Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychiatry, 20 September 2022
Sec. Mood Disorders

Basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration in major depressive disorder

  • 1Social and Preventive Medicine, Department of Sports and Health Sciences, Faculty of Human Science, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
  • 2Centre for Emotional Health, School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 3Division of Training and Movement Sciences, Research Focus Cognition Sciences, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
  • 4Social and Preventive Medicine, Department of Sports and Health Science, Intra-Faculty Unit “Cognitive Sciences”, Faculty of Human Science, and Faculty of Health Sciences Brandenburg, Research Area Services Research and e-Health, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
  • 5Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 6Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway

Basic psychological needs theory postulates that a social environment that satisfies individuals’ three basic psychological needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness leads to optimal growth and well-being. On the other hand, the frustration of these needs is associated with ill-being and depressive symptoms foremost investigated in non-clinical samples; yet, there is a paucity of research on need frustration in clinical samples. Survey data were compared between adult individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD; n = 115; 48.69% female; 38.46 years, SD = 10.46) with those of a non-depressed comparison sample (n = 201; 53.23% female; 30.16 years, SD = 12.81). Need profiles were examined with a linear mixed model (LMM). Individuals with depression reported higher levels of frustration and lower levels of satisfaction in relation to the three basic psychological needs when compared to non-depressed adults. The difference between depressed and non-depressed groups was significantly larger for frustration than satisfaction regarding the needs for relatedness and competence. LMM correlation parameters confirmed the expected positive correlation between the three needs. This is the first study showing substantial differences in need-based experiences between depressed and non-depressed adults. The results confirm basic assumptions of the self-determination theory and have preliminary implications in tailoring therapy for depression.

Introduction

Affective disorders are emotional disorders in which excessive sadness or severely elevated mood is exhibited over prolonged periods of time. The most common affective disorder is major depressive disorder (MDD). The World Health Organization names MDD as a leading cause of disability worldwide (1). Globally, over 250 million people (3.8% of the population) are currently affected by MDD, with 16% experiencing MDD at some point in their life (2). Phenomenologically, individuals with MDD show a persistent depressed mood, as well as a lack of drive and joy (3, 4). Given the relatively low level of remittance (5) and the high recurrence rate (6) of MDD, it is imperative to increase knowledge concerning the etiology and maintaining factors of MDD which can further improve treatment efforts.

The etiology of depression is multifactorial and involves the interaction of social, psychological, and biological factors. A growing volume of research has shown that MDD is associated with impairments in emotion regulation and cognitive control (711), which are associated with altered neural activation and connectivity patterns in fronto-parietal and fronto-limbic circuits (1214). On a physiological level, recent literature examines the dynamics of the central and autonomic nervous systems in fear-related disorders and threat experience. The neurovisceral integration model of fear (NVI-f) (15) assumes a complex interplay between the central and peripheral nervous systems, which, in the case of a fear-inducing stimulus, passes through the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus to the heart and thus initiates fear reactions through sympathetic and parasympathetic projections (16). Here, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) contributes to the learned distinction between threat and safety signals, in that the vmPFC supports positive affective processing of safety signals in conjunction with their implicit stress-relieving properties (17). As recent research shows, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) plays a role in Pavlovian threat conditioning in humans not only in the extinction or reversal of previously acquired stimulus-outcome associations but also during threat acquisition (1720). In patients with MDD, reduced volume and altered activity patterns were observed in the vmPFC (2123). Importantly, recent work by Grahek et al. (24) highlights the need to investigate the motivational factors underlying depression. A leading theory concerning human motivation is the self-determination theory (SDT) (25), which details contexts and factors that contribute to both well-being and ill-being including psychopathology, such as MDD.

According to basic psychological needs theory (BPNT) (26), a subtheory within the broader SDT, there are three universally inherent basic psychological needs that are essential for individuals’ psychological growth, integrity, and well-being: autonomy, competence, and relatedness. The need for autonomy refers to the desire to feel volitional in the regulation of one’s behavior and experiencing one’s behavior as self-initiated; the need for competence refers to a perception of mastery through effectively interacting with the environment; and the need for relatedness refers to an experience of belonging and care with significant others (26). Well-integrated motivation, based on interest, joy or a sense of value, arises on the basis of fulfilled basic psychological needs (27). Some time ago, BPNT was extended to include the dimension of need frustration in addition to need satisfaction. Whereas, low psychological need satisfaction denotes the absence of need fulfillment, need frustration describes an experience of threat (28). Frustration of the need for autonomy involves the feeling of being pressured or even forced to think, feel, or act a certain way; frustration of the need for competence involves the experience of being defenseless and feeling like a failure; and frustration of the need for relatedness refers to an experience of being rejected or even ostracized (30). Need satisfaction and need frustration are asymmetrical related to each other, with high levels of need frustration always coinciding with low levels of need satisfaction, but low levels of need satisfaction not necessarily implicating a high level of need frustration. For example, although an individual might not feel connected to colleagues (i.e., low level of relatedness satisfaction), this person does not necessarily feel actively excluded by these colleagues (i.e., low level of relatedness frustration). While need satisfaction has been found to be especially conducive to adaptive outcomes (e.g., vitality), need frustration is implicated in ill-being and even psychopathology (28, 29). To illustrate, need frustration has been linked to stress (31), burn-out and eating symptomatology (32, 33), anxiety (34), and disengagement (35). Further, a recent study found need frustration being a partial mediator in the relation between emotion regulation and psychopathology (36).

Based on non-clinical samples, a large number of studies demonstrate a strong link between need frustration and depressive symptoms, showing the incremental (and sometimes sole) predictive value of need frustration over a lack of need satisfaction (30, 32, 3739). A few studies have also examined the basic psychological needs in clinical samples of depressed individuals. A recent longitudinal study found that an increase in basic need satisfaction was associated with a decrease in depressive symptoms during the course of treatment (combined psychotherapy with medication) in a clinical sample with MDD (40). Similarly, Dwyer et al. (41) showed that a higher level of autonomy satisfaction during group therapy related to decreased depressive symptoms through a reduction in negative automatic thoughts in depressed adults.

In summary, the frustration of the basic psychological needs is proposed to be a general vulnerability factor for psychopathological symptom occurrence. However, disorder-specific research is needed to further investigate need-based functioning in the development, maintenance, and recovery of psychiatric disorders including MDD. However, to date, there is a notable paucity of research that has examined both need satisfaction and need frustration in a clinical sample with MDD. Accordingly, the aim of this study was to address this gap and to specifically examine whether individuals with depression show higher levels of need frustration alongside lower levels of need satisfaction when compared to individuals without depressive symptoms. On the basis of BPNT, we hypothesized that individuals without depressive symptoms would report being significantly more satisfied and less frustrated with regard to their needs than individuals with depressive symptoms on all three scales. We also expected that the difference between the two groups would be larger for frustration than satisfaction ratings for all three needs. Finally in an exploratory step, we added covariates of age, gender, and education to assess the generalizability of the main effects.

Materials and methods

Participants

This study was based on data of two adult samples from two recent German studies in order to comparatively evaluate the need domains (autonomy, competence, and relatedness) and dimensions (satisfaction and frustration) between clinically depressed and non-depressed adults.

The clinical sample with mild-to-moderate MDD was recruited from the SPeED study [Sport/Exercise Therapy and Psychotherapy—evaluating treatment Effects in Depressive patients; (42)]. The diagnosis of a mild or moderate depression episode in the SPeED study sample was confirmed using the Structured Clinical Interview, Axis I (SCID-I) (43, 44) according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th Edition (DSM-IV) (3). Individuals with Becks Depression Inventory II (BDI-II) (45) scores < 14 [indicating no depressive symptoms; (46)] were excluded from the analyses (n = 4). The clinical sample at baseline/pre-treatment (T1) consisted of n = 115 (48.69% female) adults with a mean age of 38.46 years (SD = 10.46; range 21–65 years). The SPeED-Study protocol was approved by the local ethics committee of Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany (No EA1/113/15). After detailed study description, written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

The non-clinical sample was recruited from the German Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration Scale (BPNSFS) validation study (39), which included university students, working adults, and older adults. Data were collected in January and February of 2016. For the purposes of this study, participants were excluded if they reported depressive symptom scores greater than the cut-off value of 17 (n = 35) using the Center of Epidemiological Studies—Depression Scale (CES-D), and were older than 65 years (n = 103). The latter exclusion criterion was used to ensure that the samples between the two datasets were matched in age range, given the clinical sample was based on a sample younger than 65 years. The non-depressed sample consisted of n = 201 participants (53.23% female) with a mean age of 30.16 years (SD = 12.81; range 18–64). Participants provided written informed consent to take part in the study (39). The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Potsdam (No. 41/2015).

Table 1 reports characteristics of the two samples. Significant statistical differences between the two samples were evident for depressive symptoms, age, and educational level.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics by group.

Main outcome measures

Depressive symptoms

In the two studies, depressive symptoms were assessed with different measurement instruments. In the clinical sample, the Becks Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (45) German version (47) was used. The BDI-II is a 21-item self-report questionnaire assessing depression symptom severity over a 2-week period, using a 4-point rating scale ranging from 0 (indicating absence of a symptom) to 3 (indicating a serve symptom occurrence), with elevated scores reflecting greater depressive symptom severity (total range of scores: 0–63). Sum scores 14–19 indicate mild depression, between 20 and 28, moderate depression, and between 29 and 63, severe depressive disorder (46). The German version of the BDI-II has demonstrated strong internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha > or = 0.84) for clinical and non-clinical samples (48). Internal consistency in the present sample was also very good, α = 0.81.

In the non-clinical sample, depressive symptoms were measured using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (49, 50). This scale assesses depressive symptoms in the general population by asking for the frequency of occurrence during the last 7 days (using a 4-point Likert scale with 0 = rarely or none of the time to 3 = most or all of the time). Scores range from 0 to 45, with scores above 17 indicating a depressive disorder. The German version has been found to have good internal consistency in the general population [Cronbach’s alpha = 0.90; (50)]. The reliability in the present sample was also adequate, α = 0.73. Table 2 reports relevant descriptive statistics and Cronbach’s α.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means (M), standard deviations (SD), and Cronbach’s alpha (α) by group for the six subscales of the Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration Scale (BPNSFS).

Basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration scale

To assess basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration, the validated 24-item German version (39) of the BPNSFS (30) was used. Each of the three needs is operationalized with eight items, comprising two subscales, with items focusing on satisfaction or frustration of each need respectively (e.g., “I feel connected with people who care for me, and for whom I care.” —relatedness satisfaction and “I feel that people who are important to me are cold and distant toward me.” —relatedness frustration). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). The internal consistency for each scale proved to be satisfactory, with Cronbach’s α ranging between 0.66 and 0.81 in the German sample (39) and between 0.64 and 0.89 in the original study by Chen et al. (30). The internal consistency in the present combined sample ranged between α = 0.74 for relatedness frustration and α = 0.89 for competence satisfaction. x

Data analysis

Data analysis was conducted using International Business Machines Corporation (IBM) Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) statistics software (Version 20) and the R system for statistical computing (R Version 4.1.3, R Studio 2022.02.1). We primarily used the following R packages: dyplr (51) to transform the data frame, lme4 (52) for fitting the linear mixed effects models, sjPlot (53) for data visualization and graphics derived from the ggplot package (54).

The six subscale scores from the BPNSFS served as dependent measures in a Scale (3; autonomy vs. competence vs. relatedness) × Dimension (2; satisfaction vs. frustration) within-subject design. Group (2; non-depressed vs. depressed) was included as a between-subject factor. To maintain the same direction of effects for the six subscales (i.e., have them all correlate positively), the three frustration subscales were reversed (i.e., multiplied with –1). Adding four points to the scores of frustration subscales and subtracting four points from the scores of satisfaction subscales, scores ranged from 0 to –16 for the frustration subscales and 0 to +16 for the satisfaction subscales. Thus, higher scores indicate being better off in all six scales.

Inferential statistics were based on a linear mixed model (LMM) estimated with the lme4 package in R (52). This approach makes it possible to estimate the main effects and interactions of group and dimension for each of the three need domains in a single analysis. In other words, we specified the group × dimension interaction as nested within the three levels of the factor scale, which is for autonomy, competence, and relatedness. All main effects and interactions of this Group (2) × Type of need (3) × Dimension (2) design were estimated as fixed effects. In a second step, this model was extended to check main effects of age, gender, and education and their moderation of group and dimension effects. In the random-effect structure, we estimated correlation parameters between the scores of the three scales and with the effect of dimension. This complex LMM was supported by the data. There was no need for parsimonious model selection (55). To test expected group difference in each of the six subscales, a post hoc LMM was performed and alpha error accumulation was taken into account with the Bonferroni method.

To assess the power of our analyses, we conducted sensitivity analyses using G*Power. Since we focus on group differences and directed hypotheses were formulated, the simulations are based on the t-test type with two independent means, one-tailed. Due to the required correction for multiple testing, the simulations also cover the range of smaller assumed alpha than α = 0.05. Sensitivity analysis revealed that even with a very low alpha of α = 0.001 and a desired power of (1–β) = 0.95, effects of moderate size (d = 0.55) are found with the available sample size.

Results

Complete scores were available from 301 individuals (i.e., there were 1,806 observations). Table 3 lists fixed-effect estimates for main effects of group, dimension, age, gender, and a proxy for education (i.e., qualification for university entrance) as well as interactions of group with dimension and age. Table 3 also provides 95% credibility intervals for each effect, based on a likelihood profile and the appropriate cutoffs based on likelihood ratio tests, and p-values based on Wald statistics (i.e., estimate and standard error). Adding other interaction terms to the model did not significantly improve the goodness of fit.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Fixed-effect estimates of three linear mixed models for each score of the basic psychological needs (Autonomy, Competence, Relatedness) including effects of the group (depressed vs. non-depressed), the dimension (satisfaction vs. frustration), as well as their interactions and covariate effects (age, gender, and university qualification).

Scores were higher for the satisfaction than the frustration dimension and for the non-depressed than the depressed group for each of the three need domains. Interactions between group and dimension were significant for competence and for relatedness. For both scales, the difference between the non-depressed and depressed groups was larger for the frustration than the satisfaction dimension (see Figure 1). A post hoc LMM showed that there were significant group differences for each of the six scales in the expected direction, even after Bonferroni Correction.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Observed group × dimension interactions for the three need domains. The interactions are significant for needs of competence and relatedness. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals.

Further, the profile of means in Figure 1 exhibits large and significant group differences for the three scales; group differences between non-depressed and depressed were 4.7, 5.6, and 3.4 for autonomy, competence, and relatedness, respectively. In a subsequent exploratory post hoc analysis, the group difference on need of competence was significantly larger (t = 2.93) and the group difference for need of relatedness significantly smaller (t = –4.03) than the need for autonomy.

There were significant effects associated with covariates gender, education, and age. Scores were significantly larger for males than females for need of autonomy (1.7 vs. 1.3) and need of competence (3.4 vs. 2.6) and significantly larger for females than males for need of relatedness (5.4 vs. 4.7). There was also a significant difference between individuals with and without qualification for university entrance (1.8 vs. –0.2). None of the interactions of gender and education with group and dimension were significant. For age, needs for competence and relatedness increased significantly with age and, moreover, the age-related increase in competence was larger for depressed than non-depressed individuals (see Supplementary Figure 1). There were no significant interactions involving dimension.

The data supported the estimation of reliable variance components for the three scale scores and the dimension effect, as well as the associated correlation parameters (CPs) (i.e., individual differences; see Supplementary Table 1, left column and values below diagonal). The CPs among the scales were uniformly positive between 0.47 and 0.59, supporting an interpretation as a latent construct of basic psychological need. Note that the scale scores are averaged over satisfaction and frustration subscales. Two of the scales, autonomy and competence were significantly negatively correlated with the difference between satisfaction and frustration subscales. Thus, across this set of observations, it appears that the smaller the difference between the dimensions, the better off a person is. The corresponding zero-order correlations of scores are somewhat larger because they are not adjusted for differences due to group, sex, and age; the significant dimension-related CPs, however, are numerically slightly more negative than the corresponding zero-order correlations (–0.35 vs. –0.22; –0.25 vs. –0.22) because shrinkage correction increases the reliability of difference scores.

Discussion

The present study investigated basic psychological need frustration and satisfaction in a sample of individuals with depression in comparison with a non-depressed control group for the three domains autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Individuals without depressive symptoms reported being significantly more satisfied and less frustrated than individuals with depressive symptoms with respect to all three needs. The results further showed that the group difference was significantly larger on the frustration than on the satisfaction dimension for competence and relatedness, illustrating the specific role of need frustration in the occurrence of psychopathological symptoms. Strengthening the benefit of implementing the frustration dimension in BPNT, this result aligns with previous research (28, 30, 39) and assumptions of BPNT. However, an interaction effect was not found for the need for autonomy in the present sample. Additionally, major moderation of the observed effects by age and educational status was not observed. The size of the group difference depends on both need domain and dimension. Although it might be assumed that scores are comparable across the three need scales, it is noteworthy that large differences for the size of group difference on the three scales emerged, with the largest group difference evident for competence experience and the smallest for relatedness experience.

The findings from this study make two novel, incremental advances in furthering our understanding of experienced basic need satisfaction and frustration in the context of MDD: First, competence frustration may play a critical role in at least maintaining symptoms in individuals with mild-to-moderate MDD. This concurs with psychopathology research findings and specifically aligns with depression-specific characteristics, such as a low self-efficacy-expectancy to the point of learned helplessness (5660). Second, unlike the other two need domains for individuals with elevated depressive symptoms, the experience of relatedness also shows less satisfaction and more frustration in comparison to non-depressed adults, although it remains relatively stable in a mild to moderate Major Depressive Episode. Accordingly, not all need domains are affected equally. A related assumption that has received less attention to date is that not only a high but also a balanced satisfaction of needs is presumed to be conducive to well-being and growth-processes, with balanced need satisfaction referring to similar scores on need satisfaction across the three needs (61). Although satisfaction of one need typically parallels the satisfaction of the other needs, this is not always the case. Ryan and Deci (62) state that basic psychological needs can be experienced in conflict, even if not being inherently contradictory. Unbalanced need satisfaction indicates a conflictual experience in that the satisfaction of one need may be foregone in favor of another (e.g., excessive focus on externally imposed performance). Initial research results support this proposition and studies have shown that both need satisfaction and balanced need satisfaction were predictive of well-being (6365). Thus, in addition to the score of the individual scales, the between-need-balance merits further investigation. One possible explanation for the present findings would be a tendency to accept a loss in the needs for autonomy and competence while a certain experience of relatedness is maintained. Autonomous behavior is sometimes accompanied by a detachment from a particular mode of relationship (e.g., interpersonal dependence). Interpersonal dependency on the other hand has been cited as an individual vulnerability factor for depression (6668). Accordingly, it is conceivable that especially the conflictual experience of basic psychological needs (e.g., when an individual can only fulfill his/her need for relatedness by sacrificing his/her autonomy) leads to the inhibition of growth processes, maladaption and potentially ill-being. However, further longitudinal research is needed to determine whether this is a depression- and/or course-specific pattern.

Limitations and direction for future research

The findings from this study need to be interpreted in relation to some limitations. Given the cross-sectional study design, the findings are limited to group-comparison at a specific timepoint. Changes in the need profiles during the course of disease or treatment were not investigated. This study also focused on a clinical sample with mild-to-moderate MDD compared to a non-depressed sample, while not representing the broader mental health status of the control group nor including participants with severe major depressive episodes. Further, individuals in the non-depressed sample overall had a higher level of education and were younger than the clinical sample. Due to the large age range of the present sample and the observation that age itself has an influence on the experience of basic psychological needs future research could, therefore, incorporate a longitudinal design thereby employing a matched control group in addition to a clinical sample displaying mild-to-severe MDD.

Further, with the nested LMM, we do not provide direct tests between the different needs. The decision for the nested LMM was motivated by the assumption that, for example, the degree of autonomy satisfaction is not comparable with the degree of competence satisfaction. A meaningful avenue for future research is to consider how such comparisons might be achieved and broaden the theoretical scope of BPNT. Assuming that the scales can be compared with each other in terms of their magnitude, future research with the BPNSFS could contain nested specification of group × dimension with tests of interactions involving each scale. Group-specific need profiles have great potential to further elucidate psychological disorder symptom severity, progression, and processes and to enable targeted, profile-based interventions. Thereby, need profiles should be investigated in diverse samples on the continuum of well-being to ill-being and across various psychological disorders. Finally, future research is warranted to examine the specific role of the needs in relation to other factors found to be important in depression, such as emotion regulation (8, 36), cognitive control (7, 10), and neural deficits (13, 14, 69).

Conclusion and practical implications

The study of need frustration as a separate dimension from need satisfaction has great potential to investigate threat experience and its motivational and behavioral consequences underlying psychopathology (i.e., MDD). This is the first study to demonstrate that there is a substantial difference between non-depressed adults and adults with mild-to-moderate depression in terms of their need satisfaction and need frustration. As hypothesized, people with MDD show lower satisfaction and higher frustration of the basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness. Assuming that need frustration plays a transdiagnostic role accounting for a diversity of pathological symptoms, the question remains open whether disorder-specific need-based dynamics can be found that allow a deeper understanding and more targeted interventions. The present findings highlight the potential benefits of examining the amount as well as the balance of basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration in disorder-specific samples to further investigate compensatory, substitute, and reactive processes associated with need frustration as well as experienced conflict between the needs in detail.

Specifically, our findings suggest that people experiencing MDD would profit from social environments supporting all basic psychological needs with a particular focus on competence experience. For example, this could take the form of psychotherapy (5, 70), in which the meaning of failures could be discussed or exercise therapy (7173), in which successes can be celebrated together. An assumption that would be consistent with the present findings is that individuals with MDD experience their psychological needs as being in conflict with each other. Then, it would also be conceivable to work with patients on a perceived conflict between basic psychological needs so that a balanced satisfaction of needs can be achieved.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, upon reasonable request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany, and Ethics Committee of the University of Potsdam. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

AH, AP, and RK made substantial contributions to conception and design and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AP and RK carried out data analysis. AH, AP, MK, JK-D, and RK interpreted the data. All authors were involved in revising the manuscript critically for important intellectual content and provided final approval of the version to be published.

Funding

AH and AP were partly funded by a grant at the Potsdam Graduate School, University of Potsdam. AP received a final doctoral fellowship for 6 months from the Potsdam Graduate School, University of Potsdam. The publication fees are funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – Projektnummer 491466077.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.962501/full#supplementary-material

References

1. World Health Organization. Depression. (2021). Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/depression (accessed June 1, 2022).

Google Scholar

2. Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Walters EE. Prevalence, Severity, and Comorbidity of 12-Month DSM-IV Disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2005) 62:617. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fifth ed. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. World Health Organization. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems. 11th ed. Geneva: World Health Organization (2019).

Google Scholar

5. Cuijpers P, Karyotaki E, Ciharova M, Miguel C, Noma H, Furukawa TA. The effects of psychotherapies for depression on response, remission, reliable change, and deterioration: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2021) 144:288–99. doi: 10.1111/acps.13335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Bockting CL, Hollon SD, Jarrett RB, Kuyken W, Dobson K. A lifetime approach to major depressive disorder: The contributions of psychological interventions in preventing relapse and recurrence. Clin Psychol Rev. (2015) 41:16–26. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Joormann J, Quinn ME. Cognitive processes and emotion regulation in depression: review: cognitive processes in depression. Depress Anxiety. (2014) 31:308–15. doi: 10.1002/da.22264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Joormann J, Stanton CH. Examining emotion regulation in depression: A review and future directions. Behav Res Ther. (2016) 86:35–49. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Ahern E, Semkovska M. Cognitive functioning in the first-episode of major depressive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology (2017) 31:52–72. doi: 10.1037/neu0000319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Grahek I, Everaert J, Krebs RM, Koster EHW. Cognitive control in depression: toward clinical models informed by cognitive neuroscience. Clin Psychol Sci. (2018) 6:464–80. doi: 10.1177/2167702618758969

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. LeMoult J, Gotlib IH. Depression: A cognitive perspective. Clin Psychol Rev. (2019) 69:51–66. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Picó-Pérez M, Radua J, Steward T, Menchón JM, Soriano-Mas C. Emotion regulation in mood and anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis of fMRI cognitive reappraisal studies. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 79:96–104. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. He Z, Lu F, Sheng W, Han S, Pang Y, Chen Y, et al. Abnormal functional connectivity as neural biological substrate of trait and state characteristics in major depressive disorder. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2020) 102:109949. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.109949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Wackerhagen C, Veer IM, Erk S, Mohnke S, Lett TA, Wüstenberg T, et al. Amygdala functional connectivity in major depression – disentangling markers of pathology, risk and resilience. Psychol Med. (2020) 50:2740–50. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719002885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Battaglia S, Thayer JF. Functional interplay between central and autonomic nervous systems in human fear conditioning. Trends Neurosci. (2022) 45:504–6. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2022.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Battaglia S, Orsolini S, Borgomaneri S, Barbieri R, Diciotti S, di Pellegrino G. Characterizing cardiac autonomic dynamics of fear learning in humans. Psychophysiology. (2022) 7:e14122. doi: 10.1111/psyp.14122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Harrison BJ, Fullana MA, Via E, Soriano-Mas C, Vervliet B, Martínez-Zalacaín I, et al. Human ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the positive affective processing of safety signals. NeuroImage. (2017) 152:12–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Fullana MA, Harrison BJ, Soriano-Mas C, Vervliet B, Cardoner N, Àvila-Parcet A, et al. Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Mol Psychiatry. (2016) 21:500–8. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Dunsmoor JE, Kroes MCW, Li J, Daw ND, Simpson HB, Phelps EA. Role of human ventromedial prefrontal cortex in learning and recall of enhanced extinction. J Neurosci. (2019) 39:3264–76. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2713-18.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Battaglia S, Garofalo S, di Pellegrino G, Starita F. Revaluing the Role of vmPFC in the Acquisition of Pavlovian Threat Conditioning in Humans. J Neurosci. (2020) 40:8491–500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0304-20.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Drevets WC, Savitz J, Trimble M. The subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in mood disorders. CNS Spectr. (2008) 13:663–81. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900013754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Price JL, Drevets WC. Neurocircuitry of Mood Disorders. Neuropsychopharmacol. (2010) 35:192–216. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wise T, Radua J, Via E, Cardoner N, Abe O, Adams TM, et al. Common and distinct patterns of grey-matter volume alteration in major depression and bipolar disorder: evidence from voxel-based meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. (2017) 22:1455–63. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.72

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Grahek I, Shenhav A, Musslick S, Krebs RM, Koster EHW. Motivation and cognitive control in depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 102:371–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Deci EL, Ryan RM. Toward an organismic integration theory. In: Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior Boston, MA: Springer (1985). p. 113–48. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-2271-7_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Deci EL, Ryan RM. The “What” and “Why” of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Behavior. Psychol Inq. (2000) 11:227–68. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Ryan RM, Deci EL. (2020). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: Definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 61:101860. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Vansteenkiste M, Ryan RM. On psychological growth and vulnerability: Basic psychological need satisfaction and need frustration as a unifying principle. J Psychother Integr. (2013) 23:263–80. doi: 10.1037/a0032359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Vansteenkiste M, Ryan RM, Soenens B. Basic inside the text psychological need theory: Advancements, critical themes, and future directions. Motiv Emot. (2020) 44:1–31. doi: 10.1007/s11031-019-09818-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Chen B, Vansteenkiste M, Beyers W, Boone L, Deci EL, Van der Kaap-Deeder J, et al. Basic psychological need satisfaction, need frustration, and need strength across four cultures. Motiv Emot. (2015) 39:216–36. doi: 10.1007/s11031-014-9450-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Campbell R, Tobback E, Delesie L, Vogelaers D, Mariman A, Vansteenkiste M. Basic psychological need experiences, fatigue, and sleep in individuals with unexplained chronic fatigue. Stress Health. (2017) 33:645–55. doi: 10.1002/smi.2751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bartholomew KJ, Ntoumanis N, Ryan RM, Bosch JA, Thogersen-Ntoumani C. Self-Determination Theory and Diminished Functioning: The Role of Interpersonal Control and Psychological Need Thwarting. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. (2011) 37:1459–73. doi: 10.1177/0146167211413125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Campbell R, Boone L, Vansteenkiste M, Soenens B. Psychological need frustration as a transdiagnostic process in associations of self-critical perfectionism with depressive symptoms and eating pathology. J Clin Psychol. (2018) 74:1775–90. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Haraldsen HM, Solstad BE, Ivarsson A, Halvari H, Abrahamsen FE. Change in basic need frustration in relation to perfectionism, anxiety, and performance in elite junior performers. Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2020) 30:754–65. doi: 10.1111/sms.13614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Jang H, Kim EJ, Reeve J. Why students become more engaged or more disengaged during the semester: A self-determination theory dual-process model. Learn Instr. (2016) 43:27–38. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Brenning K, Soenens B, Vansteenkiste M, De Clercq B, Antrop I. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic risk factor for (Non)Clinical adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing psychopathology: investigating the intervening role of psychological need experiences. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2022) 53:124–36. doi: 10.1007/s10578-020-01107-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Cordeiro P, Paixão P, Lens W, Lacante M, Luyckx K. The portuguese validation of the basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration scale: concurrent and longitudinal relations to well-being and ill-being. Psychol Belgica. (2016) 56:193–209. doi: 10.5334/pb.252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Nishimura T, Suzuki T. Basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration in japan: controlling for the big five personality traits: need satisfaction and frustration in japan. Japanese Psychol Res. (2016) 58:320–31. doi: 10.1111/jpr.12131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Heissel A, Pietrek A, Flunger B, Fydrich T, Rapp MA, Heinzel S, et al. The validation of the german basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration scale in the context of mental health. Eur J Health Psychol. (2018) 25:119–32. doi: 10.1027/2512-8442/a000017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Quitasol MN, Fournier MA, Di Domenico SI, Bagby RM, Quilty LC. Changes in psychological need fulfillment over the course of treatment for major depressive disorder. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2018) 37:381–404. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2018.37.5.381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Dwyer LA, Hornsey MJ, Smith LGE, Oei TPS, Dingle GA. Participant autonomy in cognitive behavioral group therapy: an integration of self-determination and cognitive behavioral theories. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2011) 30:24–46. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2011.30.1.24

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Heinzel S, Rapp MA, Fydrich T, Ströhle A, Terán C, Kallies G, et al. Neurobiological mechanisms of exercise and psychotherapy in depression: The SPeED study—Rationale, design, and methodological issues. Clin Trials. (2018) 15:53–64. doi: 10.1177/1740774517729161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders, clinician version (SCID-CV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press (1996).

Google Scholar

44. Wittchen HU, Zaudig M, Fydrich T. SKID: Strukturiertes klinisches Interview für DSM-IV; Achse I und II; Handanweisung. Göttingen: Hogrefe (1997).

Google Scholar

45. Beck AT. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1961) 4:561. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Worall H, Keller F. BDI Beck-Depressions-Inventar Testhandbuch. Bern: Hans Huber (1995).

Google Scholar

47. Beck M, Keller F, Kühner C. Das Beck Depressionsinventar II. Deutsche Bearbeitung und Handbuch zum BDI II. Frankfurt: Harcourt Test Services (2006).

Google Scholar

48. Kühner C, Bürger C, Keller F, Hautzinger M. Reliabilität und Validität des revidierten Beck-Depressionsinventars (BDI-II): Befunde aus deutschsprachigen Stichproben. Der Nervenarzt. (2007) 78:651–6. doi: 10.1007/s00115-006-2098-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Appl Psychol Meas. (1977) 1:385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Hofmeister D, Keller F. Allgemeine Depressionsskala (ADS). Psychiatr Prax. (2012) 39:302–4. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1326702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Wickham H, François R, Henry L, Müller K. dplyr.: A Grammar of Data Manipulation. (2018). Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr (accessed May 20, 2022).

Google Scholar

52. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J Stat Softw. (2015) 67:1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lüdecke D. sjPlot.: Data Visualization for Statistics in Social Science. (2021). Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sjPlot (accessed accessed May 22, 2022).

Google Scholar

54. Wickham H. ggplot2: elegant graphics for data analysis. Second ed. Cham: Springer (2016).

Google Scholar

55. Bates D, Kliegl R, Vasishth S, Baayen H. Parsimonious Mixed Models. ArXiv preprint (2015): doi: 10.48550/ARXIV.1506.04967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Klein DC, Seligman ME. Reversal of performance deficits and perceptual deficits in learned helplessness and depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (1976) 85:11–26. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.85.1.11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kavanagh DJ, Bower GH. Mood and self-efficacy: Impact of joy and sadness on perceived capabilities. Cogn Ther Res. (1985) 9:507–25. doi: 10.1007/BF01173005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Tak YR, Brunwasser SM, Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, Engels RCME. The Prospective associations between self-efficacy and depressive symptoms from early to middle adolescence: a cross-lagged model. J Youth Adolesc. (2017) 46:744–56. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0614-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Smallheer BA, Vollman M, Dietrich MS. Learned helplessness and depressive symptoms following myocardial infarction. Clin Nurs Res. (2018) 27:597–616. doi: 10.1177/1054773816689752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Trindade IA, Mendes AL, Ferreira NB. The moderating effect of psychological flexibility on the link between learned helplessness and depression symptomatology: A preliminary study. J Contextual Behav Sci. (2020) 15:68–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2019.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Ryan RM, Deci EL, Grolnick WS. Autonomy, relatedness, and the self: Their relation to development and psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ editors. Wiley series on personality processes. Developmental psychopathology, Vol. Theory and Methods. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons (1995). p. 618–55. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Ryan RM, Deci EL. The darker and brighter sides of human existence: basic psychological needs as a unifying concept. Psychol Inq. (2000) 11:319–38. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_03

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Sheldon KM, Niemiec CP. It’s not just the amount that counts: Balanced need satisfaction also affects well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2006) 91:331–41. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.2.331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Perreault S, Gaudreau P, Lapointe M-C, Lacroix C. Does it take three to tango? Psychological need satisfaction and athlete burnout. Int J Sport Psychol. (2007) 38:437–50.

Google Scholar

65. Church AT, Katigbak MS, Locke KD, Zhang H, Shen J, de Jesús Vargas-Flores J, et al. Need satisfaction and well-being: testing self-determination theory in eight cultures. J Cross Cult Psychol. (2013) 44:507–34. doi: 10.1177/0022022112466590

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Hirschfeld RMA, Klerman GL, Gouch HG, Barrett J, Korchin SJ, Chodoff P. A Measure of Interpersonal Dependency. J Pers Assess. (1977) 41:610–8. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4106_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Nietzel M, Harris M. Relationship of dependency and achievement/autonomy to depression. Clin Psychol Rev. (1990) 10:279–97. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(90)90063-G

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Skodol AE, Gallaher PE, Oldham JM. Excessive Dependency and Depression: Is the Relationship Specific? J Nerv Mental Dis. (1996) 184:165–71. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199603000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Fitzgerald JM, Klumpp H, Langenecker S, Phan KL. Transdiagnostic neural correlates of volitional emotion regulation in anxiety and depression. Depress Anxiety. (2019) 36:453–64. doi: 10.1002/da.22859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Gelenberg AJ. A review of current guidelines for depression treatment. J Clin Psychiatry. (2010) 71:e15. doi: 10.4088/JCP.9078tx1c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Cooney GM, Dwan K, Greig CA, Lawlor DA, Rimer J, Waugh FR, et al. Exercise for depression. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd (2013).

Google Scholar

72. Knapen J, Vancampfort D, Moriën Y, Marchal Y. Exercise therapy improves both mental and physical health in patients with major depression. Disabil Rehabil. (2015) 37:1490–5. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2014.972579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Schuch FB, Vancampfort D, Richards J, Rosenbaum S, Ward PB, Stubbs B. Exercise as a treatment for depression: A meta-analysis adjusting for publication bias. J Psychiatr Res. (2016) 77:42–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.02.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: basic need satisfaction and frustration, depressive symptoms, clinical sample, need profiles, social environment

Citation: Pietrek A, Kangas M, Kliegl R, Rapp MA, Heinzel S, van der Kaap-Deeder J and Heissel A (2022) Basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration in major depressive disorder. Front. Psychiatry 13:962501. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.962501

Received: 06 June 2022; Accepted: 19 August 2022;
Published: 20 September 2022.

Edited by:

Luca Steardo, University Magna Græcia of Catanzaro, Italy

Reviewed by:

Simone Battaglia, University of Turin, Italy
Ioana Miclutia, Iuliu Haţieganu University of Medicine and Pharmacy, Romania

Copyright © 2022 Pietrek, Kangas, Kliegl, Rapp, Heinzel, van der Kaap-Deeder and Heissel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anou Pietrek, vesterling@uni-potsdam.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.