Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 13 February 2023
Sec. Schizophrenia

Functional connectivity and gray matter deficits within the auditory attention circuit in first-episode psychosis

  • Clinical Neurophysiology Research Laboratory, Department of Psychiatry, Western Psychiatric Hospital, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, United States

Background: Selective attention deficits in first episode of psychosis (FEP) can be indexed by impaired attentional modulation of auditory M100. It is unknown if the pathophysiology underlying this deficit is restricted to auditory cortex or involves a distributed attention network. We examined the auditory attention network in FEP.

Methods: MEG was recorded from 27 FEP and 31 matched healthy controls (HC) while alternately ignoring or attending tones. A whole-brain analysis of MEG source activity during auditory M100 identified non-auditory areas with increased activity. Time-frequency activity and phase-amplitude coupling were examined in auditory cortex to identify the attentional executive carrier frequency. Attention networks were defined by phase-locking at the carrier frequency. Spectral and gray matter deficits in the identified circuits were examined in FEP.

Results: Attention-related activity was identified in prefrontal and parietal regions, markedly in precuneus. Theta power and phase coupling to gamma amplitude increased with attention in left primary auditory cortex. Two unilateral attention networks were identified with precuneus seeds in HC. Network synchrony was impaired in FEP. Gray matter thickness was reduced within the left hemisphere network in FEP but did not correlate with synchrony.

Conclusion: Several extra-auditory attention areas with attention-related activity were identified. Theta was the carrier frequency for attentional modulation in auditory cortex. Left and right hemisphere attention networks were identified, with bilateral functional deficits and left hemisphere structural deficits, though FEP showed intact auditory cortex theta phase-gamma amplitude coupling. These novel findings indicate attention-related circuitopathy early in psychosis potentially amenable to future non-invasive interventions.

Introduction

Selective attention deficits are present before psychosis emerges, endure throughout illness, and are associated with poorer functional outcomes (14). The EEG-measured auditory event-related potential (or MEG-measured event-related field) ~100 ms post-stimulus (N100/M100) is enhanced with attention in healthy individuals but not in schizophrenia or first episode psychosis (FEP) (58). We recently showed that FEP have reduced M100 modulation by attention in primary auditory cortex, lateral belt, and parabelt (9). However, it is unclear if the pathology underlying this impairment is restricted to mechanisms within auditory sensory cortex or involves non-auditory executive control regions as well.

The attention-related cortical network includes prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices (1012). FMRI-based work suggests a fronto-parietal system (including prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex, and precuneus) actively modulates task-relevant information and a cingulo-opercular system (including anterior prefrontal cortex, frontal operculum, and anterior cingulate) maintains sustained attention (13, 14). The frontal and parietal areas involved in attentional control show slight localization differences for auditory and visual attention. The visual attention system engages the superior precentral sulcus and inferior precentral sulcus along with the intraparietal sulcus and visual cortex. The auditory-biased network involves the transverse gyrus, precentral gyrus, caudal inferior frontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus and gyrus, and auditory cortex (15, 16). Thus, these models agree that executive control of attention involves frontal regions communicating with parietal (medial or lateral) and enhancing activity in sensory cortices.

While fMRI provides high spatial resolution, it indirectly measures neural activity and is less sensitive to rapid changes <1 s. EEG and MEG directly measure neural activity, specifically synchronized postsynaptic currents, from groups of cortical neurons with high temporal millisecond resolution, necessary for examining sub-second sensory processes involved in N100/M100. Further, MEG provides higher spatial resolution compared to EEG because magnetic fields are unaffected by tissue boundaries (17).

In MEG and EEG, coordinated local processing and network communication are thought to be reflected by synchrony in neural oscillations. In humans and animals, attentional modulation of sensory activity is associated with increased gamma synchrony in sensory areas, which correlates with gamma synchrony in prefrontal cortex (PFC) (11, 1821). Attention also influences sensory cortex coupling between low-frequency (4–13 Hz) phase and high-frequency power, termed phase-amplitude coupling (PAC). For example, alpha phase in infragranular layers of visual cortex entrains gamma amplitude in superficial layers (2226). Communication between cortical areas occurs through coherence of the low-frequency oscillations, and this synchrony increases with attention (2730). Visual attention modulates alpha connectivity between visual, parietal, and frontal regions (31, 32), while auditory attention changes theta connectivity (25, 33, 34).

Local sensory oscillations and long-range oscillatory connectivity deficits are present in psychosis and are related to cognitive impairment (35). In auditory cortex, gamma-band activity is impaired in early psychosis (36, 37). Cognitive control-related increase in PFC gamma activity is impaired in schizophrenia (38, 39). However, not all local oscillatory mechanisms are impaired, as PAC may be intact in schizophrenia (26, 40, 41). Distributed network connectivity is also altered in schizophrenia. Resting-state alpha activity is dysfunctional within several networks as early as the first-episode of schizophrenia (42). Oscillatory connectivity deficits are also associated with cognitive impairment in psychosis (43), and one study suggests deficits in attention-related alpha desynchronization are present even prior to psychosis onset (44). However, there has been relatively little additional study of oscillatory mechanisms associated with attentional impairments in early psychosis.

In addition to functional deficits, psychosis is associated with gray matter deficits (4553), some present even before first psychosis. FEP show regional gray matter loss in frontal and temporal cortices (5460). Gray matter in temporal and frontal lobes is related to cognitive functioning in chronic schizophrenia (50, 61, 62) and is associated with functional deficits, such as reduced mismatch negativity (6366). Any functional deficits in local and long-range neural oscillations involved with failure of attentional modulation in FEP may be related to structural changes occurring at the same time.

The current study is a follow-up to our recent report that identified M100 attention modulation deficits in auditory cortex in FEP (9). To investigate functional and structural deficits in local and long-range networks underlying the M100 attentional modulation deficit in FEP, we used a multi-stepped strategy. In Analysis 1, whole-brain MEG broadband source activity during the M100 interval was examined to detect extra-auditory areas similarly activated with attention as sensory cortex. We hypothesized canonical attention-related regions, such as the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices would increase activity with attention. To determine whether these areas were functionally connected to auditory cortex in the service of attention, we turned to the spectral domain of the MEG signal. In Analysis 2, a time-frequency analysis was conducted, and PAC between low-frequency phase and high-frequency gamma amplitude in primary auditory cortex were investigated to identify the carrier frequency driving the increased local sensory gamma activity during the M100, putatively sent from executive areas. We hypothesized that attention would increase coupling between gamma amplitude and theta or alpha low frequency phase. In Analysis 3, attention networks were defined by synchrony at the identified carrier frequency between regions. Precuneus subregions were used as seed regions due to their identification in Analysis1 (see below) and their known role in attention (14). Spectral connectivity deficits within this network were then investigated in FEP. We hypothesized that there would be a cortical network with increased low frequency connectivity with attention, and connectivity deficits within this network would be present in FEP. In Analysis 4, gray matter deficits within the attention network were investigated in FEP, and we hypothesized gray matter within this network would be related to functional attention-related deficits in FEP.

General methods and materials

Participants

This study included the same participants as Curtis and colleagues (9), comprising 27 FEP (recruited from Western Psychiatric Hospital with <2 months lifetime antipsychotic exposure) and 31 healthy controls (HC). No participant had a history of concussion or head injury with sequelae, history of alcohol or drug addiction or detox in the last 5 years, or neurological comorbidity. Groups were matched for age, gender, parental social economic status, and premorbid IQ, estimated by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) vocabulary t-score (Table 1). Participants had healthy hearing confirmed with audiometry. Participants provided informed consent and were paid for participation. The work was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic, neuropsychological, and clinical information (previously reported in Curtis et al., 2022).

FEP diagnoses were based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-P) (67). Provisional diagnoses at baseline were confirmed 5–7 months later. Symptoms were rated using the using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (Table 1) (68). Cognitive ability on tests sensitive to psychosis was assessed with the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (69).

Of the 27 FEP participants,17 received a diagnosis of schizophrenia (paranoid: n = 7; undifferentiated: n = 10), 1 of schizoaffective disorder (bipolar subtype), 2 of schizophreniform disorder, and 2 of psychotic disorder NOS. Five individuals received affective disorder diagnoses (Bipolar I disorder = 4; Major depressive disorder = 1), all with psychotic features. The majority of the FEP participants were medicated (21/27, 78%). Due to the differences in clinical manifestations between schizophrenia and other diagnoses for FEP, preliminary analyses investigating potential differences between FEP with a schizophrenia diagnosis and FEP without a schizophrenia diagnosis are reported in the Supplementary materials (Supplementary Tables S1, S2).

Task

An oddball task with 340 standard tones (1 kHz, 50 ms duration, 10 ms rise/fall) and 60 deviant tones (1.2 kHz, 50 ms duration, 10 ms rise/fall) with stimulus onset asynchrony of 1,050–1,550 ms was presented twice. In one condition, participants ignored tones and attended a silent video. In the second condition, participants ignored the silent video and attended tones, pressing a button to deviant tones. Blocks were counterbalanced. Responses to standard tones, where M100 enhancement via attention is observed, were analyzed. To demonstrate participants were attending, hit rate to the target tones and false alarms were calculated. HC were more accurate (90.5 ± 12.6%) than FEP (77.6 ± 23.7%, p = 0.02), while groups did not differ on false alarms (HC: 2.2 ± 7.3, FEP: 3.6 ± 5.7, p = 0.44). This performance with relatively low false alarms demonstrates that all individuals were attempting to attend to the tones.

MEG data acquisition and processing

MEG data were recorded in a magnetically shielded room with a 306-channel whole-head system (Elekta Neuromag), consisting of 102 triplets (1 magnetometer and 2 planar gradiometers) at 1000 Hz digitization with a bandpass filter of 0.1–330 Hz. Eye blinks and movements were recorded with bipolar leads placed above and below the left eye and lateral to the outer canthi of both eyes. Cardiac activity was recorded with bipolar ECG leads. A 3D-digitizer (ISOTRAK; Polhemus, Inc., Colchester, VT) was used to continuously record the location of 4 head position indicator coils. Neuromag MaxFilter software1 was used to correct for head motion. Temporal signal space separation was used to remove electromagnetic noise originating from outside the MEG helmet (70). Channels and segments of data with excessive noise via visual inspection were removed with EEGLAB (71). A high-pass filter (0.5 Hz; 12 dB/oct) was applied to the data, and an adaptive mixture independent component analysis was performed to remove eye-blink and ECG components. Any removed channels were then interpolated with spherical channel interpretation in EEGLAB.

MEG processing was performed with Brainstorm (72). A low-pass (20 Hz) filter was applied to remove muscle and other high-frequency artifacts. Trials were segmented from 100 ms before to 1,000 ms after stimulus onset. The average baseline voltage was subtracted, and trials that exceeded ±5pT were rejected.

For source activity, MEG sensor data was registered to each participant’s structural MRI (see MRI methods). Sources were constrained to the individual’s cortical surface. The forward solution was modeled as overlapping spheres, and a noise covariance matrix was calculated from the baseline window of all trials. Source activity was estimated using minimum norm estimation with a dipole constraint of 0.4 and depth weighting applied. Current density values were normalized with dynamic statistical parametric maps based on the variance in the prestimulus baseline.

MRI data acquisition and processing

MRI acquisition and processing steps were the same as Curtis and colleagues (9). The following data were acquired on a Siemens 3T MAGNETOM Prisma scanner using a 32-channel phase array head coil: A single-band reference image with no slice acceleration at the beginning of each run; T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE images (TR/TE/TI = 2400/2.22/1000 ms, flip angle = 7°, FOV = 256 × 240mm, voxel size = 0.8 mm3, 208 slices, GRAPPA acceleration factor = 2); T2-SPACE images (TR = 3,200 ms TE = 563 ms, FOV = 256 × 240, voxel size = 0.8 mm3, 208 slices); A fieldmap (TR = 731 ms, TE = 4.92/7.38 ms, FOV = 208 × 180, voxel size = 2.0 mm3 voxel size, 72 slices); Ten minutes eyes-open, resting BOLD fMRI multiband data (TR = 800 ms, TE = 37 ms, multiband factor = 8, flip angle = 52°, FOV = 208 × 208mm, voxel size = 2.0 mm3, 72 slices); and Spin echo EPI images with reversed phase encoding directions (TR = 8,000 ms, TE = 66 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 208 × 208mm, voxel size = 2.0 mm3, 72 slices).

The HCP-pipelines2 were used for MRI processing (73). Briefly, structural images were corrected for gradient nonlinearity, readout, and bias field, followed by AC-PC alignment. Myelin maps were created by the T1w/T2w ratio. White and pial surfaces were generated with Freesurfer (v6.0), refined using T2w data, and registered with a multimodal surface matching algorithm (MSMsulc) to the Conte69 template (74, 75).

FMRI data were collected with the structural MRI and processed with the HCP pipelines. The fMRI and myelin data were not analyzed here but were utilized for the multimodal surface matching algorithm (MSMall) algorithm implemented in the HCP pipelines to improve registration, alignment between participants, and utilize the HCP parcellation. FMRI data were processed with the HCP pipelines. FMRI data were corrected for gradient-nonlinearity. A 6 DOF FLIRT registration to the single-band reference image was used for motion correction. The spin-echo images were used to correct functional distortion. The single band reference image was registered to the T1w image with FreeSurfer’s BBRegister (76). The data were brain masked and intensity normalized (whole-brain mean = 10,000). Volumetric fMRI data were sampled to the individual’s native surfaces and resampled to a standard 32 k fs_LR surface. The ICA + FIX pipeline was used to remove artifactual noise (77, 78). Individuals were registered to a group average atlas surface using a two-stage MSMall algorithm, utilizing resting-state fMRI, cortical folding, and myelination information (74). The group average HCP-Multimodal Parcellation was applied to individuals’ cortical surface in the HCP connectome workbench and resampled to fsaverage space.3 In Freesurfer, this file was resampled to the individual’s surface and imported into Brainstorm.

Analysis 1

Methods and materials

Whole brain M100 window broadband source analysis

To investigate healthy attention modulation, data from HC were analyzed over the 80–140 ms post-stimulus, when M100 enhancement was previously observed in auditory cortex (9). Cortical source activity was calculated for each vertex for the attend and ignore conditions. Statistical t-tests were calculated for differences between conditions for each vertex. Permutation testing (5,000 permutations) was used for statistical significance with FDR correction for multiple comparisons (q < 0.05).

Results

Whole brain M100 window broadband source analysis

In HC, there were significant increases with attention in many areas associated with canonical executive attention networks, including bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (8C, 46, p9-46v), inferior frontal cortex (IFJa, IFSp), lateral parietal cortex (left PF, left PFt, right PFop, right PFm), precuneus, and auditory cortex (A1, MBelt). There was also significant activity in left superior parietal, middle temporal, insular, cingulate, and somatosensory areas (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Whole-brain M100 modulation in healthy individuals. There were significant increases in source-resolved MEG activity (dspm) with attention compared to ignore in healthy individuals (q < 0.05). Outlined in black are regions aligning with the general hypothesized network including prefrontal, posterior parietal, precuneus, and auditory cortices.

Brief discussion

The whole brain analysis of source activity during the M100 time-window revealed increased activity in canonical executive attention network areas (14, 16). There was particularly strong involvement of the bilateral precuneus. The precuneus, a core component of the default mode network, is also a hub for attention, as it has a central role in changing attentional states during various cognitive tasks, including auditory attention (7982).

This analysis demonstrates activity increases with attention in extra-auditory cortical areas during the M100 but does not determine any direct role in auditory attention enhancement. Hence, there was a need to examine dynamics between areas, which was accomplished with functional connectivity in the spectral domain. The primary goal of analysis 2 is to identify the carrier frequency for subsequent connectivity analyses. To accomplish this, a time-frequency analysis in primary auditory cortex was performed and coupling between executive low-frequency carrier phase and local sensory high-frequency gamma amplitude was investigated in auditory cortex regions of interest (ROIs).

Analysis 2

Methods and materials

Time-frequency analysis

The processing steps were the same with the omission of the low-pass (20 Hz) filter. In Brainstorm, the time-frequency decomposition of broadband source data from each trial was calculated with Morlet wavelets for 40 bins logarithmically spaced between 4 and 100 Hz (central frequency: 1 Hz, FWHM: 3 s). Trials were normalized to event-related perturbation during a 600 ms pre-stimulus baseline period, to investigate low frequencies, and averaged for each individual.

Phase-amplitude coupling

Phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) was calculated with the Ozkurt and Schnitzler method (83). All trials (0–1,000 ms post-stimulus) were concatenated, and Morlet wavelets were used for low-frequency (4–13 Hz) phase and high-frequency amplitude (30–100 Hz). PAC was calculated at each vertex of the ROI and averaged within the ROI. This was done in auditory ROIs (bilateral A1, LBelt, and PBelt).

Statistical analysis

To gain power to detect the carrier frequency, all individuals were included in the initial between-task comparisons. Within each ROI, a cluster-based permutation test was used to calculate significant differences between conditions. Permutation testing was used for the PAC analysis and for the between-group comparisons. An exploratory analysis of all ROIs was performed to investigate correspondence in which low-frequency phase was involved in coupling. Uncorrected t-statistics are reported for the highest instance of PAC within each region.

Results

Time-frequency

Among all participants, after correction for multiple comparisons, there was a significant increase in theta (5–7 Hz) power in left A1 with attention from 0 to 330 ms (q < 0.05; Figure 2). There were no significant group differences. In right A1, there were no significant differences with attention and no group differences.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Time-frequency analysis in left primary auditory cortex. There was a significant increase in theta frequency power with attention in left auditory cortex from 0 to 330 ms in all individuals.

PAC exploratory analysis

In all participants, the highest instances of PAC increase in each region are reported in Table 2 and shown in Figure 3. All regions, except for right LBelt, had the greatest PAC between gamma amplitude and identified theta (5–7 Hz) phase. Right LBelt had the greatest PAC between gamma amplitude and alpha (8–10 Hz) phase.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Phase-amplitude coupling in auditory cortex.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Exploratory uncorrected phase-amplitude coupling analysis in auditory cortex. There were uncorrected significant increases in coupling between low-frequency phase and gamma power. All, except right LBelt, areas showed the greatest coupling increase with attention between theta phase and gamma power. Right LBelt showed the greatest coupling increase with attention between alpha phase and gamma power. Peak coupling within each area is outlined in black.

PAC corrected statistics

After multiple comparisons correction, in all participants there was a significant increase in PAC with attention in left A1 between theta phase (5–7 Hz) and gamma amplitude (35–40 Hz; q < 0.05). There were no significant group differences (q > 0.05; Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Significant phase-amplitude coupling analysis in left primary auditory cortex. There was a significant increase in coupling with attention between theta frequency phase and gamma frequency amplitude in left auditory cortex in all individuals.

Brief discussion

Within primary auditory cortex, theta power increased with attention from 0–330 ms, a time-window that includes the M100 (80–140 ms). In all areas, except right LBelt, theta phase was associated with local gamma synchrony, although multiple comparisons correction revealed significant effects only in left primary auditory core. This provides evidence that the low-frequency carrier frequency that modulates sensory regions with attention in this task is the theta frequency band. PAC in sensory cortex was intact in FEP, suggesting the mechanism underlying attention modulation deficits in early psychosis exists at the extra-auditory network level. To determine the nodes of the distributed attention network, the next analysis focused on attention modulation of whole-brain oscillatory network connectivity in theta band and potential functional connectivity deficits in FEP.

Analysis 3

Methods and materials

Functional connectivity

Phase locking value (PLV) was used to assess spectral synchrony and functional connectivity within the theta-band (5–7 Hz) (84, 85). Connectivity was assessed for all attend and ignore trials. Cortical source activity was band-pass filtered (5–7 Hz), and a Hilbert transform was used to obtain instantaneous phase. The PLV between two regions i and j was calculated:

P L V i j = 1 T | t = 1 T e i ( φ i ( t ) φ j ( t ) ) |

In the above equation, T is the number of data points in the time series and φ ( t ) represents the phase of each signal at timepoint t.

Precuneus clustering

With its central role in attention processing and involvement in the attention enhancement during the M100 window (Analysis 1), the precuneus was used as the seed region for functional connectivity. Noting the many roles of the precuneus, functional subsystems may exist. Thus, the precuneus was parcellated into 12 bilateral subregions with the HCP-MMP. PLV was calculated between each vertex in each seed region and each vertex in every other cortical parcel and then averaged within each parcel. This was done in HC for attend and ignore conditions, and t-values comparing conditions were calculated for each subregion. To identify potential subsystems, subregions were clustered based on connectivity difference patterns with t-distributed stochastic neighbor embedding (t-SNE) implemented in Matlab and k-means clustering (86). The elbow method determined the optimal cluster number. Within-cluster sum of squared errors (WSS) was calculated, and cluster number was chosen when greater than 90% of the variance was explained.

Once clustered, the precuneus parcels were merged, and PLV connectivity was calculated again using the entire cluster as a seed region to determine which cortical parcels had significantly greater connectivity with attention.

Functional connectivity analysis

An FDR of 0.1 was used to control for multiple comparisons of the whole brain connectivity between precuneus clusters and HCP-parcellated regions in HC. An FDR of 0.1 was selected to balance the expected relatively small effect sizes due to relatively smaller sample sizes (31 HC) and large number of comparisons (~360). Further, the goal of this first analysis was to discover areas with strong connectivity changes in HC. Then, these discovered network regions were examined in FEP using repeated measures ANOVAs with one between-subjects factor (Group: Control or FEP) and one within-subject factor (Regions).

Results

Precuneus subregion clustering

The clustering analysis yielded 4 subregions based on differential theta band whole brain connectivity (Figure 5). Cluster 1 included left hemisphere regions 23c, 31a, 5L, 5mv, 7am, and precuneus visual area (PCV). The second cluster included right hemisphere regions 23c, 31a, 31pd, 5L, 5mv, 7am, and PCV. The third cluster included left 31pd, left 7 m, left 7 pm, left posterior precuneus 2 (POS2), right 7m, right 7pm, and right POS2. The fourth cluster included left posterior precuneus 1 (POS1), left ventral 23ab, right POS1, and right ventral 23ab.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Cluster analysis. (A) K-means clustering and the elbow-method was used to determine the optimal number of clusters, which was determined to be four clusters, indicated by the arrow. (B) The two-dimensional t-SNE plot with each point representing a precuneus subregion. Each region’s location on the plot is determined by the region’s whole-brain connectivity profile in the theta band. If regions have more similar connectivity profiles, they will be located closer to each other on the t-SNE plot. The clusters are denoted by different colors. The four clusters are shown on the cortical surface. There was a left hemisphere cluster (C), a right hemisphere cluster (D), and two bilateral clusters (E,F).

Connectivity changes with attention in HC

In HC, cluster 1 showed significantly greater connectivity with attention with left medial belt (MBelt), left dorsal posterior superior temporal sulcus (STSdp), left temporo-parieto-occipital junction 1 and 2 (TPOJ1 and TPOJ2), left visual area 3b (V3B), and posterior frontal pole area 10 (p10p; q < 0.1). The right lateralized cluster 2 showed significantly greater connectivity with attention with right medial superior temporal area (MST), right lateral occipital area 1 (LO1), right visual area 7 (V7), right posterior area 9-ventral area 46 in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (p9-46v; q < 0.1; Figure 6). The other two precuneus clusters did not have connectivity increases with attention that survived FDR correction (q > 0.1).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Regions with significant connectivity increases with attention. (A) The left hemisphere precuneus cluster had significant increases in theta phase connectivity with attention with left medial belt (MBelt), left dorsal posterior superior temporal sulcus (STSdp), left temporo-parieto-occipital junction 1 and 2 (TPOJ1 and TPOJ2), left visual area 3b (V3B), and posterior frontal pole area 10 (p10p; q < 0.1). (B) The right hemisphere precuneus cluster had significant increases in theta phase connectivity with attention with right medial superior temporal area (MST), right lateral occipital area 1 (LO1), right visual area 7 (V7), right posterior area 9-ventral area 46 in the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (p9-46v; q < 0.1).

Connectivity group differences

FEP were significantly impaired in the ability to enhance connectivity with attention in both the left hemisphere (F1,56 = 8.58, p < 0.01; Figure 7; Table 3) and the right hemisphere (F1,56 = 7.67, p < 0.01; Figure 8; Table 3).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Left hemisphere network connectivity changes in HC and FEP. There was a significant reduction in the modulation of theta band connectivity with attention (measured with change in phase-locking value) across regions in the left hemisphere network in FEP (A–F).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Connectivity changes with attention.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Right hemisphere network connectivity changes in HC and FEP. Similar to the left hemisphere network, there was a significant reduction in the modulation of theta band connectivity with attention (measured with change in phase-locking value) across regions in the right hemisphere network in FEP (A–D).

Brief discussion

Cluster analysis of whole brain synchrony using precuneus seed regions identified a left hemisphere and a right hemisphere network that showed increased theta synchrony with attention in HC. Importantly, FEP did not modulate theta synchrony within these networks. These novel findings indicate a distributed circuit involved in auditory attention modulation that is functionally impaired in early psychosis. The final analysis investigated if these functional measures, and corresponding deficits in FEP, were related to underlying gray matter.

Analysis 4

Methods and materials

Gray matter analysis

Gray matter volume and thickness differences were investigated in regions with significant functional connectivity enhancement from Analysis 3 (Left Hemisphere: MBelt, STSdp, TPOJ1, TPOJ2, V3B, and p10p: Right Hemisphere: MST, LO1, V7, and p9-46v). Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was performed on relative volumes [absolute volume/intracranial content (ICC)] and thickness with gender and age as covariates (The statistical conclusions remained the same when using ANCOVA on absolute volumes with intracranial content (ICC), gender, and age as covariates).

In HC and FEP, absolute volumes and thicknesses were correlated (Spearman’s rho) with PAC modulation in left A1, and network theta connectivity modulation. Correlations were also examined between gray matter in the left hemisphere network regions and symptom severity in FEP (PANSS Total, Positive, Negative, and General factors).

Results

In the left hemisphere network, FEP had significantly thinner gray matter (F1,54 = 5.14, p = 0.02; Figure 9). There were no differences in gray matter volume (p > 0.1). There were no group differences in gray matter volume or thickness within the right hemisphere network (p’s > 0.1). Thickness values are reported in Table 4, and volumes are reported in Table 5.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Gray matter thickness in the left hemisphere attention network. Compared to healthy controls, individuals with first-episode of psychosis (FEP) showed significant reductions in gray matter thickness across the regions in the left hemisphere precuneus attention network (p < 0.05; A–F).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Gray matter thickness.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Gray matter volume.

There were no significant correlations between PAC and gray matter in left A1 (p’s > 0.1) or between PLV and gray matter in the network regions (p’s > 0.1). Likewise, there were no significant correlations with symptoms.

Brief discussion

FEP had significantly thinner cortices within the left hemisphere attention network. This identifies a left hemisphere network important for attention modulation with both functional and structural deficits in FEP, while the right hemisphere network only showed functional deficits.

General discussion

In HC, auditory attention increased activity during the M100 time-window in the dorsolateral prefrontal, parietal, and precuneus areas, corresponding to canonical executive attention network areas (14, 16), with strong effects in the precuneus, a core component of attentional control (7982). However, a direct role in the enhancement of auditory cortex M100 could not be determined without examining network dynamics.

The low-frequency executive signal was determined via PAC analysis, that demonstrated every auditory area, except for right LBelt, experienced increased coupling between gamma amplitude and theta phase, with a significantly increased coupling in left A1 with attention. This provides strong evidence theta frequency is the low-frequency carrier that modulates auditory regions via attention, consistent with previous studies (24, 25, 40, 87, 88). The right LBelt experienced maximal coupling between gamma amplitude and alpha phase. In addition, there was a potential difference between FEP with a schizophrenia diagnosis and FEP without a schizophrenia diagnosis in PAC in the right LBelt. Other attention tasks have been shown to increase alpha-gamma coupling in sensory areas, and in general, gamma power bursts are phase locked to traveling alpha waves across cortex (22, 23). Future research can investigate these differences in right LBelt further. There were no significant PAC group differences, suggesting the mechanisms underlying PAC are relatively intact in early psychosis (26, 40).

Analysis of whole brain theta coupling using the precuneus seed regions identified two unilateral cortical attention networks. A left hemisphere network included left auditory cortex, temporo-parieto-occipital junction, superior occipital, and posterior frontal pole. A right hemisphere network included right prefrontal cortex and lateral occipital cortex. FEP were significantly impaired in the ability to enhance theta connectivity between these regions in the service of attention.

These networks shared some correspondence but also had divergence with canonical auditory attention networks. The precuneus, medial belt, and temporo-parieto-occipital junction have been implicated in auditory attention (14, 16, 79, 80, 89, 90). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and frontal pole are key components of canonical networks for cognitive control (13, 9193). However, several regions identified were in occipital cortex. This may be related to the cross-modal nature of the task, as these regions are implicated in visual attention modulation (89, 9496). Thus, the precuneus may increase connectivity with these regions while attending to auditory stimuli (and ignoring visual stimuli) to downmodulate local neuronal activity within these regions. Future studies can investigate this more directly.

FEP had thinner cortices within the left hemisphere attention network. Gray matter deficits in the left hemisphere are prevalent early in the disorder, though not always identified (97101), and correlate with positive symptoms and neurophysiological measures (64, 66, 102105). Gray matter loss in schizophrenia is likely due to reduced neuropil (106), hypothesized to reflect dendritic regression and reduced synaptic connectivity (66, 107, 108). We speculate that neuropil reduction within this left attention network reduces synaptic connectivity with incoming network signals controlling attention-related theta increases. However, there were no direct correlations with theta connectivity increases. There may be a mediator between the two measures, a nonlinear relationship in which gray matter thins to a threshold resulting in functional deficits and further thinning does not indicate additional deficits, or no relationship at all. Future work with larger samples may further clarify this, and ongoing longitudinal scans, where progressive gray matter loss is presumed to occur, may also provide clarification.

This study had limitations. The sample size is relatively small, and the findings need replication. Subcortical deficits exist in psychosis. However, the HCP-MMP is restricted to cortex and the reliability of MEG for subcortical structures needs further verification. Future studies can use volumetric approaches to examine subcortical contributions to these deficits. Finally, it is unknown if the identified networks are generalizable to other auditory attention tasks. Visual regions identified suggest specificity to the cross-modal aspect that may be less involved in a unimodal task (e.g., attend different auditory streams).

These novel findings indicate a distributed auditory attention circuit with functional and structural deficits in very early psychosis. This circuitopathy provides a systems-level target for novel interventions, such as non-invasive brain stimulation coupled with cognitive enhancement therapy, which can be neuroprotective in early psychosis (109). Improvement of attention modulation deficits in FEP should improve real-world functioning and might improve functional outcome if targeted early in the disorder.

Conclusion

The ability to emphasize certain stimuli, here assessed with the enhancement of auditory M100 by attention, is related to a broad distributed attentional network that utilized theta-band synchrony to modulate sensory-related gamma activity. FEP individuals show intact phase-amplitude coupling between theta and gamma in auditory cortices but show theta-band abnormalities in the extra-auditory attentional network. Whereas psychiatrically well individuals increased theta coupling within the attention network when attending, FEP did not. This suggests that the higher-order executive signal is not arriving in auditory cortices. Further, the executive areas showed gray matter thinning in left, but not right, hemisphere, although the degree of thinning did not correlate with the severity of failure to modulate theta phase-locking. Identifying the nodes of the network underlying this attentional enhancement deficit provide novel targets for selective brain stimulation treatments.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The work described was carried out in accordance with The Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) for experiments involving humans.

Author contributions

DS designed the study, interpreted findings, and contributed to the critical revision of the manuscript. MC and BC helped collect data. MC wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MC performed analyses with the aid and instruction of DS, BC, and AS. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by NIMH R01 MH108568 (DS), RO1 MH113533 (DS), and F31 MH119718 (MC). The NIH played no role in the collection or analysis of data or in the preparation of this manuscript. We thank the faculty and staff of the WPH Psychosis Recruitment and Assessment Core, the Conte Center for Translational Mental Health Research (P50 MH103204, David Lewis, Director), and the University of Pittsburgh Clinical Translational Science Institute (UL1 RR024153, Steven E. Reis) for their assistance in recruitment, diagnostic and psychopathological assessments, and neuropsychological evaluations.

Acknowledgments

We thank Vanessa Fishel, Natasha Torrence, Yiming Wang, Dylan Seebold, Rebekah Farris, Timothy Murphy, Justin Leiter, and Maria Jalbrzikowski for their assistance in collecting the MEG and MRI data.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1114703/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

References

1.Caspi, A, Reichenberg, A, Weiser, M, Rabinowitz, J, Kaplan, Z, Knobler, H, et al. Cognitive performance in schizophrenia patients assessed before and following the first psychotic episode. Schizophr Res. (2003) 65:87–94. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00056-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2.Cornblatt, BA, Lencz, T, Smith, CW, Correll, CU, Auther, AM, and Nakayama, E. The schizophrenia prodrome revisited: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Schizophr Bull. (2003) 29:633–51. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3.Fett, AK, Viechtbauer, W, Dominguez, MD, Penn, DL, van Os, J, and Krabbendam, L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:573–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4.Kraepelin, E . Psychiatrie: ein lehrbuch für studirende und aerzte. 3rd ed. Leipzig: Abel (1889).

Google Scholar

5.Rosburg, T, Boutros, NN, and Ford, JM. Reduced auditory evoked potential component N100 in schizophrenia--a critical review. Psychiatry Res. (2008) 161:259–74. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6.Foxe, JJ, and Snyder, AC. The role of alpha-band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. Front Psychol. (2011) 2:154. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7.O'Donnell, BF, Hokama, H, McCarley, RW, Smith, RS, Salisbury, DF, Mondrow, E, et al. Auditory ERPs to non-target stimuli in schizophrenia: relationship to probability, task-demands, and target ERPs. Int J Psychophysiol. (1994) 17:219–31. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(94)90065-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8.Ren, X, Fribance, SN, Coffman, BA, and Salisbury, DF. Deficits in attentional modulation of auditory N100 in first-episode schizophrenia. Eur J Neurosci. (2021) 53:2629–38. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9.Curtis, MT, Ren, X, Coffman, BA, and Salisbury, DF. Attentional M100 gain modulation localizes to auditory sensory cortex and is deficient in first-episode psychosis. Hum Brain Mapp. (2023) 44:218–28. doi: 10.1002/hbm.26067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10.Fiebelkorn, IC, and Kastner, S. Functional specialization in the attention network. Annu Rev Psychol. (2020) 71:221–49. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11.Gregoriou, GG, Gotts, SJ, Zhou, H, and Desimone, R. High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science. (2009) 324:1207–10. doi: 10.1126/science.1171402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12.Saalmann, YB, Pigarev, IN, and Vidyasagar, TR. Neural mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant locations. Science. (2007) 316:1612–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1139140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13.Dosenbach, NU, Fair, DA, Cohen, AL, Schlaggar, BL, and Petersen, SE. A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn Sci. (2008) 12:99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14.Petersen, SE, and Posner, MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2012) 35:73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15.Michalka, SW, Kong, L, Rosen, ML, Shinn-Cunningham, BG, and Somers, DC. Short-term memory for space and time flexibly recruit complementary sensory-biased frontal lobe attention networks. Neuron. (2015) 87:882–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16.Tobyne, SM, Osher, DE, Michalka, SW, and Somers, DC. Sensory-biased attention networks in human lateral frontal cortex revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. NeuroImage. (2017) 162:362–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17.Coffman, BA, and Salisbury, DF. MEG methods: A primer of basic MEG analysis In: M Kubicki and ME Shenton, editors. Neuroimaging in Schizophrenia. (2020) Cham: Springer International Publishing 191–210.

Google Scholar

18.Gregoriou, GG, Gotts, SJ, and Desimone, R. Cell-type-specific synchronization of neural activity in FEF with V4 during attention. Neuron. (2012) 73:581–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19.Gruber, T, Muller, MM, Keil, A, and Elbert, T. Selective visual-spatial attention alters induced gamma band responses in the human EEG. Clin Neurophysiol. (1999) 110:2074–85. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00176-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20.Shibata, T, Shimoyama, I, Ito, T, Abla, D, Iwasa, H, Koseki, K, et al. Attention changes the peak latency of the visual gamma-band oscillation of the EEG. NeuroReport. (1999) 10:1167–70. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199904260-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21.Tallon-Baudry, C, Bertrand, O, Henaff, MA, Isnard, J, and Fischer, C. Attention modulates gamma-band oscillations differently in the human lateral occipital cortex and fusiform gyrus. Cereb Cortex. (2005) 15:654–62. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22.Voytek, B, Canolty, RT, Shestyuk, A, Crone, NE, Parvizi, J, and Knight, RT. Shifts in gamma phase-amplitude coupling frequency from theta to alpha over posterior cortex during visual tasks. Front Hum Neurosci. (2010) 4:191. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23.Bahramisharif, A, van Gerven, MA, Aarnoutse, EJ, Mercier, MR, Schwartz, TH, Foxe, JJ, et al. Propagating neocortical gamma bursts are coordinated by traveling alpha waves. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:18849–54. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2455-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24.Cho, RY, Walker, CP, Polizzotto, NR, Wozny, TA, Fissell, C, Chen, CM, et al. Development of sensory gamma oscillations and cross-frequency coupling from childhood to early adulthood. Cereb Cortex. (2015) 25:1509–18. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25.Doesburg, SM, Green, JJ, McDonald, JJ, and Ward, LM. Theta modulation of inter-regional gamma synchronization during auditory attention control. Brain Res. (2012) 1431:77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26.Hirano, S, Nakhnikian, A, Hirano, Y, Oribe, N, Kanba, S, Onitsuka, T, et al. Phase-amplitude coupling of the electroencephalogram in the auditory cortex in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2018) 3:69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27.Fries, P . A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci. (2005) 9:474–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28.Singer, W, and Gray, CM. Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci. (1995) 18:555–86. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.003011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29.Brookes, MJ, Woolrich, M, Luckhoo, H, Price, D, Hale, JR, Stephenson, MC, et al. Investigating the electrophysiological basis of resting state networks using magnetoencephalography. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2011) 108:16783–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112685108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30.de Pasquale, F, Della Penna, S, Snyder, AZ, Lewis, C, Mantini, D, Marzetti, L, et al. Temporal dynamics of spontaneous MEG activity in brain networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2010) 107:6040–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913863107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31.Lobier, M, Palva, JM, and Palva, S. High-alpha band synchronization across frontal, parietal and visual cortex mediates behavioral and neuronal effects of visuospatial attention. NeuroImage. (2018) 165:222–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32.Marzetti, L, Basti, A, Chella, F, D'Andrea, A, Syrjala, J, and Pizzella, V. Brain functional connectivity through phase coupling of neuronal oscillations: a perspective from magnetoencephalography. Front Neurosci. (2019) 13:964. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33.Cavanagh, JF, and Frank, MJ. Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci. (2014) 18:414–21. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34.Keller, AS, Payne, L, and Sekuler, R. Characterizing the roles of alpha and theta oscillations in multisensory attention. Neuropsychologia. (2017) 99:48–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35.Boutros, NN, Arfken, C, Galderisi, S, Warrick, J, Pratt, G, and Iacono, W. The status of spectral EEG abnormality as a diagnostic test for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2008) 99:225–37. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36.Taylor, GW, McCarley, RW, and Salisbury, DF. Early auditory gamma band response abnormalities in first hospitalized schizophrenia. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol. (2013) 62:131–45. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-7020-5307-8.00009-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37.Spencer, KM, Salisbury, DF, Shenton, ME, and McCarley, RW. Gamma-band auditory steady-state responses are impaired in first episode psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 64:369–75. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38.Cho, RY, Konecky, RO, and Carter, CS. Impairments in frontal cortical gamma synchrony and cognitive control in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2006) 103:19878–83. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609440103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39.Gonzalez-Burgos, G, Cho, RY, and Lewis, DA. Alterations in cortical network oscillations and parvalbumin neurons in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. (2015) 77:1031–40. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40.Murphy, N, Ramakrishnan, N, Walker, CP, Polizzotto, NR, and Cho, RY. Intact auditory cortical cross-frequency coupling in early and chronic schizophrenia. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:507. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41.Kirihara, K, Rissling, AJ, Swerdlow, NR, Braff, DL, and Light, GA. Hierarchical organization of gamma and theta oscillatory dynamics in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. (2012) 71:873–80. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42.Phalen, H, Coffman, BA, Ghuman, A, Sejdic, E, and Salisbury, DF. Non-negative matrix factorization reveals resting-state cortical alpha network abnormalities in the first-episode schizophrenia spectrum. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2019) 5:961–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.06.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43.Uhlhaas, PJ, Haenschel, C, Nikolic, D, and Singer, W. The role of oscillations and synchrony in cortical networks and their putative relevance for the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2008) 34:927–43. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44.Koh, Y, Shin, KS, Kim, JS, Choi, JS, Kang, DH, Jang, JH, et al. An MEG study of alpha modulation in patients with schizophrenia and in subjects at high risk of developing psychosis. Schizophr Res. (2011) 126:36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45.Birur, B, Kraguljac, NV, Shelton, RC, and Lahti, AC. Brain structure, function, and neurochemistry in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder-a systematic review of the magnetic resonance neuroimaging literature. NPJ Schizophr. (2017) 3:15. doi: 10.1038/s41537-017-0013-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46.Cropley, VL, Klauser, P, Lenroot, RK, Bruggemann, J, Sundram, S, Bousman, C, et al. Accelerated Gray and white matter deterioration with age in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (2017) 174:286–95. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16050610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47.Goldman, AL, Pezawas, L, Mattay, VS, Fischl, B, Verchinski, BA, Chen, Q, et al. Widespread reductions of cortical thickness in schizophrenia and spectrum disorders and evidence of heritability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2009) 66:467–77. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48.Haijma, SV, Van Haren, N, Cahn, W, Koolschijn, PC, Hulshoff Pol, HE, and Kahn, RS. Brain volumes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis in over 18 000 subjects. Schizophr Bull. (2013) 39:1129–38. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49.Rimol, LM, Hartberg, CB, Nesvåg, R, Fennema-Notestine, C, Hagler, DJ, Pung, CJ, et al. Cortical thickness and subcortical volumes in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 68:41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.03.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50.Shenton, ME, Dickey, CC, Frumin, M, and McCarley, RW. A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2001) 49:1–52. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00163-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51.van Erp, TG, Hibar, DP, Rasmussen, JM, Glahn, DC, Pearlson, GD, Andreassen, OA, et al. Subcortical brain volume abnormalities in 2028 individuals with schizophrenia and 2540 healthy controls via the ENIGMA consortium. Mol Psychiatry. (2016) 21:585. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52.van Erp, TGM, Walton, E, Hibar, DP, Schmaal, L, Jiang, W, Glahn, DC, et al. Cortical brain abnormalities in 4474 individuals with schizophrenia and 5098 control subjects via the enhancing neuro imaging genetics through meta analysis (ENIGMA) consortium. Biol Psychiatry. (2018) 84:644–54. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53.van Haren, NE, Schnack, HG, Cahn, W, van den Heuvel, MP, Lepage, C, Collins, L, et al. Changes in cortical thickness during the course of illness in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2011) 68:871–80. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.88

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54.Curtis, MT, Coffman, BA, and Salisbury, DF. Parahippocampal area three gray matter is reduced in first-episode schizophrenia spectrum: discovery and replication samples. Hum Brain Mapp. (2021) 42:724–36. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55.DeLisi, LE, Sakuma, M, Tew, W, Kushner, M, Hoff, AL, and Grimson, R. Schizophrenia as a chronic active brain process: a study of progressive brain structural change subsequent to the onset of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. (1997) 74:129–40. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(97)00012-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56.Ellison-Wright, I, Glahn, DC, Laird, AR, Thelen, SM, and Bullmore, E. The anatomy of first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. (2008) 165:1015–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57.Hirayasu, Y, Shenton, ME, Salisbury, DF, Dickey, CC, Fischer, IA, Mazzoni, P, et al. Lower left temporal lobe MRI volumes in patients with first-episode schizophrenia compared with psychotic patients with first-episode affective disorder and normal subjects. Am J Psychiatry. (1998) 155:1384–91. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.10.1384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58.Kasai, K, Shenton, ME, Salisbury, DF, Hirayasu, Y, Lee, CU, Ciszewski, AA, et al. Progressive decrease of left superior temporal gyrus gray matter volume in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (2003) 160:156–64. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59.Keshavan, MS, Haas, GL, Kahn, CE, Aguilar, E, Dick, EL, Schooler, NR, et al. Superior temporal gyrus and the course of early schizophrenia: progressive, static, or reversible? J Psychiatr Res. (1998) 32:161–7. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(97)00038-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60.Vita, A, De Peri, L, Silenzi, C, and Dieci, M. Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of quantitative magnetic resonance imaging studies. Schizophr Res. (2006) 82:75–88. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61.Baare, WF, Hulshoff Pol, HE, Hijman, R, Mali, WP, Viergever, MA, and Kahn, RS. Volumetric analysis of frontal lobe regions in schizophrenia: relation to cognitive function and symptomatology. Biol Psychiatry. (1999) 45:1597–605. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00266-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62.Nestor, PG, Shenton, ME, McCarley, RW, Haimson, J, Smith, RS, O'Donnell, B, et al. Neuropsychological correlates of MRI temporal lobe abnormalities in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (1993) 150:1849–55. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.12.1849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63.Rasser, PE, Schall, U, Todd, J, Michie, PT, Ward, PB, Johnston, P, et al. Gray matter deficits, mismatch negativity, and outcomes in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2011) 37:131–40. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64.Curtis, MT, Coffman, BA, and Salisbury, DF. Pitch and duration mismatch negativity are associated with distinct auditory cortex and inferior frontal cortex volumes in the first-episode schizophrenia spectrum. Schizophr Bull Open. (2021) 2:sgab005. doi: 10.1093/schizbullopen/sgab005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65.Murphy, TK, Haigh, SM, Coffman, BA, and Salisbury, DF. Mismatch negativity and impaired social functioning in long-term and in first episode schizophrenia spectrum psychosis. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:544. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66.Salisbury, DF, Kuroki, N, Kasai, K, Shenton, ME, and McCarley, RW. Progressive and interrelated functional and structural evidence of post-onset brain reduction in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2007) 64:521–9. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67.First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M, and Williams, J. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders New York State Psychiatric Institute: Biometrics Research Department (1997).

Google Scholar

68.Kay, SR, Fiszbein, A, and Opler, LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (1987) 13:261–76. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69.Nuechterlein, KH, Green, MF, Kern, RS, Baade, LE, Barch, DM, Cohen, JD, et al. The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry. (2008) 165:203–13. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70.Uusitalo, MA, and Ilmoniemi, RJ. Signal-space projection method for separating MEG or EEG into components. Med Biol Eng Comput. (1997) 35:135–40. doi: 10.1007/BF02534144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71.Delorme, A, and Makeig, S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. (2004) 134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72.Tadel, F, Baillet, S, Mosher, JC, Pantazis, D, and Leahy, RM. Brainstorm: a user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Comput Intell Neurosci. (2011) 2011:879716:1–13. doi: 10.1155/2011/879716

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73.Glasser, MF, Sotiropoulos, SN, Wilson, JA, Coalson, TS, Fischl, B, Andersson, JL, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the human connectome project. NeuroImage. (2013) 80:105–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74.Robinson, EC, Jbabdi, S, Glasser, MF, Andersson, J, Burgess, GC, Harms, MP, et al. MSM: a new flexible framework for multimodal surface matching. NeuroImage. (2014) 100:414–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75.Van Essen, DC, Glasser, MF, Dierker, DL, Harwell, J, and Coalson, T. Parcellations and hemispheric asymmetries of human cerebral cortex analyzed on surface-based atlases. Cereb Cortex. (2012) 22:2241–62. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76.Greve, DN, and Fischl, B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. NeuroImage. (2009) 48:63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77.Salimi-Khorshidi, G, Douaud, G, Beckmann, CF, Glasser, MF, Griffanti, L, and Smith, SM. Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage. (2014) 90:449–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78.Griffanti, L, Salimi-Khorshidi, G, Beckmann, CF, Auerbach, EJ, Douaud, G, Sexton, CE, et al. ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage. (2014) 95:232–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79.Utevsky, AV, Smith, DV, and Huettel, SA. Precuneus is a functional core of the default-mode network. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:932–40. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4227-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80.Degerman, A, Rinne, T, Salmi, J, Salonen, O, and Alho, K. Selective attention to sound location or pitch studied with fMRI. Brain Res. (2006) 1077:123–34. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81.Krumbholz, K, Nobis, EA, Weatheritt, RJ, and Fink, GR. Executive control of spatial attention shifts in the auditory compared to the visual modality. Hum Brain Mapp. (2009) 30:1457–69. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82.Cavanna, AE, and Trimble, MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. (2006) 129:564–83. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83.Ozkurt, TE, and Schnitzler, A. A critical note on the definition of phase-amplitude cross-frequency coupling. J Neurosci Methods. (2011) 201:438–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84.Mormann, F, Lehnertz, K, David, PE, and Elger, C. Mean phase coherence as a measure for phase synchronization and its application to the EEG of epilepsy patients. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena. (2000) 144:358–69. doi: 10.1016/S0167-2789(00)00087-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85.Lachaux, JP, Rodriguez, E, Martinerie, J, and Varela, FJ. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp. (1999) 8:194–208. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<194::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-C

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86.Lloyd, S . Least squares quantization in PCM. IEEE Trans Inf Theory. (1982) 28:129–37. doi: 10.1109/TIT.1982.1056489

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87.Tseng, YL, Liu, HH, Liou, M, Tsai, AC, Chien, VSC, Shyu, ST, et al. Lingering sound: event-related phase-amplitude coupling and phase-locking in Fronto-Temporo-parietal functional networks during memory retrieval of music melodies. Front Hum Neurosci. (2019) 13:150. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88.Lakatos, P, Shah, AS, Knuth, KH, Ulbert, I, Karmos, G, and Schroeder, CE. An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. (2005) 94:1904–11. doi: 10.1152/jn.00263.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89.Kong, L, Michalka, SW, Rosen, ML, Sheremata, SL, Swisher, JD, Shinn-Cunningham, BG, et al. Auditory spatial attention representations in the human cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex. (2014) 24:773–84. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90.Alho, K, Vorobyev, VA, Medvedev, SV, Pakhomov, SV, Starchenko, MG, Tervaniemi, M, et al. Selective attention to human voice enhances brain activity bilaterally in the superior temporal sulcus. Brain Res. (2006) 1075:142–50. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91.Burgess, PW, Veitch, E, de Lacy, CA, and Shallice, T. The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia. (2000) 38:848–63. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00134-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92.Dreher, JC, Koechlin, E, Tierney, M, and Grafman, J. Damage to the fronto-polar cortex is associated with impaired multitasking. PLoS One. (2008) 3:e3227. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93.Mansouri, FA, Buckley, MJ, Mahboubi, M, and Tanaka, K. Behavioral consequences of selective damage to frontal pole and posterior cingulate cortices. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2015) 112:E3940–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422629112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94.Karten, A, Pantazatos, SP, Khalil, D, Zhang, X, and Hirsch, J. Dynamic coupling between the lateral occipital-cortex, default-mode, and frontoparietal networks during bistable perception. Brain Connect. (2013) 3:286–93. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95.Baker, CM, Burks, JD, Briggs, RG, Stafford, J, Conner, AK, Glenn, CA, et al. A connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum-chapter 9: the occipital lobe. Oper Neurosurg. (2018) 15:S372–406. doi: 10.1093/ons/opy263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96.Tootell, RB, Hadjikhani, N, Hall, EK, Marrett, S, Vanduffel, W, Vaughan, JT, et al. The retinotopy of visual spatial attention. Neuron. (1998) 21:1409–22. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80659-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97.Job, DE, Whalley, HC, Johnstone, EC, and Lawrie, SM. Grey matter changes over time in high risk subjects developing schizophrenia. NeuroImage. (2005) 25:1023–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98.Pantelis, C, Velakoulis, D, McGorry, PD, Wood, SJ, Suckling, J, Phillips, LJ, et al. Neuroanatomical abnormalities before and after onset of psychosis: a cross-sectional and longitudinal MRI comparison. Lancet. (2003) 361:281–8. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12323-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99.Takahashi, T, Wood, SJ, Yung, AR, Soulsby, B, McGorry, PD, Suzuki, M, et al. Progressive gray matter reduction of the superior temporal gyrus during transition to psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2009) 66:366–76. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100.Cannon, TD, Chung, Y, He, G, Sun, D, Jacobson, A, van Erp, TG, et al. Progressive reduction in cortical thickness as psychosis develops: a multisite longitudinal neuroimaging study of youth at elevated clinical risk. Biol Psychiatry. (2015) 77:147–57. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101.Fortea, A, Batalla, A, Radua, J, van Eijndhoven, P, Baeza, I, Albajes-Eizagirre, A, et al. Cortical gray matter reduction precedes transition to psychosis in individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis: a voxel-based meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. (2021) 232:98–106. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2021.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102.Barta, PE, Pearlson, GD, Powers, RE, Richards, SS, and Tune, LE. Auditory hallucinations and smaller superior temporal gyral volume in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (1990) 147:1457–62. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.11.1457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103.Salisbury, DF, Shafer, AR, Murphy, TK, Haigh, SM, and Coffman, BA. Pitch and duration mismatch negativity and Heschl's gyrus volume in first-episode schizophrenia-spectrum individuals. Clin EEG Neurosci. (2020) 51:359–64. doi: 10.1177/1550059420914214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104.Shenton, ME, Kikinis, R, Jolesz, FA, Pollak, SD, LeMay, M, Wible, CG, et al. Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought disorder in schizophrenia. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. N Engl J Med. (1992) 327:604–12. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199208273270905

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105.Takahashi, T, Suzuki, M, Zhou, SY, Tanino, R, Hagino, H, Niu, L, et al. Temporal lobe gray matter in schizophrenia spectrum: a volumetric MRI study of the fusiform gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, and middle and inferior temporal gyri. Schizophr Res. (2006) 87:116–26. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106.Selemon, LD, and Goldman-Rakic, PS. The reduced neuropil hypothesis: a circuit based model of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. (1999) 45:17–25. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00281-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107.Lewis, DA, and Sweet, RA. Schizophrenia from a neural circuitry perspective: advancing toward rational pharmacological therapies. J Clin Invest. (2009) 119:706–16. doi: 10.1172/JCI37335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108.Todd, J, Harms, L, Schall, U, and Michie, PT. Mismatch negativity: translating the potential. Front Psychol. (2013) 4:171. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109.Eack, SM, Hogarty, GE, Cho, RY, Prasad, KM, Greenwald, DP, Hogarty, SS, et al. Neuroprotective effects of cognitive enhancement therapy against gray matter loss in early schizophrenia: results from a 2-year randomized controlled trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2010) 67:674–82. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: selective attention, first episode psychosis, phase-locking value, functional connectivity, phase-amplitude coupling, gray matter

Citation: Curtis MT, Sklar AL, Coffman BA and Salisbury DF (2023) Functional connectivity and gray matter deficits within the auditory attention circuit in first-episode psychosis. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1114703. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1114703

Received: 02 December 2022; Accepted: 24 January 2023;
Published: 13 February 2023.

Edited by:

Chao Yan, East China Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Yegang Hu, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China
Ya Wang, Institute of Psychology (CAS), China
Matthew J. Hoptman, Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, United States

Copyright © 2023 Curtis, Sklar, Coffman and Salisbury. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dean F. Salisbury, ✉ salisburyd@upmc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.