Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 25 August 2022
Sec. Schizophrenia

Caregivers’ burden and schizophrenia patients’ quality of life: Sequential mediating effects of expressed emotion and perceived expressed emotion

  • 1Shanghai Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Psychological Crisis Intervention, Affiliated Mental Health Center (ECNU), School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 2Shanghai Changning Mental Health Center, Shanghai, China

Background: Increasing attention has been paid to the role of caregivers’ burden in affecting quality of life (QoL) of schizophrenic patients. However, less is known about potential mediation mechanisms underlying this relationship. The current study aimed to explore the sequential mediating effect of expressed emotion and perceived expressed emotion on the relationship between care burden and QoL among people with schizophrenia.

Methods: 135 Chinese families (one patient and one caregiver) participated in this study. Caregivers reported their care burden and expressed emotion, patients reported their perceived expressed emotion and QoL.

Results: The results of the correlation analysis showed that care burden was negatively related to patients’ QoL, including physical, psychological, and social relationships domains, with patients’ sex, age, educational level, employment status, and medication-taking as covariates. The sequential mediating effects of criticism and perceived criticism between care burden and QoL were not significant. However, the sequential mediating effects of emotional over-involvement and perceived emotional over-involvement (EOI) between care burden and QoL (including physical and psychological domain) were significant.

Conclusion: The results indicated that reducing the burden and expressed emotion of caregivers could be helpful to improve schizophrenia patients’ QoL.

Introduction

The 2017 Global Burden of Disease report showed that about 20 million people worldwide are affected by schizophrenia (1). According to the National Health Commission (NHC) of the People’s Republic of China (2), the number in China is more than 4 million. Presently, the elimination of symptoms is no longer the only goal of schizophrenia treatment; the social function and adaptation of people with schizophrenia is also an essential issue (3). Quality of life (QoL), which reflects the individual’s perception of their life status, is a good proxy for social adaptation (4).

Family members are caregivers for many outpatients with schizophrenia (5). The caring experience may bring physical, emotional, and social pressure, which results in a psychological state called caregiver burden (6, 7). Specifically, caregivers of people with schizophrenia tend to face more challenges than those of patients with other disorders. They reported higher levels of subjective care burden (8, 9) and experienced poorer mental and physical health (5, 10). Previous research has focused on factors that exacerbate the care burden (both caregiver-level and patient-level factors) and the impact of the care burden on the caregivers (5, 6, 1013). However, the caregiver burden could also be a predictor of the patient’s recovery, considering that family is a system that affects every member living in it. A few studies have attempted to explore the relationship between caregiver burden and recovery in schizophrenia. Levene et al. (14) found that the care burden reported by caregivers at discharge could positively predict the patient’s symptoms after 9 months. Nuttall et al. (15) found a covariation in change over time in the caregiver burden and QoL. These pieces of evidence suggested that caregiver burden may be one of the essential family factors affecting the QoL of schizophrenia patients.

Care burden may affect patients’ QoL through family interaction, and expressed emotion may play an important role. Expressed emotion refers to the attitudes and emotions of relatives toward mentally ill family members (1618). It is comprised of three components: emotional over-involvement (EOI), criticism, and hostility. Expressed emotion is a robust predictor of schizophrenia relapse. Specifically, patients who live in a high expressed emotion family had higher rates of relapse. This conclusion has been supported by evidence from many empirical studies and meta-analyses (1922). A number of studies had also found that care burden was significantly positively correlated with expressed emotion, and caregivers with high expressed emotion reported higher care burden scores (2327).

As a member of the family system, people with schizophrenia could also feel and perceive the expressed emotion of caregivers during interactions with them. Patients are not simply passive recipients of other peoples’ behavior (28). According to Lazarus’ stress influence model, individuals’ appraisals mediate the relationship between stress and the outcome of stress (29). Therefore, it is also necessary to include patients’ perceptions of expressed emotion in research. On the one hand, previous studies have found that patients’ perceptions of expressed emotion were significantly related to their recovery. For example, caregivers’ criticism reported by first-episode schizophrenia adolescents negatively predicted their QoL (30). Patients who rated their caregivers to be low in care (i.e., indifferent or rejecting) or high in protection (i.e., controlling or intrusive) had more severe symptoms compared with those who reported their caregivers as high in care and low in protection (31). Such a relationship was also found between patient relapse and expressed emotion of institutional caregivers (32). On the other hand, in some comparative studies, patients’ perceived expressed emotion was a better predictor of relapse than relatives’ expressed emotion. Lebell et al. (33) found that patients’ feelings toward their family members and their perceptions of relatives’ attitudes toward them were significantly related to patients’ relapse during a 1-year-follow up, whereas caregiver’s self-reported attitudes did not predict the outcomes. Compared to caregivers’ expressed emotions, patients’ perceptions of criticism and EOI show a more robust predictive effect to relapse in some studies (3436). In a word, the patients’ perception of expressed emotion should not be overlooked in exploring the relationship between care burden and patients’ QoL.

A meta-analysis revealed that people with schizophrenia in China had a significantly poorer QoL than healthy controls; factors associated with their poorer QoL include diagnostic criteria, study location, female gender, older age, and inpatient status (37). However, these factors focus more on the patient’s situation and lack the family system’s perspective. In China, most people with schizophrenia live with their families (38), and their QoL may also be associated with caregivers. On the other hand, Confucian values require people to be responsible to their families, and the burden of caregivers in Chinese schizophrenia families may be even heavier (39). Caregivers who perceive more burden may show higher levels of expressed emotion (40). Several studies found that expressed emotion was related to relapse in Chinese samples of people (4143). Given the family system and expectation on caregivers of people with schizophrenia in China, we believe it is essential to explore the relationship between Chinese caregiver burden and patients’ QoL from the perspective of family interaction.

This study aimed to investigate the mechanism between caregiver burden and patients’ QoL by examining the sequential mediating effect of caregiver expressed emotion and patients’ perception of expressed emotion in the Chinese context. In summary, the hypotheses of this study were as follows:

Hypothesis 1: Caregiver burden can negatively predict patients’ QoL.

Hypothesis 2: Caregiver burden can positively predict caregivers’ expressed emotion.

Hypothesis 3: Caregivers’ expressed emotion can positively predict patients’ perception of expressed emotion.

Hypothesis 4: Patients’ perception of expressed emotion can negatively predict patients’ QoL.

Hypothesis 5: Caregivers’ expressed emotion and patients’ perception of expressed emotion play sequential mediating effects between caregiver burden and patients’ QoL.

Materials and methods

Procedure and participants

Participants were recruited from Changning district in Shanghai, China. The inclusion criteria of patients were as follows: (1) were 18 years and older; (2) diagnosed with schizophrenia at a local mental health center according to Chinese Classification of Mental Disorder-3 (CCMD-3), which was edited by the Chinese Psychiatric Association according to the clinical description and diagnostic criteria of ICD-10 (44); (3) living with family members; (4) were in good condition and had sufficient capacity to participate in the study according to the evaluation of a qualified psychiatrist. The inclusion criteria of caregivers were as follows: (1) living with the patients; (2) being the primary caregivers of patients.

The research assistant would visit the family of schizophrenics in the company of community doctors (the staff at the mental health center) when the doctors went to the family to do a routine check. The research assistant briefly introduced the study to those patients and their family members after the community doctor finished the visit. If the family was interested in participating in the study, the research assistant would explain the purpose and procedure of the study, data confidentiality and security, participants’ rights, and incentives for the study. If more than one caregiver was interested in participating in the study, we would invite the primary caregiver (who is responsible for taking care of the health of the patient) to fill in the questionnaire. Then the research assistant conducted informed consent and assessment with both participants (patient and caregiver). All participants signed a paper version of the informed consent document. All the questionnaires were administered at a single time point. The family would get a gift valued at 50 yuan (the unit of currency in China) as a reward for participation. The study was approved by the authors’ university committee on human research protection as well as the mental health hospital’s ethics committee.

A total of 219 families were approached by the researcher and eventually 135 families’ data (one patient and one caregiver) were included in the analysis. The process of participant selection is summarized in Figure 1. The demographic information of patients and their caregivers are shown in Table 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of the participants selection.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic information of patients (N = 135) and their caregivers (N = 135).

Measurement

Caregiver burden

Caregiver burden was assessed using the Zarit Burden Interview Chinese version (45). Twenty-two items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 4 (always), and higher scores indicated a higher level of burden. The Cronbach’s alpha was set at 0.95 in the present study.

Expressed emotion

Caregivers’ expressed emotion was measured by the Family Questionnaire (FQ) (46). This questionnaire has shown good psychometric properties in different cultures (4750). In this study, we translated the questionnaire into Chinese following the back-translation procedure. Translation and back translation procedure was applied to translate the FQ into Chinese. To start, two bilingual psychologists with advanced knowledge of the English language and Chinese as their native language independently translated the questionnaire into Chinese (forward translation). Then, a reconciliation meeting was conducted to develop a consensus version (reconciliated Chinese version) with the help of a third reviewer. After that, two psychologists who were blinded to the original version translated the reconciliated Chinese version back into English (backward translation). A third reviewer compared the backward translation and the original English version and determined there were no significant discrepancies between the two versions. The questionnaire consists of two subscales: EOI (10 items) and criticism (10 items). All items were rated on a 4-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The specific items are presented in Supplementary material. Higher scores indicated higher expressed emotion. The Cronbach’s alphas of EOI and criticism were 0.90 and 0.87, respectively. We also performed the confirmatory factor analysis to test the validity of this Chinese version. The model provided a good fit to the data, χ2/df = 1.63, RMSEA = 0.07, CFI = 0.92, TLI = 0.91, SRMR = 0.07, and the factorial loading of all items was significant (p < 0.001).

Perceived expressed emotion

We measured patients’ perception of expressed emotion by adapting the FQ. In order to reduce the burden on the patients who participated in the study, we shortened our questionnaire. We selected eight items from the initial twenty items of the FQ based on factor loading while taking into consideration language variety. Specifically, we analyzed the factor loading results of the two subscales in Wiedemann et al. (46). For the subscale for EOI, we selected items based on the rank of factor loadings, with the highest factor loading item selected first. If one item has a similar meaning to the last item in the ranking of factor loadings, we would skip this item and chose another item. For example, the item “He/she irritates me.” (With the second highest loadings among the items of criticism) was literally similar to the item “I’m often angry with him/her” (the item with top loading factor). Thus “He/she irritates me” was not chosen. The item selection procedure for the criticism subscale followed a similar procedure. After that, we adapted the items into a patient report version. For example, the item “I’m often angry with him/her” was adapted into “He/she is often angry with me.” The specific items are presented in Supplementary material. These eight items were also rated on a 4-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicated a higher perceived level of expressed emotion. The Cronbach’s alphas of perceived EOI and perceived criticism were both 0.83. The result of confirmatory factor analysis showed that the model provided an acceptable fit to the data, χ2/df = 2.98, RMSEA = 0.12, CFI = 0.92, TLI = 0.88, SRMR = 0.06, and the factorial loading of all items was significant (p < 0.001).

Quality of life

QoL was measured using three subscales of the World Health Organization Quality of Life (WHOQOL-BREF) questionnaire Chinese version (51). This measurement has four subscales: physical domain, psychological domain, social relationships, and environmental domain. We selected items from the physical, psychological, and social relationship domains, to reduce the response burden for patients (52). The environmental domain was excluded because it is more affected by the public environment than the family system. We also removed an item (Are you satisfied with your sex life?) that measured social relationships. This item is difficult to answer for the patients because most people with schizophrenia are unmarried or single. In the current study, the Cronbach’s alphas of the physical, psychological, and social relationship domain were 0.77, 0.78, and 0.86.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed using SPSS 23.0 and Mplus 7.0. The code for Mplus is presented in Supplementary material. The rate of missing data for the demographic information ranged from 0.74 to 4.44% among patients and from 1.48 to 4.44% among caregivers. The rate of missing data was 1.48% for care burden and 0.74% for the social relationship domain of QoL. There were no missing data for the other main study variables. Full information maximum likelihood (FIML) was used to handle missing data in the analyses. Analyses were performed in three steps. First, we calculated the mean and standard deviation of all variables and the Pearson correlation between variables. Second, we examined the direct effect of caregiver burden on patients’ QoL. Third, we examined the sequential mediating effects of caregivers’ expressed emotion and patients’ perceptions of expressed emotion between burden and patients’ QoL.

Results

Demographic information is presented in Table 1. The average age of the patients was 42.78 ± 8.06 years, 50.37% were male, the average years of illness was 19.42 ± 8.62 years and number of hospitalizations was 1.61 ± 2.10. The average age of the caregivers was 66.28 ± 11.19 years, 51.85% were male and 78.52% were the parents of the patients.

Table 2 displays the means and standardized deviation of all variables and the Pearson relation between all variables. All correlations are statistically significant. We used Mplus to analyze the sequential mediating effects of caregivers’ expressed emotion and patients’ perceptions of expressed emotion. All variables and outcomes were standardized. First, we examined the prediction of caregivers’ expressed emotion to patients’ QoL with patients’ sex, age, educational level, employment status, and medication-taking as covariates (these covariates were significantly related to patients’ QoL, the estimates for coefficients of covariates are presented in Supplementary material). Supporting hypothesis 1, we found that caregiver burden could significantly negatively predict QoL of patients, including: physical domain [β = −0.39, p < 0.001, 95% CI = (−0.543, −0.239)], psychological domain [β = −0.35, p < 0.001, 95% CI = (−0.506, −0.190)], and social relationships domain [β = −0.18, p = 0.046, 95% CI = (−0.357, −0.004)].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations (N = 135).

Second, we examined the sequential mediating effect of caregivers’ EOI and patients’ perception of EOI. We fitted a model where care burden served as the independent variable, caregivers’ EOI and patients’ perception of EOI as mediators, and the three subscales of QoL as dependent variables. The patients’ sex, age, educational level, employment status, and medication-taking were control variables. The results are shown in Figure 2. The model provided a good fit to the data, χ2/df = 1.35, RMSEA = 0.05, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.96, SRMR = 0.04. Caregiver burden significantly positively predicted caregivers’ EOI, supporting hypothesis 2. Caregivers’ EOI significantly positively predicted patients’ perception of EOI, supporting hypothesis 3. Patients’ perception of EOI significantly negatively predicted the psychical and psychological domain of QoL. However, the path between patients’ perception of EOI and social relationships was not significant. Hypothesis 4 was only partially supported. We also tested the significant levels of indirect effects in the model. As shown in Table 3, the sequential mediating effects of caregivers’ EOI and patients’ perception of EOI on the relationship between care burden and patients’ physical and psychological QoL were significant. However, the sequential mediating effect on the relationship between care burden and social relationships domain was not significant. Therefore, hypothesis 5 was only partially supported.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The mediating model of EOI and PEOI between CB and QoL (PHY, PSY, and SOC). CB, care burden; EOI, caregivers’ emotional over-involvement; PEOI, patients’ perceived emotional over-involvement; PSY, psychological domain of patients’ QoL; SOC, social relationships domain of patients’ QoL. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Standardized mediating path analysis.

Third, we examined the sequential mediating effect of caregivers’ criticism and patients’ perception of criticism. We fitted a model where care burden served as the independent variable, caregivers’ criticism and patients’ perception of criticism were mediators, and the three subscales of QoL were dependent variables. The patients’ sex, age, educational level, employment status, and medication-taking were control variables. The results are shown in Figure 3. The model provided a good fit to the data, χ2/df = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.00, CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00, SRMR = 0.03. Caregiver burden significantly positively predicted caregivers’ criticism, supporting hypothesis 2. Caregivers’ criticism significantly positively predicted patients’ perception of criticism, supporting hypothesis 3. Patients’ perception of criticism did not significantly predict the three domains of QoL. The direct and indirect effects of all paths are shown in Table 3. The results indicated that the sequential mediating effects of caregivers’ criticism and patients’ perception of criticism on the relationship between care burden and patients’ QoL were non-significant.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The mediating model of CC and PCC between CB and QoL (PHY, PSY, and SOC). CB, care burden; CC, caregivers’ criticism; PCC, patients’ perceived criticism; PHY, physical domain of patients’ QoL; PSY, psychological domain of patients’ QoL; SOC, social relationships domain of patients’ QoL. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

The current study aimed to explore the relationship between care burden and the QoL of people with schizophrenia and the family interaction process. Specifically, we examined the sequential mediating effects of caregivers’ expressed emotion (EOI and criticism) and patients’ perception of expressed emotion (perceived EOI and perceived criticism) on the relationship between care burden and patients’ QoL (physiological domain, psychological domain, and social relationships domain). Hypothesis 1 was supported by our results. Care burden significantly negatively predicted the physical, psychological, and social relationships domain of patients’ QoL. Hypothesis 2 was supported. Care burden significantly positively predicted the EOI and criticism of caregivers. Hypothesis 3 was supported. The EOI and criticism of caregivers significantly positively predicted patients’ perceptions of EOI and criticism. Hypothesis 4 was partially supported. The patients perceived emotional involvement significantly negatively predicted their physical and psychological QoL, but not the social relationships domain. The predictive effect of patients’ perceived criticism on QoL was non-significant. Hypothesis 5 was partially supported. The sequential mediating effects of caregivers’ EOI and patients’ perception of EOI on the relationship between care burden and patients’ physical and psychological QoL were significant. However, the sequential mediating effects of caregivers’ criticism and patients’ perception of criticism were non-significant.

The regression analysis results showed that the care burden significantly negatively predicted the patients’ QoL. It was consistent with the results of previous studies (15, 53). A study in Chinese families of people with schizophrenia also found that higher caregiving burden was associated with poorer family functioning (54), which means it would be hard for caregivers to give a patient enough social support and care. Caregivers may also be unable to support the patient to go out and return to society, which leads to a poorer QoL for patients. This was also found in an empirical study with a Chinese sample (55).

The path analysis of the mediating model showed that care burden significantly positively predicted expressed emotion and expressed emotion also positively predicted patients’ perceptions of expressed emotion. Previous studies have found that caregivers who reported high care burden also show a high level of expressed emotion (2326). This is consistent with the cross-sectional data results in our study. A longitudinally designed study also found that change in burden is associated with the change in EE among relatives of people with schizophrenia (56). The direction of effect in the association of burden and EE could not be determined from our study or existing studies. A possible explanation is that caregivers of individuals with schizophrenia often feel overwhelmed, stressed, drained, burdened, frustrated, or angry (5). A meta-analysis showed that feelings and emotions are at the core of the caregiver’s caring experience (57). Under these circumstances, coping skills to help manage pressure and negative feelings would be important for caregivers to improve their interaction with the patient. However, those who reported high subjective care burden may lack coping skills brought on by the caring role (58). They may therefore experience worry and frustration with the patient and then express these emotions to the patient.

Although some studies indicated that schizophrenia patients have difficulties in recognizing others’ emotions (59, 60), research on expressed emotion showed that people with schizophrenia can still detect and perceive emotions and attitudes of close family members (24, 61). Therefore, it is reasonable that expressed emotion of family members positively predicted patients’ perceived expressed emotion in this study.

The results of our study found that EOI and patients’ perceived EOI served as sequential mediators on the relationship between care burden and patients’ QoL. However, the other aspect of expressed emotion, criticism, did not show such effects. This difference might result from patients’ different appraisals toward caregivers’ EOI and criticism in Chinese culture. Expressed emotion is related to relapse of schizophrenia because high expressed emotion in the family environment is a kind of stress for patients (21). In our study, nearly 80% of the caregivers were parents. Asian parents tend to show more control and critical attitudes when they raise their children; they also tend to express their dissatisfaction with children due to high expectations for them (62, 63). People with schizophrenia in China may get used to parents’ criticism and not appraise it as pressure. Thus, in our study, patients’ perception of criticism did not significantly predict their QoL. However, the mediating effects of EOI and patients’ perceived EOI were significant. The impact of EOI may be due to its impairment on patients’ agency. Caregivers high in EOI tend to perceive the patient as less capable of participating and completing tasks associated with the recovery process (64). This may lead the patients to lose confidence in getting back to society and reduce participation in activities that help improve their QoL. Some studies found that expressed emotion did not significantly predict schizophrenia patients’ relapse when caregivers’ perception of patients’ agency was controlled (65). Cross-cultural studies of expressed emotion also found differences in the predictive effects of EOI and criticism on schizophrenia relapse (66, 67).

Strengths and limitations

This study also had several limitations. First, it was not possible to infer causality due to the cross-sectional nature of the study. Longitudinal design could be more helpful to explore the causal relationship between care burden and QoL of people with schizophrenia. Second, the sample in our study consisted of patients with different ages and years of illness duration. The average age was 43. The finding from the current study may not translate to other samples, such as first-episode schizophrenia or early-phase schizophrenia. Third, although we have controlled for multiple covariates in the mediation analysis, unmeasured confounders may also affect the mediation effects of expressed emotion and perceived expressed emotion. For example, some individual traits related to coping, such as resilience, might confound patients’ perceived EE and QoL. The illness belief about schizophrenia (whether it could be controlled or not) might be a confounder of caregivers’ burden and expressed emotion. Future studies could explore the mediating effects of EE and patients’ perceived EE by measuring and controlling known confounders.

Despite limitations in this study, our study has several strengths. To our knowledge, the current study is the first to explore the relationship between care burden and patients’ QoL by examining the sequential mediating effects of expressed emotion and perceived expressed emotion. By including the measure of patients’ perceptions of expressed emotion, the current study provided insight into the family interaction’s impact on the QoL of schizophrenia patients. The present study also implied that the two aspects of expressed emotion (i.e., EOI and criticism) might have different effects on patients’ QoL, which expands our understanding of expressed emotion and enriches relevant empirical evidence. Future studies can further explore whether schizophrenia patients’ appraisal of EOI and criticism is different in a Chinese cultural context. In addition, whether different roles of caregivers may moderate the impact of care burden on patients’ QoL via expressed emotion could also be explored in future studies, given that a caregiving experience of a parent might be different from that of a spouse. The results of our study also provide some implications for improving the QoL of patients with schizophrenia. Psychoeducation targeted for caregivers could be helpful to reduce their level of expressed emotion, which may enhance patients’ QoL. Additional support systems and resources (such as social workers or professional caregivers) may aid Chinese families in reducing care burden and improve QoL for patients with schizophrenia.

Conclusion

In summary, our results examined the sequential mediating effects of expressed emotion and patients’ perception of expressed emotion on the relationship between care burden and the QoL of people with schizophrenia. Care burden was negatively related to patients’ QoL, including the physical, psychological, and social relationships domain. The EOI of caregivers and patients’ perception of EOI served as mediators between care burden and patients’ QoL. However, criticism of caregivers and patients’ perception of criticism did not mediate this effect.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the East China Normal University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

YW and YP analyzed and interpreted the data and drafted the manuscript. YL, LS, and KJ contacted participants and collected data. JX designed the study and revised the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Social Science Foundation of China (18BSH129), the Shanghai Philosophy and Social Science Planning Project (2017BSH004), the Research Project of Shanghai Science and Technology Commission (20DZ2260300), and the Science and Technology Innovation Action Plan of Shanghai Science and Technology Commission (20DZ2304400).

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge all the participants with schizophrenia and their families in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.961691/full#supplementary-material

References

1. GBD Collaborators. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990-2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet. (2018) 392:1789–858. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32279-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. National Health Commission of the People’s Republic of China. National Mental Health Work Progress. (2017). Available online at: http://www.nhc.gov.cn/xcs/s3574/201704/20cf8768e05445a387a1c4bf1be9ec6d.shtml (accessed June 4, 2022).

Google Scholar

3. Insel TR. Rethinking schizophrenia. Nature. (2010) 468:187–93. doi: 10.1038/nature09552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Whoqol Group. The World Health Organization quality of life assessment (WHOQOL): position paper from the World Health Organization. Soc Sci Med. (1995) 41:1403–9. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(95)00112-k

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kamil SH, Velligan DI. Caregivers of individuals with schizophrenia: who are they and what are their challenges? Curr Opin Psychiatr. (2019) 32:157–63. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Caqueo-Urízar A, Gutiérrez-Maldonado J. Burden of care in families of patients with schizophrenia. Qual Life Res. (2006) 15:719–24. doi: 10.1007/s11136-005-4629-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Dillehay RC, Sandys MR. Caregivers for Alzheimer’s patients: what we are learning from research. Int J Aging Hum Dev. (1990) 30:263–85. doi: 10.2190/2P3J-A9AH-HHF4-00RG

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Magliano L, Fiorillo A, De Rosa C, Malangone C, Maj M. Family burden in long-term diseases: a comparative study in schizophrenia vs physical disorders. Soc Sci Med. (2005) 61:313–22. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Möller-Leimkühler AM. Burden of relatives and predictors of burden. Baseline results from the Munich 5–year–follow–up study on relatives of first hospitalized patients with schizophrenia or depression. Eur Arch Psych Clin Neurosci. (2005) 255:223–31. doi: 10.1007/s00406-004-0550-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Gupta S, Isherwood G, Jones K, Van Impe K. Assessing health status in informal schizophrenia caregivers compared with health status in non-caregivers and caregivers of other conditions. BMC Psychiatry. (2015) 15:1–162. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0547-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Fadden G, Bebbington P, Kuipers L. The burden of care: the impact of functional psychiatric illness on the patient’s family. Br J Psychiatry. (1987) 150:285–92. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.3.285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Mantovani LM, Ferretjans R, Marçal IM, Oliveira AM, Guimarães FC, Salgado JV. Family burden in schizophrenia: the influence of age of onset and negative symptoms. Trends Psychiatr Psychother. (2016) 38:96–9. doi: 10.1590/2237-6089-2015-0082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Yamazaki S, Sokejima S, Mizoue T, Eboshida A, Fukuhara S. Health-related quality of life of mothers of children with leukemia in Japan. Qual Life Res. (2005) 14:1079–85. doi: 10.1007/s11136-004-3288-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Levene JE, Lancee W, Seeman MV, Skinner H, Freeman SJJ. Family and patient predictors of symptomatic status in schizophrenia. Can J Psychiatry. (2009) 54:446–51. doi: 10.1177/070674370905400705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Nuttall AK, Thakkar KN, Luo X, Mueser KT, Glynn SM, Achtyes ED, et al. Longitudinal associations of family burden and patient quality of life in the context of first-episode schizophrenia in the RAISE-ETP study. Psychiatry Res. (2019) 276:60–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Brown GW, Birley JL, Wing JK. Influence of family life on the course of schizophrenic disorders: a replication. Br J Psychiatry. (1972) 121:241–58. doi: 10.1192/bjp.121.3.241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Brown GW, Carstairs GM, Topping G. Post-hospital adjustment of chronic mental patients. Lancet. (1958) 2:685–8. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(58)92279-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Brown GW, Rutter M. The measurement of family activities and relationships: a methodological study. Hum Relat. (1966) 19:241–63. doi: 10.1177/001872676601900301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Butzlaff RL, Hooley JM. Expressed emotion and psychiatric relapse. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1998) 55:547–52. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.6.547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Cechnicki A, Bielańska A, Hanuszkiewicz I, Daren A. The predictive validity of expressed emotions (EE) in schizophrenia A 20-year prospective study. J Psychiatr Res. (2013) 47:208–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hooley JM. Expressed emotion and relapse of psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2007) 3:329–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Weintraub MJ, Hall DL, Carbonella JY, Weisman de Mamani A, Hooley JM. Integrity of literature on expressed emotion and relapse in patients with schizophrenia verified by a p-curve analysis. Fam Process. (2017) 56:436–44. doi: 10.1111/famp.12208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Carrà G, Cazzullo CL, Clerici M. The association between expressed emotion, illness severity and subjective burden of care in relatives of patients with schizophrenia Findings from an Italian population. BMC Psychiatry. (2012) 12:140. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Möller-Leimkühler AM, Jandl M. Expressed and perceived emotion over time: does the patients’ view matter for the caregivers’ burden? Eur Arch Psych Clin Neurosci. (2011) 261:349–55. doi: 10.1007/s00406-010-0178-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Möller-Leimkühler AM, Obermeier M. Predicting caregiver burden in first admission psychiatric patients. Eur Arch Psych Clin Neurosci. (2008) 258:406–13. doi: 10.1007/s00406-008-0818-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Scazufca M, Kuipers E. Links between expressed emotion and burden of care in relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. (1996) 168:580–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.168.5.580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wang X, Chen Q, Yang M. Effect of caregivers’ expressed emotion on the care burden and rehospitalization rate of schizophrenia. Patient Prefer Adherence. (2017) 11:1505–11. doi: 10.2147/PPA.S143873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Miklowitz DJ, Goldstein MJ, Doane JA, Nuechterlein KH, Strachan AM, Snyder KS, et al. Is expressed emotion an index of a transactional process? I Parents’ affective style. Fam Process. (1989) 28:153–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.1989.00153.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lazarus RS. From psychological stress to the emotions: a history of changing outlooks. Annu Rev Psychol. (1993) 44:1. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.000245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. von Polier GG, Meng H, Lambert M, Strauss M, Zarotti G, Karle M, et al. Patterns and correlates of expressed emotion, perceived criticism, and rearing style in first admitted early-onset schizophrenia spectrum disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2014) 202:783–7. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Warner R, Atkinson M. The relationship between schizophrenic patients’ perceptions of their parents and the course of their illness. Br J Psychiatry. (1988) 153:344–53. doi: 10.1192/bjp.153.3.344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Fan BWS. Measuring perceived expressed emotion of elderly with schizophrenia. Int J Ment Health. (2014) 43:3–16. doi: 10.2753/IMH0020-7411430201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Lebell MB, Marder SR, Mintz J, Mintz LI, Tompson M, Wirshing W, et al. Patients’ perceptions of family emotional climate and outcome in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. (1993) 162:751–4. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.6.751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Hooley JM, Teasdale JD. Predictors of relapse in unipolar depressives: expressed emotion, marital distress, and perceived criticism. J Abnorm Psychol. (1989) 98:229–35. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.98.3.229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Scott RD, Fagin L, Winter D. The importance of the role of the patient in the outcome of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. (1993) 163:62–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.163.1.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Tompson MC, Goldstein MJ, Lebell MB, Mintz LI, Marder SR, Mintz J. Schizophrenic patients’ perceptions of their relatives’ attitudes. Psychiatry Res. (1995) 57:155–67. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02598-q

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lu L, Zeng LN, Zong QQ, Rao WW, Ng CH, Ungvari GS, et al. Quality of life in Chinese patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 268:392–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Phillips MR. Characteristics, experience, and treatment of schizophrenia in China. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2001) 3:109–19. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2001.3.2/mrphillips

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Zhou Y, Ning Y, Rosenheck R, Sun B, Zhang J, Ou Y, et al. Effect of living with patients on caregiver burden of individual with schizophrenia in China. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 245:230–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.08.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Nuralita NS, Camellia V, Loebis B. Relationship between caregiver burden and expressed emotion in families of schizophrenic patients. Macedonian J Med Sci. (2019) 7:2583. doi: 10.3889/oamjms.2019.394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. O’Driscoll C, Sener SB, Angmark A, Shaikh M. Caregiving processes and expressed emotion in psychosis, a cross-cultural, meta-analytic review. Schizophr Res. (2019) 208:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Phillips MR, Xiong W. Expressed emotion in mainland China: Chinese families with schizophrenic patients. Int J Ment Health. (1995) 24:54–75. doi: 10.1080/00207411.1995.11449319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Yang LH, Phillips MR, Licht DM, Hooley JM. Causal attributions about schizophrenia in families in China: expressed emotion and patient relapse. J Abnorm Psychol. (2004) 113:592. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.4.592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Chen YF. Chinese classification of mental disorders (CCMD-3): towards integration in international classification. Psychopathology. (2002) 35:171–5. doi: 10.1159/000065140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Wang L, Yang X, Hou Z, Feng Q, Wan J. Application and evaluation of the Chinese version of the caregiver burden scale. Chin J Public Health. (2006) 22: 970–2.

Google Scholar

46. Wiedemann G, Rayki O, Feinstein E, Hahlweg K. The family questionnaire: development and validation of a new self-report scale for assessing expressed emotion. Psychiatry Res. (2002) 109:265–80. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00023-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Koutra K, Economou M, Triliva S, Roumeliotaki T, Lionis C, Vgontzas AN. Cross-cultural adaptation and validation of the Greek version of the Family Questionnaire for assessing expressed emotion. Compr Psychiat. (2014) 55:1038–49. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ponti L, Stefanini MC, Troiani MR, Tani F. A study on the Italian validation of the family questionnaire. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 270:1116–21. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Sepúlveda AR, Anastasiadou D, Rodríguez L, Almendros C, Andrés P, Vaz F, et al. Spanish validation of the Family Questionnaire (FQ) in families of patients with an eating disorder. Psicothema. (2014) 26:321–7. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Zanetti ACG, Ciol MA, Wiedemann G, de Azevedo-Marques JM, Dantas RAS, da Silva Gherardi-Donato EC, et al. Predictive validity of the Brazilian Portuguese version of the family questionnaire for relapse in patients with schizophrenia. J Nurs Meas. (2017) 25:559–72. doi: 10.1891/1061-3749.25.3.559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Zhang M, He Y. Quality of life scale. In: H Zou editor. Handbook of Psychiatric Scales. Chang Sha: Hunan Science and Technology Press (2015). p. 352–3.

Google Scholar

52. Zhong X, Li M, Li L. Preventing and detecting insufficient effort survey responding. Adv Psychol Sci. (2021) 29:225–37.

Google Scholar

53. Perlick DA, Rosenheck RA, Kaczynski R, Swartz MS, Cañive JM, Lieberman JA. Components and correlates of family burden in schizophrenia. Psychiatr Serv. (2006) 57:1117–25. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.57.8.1117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Chien WT, Chan SW, Morrissey J. The perceived burden among Chinese family caregivers of people with schizophrenia. J Clin Nurs. (2007) 16:1151–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2007.01501.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Xiang YT, Hou YZ, Yan F, Dixon LB, Ungvari GS, Dickerson F, et al. Quality of life in community-dwelling patients with schizophrenia in China. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2012) 200:584–7. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31825bfc71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Scazufca M, Kuipers E. Stability of expressed emotion in relatives of those with schizophrenia and its relationship with burden of care and perception of patients’ social functioning. Psychol. Med. (1998) 28:453–61. doi: 10.1017/S0033291797005977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Cleary M, West S, Hunt GE, McLean L, Kornhaber R. A qualitative systematic review of caregivers’ experiences of caring for family diagnosed with schizophrenia. Issues Ment Health Nurs. (2020) 41:667–83. doi: 10.1080/01612840.2019.1710012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Geriani D, Savithry KSB, Shivakumar S, Kanchan T. Burden of care on caregivers of schizophrenia patients: a correlation to personality and coping. J Clin Diagn Res. (2015) 9:VC01–04. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2015/11342.5654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Bell M, Bryson G, Lysaker P. Positive and negative affect recognition in schizophrenia: a comparison with substance abuse and normal control subjects. Psychiatry Res. (1997) 73:73–82. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(97)00111-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Watson D. Positive mood dysfunction in psychopathology: a structural perspective. In: Wood AM, Johnson J editors. The Wiley Handbook of Positive Clinical Psychology. New Jersey: Wiley Blackwell (2016). p. 59–71. doi: 10.1002/9781118468197.ch5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Cutting LP, Aakre JM, Docherty NM. Schizophrenic patients’ perceptions of stress, expressed emotion, and sensitivity to criticism. Schizophr Bull. (2006) 32:743–50. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Lau AS, Fung J. On better footing to understand parenting and family process in Asian American families. Asian Am J Psychol. (2013) 4:71–5. doi: 10.1037/a0032120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Lui PP, Rollock D. Tiger mother: popular and psychological scientific perspectives on Asian culture and parenting. Am J Orthopsychiatr. (2013) 83:450–6. doi: 10.1111/ajop.12043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Breitborde NJK, López SR, Aguilera A, Kopelowicz A. Perceptions of efficacy, expressed emotion, and the course of schizophrenia: the case of emotional overinvolvement. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2013) 201:833–40. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182a5bf1d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Breitborde NJK, López SR, Nuechterlein KH. Expressed emotion, human agency, and schizophrenia: toward a new model for the ee-relapse association. Cult Med Psychiatr. (2009) 33:41–60. doi: 10.1007/s11013-008-9119-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Hashemi AH, Cochrane R. Expressed emotion and schizophrenia: a review of studies across cultures. Int Rev Psych. (1999) 11:219–24. doi: 10.1080/09540269974401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Singh SP, Harley K, Suhail K. Cultural specificity of emotional overinvolvement: a systematic review. Schizophr Bull. (2013) 39:449–63. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizophrenia, care burden, quality of life, expressed emotion, family wellbeing

Citation: Wei Y, Peng Y, Li Y, Song L, Ju K and Xi J (2022) Caregivers’ burden and schizophrenia patients’ quality of life: Sequential mediating effects of expressed emotion and perceived expressed emotion. Front. Psychiatry 13:961691. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.961691

Received: 05 June 2022; Accepted: 01 August 2022;
Published: 25 August 2022.

Edited by:

Eric D. Achtyes, Michigan State University College of Human Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Katharine Thakkar, Michigan State University, United States
Zhehui Luo, Michigan State University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Wei, Peng, Li, Song, Ju and Xi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Juzhe Xi, jzxi@psy.ecnu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.