic model invokes an unconscious mechanism acting independently of consciousness. Thus a disordered unconscious intention might give rise to a disordered will. From these hypotheses, an unconscious intention to action would rely on a simultaneous (conscious) intention to act. Lacanian interpretations (129, 130) of FND talked of “failed acts” either as identification with an external will or as an act within a fantasized body space. Attention to the motor act, as part of intention, was then called into a mechanistic cause, also to explain the distractibility of FND. It must be pointed out that when invoking attention into a mechanistic cause, a paradox surfaces, as the maintenance of unconsciously derived FND symptoms requires conscious attention (2, 137) as shown by the fact that FND are, by definition, distractible by entrainment maneuvers.

More recently, the focus (1, 32, 33) has been on agency, which was considered as the expression of disordered perception, altered by insufficient suppression of the “priors” recruited by top-down processing of perception (32, 33, 138). The interpretation would be that there is impaired access to consciousness of the somatic symptom in FND.

In patients with Factitious Disorders, the agency of feigning and perhaps volition of feigning are denied and canceled from consciousness; intention should be consequently unconscious.

Further attempts at interpretation with psychodynamic analyses focused on self-deception, avowal, and disavowal of action, suggesting that disavowal of action (18), which becomes not intended, is acted to avoid disturbing the patient’s image of his/herself (18, 48, 49). The concept of avowal did not separate FND from feigning, and, unsurprisingly, the dissection of volition led to a discussion of moral contents, distinguishing an unconscious, morally neutral deception, where it is the self and not the other, whom the deceiver is deceiving; this is different from a deception being a willed act, which is pure malingering.

The boundaries between the various discussed disorders were therefore considered unclear and a possible continuum was suggested, and the “Hystero-malingering continuum” appears in the literature (10, 16, 17, 48, 67, 98). For example, it was shown that hysteric patients misinterpret evidence and selectively attend to only a certain part of the overall evidence. These patients who employ such strategies as positive and negative misinterpretations, selective attending, and selective evidence gathering were considered motivationally biased (primary, secondary/economic gain), and can therefore be self-deceived (48).

Psychotherapeutic treatments

A throughout review of 2017 (139) analyzed the different treatment options for SSD/FND.

FND management and treatment strategies include: (1) patients education, (2) psychotherapy, (3) psychopharmacological treatment, and (4) physical rehabilitation, with basic (and essential) avoidance of medical harm (66). Thorough communication is required to enhance the chances of successful treatment. The patients must be informed about the nature of their disturbances (140) and the existence of a “real” problem must be acknowledged with the patient, as well as its possible reversibility with appropriate treatments (25), to improve compliance with therapies and the final outcomes. Subsequently, the therapy should be tailored to the patient’s necessities and clinical features, which can be assessed by the Psychosomatic Evaluation (PE) (139). PE encompasses the patient’s intrinsic features, such as the personality and the presence of a Type A behavior (prone to competition, irritability, and psychomotor urgency), as well as the occurrence of external stressors that modify the allostatic burden causing an acute or chronic overload that ultimately impairs wellbeing (139). The absence of adequate social support should also be assessed, as it is related to worse outcomes (141). The complex ties between “external” and “internal” proneness to somatization underlie the production of one or more maladaptive disease behaviors, like health anxiety, hypochondria, thanatophobia, or disease phobia, or – on the opposite side – illness denial (139). Other peculiar manifestations include the “anniversary reaction” and, finally, FND or persistent somatizations (139). The PE may be particularly suitable to monitor the therapy response (139), while psychotherapy and pharmacological therapy may be effective for FND treatment and relieve concomitant psychiatric disorders. The psychodynamic approach has been successfully tested (142), both in acute and chronic settings (143). One of the main limitations of the psychodynamic approach is the relatively slow effect compared to other techniques. However, short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy (STPP) is also effective in treating FND, as confirmed by a meta-analysis, and may represent a promising therapeutical resource (144). Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) is now widely employed (145147). CBT shows both early and long-term efficacy (148) and is effective in the reduction of PNES (149, 150). A cognitive-behavioral intervention, namely Treatment of Anxiety and Physical Symptom (TAPS), has been successfully applied to a cohort of pediatric and adolescent patients with persistent somatic complaints (151). Other techniques have been tested in FND patients, with different degrees of success (e.g., mindfulness, stress management, hypnosis add-on, prolonged exposure therapy, and group psychoeducation (152157)). Antidepressant and concomitant anxiolytic treatment may also be helpful (158).

In the review, physical therapy is also analyzed and considered as generally well-tolerated and effective for motor symptoms (66, 159), alone or as part of other multidisciplinary approaches, considering that it can also include psychotherapeutic interventions. Symptom remission may be sustained up to one year after cessation (66, 159161). However, physiotherapy strategy should be planned according to guidelines and to the patient’s profile to retain adherence and achieve the best results (25, 160), explaining to patients the interpretations of their physical symptoms to help them with the management of their situation and physical condition. In this way, a powerful working alliance can be built between physiotherapists and patients, which can improve the patients’ quality of life.

The authors also summarized the effect of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS), which can improve the clinical picture, according to several observations (162164), but they also commented that the mechanism is still unclear, addressing methodological bias (165). Interestingly, TMS was suggested to mainly act through a cognitive-behavioral mechanism (166), rather than by cortical excitability modulation. According to this model, TMS-related improvement of FND would result from suggestion and from motor re-learning (i.e., the assessment of the capacity of normal movements), as training for self-agency during involuntary movements. Finally, antidepressant and concomitant anxiolytic treatments were analyzed (23) and benzodiazepines for catatonia (167).

Other studies discuss using Narrative Exposure Treatment (NET), classical Hypnosis, Trauma Desensitization (52). Hypnosis has been studied in two randomized clinical trials in patients with FND, including symptoms of sensory loss or speech disturbance showing an improvement comparable to psychotherapy (168, 169). It can be used also to demonstrate the diagnosis of FND to patients if the symptoms disappear under hypnosis. NET is usually used as a treatment for trauma disorders, particularly in individuals suffering from complex and multiple trauma (170172). The idea behind it is that people who write or tell their own stories can also change the biological mechanisms that are at the origin of the trauma (52) because the systematic narration of their life can have a consolidating effect on how one perceives itself, as Bures writes in his “Geography of madness” (52, 173).

Other (pharmacological) treatments

Current therapeutic options for FND are mainly non-pharmacological. Although antipsychotic drugs.

have no direct indication for FND treatment, they are prescribed off-label in over 60% of cases (174). According to a national survey (174), olanzapine (42.1%), quetiapine (15.8%), and risperidone (5.3%) are the most prescribed antipsychotics for SSD treatment. Other atypical agents (like brexipiprazole, aripiprazole (175), and paliperidone (176)) are also effective as an add-on (177). These findings align with the model that considers FND and SSD as psychotic symptoms (90) arising from a dysfunctional coupling of task-negative and task-positive networks, jointly to an aberrant Salient Network activity (42) and a weakened frontal control system (178, 179). In line with this model, MRI spectroscopy (180) in SSD patients revealed the presence of high Glx (glutamate+glutamine) levels within the posterior cingulate cortex. These levels were higher than healthy age-matched control subjects and correlated with catastrophizing pain symptoms. From a speculative point of view, anti-glutamatergic agents can counteract the hyperactivation of the Default Mode Network– as demonstrated for new antidepressant agents – and ameliorate the cognitive and emotional processing of pain. (Es)ketamine (181, 182) and lamotrigine (177) have shown promising antipsychotic properties, mostly attributed to the NMDAr blockage within specific thalamo-cortical circuits. Accordingly, their properties as potential FND treatments should be thoroughly investigated. Finally, in the frequent cases of comorbid depression or anxiety, therapeutical management should be tailored to the patient’s needs.

The continuum model and possible variants

Almost two decades after the book (18) on the contemporary approach to Hysteria, Feldman and Yates provided a model for the continuum (98), pointing out where medically recognized diseases, medically unexplained symptoms, and deception would overlap. This model explains the frequent condition where SSD/FND evolve, or rather deteriorate, into Factitious Disorder, while emerging from a recognized disease (Figure 1).

While an undebatable operating instrument for clinical understanding, this model lacks a definition of the background giving origin to the behaviors. From this model, we developed an alternative representation where many variables are inserted and where the equivalence, in terms of severity, and intensity of symptoms, of the three aspects is substituted by a core element consisting of the pulsion to exert control on external and internal forces (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Proposed model. FND, Functional Neurological Disorder; SSD, Somatic Symptom Disorder.

Our model centers on the necessity to exert control, interpreted as a defense against a perceived fragmentation of the identity, i.e., because of an offense to a narcissistic idealization of the self in psychopathological conditions or because of shrinking of higher-order mental activities in neurodegenerative conditions. The attempt to regain control, sometimes accompanied by weakening frontal control networks (30, 178, 183), will produce aberrant behaviors, inclined toward passive or active responses (Figure 3).

The model assumes that the dysfunction of frontal/fronto-parietal control systems will disinhibit activities normally inhibited by Frontal lobes. These activities provide elementary coping patterns, like stimulus bound behaviors, which range from imitation and utilization to the lowest level of grasping and groping. The attempt to regain control of fragmenting forces would generate adaptive elementary coping strategies. Dependent on the extent of frontal lobe dysfunction, the resulting behaviors would include the utilization behaviors, which could be graded in elementary, low-level forms of object manipulation, to higher forms, in which manipulation is addressed to social relations and includes, as a consequence, deception (184, 185). The frontal lobe disinhibited requests for compensation-restitution will induce behaviors reaching levels that will be defined by observers as antisocial, or disruptive. This hypothesis is supported by studies showing that antisocial behavior is linked to dysfunctions of Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex, which is the part of the brain activated by social judgement and ethical tasks (186191). Ethological studies on deception, moreover, do show that this behavior does not need evolute Frontal Lobes to be enacted (192199). In animals, deception is stratified in different levels, according to the degree of cortical involvement required by the analysis of the environment (200). For instance, freezing represents an automated response to predators. This elementary deception strategy, empowered by subcortical “fear circuits” of the amygdala (201, 202), will be considered in our model as the core reflex subtending the “passive” coping strategy. But it is the higher level of deception, the “tactical deception,” that we consider in the model as the elementary behavior subtending the “active” response, stemming from stimulus-bound utilization behaviors (201, 202). Tactical deception relies on the utilization of normal repertoire contents, to manipulate other individuals of the same species. This level of deception needs a second-order thinking and is common in primates (203). The use of deception among primate species is correlated with the neocortical volumes (204), and appears despite frontal lobes do not reach, in these species, the outstanding, and possibly critical, size proper of the human species.

Therefore, in the model, we indicate two patterns of behavior: a passive coping strategy and an active strategy centered on manipulation. The first pattern is the feigned death reflex activity, which will appear with a range of manifestations including motor arrest, catatonia, Cotard, and Ganser Syndromes (205, 206).

The second pattern is manipulation and deception, which will emerge from utilization/stimulus-bound behaviors.

The two patterns of behavior, rather than randomly overlapping, are represented as stemming from fragmentation of the self, separately or as merging, according to supervening conditions (Figure 3).

We suggest that the suppression of “large world” connectivity, and its substitution with “small world” connectivity, observed in neurodegenerative disorders, explain the emergence of elementary behavioral patterns (Figure 4). This substitution is, in our model, represented by the suppression of the, long-distance, “large world” Fronto-Parietal Control Network, and the emergence of short cortico-cortical or cortico-subcortical, “small world” connections. The “Large world/Small World” connection duality was theorized in studies on human brain connectome, and found a prominent space in theories of consciousness (207216). The connection duality may explain the shift from a modality where the long-range frontoparietal connectivity governs the Posterior Cingulate Cortex to a modality where the lack of inhibition prompts short-range connections and produces behaviors that are resistant to compelling verifications required by consensual reality principles, which are proper of psychotic or dreaming states (212, 213, 217). Based on functional neuroimaging and neurophysiology studies, this model could be applicable to various conditions drawn together by psychotic symptoms and ranging from neurodegenerative disorders to psychedelic states due to the administration of tryptamines. The Posterior Cingulate Cortex is the hub of the Default Mode Network (DMN) (41), a network active during internal narration, suppressed by cognitive tasks (218). The anti-correlation between the activity of the DMN and Task-Positive Networks is necessary to shift focus from internal to external stimuli and plays a pivotal role in the continuous reality check, whose suspension can lead to the onset of psychosis (89, 90). This delicate balance is “peripherally” modulated by the Salience Network, but it heavily depends upon the thalamic pacing. An impairment of thalamo-cortical physiological activity (or ThalamoCortical dysrhythmia, TCD) alters the cortical activity and leads to a decrease of the anti-correlation between task-positive and task-negative systems, an increased propensity to “small-world” functional connections and, eventually, through the generation of random short-range connections motifs, introduces elements of internal narration into external stimuli.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Examples of “small-world” connections of the PCC (Posteriori Cingulate Cortex) (A) and representation of the Fronto-Parietal Network, a “Large-World” system involving the PCC (B1,B2). AC: auditory cortex; aPFC: anterior prefrontal cortex; dlPFC: dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; In: Insula; IPL: inferior parietal lobule; ITC: inferior-temporal cortex; OC: occipital cortex; PCC: posterior cingulate cortex; PrCe: precentral gyrus; PreCun: precuneus; S1: primary sensory cortex; TP: temporal pole. http://smart.servier.com.

Conclusion

A growing consensus indicates that FND are heterogeneous conditions that can present with motor or non-motor symptoms. In this context, implementing a multidisciplinary approach can lead to better awareness and management of these conditions. Indeed, neurologists are crucial in examining these patients, find “positive signs,” and providing a broader understanding of the neuroanatomical basis of the physical signs. On the other hand, psychiatrists provide critical neuro-psychoanalytical assessment and holistic management of the patient’s comorbidities, such as anxiety, depression, or personality disorders. This optimal approach requires a reappropriation of cultural competencies by the two disciplines. Unfortunately, in recent years the process has gone in the opposite direction. Experts have often expressed the fear that excessive nosologic parcellation of these conditions may forfeit the social costs and epidemiological relevance of SSD/FND. Furthermore, a perceived reduced relevance of these disorders within the public as produced by again nosologic parcellation is often feared to reduce the allocation of much-needed resources by the National Health systems. Our review provides the conceptual tools to reverse this process.

Our review, therefore, values the wealth of psychopathological approaches, underlining their place in understanding these disorders. By showing the complex history of psychopathological approaches, our review indicates the need to search for additional treatment strategies rather than offering physiotherapy and condescension to symptom presentation.

With an approach integrating psychiatry and neurology competencies, better chances could be offered for responsible insight and growth. Approaches, ranging from behavioral dialectic to cognitive, narrative exposure treatments, tentative inhibition, and pharmacological or interventional techniques, are based on a wealth of validated theories and experimental evidence. These combined strategies prevent the possibility of repeating older assumptions provided at the dawn of studies on the topic (18, 66, 83, 84, 106).

The proposed model mixes psychopathological core mechanisms with functional connectivity changes of cortical networks and biobehavioral expressions of disinhibition and the emergence of subcortical reactive patterns. The model indicates targets to be challenged and points to unresolved methodological issues. We provide the conceptual framework to encourage a multidisciplinary and versatile approach. Instead of a single disease model, our approach aims at bridging the gap between different cultures and disciplines like psychiatry, psychology, neurology, and clinical neurosciences.

Author contributions

MO and SS: conceptualization. AD, PA, MR, NM, and MO: methodology, writing-original draft preparation. AD, PA, MR, NM, SS, MO, GM, FF, and AT: writing-review and editing. SS and MO: supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Stone, J, Burton, C, and Carson, A. Recognising and explaining functional neurological disorder. BMJ. (2020) 371:m3745. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m3745

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Stone, J. Functional symptoms and signs in neurology: assessment and diagnosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2005) 76:i2–i12. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.061655

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Canna, M, and Seligman, R. Dealing with the unknown. Functional neurological disorder (FND) and the conversion of cultural meaning. Soc Sci Med. (2020) 246:112725. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. MacDuffie, KE, Grubbs, L, Best, T, LaRoche, S, Mildon, B, Myers, L, et al. Stigma and functional neurological disorder: a research agenda targeting the clinical encounter. CNS Spectr. (2021) 26:587–92. doi: 10.1017/S1092852920002084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Nielsen, G, Stone, J, and Edwards, MJ. Physiotherapy for functional (psychogenic) motor symptoms: a systematic review. J Psychosom Res. (2013) 75:93–102. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Gray, N, Savage, B, Scher, S, and Kozlowska, K. Psychologically informed physical therapy for children and adolescents with functional neurological symptoms: the wellness approach. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2020) 32:389–95. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19120355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Baizabal-Carvallo, JF, Hallett, M, and Jankovic, J. Pathogenesis and pathophysiology of functional (psychogenic) movement disorders. Neurobiol Dis. (2019) 127:32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2019.02.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Burke, MJ. “it’s all in your Head” - Medicine’s silent epidemic. JAMA Neurol. (2019) 76:1417–8. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.3043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Garrett, AR, Hodges, SD, and Stahlman, S. Epidemiology of functional neurological disorder, active component, U.S. armed forces, 2000–2018. MSMR. (2020) 27:16–22.

Google Scholar

10. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

11. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association (1980).

Google Scholar

12. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV). Washington: American Psychiatric Association (1994).

Google Scholar

13. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2000).

Google Scholar

14. World Health Organization. International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems: 10th revision (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO (2004).

Google Scholar

15. Merskey, H. Conversion, dissociation, or doxomorphic disorder In: P Halligan and C Bass, editors. Contemporary approaches to the study of hysteria. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2001). 171–81.

Google Scholar

16. Galli, S, Tatu, L, Bogousslavsky, J, and Aybek, S (2018) Neurologic-psychiatric syndromes in focus part II—from psychiatry to neurology pp. 72–80, Basel: Karger.

Google Scholar

17. Onofrj, M, Digiovanni, A, Ajdinaj, P, Russo, M, Carrarini, C, di Giannantonio, M, et al. The factitious/malingering continuum and its burden on public health costs: a review and experience in an Italian neurology setting. Neurol Sci. (2021) 42:4073–83. doi: 10.1007/s10072-021-05422-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Bass, CM, Halligan, PW, and Marshall, JC. Contemporary approaches to the study of hysteria. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (2001).

Google Scholar

19. Hallett, M. Functional movement disorders: is the crisis resolved? Mov Disord. (2019) 34:971–4. doi: 10.1002/MDS.27713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hallett, M, Aybek, S, Dworetzky, BA, McWhirter, L, Staab, JP, and Stone, J. Functional neurological disorder: new subtypes and shared mechanisms. Lancet Neurol. (2022) 21:537–50. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(21)00422-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Bennett, K, Diamond, C, Hoeritzauer, I, Gardiner, P, McWhirter, L, Carson, A, et al. A practical review of functional neurological disorder (FND) for the general physician. Clin Med. (2021) 21:28–36. doi: 10.7861/clinmed.2020-0987

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Head, H. An address on the diagnosis of hysteria. Br Med J. (1922) 1:827–9. doi: 10.1136/bmj.1.3204.827

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Arieff, AJ. The hoover sign. Arch Neurol. (1961) 5:673. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1961.00450180095012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Jimenez, XF, Aboussouan, A, and Johnson, J. Functional neurological disorder responds Favorably to interdisciplinary rehabilitation models. Psychosomatics. (2019) 60:556–62. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2019.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Nielsen, G, Stone, J, Matthews, A, Brown, M, Sparkes, C, Farmer, R, et al. Physiotherapy for functional motor disorders: a consensus recommendation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2015) 86:1113–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-309255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Maggio, JB, Ospina, JP, Callahan, J, Hunt, AL, Stephen, CD, and Perez, DL. Outpatient physical therapy for functional neurological disorder: a preliminary feasibility and naturalistic outcome study in a U.S. JNP. (2020) 32:85–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19030068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Jacob, AE, Kaelin, DL, Roach, AR, Ziegler, CH, and LaFaver, K. Motor retraining (MoRe) for functional movement disorders: outcomes from a 1-week multidisciplinary rehabilitation program. PM&R. (2018) 10:1164–72. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2018.05.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Gelauff, J, Stone, J, Edwards, M, and Carson, A. The prognosis of functional (psychogenic) motor symptoms: a systematic review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2014) 85:220–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-305321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. LaFaver, K. Treatment of functional movement disorders. Neurol Clin. (2020) 38:469–80. doi: 10.1016/j.ncl.2020.01.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Stone, J, Zeman, A, Simonotto, E, Meyer, M, Azuma, R, Flett, S, et al. fMRI in patients with motor conversion symptoms and controls with simulated weakness. Psychosom Med. (2007) 69:961–9. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815b6c14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Jinnah, HA, Neychev, V, and Hess, EJ. The anatomical basis for dystonia: the motor network model. Tremor Other Hyperkinet Mov (N Y). (2017) 7:506. doi: 10.7916/D8V69X3S

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Voon, V, Cavanna, AE, Coburn, K, Sampson, S, Reeve, A, and LaFrance, WC Jr, (On behalf of the American Neuropsychiatric Association Committee for Research) (2016) Functional Neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of functional neurological disorders (conversion disorder). J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 28:168–190. doi:doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.14090217

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Edwards, MJ, and Bhatia, KP. Functional (psychogenic) movement disorders: merging mind and brain. Lancet Neurol. (2012) 11:250–60. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70310-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Dreissen, YE, Lambert, F, Dijk, JM, Koelman, JHTM, and Tijssen, MAJ. Botulinum neurotoxin (BoNT) treatment in functional movement disorders: long-term follow-up. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2020) 91:1120–1. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2020-323684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. di Pietro, M, Russo, M, Dono, F, Carrarini, C, Thomas, A, di Stefano, V, et al. A critical review of alien limb-related phenomena and implications for functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:661130. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.661130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Lingiardi, V, and McWilliams, N. Psychodynamic diagnostic manual: PDM-2. New York, NY: Guilford Publications (2017).

Google Scholar

37. Onofrj, M, Bonanni, L, Manzoli, L, and Thomas, A. Cohort study on somatoform disorders in Parkinson disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology. (2010) 74:1598–606. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181df09dd

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Onofrj, M, Thomas, A, Bonanni, L, di Giannantonio, M, Gambi, F, and Sepede, G. Somatoform disorders in Parkinson’s disease and dementia with lewy bodies evidence underlying psychotic traits In:. Psychiatry of Parkinson’s disease. Basel: S. Karger AG (2012). 125–32.

Google Scholar

39. Carrozzino, D, Bech, P, Patierno, C, Onofrj, M, Morberg, BM, Thomas, A, et al. Somatization in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 78:18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.05.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Onofrj, M, Russo, M, Carrarini, C, Delli Pizzi, S, Thomas, A, Bonanni, L, et al. Functional neurological disorder and somatic symptom disorder in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci. (2022) 433:120017. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2021.120017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Franciotti, R, Delli Pizzi, S, Russo, M, Carrarini, C, Carrozzino, D, Perfetti, B, et al. Somatic symptoms disorders in Parkinson’s disease are related to default mode and salience network dysfunction. Neuroimage Clin. (2019) 23:101932. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101932

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Onofrj, M, Thomas, A, Tiraboschi, P, Wenning, G, Gambi, F, Sepede, G, et al. Updates on somatoform disorders (SFMD) in Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies and discussion of phenomenology. J Neurol Sci. (2011) 310:166–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.07.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Anzellotti, F, Dono, F, Evangelista, G, di Pietro, M, Carrarini, C, Russo, M, et al. Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures and pseudo-refractory epilepsy, a management challenge. Front Neurol. (2020) 11:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00461

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Walzl, D, Solomon, AJ, and Stone, J. Functional neurological disorder and multiple sclerosis: a systematic review of misdiagnosis and clinical overlap. J Neurol. (2022) 269:654–63. doi: 10.1007/s00415-021-10436-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hallett, M. Psychogenic parkinsonism. J Neurol Sci. (2011) 310:163–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.03.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Bonanni, L, Franciotti, R, Martinotti, G, Vellante, F, Flacco, ME, di Giannantonio, M, et al. Post traumatic stress disorder heralding the onset of semantic Frontotemporal dementia. J Alzheimers Dis. (2018) 63:203–15. doi: 10.3233/JAD-171134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Lingiardi, V, and McWilliams, N. The psychodynamic diagnostic manual - 2nd edition (PDM-2). World Psychiatry. (2015) 14:237–9. doi: 10.1002/wps.20233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Turner, M. Malingering. Br J Psychiatry. (1997) 171:409–11. doi: 10.1192/bjp.171.5.409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Mele, A. Recent work on self-deception. Am Philos Q. (1987) 24:1–17.

Google Scholar

50. Carson, AJ, Brown, R, David, AS, Duncan, R, Edwards, MJ, Goldstein, LH, et al. Functional (conversion) neurological symptoms: research since the millennium. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2012) 83:842–50. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2011-301860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kinzie, JD, and Bolton, JM. Psychiatry with the aborigines of West Malaysia. Am J Psychiatr. (1973) 130:769–73. doi: 10.1176/ajp.130.7.769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bures, F. The geography of madness: penis thieves, voodoo death, and the search for the meaning of the world’s strangest syndromes (2016). 241 p.

Google Scholar

53. Lanska, DJ. Jumping Frenchmen, Miryachit, and Latah: culture-specific Hyperstartle-plus syndromes. Front Neurol Neurosci. (2018) 42:122–31. doi: 10.1159/000475700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Saint-Hilaire, MH, Saint-Hilaire, JM, and Granger, L. Jumping Frenchmen of Maine. Neurology. (1986) 36:1269–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.36.9.1269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Yap, PM. The latah reaction: its pathodynamics and nosological position. J Ment Sci. (1952) 98:515–64. doi: 10.1192/BJP.98.413.515

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Bogousslavsky, J. Neurologic-psychiatric syndromes in focus. Part II: from psychiatry to neurology In: J Bogousslavsky, editor. Frontiers of neurology and neuroscience. Basel: Karger (2018). 122–31.

Google Scholar

57. Miller, LJ. Divine punishment or disease? Medieval and early modern approaches to the 1518 Strasbourg dancing plague (2017) 35:149–64. doi: 10.3366/DRS.2017.0199,

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Waller, J. A forgotten plague: making sense of dancing mania. Lancet. (2009) 373:624–5. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60386-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Olvera, C, Stebbins, GT, Goetz, CG, and Kompoliti, K. TikTok tics: a pandemic within a pandemic. Mov Disord Clin Pract. (2021) 8:1200–5. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.13316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Hull, M, and Parnes, M. Tics and TikTok: functional tics spread through social media. Mov Disord Clin Pract. (2021) 8:1248–52. doi: 10.1002/MDC3.13267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Lakhan, SE, and Schofield, KL. Mindfulness-based therapies in the treatment of somatization disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. (2013) 8:e71834. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Moene, F, Roelofs, K, and Nash, MRBAJ. Hypnosis in the treatment of conversion and somatization disorders In: MR Nash and AJ Barnier, editors. The Oxford handbook of hypnosis. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press (2008). 625–45.

Google Scholar

63. Masuda, A, and Tully, EC. The role of mindfulness and psychological flexibility in somatization, depression, anxiety, and general psychological distress in a nonclinical college sample. J Evid Based Complement Altern Med. (2012) 17:66–71. doi: 10.1177/2156587211423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Tinazzi, M, Geroin, C, Erro, R, Marcuzzo, E, Cuoco, S, Ceravolo, R, et al. Functional motor disorders associated with other neurological diseases: beyond the boundaries of “organic” neurology. Eur J Neurol. (2021) 28:1752–8. doi: 10.1111/ene.14674

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Dogonowski, AM, Andersen, KW, Sellebjerg, F, Schreiber, K, Madsen, KH, and Siebner, HR. Functional neuroimaging of recovery from motor conversion disorder: a case report. NeuroImage. (2019) 190:269–74. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2018.03.061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Espay, AJ, Aybek, S, Carson, A, Edwards, MJ, Goldstein, LH, Hallett, M, et al. Current concepts in diagnosis and treatment of functional neurological disorders. JAMA Neurol. (2018) 75:1132–41. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.1264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Charcot, J-M. Leçons sur les maladies du système nerveux. Leçon d’ouverture Œuvres Complètes. (1887) 3:1–22.

Google Scholar

68. Gherovici, P. Where have the hysterics gone?: Lacan’s reinvention of hysteria. Engl Stud Can. (2014) 40:47–70. doi: 10.1353/ESC.2014.0000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Goetz, CGG (2016) Charcot, hysteria, and simulated disorders. In: Handbook of clinical neurology. Netherlands, pp. 11–23

Google Scholar

70. Kanaan, RAA. Freud’s hysteria and its legacy. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:37–44. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00004-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Freud, S, and Breuer, J. Studies in hysteria. London: Penguin Press (2004).

Google Scholar

72. Silveira, A. Cerebral systems in the pathogenesis of endogenous psychoses. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. (1999) 57:1046–56. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X1999000600026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Micale, MS. Charcot and les névroses traumatiques: scientific and historical reflections. J Hist Neurosci. (1995) 4:101–19. doi: 10.1080/09647049509525630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Edwards, MJ. Neurobiologic theories of functional neurologic disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:131–7. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00012-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Maurer, CW, LaFaver, K, Ameli, R, Epstein, SA, Hallett, M, and Horovitz, SG. Impaired self-agency in functional movement disorders. Neurology. (2016) 87:564–70. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002940

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Delange, F, Roelofs, K, and Toni, I. Increased self-monitoring during imagined movements in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia. (2007) 45:2051–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Wegrzyk, J, Kebets, V, Richiardi, J, Galli, S, de Ville, DV, and Aybek, S. Identifying motor functional neurological disorder using resting-state functional connectivity. Neuroimage Clin. (2018) 17:163–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.10.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Onofrj, M, Bonanni, L, Delli Pizzi, S, Caulo, M, Onofrj, V, Thomas, A, et al. Cortical activation during levitation and Tentacular movements of Corticobasal syndrome 94:e1977. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000001977,

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Neuner, I, Werner, CJ, Arrubla, J, Stöcker, T, Ehlen, C, Wegener, HP, et al. Imaging the where and when of tic generation and resting state networks in adult Tourette patients. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:362. doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2014.00362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Sasikumar, S, and Strafella, AP. The neuroimaging evidence of brain abnormalities in functional movement disorders. Brain. (2021) 144:2278–83. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Perez, DL, Nicholson, TR, Asadi-Pooya, AA, Bègue, I, Butler, M, Carson, AJ, et al. Neuroimaging in functional neurological disorder: state of the field and research agenda. Neuroimage Clin. (2021) 30:102623. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Stone, J, Zeman, A, Simonotto, E, Meyer, M, Azuma, R, Flett, S, et al. FMRI in patients with motor conversion symptoms and controls with simulated weakness. Psychosom Med. (2007) 69:961–9. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815b6c14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Spence, SA, Crimlisk, HL, Cope, H, Ron, MA, and Grasby, PM. Discrete neurophysiological correlates in prefrontal cortex during hysterical and feigned disorder of movement. Lancet. (2000) 355:1243–4. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02096-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Babinski, J. Démembrement de l’hystérie traditionnelle: pithiatisme. Paris: Imprimerie de la Semaine Médicale. (1909).

Google Scholar

85. Babinski, J. Définition de l’hystérie. Rev Neurol (Paris). (1901) 9:1074–80.

Google Scholar

86. Kranick, S, Ekanayake, V, Martinez, V, Ameli, R, Hallett, M, and Voon, V. Psychopathology and psychogenic movement disorders. Mov Disord. (2011) 26:1844–50. doi: 10.1002/mds.23830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Freud, S. Fragment of an analysis of a case of hysteria. London: Vintage (1905).

Google Scholar

88. Masson, J, Bernoussi, A, and Regourd-Laizeau, M. From the influence of traumas to therapeutic letting go: the contribution of hypnosis and EMDR. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. (2016) 64:350–64. doi: 10.1080/00207144.2016.1171108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Wissel, BD, Dwivedi, AK, Merola, A, Chin, D, Jacob, C, Duker, AP, et al. Functional neurological disorders in Parkinson disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2018) 89:566–71. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2017-317378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Onofrj, M, Espay, AJ, Bonanni, L, Delli Pizzi, S, and Sensi, SL. Hallucinations, somatic-functional disorders of PD-DLB as expressions of thalamic dysfunction. Mov Disord. (2019) 34:1100–11. doi: 10.1002/mds.27781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Levy, S. Post-encephalitic behavior disorder; a forgotten entity: a report of 100 cases. Am J Psychiatry. (1959) 115:1062–7. doi: 10.1176/ajp.115.12.1062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Hoffman, LA, and Vilensky, JA. Encephalitis lethargica: 100 years after the epidemic. Brain. (2017) 140:2246–51. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Vilensky, JA, Gilman, S, and McCall, S. Does the historical literature on encephalitis lethargica support a simple (direct) relationship with postencephalitic parkinsonism? Mov Disord. (2010) 25:1124–30. doi: 10.1002/mds.22991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Russo, M, Carrarini, C, Dono, F, Rispoli, MG, di Pietro, M, di Stefano, V, et al. The pharmacology of visual hallucinations in Synucleinopathies. Front Pharmacol. (2019) 10. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.01379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Bass, C, and Wade, DT. Malingering and factitious disorder. Pract Neurol. (2019) 19:96–105. doi: 10.1136/practneurol-2018-001950

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Schnellbacher, S, and O’Mara, H. Identifying and managing malingering and factitious disorder in the military. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2016) 18:105. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0740-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Asher, R. Munchausen’s syndrome. Lancet. (1951) 257:339–41. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(51)92313-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Feldman, MD, and Yates, GP. Dying to be ill: True stories of medical deception. London: Routledge (2018).

Google Scholar

99. Cloninger, CR. The origins of DSM and ICD criteria for conversion and somatization disorders In: PW Halligan, C Bass, and JC Marshall, editors. Contemporary approaches to the study of hysteria. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (2001). 49–62.

Google Scholar

100. Sigvardsson, S. An adoption study of somatoform disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1984) 41:853. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790200035005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. World Health Organization ICD-11 revision. Available at: https://icd.who.int/en (Accessed October 6, 2022).

Google Scholar

102. Nonis, M, Bertinato, L, Arcangeli, L, Cadeddu, C, Palinuro, A, Frattura, L, et al. The evolution of drg system in Italy: the it-drg project. Eur J Pub Health. (2018) 28. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cky218.095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Rukovets, O. Is it time to update psychiatry training for neurologists? Neurol Today. (2022) 22:16–7. doi: 10.1097/01.NT.0000819848.93457.71

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Cloninger, CR, von Knorring, A-L, Sigvardsson, S, Bohman, M, and Conneally, PM. Symptom patterns and causes of somatization in men: II. Genetic and environmental independence from somatization in women. Genet Epidemiol. (1986) 3:171–85. doi: 10.1002/gepi.1370030304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Guze, S. Genetics of Briquet’s syndrome and somatization disorder: a review of family, adoption, and twin studies. Ann Clin Psychiatry. (1993) 5:225–30. doi: 10.3109/10401239309148821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Pal, R, Romero, E, He, Z, Stevenson, T, and Campen, CJ. Pediatric functional neurological disorder: demographic and clinical factors impacting care. J Child Neurol. (2022) 37:669–76. doi: 10.1177/08830738221113899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Babinski, J, and Froment, J. Hystérie-pithiatisme et troubles nerveux d’ordre réflexe en neurologie de guerre. In: Masson et C, Editeurs. Paris: Libraires de l’Academie de Medecine. (1917).

Google Scholar

108. Briquet, P. Traité clinique et thérapeutique de l’hystérie. Paris: J.-B. Baillière et Fils, Libraires De L’académie Impériale De Médecine. (1859).

Google Scholar

109. Bratanov, C, Hot, P, and Vercueil, L. The natural history of terms describing functional (neurological) disorders in the medical literature of the last 60 years. J Neurol. (2022). doi: 10.1007/s00415-022-11526-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Kuhn and Feyerabend (2007). In: Theories of scientific method. Buckinghamshire: Acumen Publishing Limited, pp. 285–310.

Google Scholar

111. Kuhn, TS. The structure of scientific revolutions. 1st ed. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press (1962).

Google Scholar

112. Sussmen, N, and Hyler, SE. Kaplan and Sadock In:. Factitious disorders in comprehensive textbook of psychiatry. 4th ed. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins (1987)

Google Scholar

113. Jung, CG. Volume 6 Collected Works of C.G. Jung. In: Adler G, Hull, RFC, editors. Princeton: Princeton University Press. (1971). doi: 10.1515/9781400850860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Foucher, JR, Gawlik, M, Roth, JN, de Crespin de Billy, C, Jeanjean, LC, Obrecht, A, et al. Wernicke-Kleist-Leonhard phenotypes of endogenous psychoses: a review of their validity. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2020) 22:37–49. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2020.22.1/jfoucher

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Kendler, KS, and Engstrom, EJ. Kahlbaum, Hecker, and Kraepelin and the transition from psychiatric symptom complexes to empirical disease forms. Am J Psychiatry. 174:102–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Kraepelin, E, and Defendorf, AR. Clinical psychiatry: A text-book for students and physicians: Abstracted and adapted from the 6th German edition of Kraepelin’s Lehrbuch der psychiatrie. New York, London: Macmillan (1904).

Google Scholar

117. Kraepelin, E. Dementia praecox and paraphrenia. Chicago, IL: Chicago Medical Book Co. (1919).

Google Scholar

118. Bleuler, E (1911). Dementia praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien.

Google Scholar

119. Leonhard, K, and Eli, R. The classification of endogenous psychoses. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (1979). 452 p.

Google Scholar

120. Trostorff, VS, and Leonhard, K. Catamnesis of endogenous psychoses according to the differential diagnostic method of Karl Leonhard. Psychopathology. (1990) 23:259–62. doi: 10.1159/000284669

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Leonhard, K. Cycloid psychoses--endogenous psychoses which are neither schizophrenic nor manic-depressive. J Ment Sci. (1961) 107:633–48. doi: 10.1192/BJP.107.449.633

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Leonhard, K. Pathogenesis of manic-depressive disease. Nervenarzt. (1957) 28:271–2.

Google Scholar

123. Freud, S. Chapter IX. My views on the Rôle of sexuality in the Etiology of the neuroses In:. Selected papers on hysteria and other psychoneuroses. New York: The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Company (1912)

Google Scholar

124. Chodoff, P, and Lyons, H. Hysteria, the hysterical personality and hysterical conversion. Am J Psychiatry. (1958) 114:734–40. doi: 10.1176/AJP.114.8.734

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Rangell, L. The nature of conversion. J Am Psychoanal Assoc. (1959) 7:632–62. doi: 10.1177/000306515900700403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Zetzel, ER. The so called good hysteric. Int J Psychoanal. (1968) 49:256–60.

Google Scholar

127. Kernberg Otto, F (1985). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. New York: Jason Aronson, Inc. 3–49

Google Scholar

128. Khan, R. Grudge and the hysteric. Int J Psychoanal Psychother. (1975) 4:349–57.

Google Scholar

129. Lacan, J. Remarks on hysteria. In: Stone JW, Translator. Quarto: Intervention of Jacques Lacan in Brussels (Belgian Supplement to La lettre mensuelle de l’École de la cause freudienne). (1981).

Google Scholar

130. Jacques, L. Le Séminaire. Livre XVII: L’Envers de la psychanalyse. 1969–1970. Paris: Seuil (1991).

Google Scholar

131. Cloninger, CR. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1987) 44:573. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800180093014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Cloninger, C. Temperament and personality. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1994) 4:266–73. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90083-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Richard, M, Bass, CM, and Michael, S. Treatment of functional somatic symptoms. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1995). 452 p.

Google Scholar

134. Teasell, RW, and Shapiro, AP. Strategic-behavioral intervention in the treatment of chronic nonorganic motor disorders. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. (1994) 73:44–50.

Google Scholar

135. Selby-Bigge, LA. Enquiries concerning human understanding and concerning the principles of morals. Oxford University Press. (1975).

Google Scholar

136. Spence, SA. Hysteria: a new look. Psychiatry. (2006) 5:56–60. doi: 10.1383/psyt.2006.5.2.56

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Spence, SA. Hysterical paralyses as disorders of action. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (1999) 4:203–26. doi: 10.1080/135468099395936

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Abe, N. The neurobiology of deception: evidence from neuroimaging and loss-of-function studies. Curr Opin Neurol. (2009) 22:594–600. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328332c3cf

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Fava, GA, Cosci, F, and Sonino, N. Current psychosomatic practice. Psychother Psychosom. (2017) 86:13–30. doi: 10.1159/000448856

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Bègue, I, Adams, C, Stone, J, and Perez, DL. Structural alterations in functional neurological disorder and related conditions: a software and hardware problem? Neuroimage Clin. (2019) 22:101798. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Schovsbo, SU, Dantoft, TM, Thuesen, BH, Leth-Møller, KB, Eplov, LF, Petersen, MW, et al. Social position and functional somatic disorders: the DanFunD study. Scand J Public Health. (2023) 51:225–32. doi: 10.1177/14034948211056752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Hinson, VK, Weinstein, S, Bernard, B, Leurgans, SE, and Goetz, CG. Single-blind clinical trial of psychotherapy for treatment of psychogenic movement disorders. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2006) 12:177–80. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2005.10.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Kompoliti, K, Wilson, B, Stebbins, G, Bernard, B, and Hinson, V. Immediate vs. delayed treatment of psychogenic movement disorders with short term psychodynamic psychotherapy: randomized clinical trial. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2014) 20:60–3. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2013.09.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Abbass, A, Lumley, MA, Town, J, Holmes, H, Luyten, P, Cooper, A, et al. Short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy for functional somatic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis of within-treatment effects. J Psychosom Res. (2021) 145:110473. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2021.110473

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Goldstein, LH, Chalder, T, Chigwedere, C, Khondoker, MR, Moriarty, J, Toone, BK, et al. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a pilot RCT. Neurology. (2010) 74:1986. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181e39658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Hinson, VK, and Haren, WB. Psychogenic movement disorders. Lancet Neurol. (2006) 5:695–700. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70523-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Kroenke, K, and Swindle, R. Cognitive-Behavioral therapy for somatization and symptom syndromes: a critical review of controlled clinical trials. Psychother Psychosom. (2000) 69:205–15. doi: 10.1159/000012395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Christensen, SS, Frostholm, L, Ørnbøl, E, and Schröder, A. Changes in illness perceptions mediated the effect of cognitive behavioural therapy in severe functional somatic syndromes. J Psychosom Res. (2015) 78:363–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. LaFrance, WC, Miller, IW, Ryan, CE, Blum, AS, Solomon, DA, Kelley, JE, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2009) 14:591–6. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.02.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Cope, SR, Smith, JG, King, T, and Agrawal, N. Evaluation of a pilot innovative cognitive-behavioral therapy-based psychoeducation group treatment for functional non-epileptic attacks. Epilepsy Behav. (2017) 70:238–44. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2017.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Warner, CM, Colognori, D, Kim, RE, Reigada, LC, Klein, RG, Browner-Elhanan, KJ, et al. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of persistent functional somatic complaints and pediatric anxiety: an initial controlled trial. Depress Anxiety. (2011) 28:551–9. doi: 10.1002/da.20821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Fjorback, LO, Arendt, M, Ørnbøl, E, Walach, H, Rehfeld, E, Schröder, A, et al. Mindfulness therapy for somatization disorder and functional somatic syndromes — randomized trial with one-year follow-up. J Psychosom Res. (2013) 74:31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Baslet, G, Dworetzky, B, Perez, DL, and Oser, M. Treatment of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Clin EEG Neurosci. (2015) 46:54–64. doi: 10.1177/1550059414557025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Markert, C, Gomm, C, Ehlert, U, Gaab, J, and Nater, UM. Effects of cognitive-behavioral stress management training in individuals with functional somatic symptoms – an exploratory randomized controlled trial. Stress. (2019) 22:696–706. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2019.1625329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Moene, FC, Spinhoven, P, Hoogduin, KAL, and van Dyck, R. A randomised controlled clinical trial on the additional effect of hypnosis in a comprehensive treatment programme for in-patients with conversion disorder of the motor type. Psychother Psychosom. (2002) 71:66–76. doi: 10.1159/000049348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Myers, L, Vaidya-Mathur, U, and Lancman, M. Prolonged exposure therapy for the treatment of patients diagnosed with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Epilepsy Behav. (2017) 66:86–92. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.10.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Zaroff, CM, Myers, L, Barr, WB, Luciano, D, and Devinsky, O. Group psychoeducation as treatment for psychological nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2004) 5:587–92. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2004.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Voon, V, and Lang, AE. Antidepressant treatment outcomes of psychogenic movement disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. (2005) 66:1529–34. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v66n1206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Czarnecki, K, Thompson, JM, Seime, R, Geda, YE, Duffy, JR, and Ahlskog, JE. Functional movement disorders: successful treatment with a physical therapy rehabilitation protocol. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2012) 18:247–51. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2011.10.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Nielsen, G, Ricciardi, L, Demartini, B, Hunter, R, Joyce, E, and Edwards, MJ. Outcomes of a 5-day physiotherapy programme for functional (psychogenic) motor disorders. J Neurol. (2015) 262:674–81. doi: 10.1007/s00415-014-7631-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Jordbru, AA, Smedstad, LM, Klungsøyr, O, and Martinsen, EW. Psychogenic gait disorder: a randomized controlled trial of physical rehabilitation with one-year follow-up. J Rehabil Med. (2014) 46:181–7. doi: 10.2340/16501977-1246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Pick, S, Hodsoll, J, Stanton, B, Eskander, A, Stavropoulos, I, and Samra, K. Trial of Neurostimulation in conversion symptoms (TONICS): a feasibility randomised controlled trial of transcranial magnetic stimulation for functional limb weakness. BMJ Open. (2020) 10:e037198. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-037198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Garcin, B, Roze, E, Mesrati, F, Cognat, E, Fournier, E, Vidailhet, M, et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation as an efficient treatment for psychogenic movement disorders. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2013) 84:1043–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-304062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Nicholson, TRJ, and Voon, V. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and sedation as treatment for functional neurologic disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. (2016) 139:619–29. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801772-2.00050-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Pollak, TA, Nicholson, TR, Edwards, MJ, and David, AS. A systematic review of transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of functional (conversion) neurological symptoms. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2014) 85:191–7. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-304181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Garcin, B, Mesrati, F, Hubsch, C, Mauras, T, Iliescu, I, Naccache, L, et al. Impact of Transcranial magnetic stimulation on functional movement disorders: cortical modulation or a Behavioral effect? Front Neurol. (2017) 8:338. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Wijemanne, S, and Jankovic, J. Movement disorders in catatonia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2015) 86:825–32. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-309098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Moene, FC, Spinhoven, P, Hoogduin, KAL, and van Dyck, RA. Randomised controlled clinical trial on the additional effect of hypnosis in a comprehensive treatment programme for in-patients with conversion disorder of the motor type. Psychother Psychosom. 71:66–76. doi: 10.1159/000049348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Moene, FC, Spinhoven, P, Hoogduin, KAL, and van Dyck, R. A randomized controlled clinical trial of a hypnosis-based treatment for patients with conversion disorder, motor type. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. (2003) 51:29–50. doi: 10.1076/iceh.51.1.29.14067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. McLean, CP, Levy, HC, Miller, ML, and Tolin, DF. Exposure therapy for PTSD: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2022) 91. doi: 10.1016/J.CPR.2021.102115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Lely, JCG, Smid, GE, Jongedijk, RA, Knipscheer, JW, and Kleber, RJ. The effectiveness of narrative exposure therapy: a review, meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis. Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2019) 10. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2018.1550344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Giotakos, O. Neurobiology of emotional trauma. Psychiatriki. (2020) 31:162–71. doi: 10.22365/JPSYCH.2020.312.162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Keough, KA, and Markus, HR. The role of the self in building the bridge from philosophy to biology. Psychol Inq. (1998) 9:49–53. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0901_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Wang, J, Jiang, F, Yang, Y, Zhang, Y, Liu, Z, Qin, X, et al. Off-label use of antipsychotic medications in psychiatric inpatients in China: a national real-world survey. BMC Psychiatry. (2021) 21:375. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03374-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. Nagoshi, Y, Tominaga, T, and Fukui, K. Effect of Aripiprazole augmentation for treatment-resistant somatoform disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2014) 34:397–8. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0000000000000063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Huang, M, Luo, B, Hu, J, Wei, N, Chen, L, Wang, S, et al. Combination of citalopram plus paliperidone is better than citalopram alone in the treatment of somatoform disorder. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. (2012) 27:151–8. doi: 10.1097/YIC.0b013e328351c7e8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

177. Chiappini, S, Mosca, A, Martinotti, G, di Carlo, F, Miuli, A, Dattoli, L, et al. Brexpiprazole as a new approach of treatment in somatization disorder. Emerging Trends Drugs Addict Health. (2022) 2:100031. doi: 10.1016/j.etdah.2022.100031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. Delli Pizzi, S, Franciotti, R, Chiacchiaretta, P, Ferretti, A, Edden, RA, Sestieri, C, et al. Altered medial prefrontal connectivity in parkinson’s disease patients with somatic symptoms. Mov Disord. (2022) 37:2226–35. doi: 10.1002/mds.29187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Delli Pizzi, S, Franciotti, R, Ferretti, A, Edden, RAE, Zöllner, HJ, Esposito, R, et al. Highγ‐AminobutyricAcid content within the medial prefrontal cortex is a functional signature of somatic symptoms disorder in patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2020) 35:2184–92. doi: 10.1002/mds.28221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Fayed, N, Andres, E, Rojas, G, Moreno, S, Serrano-Blanco, A, Roca, M, et al. Brain dysfunction in fibromyalgia and somatization disorder using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy: a controlled study. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2012) 126:115–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01820.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Höflich, A, Hahn, A, Küblböck, M, Kranz, GS, Vanicek, T, Windischberger, C, et al. Ketamine-induced modulation of the Thalamo-cortical network in healthy volunteers as a model for schizophrenia. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. (2015) 18:pyv040. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyv040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Nunes, MV, Adelino, MPM, Ajub, E, Quarantini, LC, and Lacerda, ALT. Efficacy of esketamine in the treatment of negative symptoms in schizophrenia – a case series. Schizophr Res. (2018) 202:394–6. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.06.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

183. Delli Pizzi, S, Chiacchiaretta, P, Mantini, D, Bubbico, G, Edden, RA, Onofrj, M, et al. GABA content within medial prefrontal cortex predicts the variability of fronto-limbic effective connectivity. Brain Struct Funct. (2017) 222:3217–29. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1399-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

184. Lhermitte, F, Pillon, B, and Serdaru, M. Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: imitation and utilization behavior: a neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Ann Neurol. (1986) 19:326–34. doi: 10.1002/ANA.410190404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. Abe, N. The neurobiology of deception: evidence from neuroimaging and loss-of-function studies. Curr Opin Neurol. (2009) 22:594–600. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0B013E328332C3CF

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

186. Alegria, AA, Radua, J, and Rubia, K. Meta-analysis of fMRI studies of disruptive behavior disorders. Am J Psychiatr. (2016) 173:1119–30. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15081089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Ogilvie, JM, Stewart, AL, Chan, RCK, and Shum, DHK. Neuropsychological measures of executive function and antisocial behavior: a META-analysis*. Criminology. (2011) 49:1063–107. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00252.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

188. Morgan, AB, and Lilienfeld, SO. A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clin Psychol Rev. (2000) 20:113–36. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00096-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

189. Lisofsky, N, Kazzer, P, Heekeren, HR, and Prehn, K. Investigating socio-cognitive processes in deception: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia. (2014) 61:113–22. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2014.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

190. Volz, KG, Vogeley, K, Tittgemeyer, M, von Cramon, DY, and Sutter, M. The neural basis of deception in strategic interactions. Front Behav Neurosci. (2015) 9. doi: 10.3389/FNBEH.2015.00027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

191. Zeki, S, Goodenough, OR, Spence, SA, Hunter, MD, Farrow, TFD, Green, RD, et al. A cognitive neurobiological account of deception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B. (2004) 359:1755–62. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2004.1555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

192. Humphreys, RK, and Ruxton, GD. A review of thanatosis (death feigning) as an anti-predator behaviour. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. (2018) 72:22. doi: 10.1007/s00265-017-2436-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

193. Byrne, RW, and Corp, N. Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proc Biol Sci. (2004) 271:1693–9. doi: 10.1098/RSPB.2004.2780

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

194. Smith, WJ (William, J (1977). The behavior of communicating: an ethological approach. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 545

Google Scholar

195. Kirkpatrick, CL. Tactical deception and the great apes: insight into the question of theory of mind. UWOJA. (2007) 15:31–4. doi: 10.5206/uwoja.v15i1.8872

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

196. Srinivasan, MV, and Davey, M. Strategies for active camouflage of motion. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (1995) 259:19–25. doi: 10.1098/RSPB.1995.0004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

197. Hauser, MD. Costs of deception: cheaters are punished in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (1992) 89:12137–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.24.12137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

198. Angilletta, MJ, Kubitz G, Wilson, RS. Self-deception in nonhuman animals: weak crayfish escalated aggression as if they were strong. Behav Ecol 30, 1469–1476. doi:doi: 10.1093/beheco/arz103.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

199. Šekrst, K. Everybody lies: deception levels in various domains of life. Biosemiotics. 15:309–24. doi: 10.1007/s12304-022-09485-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

200. Bixler, RH. Robert W. Mitchell & Nicholas S. Thompson (Eds.) deception: perspectives on human and non-human deceit. Albany: State University of new York, 1986, 388 pp. Behav Sci. (1987) 32:238–9. doi: 10.1002/BS.3830320308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

201. LeDoux, JBJR. The emotional brain. J Am Acad Psychoanal. (1997) 25:525–8.

Google Scholar

202. LeDoux, J (2003) Synaptic self: how our brains become who we are.

Google Scholar

203. Whiten, A, and Byrne, RW. Tactical deception in primates. Behav Brain Sci. (1988) 11:233–44. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00049682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

204. Byrne, RW, and Corp, N. Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2004) 271:1693–9. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2780

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

205. Debruyne, H, Portzky, M, van den Eynde, F, and Audenaert, K. Cotard’s syndrome: a review. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2009) 11:197–202. doi: 10.1007/s11920-009-0031-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

206. Dieguez, S. Ganser syndrome. Front Neurol Neurosci. (2018) 42:1–22. doi: 10.1159/000475676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

207. Bassett, DS, and Bullmore, ET. Small-world brain networks revisited. Neuroscientist. (2017) 23:499–516. doi: 10.1177/1073858416667720

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

208. Glasser, MF, Smith, SM, Marcus, DS, Andersson, JLR, Auerbach, EJ, Behrens, TEJ, et al. The human Connectome Project’s neuroimaging approach. Nat Neurosci. (2016) 19:1175–87. doi: 10.1038/NN.4361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

209. Bijsterbosch, JD, Valk, SL, Wang, D, and Glasser, MF. Recent developments in representations of the connectome. NeuroImage. (2021) 243. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2021.118533

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

210. Liao, X, Vasilakos, AV, and He, Y. Small-world human brain networks: perspectives and challenges. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 77:286–300. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2017.03.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

211. Bullmore, E, and Sporns, O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:186–98. doi: 10.1038/NRN2575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

212. Carhart-Harris, RL. The entropic brain - revisited. Neuropharmacology. (2018) 142:167–78. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPHARM.2018.03.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

213. Amico, E, Marinazzo, D, di Perri, C, Heine, L, Annen, J, Martial, C, et al. Mapping the functional connectome traits of levels of consciousness. Neuroimage. (2017) 148:201–11. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2017.01.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

214. Tononi, G, and Koch, C. Consciousness: here, there and everywhere? Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. (2015) 370. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2014.0167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

215. Seth, AK, and Bayne, T. Theories of consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2022) 23:439–52. doi: 10.1038/s41583-022-00587-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

216. Koch, C, Massimini, M, Boly, M, and Tononi, G. Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2016) 17:307–21. doi: 10.1038/NRN.2016.22

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

217. Carhart-Harris, RL, Leech, R, Hellyer, PJ, Shanahan, M, Feilding, A, Tagliazucchi, E, et al. The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

218. Hacker, CD, Laumann, TO, Szrama, NP, Baldassarre, A, Snyder, AZ, Leuthardt, EC, et al. Resting state network estimation in individual subjects. NeuroImage. (2013) 82:616–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: somatic symptom and related disorders, factitious disorder, utilization behavior, deception, hysteria

Citation: Onofrj M, Ajdinaj P, Digiovanni A, Malek N, Martinotti G, Ferro FM, Russo M, Thomas A and Sensi SL (2023) Functional Neurologic Disorders, disorders to be managed by neurologists, or are neurologists wandering in a dangerous field with inadequate resources? Front. Psychiatry. 14:1120981. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1120981

Received: 10 December 2022; Accepted: 24 February 2023;
Published: 17 March 2023.

Edited by:

Martina Ardizzi, University of Parma, Italy

Reviewed by:

Antonio Bulbena, Autonomous University of Barcelona, SpainJeroen Antonius Van Waarde, Rijnstate Hospital, Netherlands

Copyright © 2023 Onofrj, Ajdinaj, Digiovanni, Malek, Martinotti, Ferro, Russo, Thomas and Sensi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marco Onofrj, onofrj@unich.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.