Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 29 September 2022
Sec. Adolescent and Young Adult Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Suicide and Self Harm in Young People View all 18 articles

The relationship between daily positive future thinking and past-week suicidal ideation in youth: An experience sampling study

  • 1Department of Neurosciences, Center for Contextual Psychiatry, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 2Leuven Brain Institute, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 3Leuven Child and Youth Institute, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 4Research Group of Quantitative Psychology and Individual Differences, Faculty of Psychology, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 5Department of Psychology, Health, and Technology, Center for eHealth and Well-being Research, University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
  • 6Psychiatric Care Sint-Kamillus, Bierbeek, Belgium
  • 7Department of Human Genetics, University Hospital Gasthuisberg, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 8Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Ghent University Hospital, Ghent, Belgium
  • 9Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Medical Center, Maastricht, Netherlands
  • 10Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 11University Psychiatric Centre, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Kortenberg, Belgium
  • 12Department of Neurosciences, Centre for Clinical Psychiatry, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 13Antwerp Health Law and Ethics Chair, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
  • 14Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, South Limburg Mental Health and Teaching Network, EURON, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
  • 15Faculty of Psychology, Open University of the Netherlands, Heerlen, Netherlands
  • 16Department of Neurology, Ghent University Hospital, Ghent, Belgium
  • 17Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Centre Utrecht, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 18Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s Health Partners, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 19Interdisciplinary Center Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

Reduced positive future thinking has been associated with suicidal ideation and behavior in adults, and appears to be exacerbated by negative affect. Yet, this has received little attention in youth. Prior research has also focused on longer-term future thinking, e.g., months and years, and relied on lab-based assessments. Using the experience sampling method (ESM), we investigated whether short-term future thinking in daily life was associated with suicidal ideation in youth and explored the role of affect in the future thinking–suicidal ideation relationship. A community sample of N = 722 adolescent twins and their non-twin siblings completed ESM as part of the TwinssCan study (n = 55 with, and n = 667 without, past-week suicidal ideation). Participants completed self-report questionnaires, including on past-week suicidal ideation as part of the SCL-90. Subsequently, daily future thinking was assessed each morning for six days with ESM. To investigate the relationship between daily positive future thinking and past-week suicidal ideation, we estimated a mixed-effects linear regression model with a random intercept for participant, including age and sex as covariates. The relationship between daily positive future thinking, past-week suicidal ideation, and average positive and negative affect from the previous day was investigated by estimating two separate mixed-effects linear regression models (one for negative affect, one for positive affect), with a random intercept for participant, and random slopes for average positive and negative affect. Our results showed that participants reporting higher past-week suicidal ideation also reported significantly less daily positive future thinking during the ESM period, and this association remained significant when controlling for previous-day average positive and negative affect. Higher average positive affect from the previous day was significantly associated with higher positive future thinking. Although average negative affect from the previous day was associated with lower positive future thinking, this association was not statistically significant. Our findings indicate that short-term future thinking relates to suicidal ideation among a non-clinical sample of adolescents. Future research should investigate the directionality of the future thinking–suicidal ideation relationship, in order to investigate whether impaired future thinking may be an early warning signal for escalating suicidal ideation in youth.

Introduction

Being unable to anticipate positive future experiences has been consistently associated with self-harm ideation and behaviors, as have a range of other future oriented constructs (17). Previous research has found that individuals who ideate about or engage in self-harm behaviors, generate fewer positive–but not more negative-future thoughts than those without a history of self-harm thoughts or behaviors (811). Whilst this association appears to be robust, there are several critical unknowns regarding the association between positive future thinking and suicidal ideation that must be addressed if research on positive future thinking is to be translated into clinical practice.

The vast majority of previous studies in this area have measured positive future thinking using the Future Thinking Task [FTT; (8)], in which individuals are asked to free-generate responses to questions regarding what they are looking forward to in one week (including today), one year, and five to ten years. Whilst future thinking within these time-windows prospectively predicts suicidal ideation in adults (12), the extent to which more short-term future thinking, e.g., the next day, relates to suicidal ideation is unknown. Additionally, findings from two studies have demonstrated that positive future thinking decreases following a negative mood manipulation (13, 14), suggesting that the ability to anticipate positive future experiences may be a dynamic process that fluctuates with changes in negative affect. Other research with community samples of adults (15), adolescents (16), and adults with panic disorder and depression (17) has found associations between positive, but not negative, affect, and positive future thinking. To our knowledge, the relationship between naturally occurring, i.e., non-induced, positive and negative affect and positive future thinking has not been examined in individuals experiencing suicidal ideation. Furthermore, as all previous research on future thinking and suicidal thoughts and behaviors has been conducted in the laboratory, we do not know whether positive future thinking as it occurs naturally, in individuals’ daily lives, relates similarly to suicidal ideation.

The laboratory-and survey-based nature of the majority of research on suicidal ideation and behavior limits our capacity to achieve a truly fine-grained understanding of the psychological factors that relate to suicidal ideation and behaviors. To address this, we need to employ methodologies capable of capturing dynamic psychological processes. The Experience Sampling Method [ESM; (18, 19)]—also referred to as Ecological Momentary Assessment [EMA; (20)]—offers an invaluable opportunity to gain insights into individuals’ everyday lives by allowing the collection of dynamic data on individuals’ behaviors and experiences (19). Use of ESM to investigate suicidal ideation has increased rapidly (2125), building on seminal work by Nock et al. (26), which used ESM to investigate self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. ESM techniques therefore represent an innovative method of investigating dynamic variations in suicidal ideation and behaviors, as well as the psychosocial and environmental factors associated with these thoughts and behaviors.

Whilst there have been a number of studies investigating the relationship between future thinking, and suicidal thoughts and behaviors in adults (8, 9, 14), there has been only one investigation of this in adolescents (27). Intriguingly and contrary to predictions based on the Integrated Motivational Volitional model of suicide [IMV; (28)], 3-month suicidal ideation was associated with defeat and entrapment when positive future thinking was higher; an effect which seemed to be explained by adolescents reporting higher defeat and entrapment generating more unrealistic positive future thoughts (27). That positive future thinking among suicidal youth has received so little attention is surprising, considering that adolescence is an important developmental period for future thinking abilities (2931). Further, there is a pressing need to better understand factors associated with suicidal ideation and behavior within this age group. In youth, prevalence estimates for suicidal ideation range from 19.8 to 24% (32), and although not all young people who think about suicide will go on to engage in suicidal behavior, among adolescents who experience suicidal ideation, more than one third go on to make a suicide attempt (33). Given that suicide is the fourth leading cause of death worldwide for young people aged 15–29 (34), identification of targets for early intervention in youth—especially for short-term suicide risk—is of critical importance (35). In a recent review of research on youth suicide, Cha et al. (36) further emphasized the need to devote greater attention to the psychological factors associated with suicidal ideation and behaviors in youth in the short-term. Moreover, they also highlighted the overreliance on traditional self-report methods when investigating factors associated with suicidal ideation and behavior in youth and encouraged researchers to “move beyond traditional tools used in psychology research…” [p473; (36)].

In order to address these open questions and issues regarding the relationship between short-term positive future thinking and suicidal ideation in youth, we conducted a study using pre-existing ESM data to investigate whether daily positive future thinking was associated with past-week history of suicidal ideation in youth. We hypothesized that higher past-week suicidal ideation would be significantly and negatively associated with daily positive future thinking. Based on previous research [e.g., (13, 14)] suggesting that affect influences and, in some cases, potentiates a decrease in positive future thinking, we also conducted exploratory analysis to investigate the relationship between past-week suicidal ideation and daily positive future thinking, with and without positive and negative affect.

Materials and methods

Participants and recruitment

Participants were from the TwinssCan study (37), a large general population-based cohort of adolescent twins and their young adult non-twin siblings, which formed part of the East Flanders Prospective Twin Survey [EFPTS; (38)]. Twin participants were aged 15–18 years old at the time of enrollment and their non-twin siblings were aged 15–35 years old. Exclusion criteria were: (1) not understanding the purpose of the study and being unable to provide informed assent/consent; (2) not having parent/caregiver consent (if under 18 years old); (3) presence of pervasive mental disorder; and (4) being unable to complete the study procedure, or providing invalid/unreliable data on measures. The total TwinssCan ESM dataset (version 2.3) sample comprised N = 840 individuals; however, n = 13 participants had missing data in ESM design variables required for multilevel modeling of the data, so were excluded. For the purposes of this study we specifically focused on youth,1 so excluded n = 39 participants older than 25, leaving a sample of 788 participants before additional data cleaning (see Data Cleaning section below for further details). No information regarding participants’ ethnicity was available; however, as a proxy, participants were asked whether they spoke any languages other than Dutch at home. A small number of participants indicated yes (n = 27), the majority indicated no (n = 705), and n = 11 participants’ responses were missing. Participants’ perceived social status was assessed using the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status (39, 40), wherein they were shown an illustration of a ten-rung ladder, with each rung corresponding to individuals’ position within the community. Using a 1–100 visual analogue scale, researchers asked participants to indicate their position on the ladder for the community that was most relevant to them. Within the current study, participants’ mean score on the ladder was 37.09 (SD: 27.57), and the median score was 50. The study received local ethics committee approval (Medical Ethics Committee UZ/KU Leuven, No. B32220107766), and informed consent was obtained from all participants (and parents where participants were aged < 18 years old).

Procedure

The procedure for the TwinssCan study is fully described elsewhere (37). During a baseline interview session with a member of the TwinssCan research team, participants completed a battery of self-report questionnaires, including questions regarding various psychosocial factors and experiences of psychopathology symptoms. At the end of the baseline session, the researcher briefed participants regarding the ESM protocol, where participants were asked to complete a series of brief questionnaires, ten times per day for six days, plus additional shorter morning and evening questionnaires. Future thinking was measured during the morning questionnaire and positive and negative affect were assessed in the momentary questionnaires. All ESM measures were administered using Psy-mate©, a custom-made Personal Digital Assistant (41), which emitted a notification prompting participants to complete a questionnaire. Notifications were given between 7:30 am and 10:30 pm, thus the morning questionnaire was available to participants from 7:30 am.

Measures

Past-week suicidal ideation

Past week suicidal ideation was assessed using a single item from the Dutch version of the SCL-90-R (42) administered at baseline, which asked “[How often in the past week including today have you been troubled by] thoughts of ending your life?” Responses were given on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely).

Future thinking

To assess daily future thinking, we used the Experience Sampling Method (18, 19). In the current study, future thinking was assessed using a single item from the ESM morning questionnaire, which asked participants to indicate “How much you are looking forward to today” on a seven-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), which indicates the amount of variance that is due to between-person differences, was 0.30 for the future thinking ESM item, and the adjusted and conditional ICC values were identical. The morning questionnaire was presented to participants once a day for a period of six days and was the first questionnaire received by participants each day. The morning questionnaire included a total of five items.

Previous day mean positive and negative affect

Between the morning and evening questionnaires, 10 momentary questionnaires were administered consisting of a maximum of 57 items (depending upon answers to conditionally branched questions). Following receipt of the prompt, participants had 15 min to respond to questionnaires. During each of the 10 momentary ESM questionnaires, participants were presented with items assessing positive and negative affect. All affect items began with the stem “I feel…” followed by five items assessing positive affect (cheerful, relaxed, satisfied, enthusiastic, and generally well) and four items assessing negative affect (insecure, lonely, anxious, and annoyed). Responses to all items were provided on a seven-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much so). Separate means were calculated for positive and negative affect at each beep by averaging item scores. These questionnaire-level means were then aggregated within days to create average daily positive and negative affect scores, which were then lagged to provide average positive and daily negative affect scores for the day prior to completion of the morning questionnaire. ICCs for the mean positive and negative affect variables were 0.58 and 0.65, respectively, and values for adjusted and conditional ICCs were identical. Within-and between-person reliability (ω) for positive affect were 0.82 and 0.92, and 0.78 and 0.91 for negative affect.

Data cleaning

Participants who completed fewer than 30% of questionnaires during the ESM period were asked to continue with ESM for longer than the 6-day protocol. Compliance with ESM protocols, defined as the proportion of completed questionnaires out of the total number of questionnaires delivered, is related to a number of different participant characteristics, including presence of psychopathology symptoms (43), therefore to minimize heterogeneity within the sample, we excluded participants with ESM compliance below 30%. This resulted in a sample size of N = 743. Following concerns regarding use of the 30% compliance rule of thumb as a basis for excluding participants (44), we also conducted an exploratory sensitivity analysis in which we re-estimated all models including participants with compliance below 30%. The results of these sensitivity analyses are presented in Supplementary 1 and were virtually identical to the results of the analyses excluding participants with < 30% compliance.

Statistical analysis

Open research practices

The research questions, hypotheses, variables, data inclusion/exclusion criteria, and statistical analysis plan were postregistered on the Open Science Framework,1 a type of preregistration occurring after data collection, but before data analysis (45). Prior to postregistration, data had not been accessed.

Subsequent to registration and data access, a number of issues emerged and consequently, several major aspects of our preregistered plan were changed. These changes are detailed in a supplement to our original registration2 and in more detail in the Supplementary materials (Supplementary 2). Briefly, the major deviation from our original postregistered analysis plan was a change in our independent variable from past-year suicidal ideation and attempts, to past-week suicidal ideation. This was due to unexpected conditional branching within the CIDI questionnaire, which meant that too few individuals had received the questions pertaining to past-year suicidal ideation and behavior for meaningful analysis to be feasible. For transparency, we report the results of these original postregistered analyses in Supplementary 3.

The full ESM questionnaire from the TwinssCan study (37) and R Markdown files for all analyses from the current study are available on the Open Science Framework.3 The Supplementary materials for the current manuscript are also available on the OSF project page for this study.

Analysis plan

The statistical analysis plan presented here is that outlined in the supplementary registration, not the original registration. Data were cleaned and analyzed using R v4.1.2 (46) via R Studio v2022.2.0.443 (47). As observations (level 1) were nested within participants (level 2), mixed effects linear regressions with random intercepts were conducted using the “lmer” function from the “lme4” package (48) for analyses of past week suicidal ideation and daily future thinking. Tables of model summaries were generated with the “SjPlot” package v2.8.10 (49) and plots were generated using “ggeffects” v1.1.1.1 (50) and “cowplot” v1.1.1 (51). Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICCs) and (where possible) reliability for ESM items were calculated using the “psych” package (52) v1.5.8 and the “multilevel tools” package (53) v0.1.1. Appropriateness of multilevel modeling for these data was assessed by estimating an unconditional model, which included only the outcome variable (daily future thinking) and the random intercept (participant). As the unconditional model was statistically significant (p < 0.001), multilevel modeling was considered appropriate for these data (54, 55). For analyses of the relationship between past week suicidal ideation and daily future thinking including affect covariates, two separate mixed effects linear regressions with random intercepts and random slopes were estimated, one including average positive affect and one including average negative affect, from the previous day. In all analyses, daily future thinking was the dependent variable and past-week suicidal ideation was the independent variable. The past-week suicidal ideation variable was grand mean-centered, i.e., centered using the mean calculated across all participants, in order to aid interpretability of the intercept (56). Average lagged positive and negative affect were participant mean-centered. This is recommended for time-varying variables in order to avoid conflation between the association between the dependent variable and variation of the independent variables at the within-and between-person level (56). Given associations between sex, age, and both suicidal ideation (57, 58) and future thinking (31), we included both age and sex as covariates. The covariance structure of the Level 1 within-person errors was assumed to be independent. As stated in our original post-registration, due to the absence of comparable published literature from which to draw parameters for simulation-based power calculations, we did not conduct an a priori or sensitivity power calculation.

Results

Fifty-five (7.4%) participants reported experiencing suicidal ideation within the past week, relative to 677 (89.8%) participants who reported no past-week suicidal ideation. Twenty-one (2.8%) participants were missing responses to the suicidal ideation item. Mean compliance, i.e., mean number of completed ESM questionnaires out of the total 60, within the full sample was 41.72 (SD: 10.49; 69.53% compliance). Neither age (β = 0.00958, SE = 0.015, p = 0.53) nor sex (β = 0.00149, SE = 0.075, p = 0.98) were significantly related to positive future thinking. However, both were related to past-week suicidal ideation: younger age was associated with higher suicidal ideation (β = −0.016, SE = 0.003, p < 0.001) and youth reporting suicidal ideation were more likely to be female (β = 0.046, SE = 0.014, p = 0.00075). For sample and variable descriptive information, see Tables 1, 2 for variable descriptives according to endorsement vs. non-endorsement of past-week suicidal ideation. Results of the analyses investigating the relationship between past-week suicidal ideation and daily future thinking (with and without affect) are reported below. Results of multilevel analyses are presented in Figure 1 and Table 3.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample and variable descriptives.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Variable descriptives for adolescents reporting vs. not reporting past-week suicidal ideation.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The association between daily positive future thinking and past-week suicidal ideation, controlling for (A) age and sex; (B) age, sex, and average positive affect from the previous day; and (C) age, sex, and average negative affect from the previous day. Shading represents 95% confidence interval.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multilevel model summaries.

There was a significant negative association between past-week suicidal ideation and daily positive future thinking, such that youth who reported higher levels of suicidal ideation within the past-week also reported lower levels of daily positive future thinking during the ESM period. Higher levels of daily positive future thinking were associated with reporting higher average positive affect on the preceding day. Higher levels of daily positive future thinking were associated with reporting lower average levels of negative affect the previous day (i.e., a negative association), however this association was not statistically significant and should therefore be interpreted with caution.

Discussion

In the current study, we found that past-week suicidal ideation was significantly associated with daily positive future thinking in a non-clinical, youth sample. Higher levels of daily positive future thinking were associated with lower levels of past-week suicidal ideation. Both average negative and positive affect from the previous day were associated with daily positive future thinking, such that youth experiencing higher positive and lower negative affect during the previous day also experienced higher levels of positive future thinking the next day. However, only the association between previous day positive affect and daily positive future thinking was statistically significant. The non-significant negative association between previous day negative affect and daily positive future thinking should therefore be interpreted with caution.

Our results are consistent with previous laboratory-based studies, finding that individuals who have thought about or engaged in self-harm also exhibit reduced positive future thinking (811). All previous research, however, has focused on positive future thinking over longer time-frames, generally including the next week, month, year, and five to ten years, and our study provides the first evidence indicating that short-term positive future thinking is also associated with recent suicidal ideation. Moreover, we show that future thinking in youths’ normal, everyday lives–outside of the controlled laboratory environment–relates to recent thoughts of suicide. Previous research using ESM has found that relationships between suicidal ideation and other risk factors, previously established with self-report questionnaires, do not always translate to everyday life. For example, despite a large body of questionnaire-based research associating perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness with suicidal ideation [e.g., (59, 60)], ESM research has not replicated these associations (24). This suggests that some risk and protective factors are less dynamic, and associated with suicidal ideation over longer timeframes, e.g., weeks, months, or years, whereas others are more dynamic and relate to suicidal ideation over minutes, hours, or days. Such dynamic factors may also be more amenable to change. That both short-term and longer-term positive future thinking both appear to be related to suicidal ideation may indicate the temporal robustness of future thinking as a correlate of suicidal ideation.

Combined with results of previous research with clinical populations [e.g., (10, 11, 14)], our finding that daily positive future thinking relates to even very low levels of recent suicidal ideation in a non-clinical youth sample, suggests that impaired positive future thinking could be present even at the very early stages of suicidal ideation. The need to examine risk factors for suicidal ideation in non-clinical samples has recently been highlighted by Millner et al. (61), who argue that risk factors for the development of suicidal ideation can likely not be derived from research with samples who have already experienced severe ideation or engaged in suicide attempts. Processes involved in the genesis of suicidal thoughts must necessarily also be present among individuals experiencing no or low levels of suicidal ideation, in order for changes in these processes to cause escalation in suicidal ideation (61). In this regard, future thinking may hold promise, as it appears to be a process associated with suicidal ideation across the spectrum of severity.

In the current study, the relationship between daily positive future thinking and past-week suicidal ideation remained significant even when controlling for previous-day positive and negative affect. This suggests that past-week suicidal ideation is associated with variance in daily future thinking beyond the effects of average positive and negative affect from the previous day. We also found a significant positive association between daily positive future thinking and positive affect from the previous day. This is consistent with previous studies demonstrating an association between positive affect and positive future thinking (1517). Although we found a negative association between negative affect from the previous day and positive future thinking, this association was not statistically significant. Early studies of future thinking (1517) found no association between negative affect and positive future thinking. Yet, these earlier studies’ findings also appear at odds with later research demonstrating that negative mood inductions reduced positive future thinking (13, 14). There is evidently heterogeneity within the literature regarding the relationship between affect and positive future thinking, which warrants further exploration.

Although previous literature indicates that adolescence is a sensitive period for the development of future orientation abilities (2931), we found no significant association between age and daily positive future thinking. The age-range of our sample was 15–25 years old, covering periods of developmental flux in future thinking identified in prior studies (31), thus our study should have been able to capture developmental differences in future thinking, had they been present. One explanation for the lack of association between age and future thinking observed in the current study is that developmental differences may be more apparent in static, as opposed to dynamic, measures of future thinking. As our study is, to the best of our knowledge, the first to investigate dynamic daily life future thinking, this hypothesis should be interrogated further in future research. It may also be possible that age-related differences in future thinking may have been apparent in a younger sample (e.g., 12–14 year-olds). Future research should also investigate future thinking in the daily lives of younger youth.

Strengths and limitations

Our study has a number of strengths. To our knowledge, this is the first investigation of positive future thinking in daily life and its association with recent suicidal ideation among a non-clinical sample of adolescents. Use of ESM to assess positive future thinking increases ecological validity (19), sheds new light on temporal aspects of future thinking, and provides the first indication that naturally occurring, positive future thinking relates to suicidal ideation. These findings therefore have theoretical and practical relevance for suicide research, where future thinking features within the IMV model of suicide (28). Only one previous study has investigated the relationship between suicidal ideation and future thinking in adolescents (27), thus our study adds to and extends the literature on this topic, as well as the broader future thinking and suicide, and developmental future orientation, literature.

The field of suicide research, as well as clinical psychology and psychiatry more broadly, have been highlighted as in need of a greater focus on transparency, reproducibility, and replicability (6266). To this end, we have made our analysis code available on the Open Science Framework (open code). We also postregistered our hypotheses and analysis plan. However, due to unexpected conditional branching in the dataset and consequently low numbers of individuals reporting suicidal ideation in our original independent variable, significant changes to our post-registration were necessary. We made every effort to document these deviations as transparently as possible, with a supplementary registration and a full description of deviations in the Supplementary materials. However, this undeniably compromised several key goals of preregistration (6769) and we fully appreciate this is a limitation of our study.

We must also acknowledge several further limitations. The number of adolescents endorsing past-week suicidal ideation was low (n = 55) in this general population adolescent sample and even among those reporting past-week suicidal ideation, the mean level of suicidal ideation was low (1.43 on a 1–4 scale). Our findings therefore require further replication in larger samples, both with adolescents from the general population, as well as individuals endorsing higher levels of suicidal ideation. The difference in mean levels of daily positive future thinking between those with vs. without past-week suicidal ideation was also small. The extent to which daily positive future thinking may be predictive of clinically relevant suicidal ideation outcomes cannot be determined from this study, and should be tested in future prospective research with clinical samples, using validated suicidal ideation scales. However, an additional challenge for determining the clinical meaningfulness of effects is that effects cannot be compared easily across ESM studies due to difficulties in obtaining standardized effect size estimates (70).

Additionally, we assessed suicidal ideation using a single-item from the SCL-90 (42) and previous research has highlighted single-item assessments of suicidal ideation and behavior as suboptimal (71). As our study used pre-existing data from the TwinssCan study (37), we were constrained by the variables available within the dataset. Future thinking was also assessed using only a single item, which naturally limits the scope and level of future thinking we could capture. However, given the intensive nature of ESM data collection, use of single-item measures to assess constructs of interest is common (72). The single item we used to assess future thinking in the current study has not been psychometrically validated and as—to the best of our knowledge—this is the first study of future thinking in daily life, the extent to which this item demonstrates convergent validity with other measures of future thinking is unknown. There are currently no standard items for assessing future thinking (or related constructs) using ESM. Indeed, ESM suffers from a general lack of validated questions for assessing constructs (7274). Although initiatives such as the ESM Item Repository (75) are underway to build sets of psychometrically valid ESM items, future research should also invest in basic measurement groundwork to develop high quality items for assessing future thinking in daily life. Other laboratory research has underscored the relevance of assessing the content of positive future thoughts (10) and the likelihood of positive future events occurring (27), in relation to suicidal ideation. When developing ESM measures to assess future thinking, content and likelihood of future thoughts should also be considered.

Positive and negative affect were not assessed at the same moment as future thinking, because future thinking was included only in the morning questionnaire and affect only in the momentary questionnaires. We used average affect from the previous day, as this was temporally closest to completion of the morning questionnaire. However, this may have been too long a time-window to detect meaningful dynamic effects of affect upon future thinking. Subsequent research should investigate the relationship between affect and future thinking contemporaneously, as well as prospectively from one moment to the next.

As post-registered, we did not conduct a power calculation, because the lack of comparable literature and available data meant we had nothing from which to draw meaningful parameters for a power calculation. Power and sample size calculations are often neglected in ESM research (64, 76, 77) and future research should aim to replicate our findings in larger samples, guided by simulation-based power analyses.

Finally, although not specifically a limitation, it is worth noting that data are drawn from a sample of twins and their non-twin siblings. There is some debate regarding whether twins are representative of the general population, termed the “twin representativeness assumption” (78). However, researchers have argued that although some differences are apparent—for example, in internalizing symptoms and eating disorders—the small to moderate effect sizes of these differences suggest that results from twins can be generalized to non-twins (79). Nevertheless, the potential effect of twinness on positive future thinking is an empirical question and future research should investigate this using dyadic models that are able to account for the interdependence between participants in twin pairs [e.g., (80)]. We also suggest that the findings of the current study should be replicated in non-twin samples.

Future research and clinical directions

Our findings provide several avenues for further research. First, future thinking has been linked to suicidal ideation and behavior among individuals with chronic pain (81), highlighting another group that could also benefit from further investigation of the relationship between short-term, positive future thinking and suicidal ideation. Second, future research should investigate future thinking in conjunction with other risk and protective factors for suicidal ideation. In the current study, we examined positive future thinking in isolation, but recent research (27, 82) and contemporary theoretical models of suicide—the IMV (28)—highlight that future thinking may influence suicidal ideation in concert with other factors. For example, within the IMV model, future thinking is posited as a “motivational moderator,” disrupting or facilitating the pathway between defeat, entrapment, and suicidal ideation (28). Third, the relationship between future thinking, suicidal ideation and affect may feasibly differ as a function of context. Research should assess future thinking and suicidal ideation at the momentary level, to determine whether the future thinking—suicidal ideation association is robust across contexts. Future studies could also substantively examine the role of context in positive future thinking, to reveal whether particular contexts, e.g., being in company, are associated with more positive future thinking. Fourth, and finally, the ICCs observed in the current study suggest that meaningful variance can also be explained at the within-as well as the between-person level. Further research would benefit from examining potential within-person relationships between future thinking and suicidal ideation.

Clinically, given the lack of knowledge regarding short-term correlates of suicidal ideation and behavior (83, 84), we consider these to be promising findings which, following further replication, may provide novel targets for rapid interventions to prevent suicidal thoughts and behaviors in young people, especially from non-clinical populations. Ecological Momentary Interventions (85), including Just-in-time adaptive interventions (86), could be an optimal approach to target positive future thinking in youths’ daily lives. These could also be blended with existing, e.g., Future Oriented Group Training (87) and emerging, e.g., “Edge of the Present” virtual reality therapy (88), interventions for future thinking.

Conclusion

The current study provides the first evidence to suggest that short-term (daily) positive future thinking is associated with past-week suicidal ideation in a non-clinical sample of adolescents. These findings are consistent with the broader experimental literature on future thinking and its association with suicidal ideation and behavior. Whilst the general population nature of the sample resulted in a relatively low number of adolescents endorsing recent suicidal ideation, our study indicates that daily positive future thinking is a promising avenue for future research.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: Data are not publicly available and are available upon application to the TwinssCan research team. Requests to access these data should be directed to TwinssCan project team: aW5mb0B0d2luc3NjYW4uZXU=.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Medical Ethics Committee UZ/KU Leuven, No. B32220107766. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by all participants or participants’ legal guardian/next of kin when participants were under 18 years old.

Author contributions

OJK and IM-G: conceptualization. SG: data curation. OJK: formal analysis, investigation, visualization, and writing – original draft. OJK, GL, TV, and IM-G: methodology. GL: software. OJK, GL, TV, JD, CD, SG, MD, NJ, CM-L, BR, ET, JO, RW, and IM-G: writing – review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

OJK was supported by a Senior Postdoctoral Fellowship from Research Foundation Flanders (FWO 1257821N). TV was supported by a Junior Postdoctoral Fellowship from Research Foundation Flanders (FWO 1243620N). GL and IM-G were supported by a Research Foundation Flanders Odysseus grant to IM-G (FWO GOF8416N).

Acknowledgments

This manuscript is available on PsyArXiv as a preprint: https://psyarxiv.com/dvnb8/.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.915007/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://osf.io/6mja2
  2. ^ https://osf.io/4nck7
  3. ^ https://osf.io/up63x/

References

1. Anestis MD, Moberg FB, Arnau RC. Hope and the interpersonal-psychological theory of suicidal behavior – replication and extenstion of prior findings. Suicide Life Threat Behav. (2013) 44:175–87.

Google Scholar

2. Chang EC, Yu EA, Lee JY, Hirsch JK, Kupfermann Y, Kahle ER. An examination of optimism/pessimism and suicide risk in primary care patients: does belief in a changeable future make a difference? Cogn Ther Res. (2012) 37:796–804. doi: 10.1007/s10608-012-9505-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Chin J, Holden RR. Multidimensional future time perspective as moderators of the relationships between suicide motivation, preparation, and its predictors. Suicide Life Threat Behav. (2013) 43:395–405. doi: 10.1111/sltb.12025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Danchin CL, MacLeod AK, Tata P. Painful engagement in deliberate self-harm: the role of conditional goal setting. Behav Res Ther. (2010) 48:915–20. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hirsch JK, Duberstein PR, Conner KR, Heisel MJ, Beckman A, Franus N, et al. Future orientation moderates the relationship between functional status and suicide ideation in depressed adults. Depress Anxiety. (2007) 24:196–201. doi: 10.1002/da.20224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kirtley OJ, Melson AJ, O’Connor RC. Future-oriented constructs and their role in suicidal ideation and enactment. In: Hirsch JK, Chang EC, Kelliher Rabon J editors. A Positive Psychological Approach to Suicide : Theory, Research, and Prevention. (Berlin: Springer International Publishing) (2018). p. 17–36. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-03225-8_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. O’Connor RC, O’Carroll RE, Ryan C, Smyth R. Self-regulation of unattainable goals in suicide attempters: a two year prospective study. J Affect Disord. (2012) 142:248–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. MacLeod AK, Pankhania B, Lee M, Mitchell D. Parasuicide, depression and the anticipation of positive and negative future experiences. Psychol Med. (1997) 27:973–7.

Google Scholar

9. MacLeod AK, Rose GS, Williams JMG. Components of hoplessness about the future in parasuicide. Cogn Ther Res. (1993) 17:441–55.

Google Scholar

10. O’Connor RC, Smyth R, Williams JM. Intrapersonal positive future thinking predicts repeat suicide attempts in hospital-treated suicide attempters. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2015) 83:169–76. doi: 10.1037/a0037846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. O’Connor RC, Whyte MC, Fraser L, Masterton G, Miles J, MacHale S. Predicting short-term outcome in well-being following suicidal behaviour: the conjoint effects of social perfectionism and positive future thinking. Behav Res Ther. (2007) 45:1543–55. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. O’Connor RC, Fraser L, Whyte MC, Machale S, Masterton G. A comparison of specific positive future expectancies and global hopelessness as predictors of suicidal ideation in a prospective study of repeat self-harmers. J Affect Disord. (2008) 110:207–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. O’Connor RC, Williams JMG. The relationship between positive future thinking, brooding, defeat and entrapment. Pers Individ Differ. (2014) 70:29–34. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Williams JMG, Van der Does AJW, Barnhofer T, Crane C, Segal ZS. Cognitive reactivity, suicidal ideation and future fluency: preliminary investigation of a differential activation theory of hopelessness/suicidality. Cogn Ther Res. (2007) 32:83–104. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9105-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Macleod AK, Conway C. Well-being and the anticipation of future positive experiences: the role of income, social networks, and planning ability. Cogn Emot. (2005) 19:357–74. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Miles H, MacLeod AK, Pote H. Retrospective and prospective cognitions in adolescents: anxiety, depression, and positive and negative affect. J Adolesc. (2004) 27:691–701. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. MacLeod AK, Tata P, Kentish J, Jacobsen H. Retrospective and prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression. Cogn Emot. (1997) 11:467–79. doi: 10.1080/026999397379881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R. Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. J Nerv Ment Dis. (1987) 175:526–36. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198709000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Myin-Germeys I, Kasanova Z, Vaessen T, Vachon H, Kirtley OJ, Viechtbauer W, et al. Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: new insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry. (2018) 17:123–32.

Google Scholar

20. Stone AA, Shiffman S. Reflections on the intensive measurement of stress, coping, and mood, with an emphasis on daily measures. Psychol Health. (1992) 7:115–29. doi: 10.1080/08870449208520014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gee BL, Han J, Benassi H, Batterham PJ. Suicidal thoughts, suicidal behaviours and self-harm in daily life: a systematic review of ecological momentary assessment studies. Digit Health. (2020) 6:2055207620963958. doi: 10.1177/2055207620963958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Kaurin A, Dombrovski AY, Hallquist MN, Wright ACG. Momentary interpersonal processes of suicidal surges in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med. (2020) 1–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720004791.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Kleiman EM, Nock MK. Advances in scientific possibilities offered by real-time monitoring technology. Psychiatry. (2017) 80:118–24. doi: 10.1080/00332747.2017.1325661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kleiman EM, Turner BJ, Fedor S, Beale EE, Huffman JC, Nock MK. Examination of real-time fluctuations in suicidal ideation and its risk factors: results from two ecological momentary assessment studies. J Abnorm Psychol. (2017) 126:726–38. doi: 10.1037/abn0000273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kleiman EM, Turner BJ, Fedor S, Beale EE, Picard RW, Huffman JC, et al. Digital phenotyping of suicidal thoughts. Depress Anxiety. (2018) 35:601–8. doi: 10.1002/da.22730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Nock MK, Prinstein MJ, Sterba SK. Revealing the form and function of self-injurious thoughts and behaviors: a real-time ecological assessment study among adolescents and young adults. J Abnorm Psychol. (2009) 118:816–27. doi: 10.1037/a0016948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Pollak OH, Guzmán EM, Shin KE, Cha CB. Defeat, entrapment, and positive future thinking: examining key theoretical predictors of suicidal ideation among adolescents. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:590388. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.590388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. O’Connor RC, Kirtley OJ. The integrated motivational-volitional model of suicidal behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2018) 373:20170268. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Gott C, Lah S. Episodic future thinking in children compared to adolescents. Child Neuropsychol. (2014) 20:625–40. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2013.840362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Seginer R. Future Orientation: Developmental and Ecological Perspectives. 1 ed. Berlin: Springer (2009).

Google Scholar

31. Steinberg L, Graham S, O’Brien L, Woolard J, Cauffman E, Banich M. Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Dev. (2009) 80:28–44.

Google Scholar

32. Nock MK, Borges G, Bromet EJ, Cha CB, Kessler RC, Lee S. Suicide and suicidal behavior. Epidemiol Rev. (2008) 30:133–54. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Nock MK, Green JG, Hwang I, McLaughlin KA, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Prevalence, correlates, and treatment of lifetime suicidal behavior among adolescents: results from the national comorbidity survey replication adolescent supplement. JAMA Psychiatry. (2013) 70:300–10. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. World Health Organization. Suicide Worldwide in 2019. Geneva: World Health Organization (2021).

Google Scholar

35. Glenn CR, Nock MK. Improving the prediction of suicidal behavior in youth. Int J Behav Consult Ther. (2014) 9:7–10.

Google Scholar

36. Cha CB, Franz PJ, M Guzmán E, Glenn CR, Kleiman EM, Nock MK. Annual research review: suicide among youth – epidemiology, (potential) etiology, and treatment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2018) 59:460–82. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Pries LK, Snijders C, Menne-Lothmann C, Decoster J, van Winkel R, Collip D, et al. TwinssCan – gene-environment interaction in psychotic and depressive intermediate phenotypes: risk and protective factors in a general population twin sample. Twin Res Hum Genet. (2019) 22:460–6. doi: 10.1017/thg.2019.96

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Derom C, Thiery E, Rutten BPF, Peeters H, Gielen M, Bijnens E, et al. The east Flanders prospective twin survey (EFPTS): 55 years later. Twin Res Hum Genet. (2019) 22:454–9. doi: 10.1017/thg.2019.64

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy, white women. Health Psychol. (2000) 19:586–92. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.19.6.586

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Klippel A, Reininghaus U, Viechtbauer W, Decoster J, Delespaul P, Derom C, et al. Sensitivity to peer evaluation and its genetic and environmental determinants: findings from a population-based twin study. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2018) 49:766–78. doi: 10.1007/s10578-018-0792-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Myin-Germeys I, Birchwood M, Kwapil T. From environment to therapy in psychosis: a real-world momentary assessment approach. Schizophr Bull. (2011) 37:244–7. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Derogatis LR. The brief symptom inventory: an introductory report. Psychol Med. (1983) 13:595–605. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700048017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Rintala A, Wampers M, Myin-Germeys I, Viechtbauer W. Response compliance and predictors thereof in studies using the experience sampling method. Psychol Assess. (2019) 31:226–35. doi: 10.1037/pas0000662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Jacobson NC. Compliance Thresholds in Intensive Longitudinal Data: Worse than Listwise Deletion: Call for Action [Oral Presentation]. Melbourne, VIC: Society of Ambulatory Assessment (SAA) (2020).

Google Scholar

45. Benning SD, Bachrach RL, Smith EA, Freeman AJ, Wright AGC. The registration continuum in clinical science: a guide toward transparent practices. J Abnorm Psychol. (2019) 128:528–40. doi: 10.1037/abn0000451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2021).

Google Scholar

47. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio Inc (2022).

Google Scholar

48. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S, Christensen RHB, Singmann H, et al. Lme4. In (Version 1.1-26). (2020).

Google Scholar

49. Lüdecke D. Package ‘sjPlot’: Data Visualization for Statistics in Social Science. In (Version 2.8.10) [R Package]. (2021). Available online at: https://strengejacke.github.io/sjPlot/

Google Scholar

50. Lüdecke D. ggeffects: tidy data frames of marginal effects from regression models. J Open Source Softw. (2018) 3:772. doi: 10.21105/joss.00772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Wilke CO. Package ‘cowplot’: Sreamlined Plot Theme and Plot Annotations for ‘ggplot2’. In (Version 1.1.1) [R package]. (2020). Available online at: https://wilkelab.org/cowplot/

Google Scholar

52. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. In (Version 1.5.8). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University (2015).

Google Scholar

53. Wiley J. Multilevel and Mixed Effects Model Diagnostics and Effect Sizes. In (Version 0.1.1) [R Package]. (2020). Available online at: http://joshuawiley.com/multilevelTools

Google Scholar

54. Garson GD. The Null Model. In Multilevel Modeling Applications in STATA, IBM SPSS, SAS, R, & HLM. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishing Inc (2020).

Google Scholar

55. Kleiman EM. Understanding and analyzing multilevel data from real-time monitoring studies: an easily– accessible tutorial using R. PsyArXiv. (2017) [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/xf2pw

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Enders CK, Tofighi D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. Psychol Methods. (2007) 12:121–38. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Adrian M, Miller AB, McCauley E, Vander Stoep A. Suicidal ideation in early to middle adolescence: sex-specific trajectories and predictors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2016) 57:645–53. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Fox KR, Millner AJ, Mukerji CE, Nock MK. Examining the role of sex in self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Clin Psychol Rev. (2018) 66:3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Chu C, Buchman-Schmitt JM, Stanley IH, Hom MA, Tucker RP, Hagan CR, et al. The interpersonal theory of suicide: a systematic review and meta-analysis of a decade of cross-national research. Psychol Bull. (2017) 143:1313–45. doi: 10.1037/bul0000123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Van Orden KA, Witte TK, Cukrowicz KC, Braithwaite SR, Selby EA, Joiner TE Jr. The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychol Rev. (2010) 117:575–600. doi: 10.1037/a0018697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Millner AJ, Robinaugh DJ, Nock MK. Advancing the understanding of suicide: the need for formal theory and rigorous descriptive research. Trends Cogn Sci. (2020) 24:704–16. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Carpenter TP, Law KC. Optimizing the scientific study of suicide with open and transparent research practices. Suicide Life Threat Behav. (2021) 51:36–46. doi: 10.1111/sltb.12665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Kirtley OJ, Janssens JJ, Kaurin A. Open science in suicide research is open for business. Crisis. (2022) doi: 10.1027/0227-5910/a000859. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kirtley OJ, Lafit G, Achterhof R, Hiekkaranta AP, Myin-Germeys I. Making the black box transparent: a template and tutorial for registration of studies using experience-sampling methods. Adv Meth Pract Psychol Sci. (2021) 4:1–16. doi: 10.1177/2515245920924686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Tackett JL, Brandes CM, Reardon KW. Leveraging the open science framework in clinical psychological assessment research. Psychol Assess. (2019) 31:1386–94. doi: 10.1037/pas0000583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Tackett JL, Lilienfeld SO, Patrick CJ, Johnson SL, Krueger RF, Miller JD, et al. It’s time to broaden the replicability conversation: thoughts for and from clinical psychological science. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2017) 12:742–56. doi: 10.1177/1745691617690042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Lakens D. The value of preregistration for psychological science: a conceptual analysis. Jpn Psychol Res. (2019) 62:221–30.

Google Scholar

68. Nosek BA, Ebersole CR, DeHaven AC, Mellor DT. The preregistration revolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2018) 115:2600–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1708274114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Wagenmakers EJ, Wetzels R, Borsboom D, van der Maas HL, Kievit RA. An agenda for purely confirmatory research. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2012) 7:632–8. doi: 10.1177/1745691612463078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Rights JD, Sterba SK. Quantifying explained variance in multilevel models: an integrative framework for defining R-squared measures. Psychol Methods. (2019) 24:309–38. doi: 10.1037/met0000184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Millner AJ, Lee MD, Nock MK. Single-item measurement of suicidal behaviors: validity and consequences of misclassification. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0141606. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Wright AGC, Zimmermann J. Applied ambulatory assessment: integrating idiographic and nomothetic principles of measurement. Psychol Assess. (2019) 31:1467–80. doi: 10.1037/pas0000685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Horstmann KT, Ziegler M. Assessing personality states: what to consider when constructing personality state measures. Eur J Pers. (2021) 34:1037–59. doi: 10.1002/per.2266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Mestdagh M, Dejonckheere E. Ambulatory assessment in psychopathology research: current achievements and future ambitions. Curr Opin Psychol. (2021) 41:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Kirtley OJ, Hiekkaranta AP, Kunkels YK, Eisele G, Verhoeven D, van Nierop M, et al. The experience sampling method (ESM) item repository. (2020) doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/KG376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Lafit G, Adolf J, Dejonckheere E, Myin-Germeys I, Viechtbauer W, Ceulemans E. Selection of the number of participants in intensivelongitudinal studies: a user-friendly shiny app and tutorial to perform power analysis in multilevel regression modelsthat account for temporal dependencies. Adv Meth Pract Psychol Sci. (2020) 4:1–24.

Google Scholar

77. Trull TJ, Ebner-Priemer UW. Ambulatory assessment in psychopathology research: a review of recommended reporting guidelines and current practices. J Abnorm Psychol. (2020) 129:56–63. doi: 10.1037/abn0000473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Christensen K, McGue M. The twin representativeness assumption. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 112:374–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Munn-Chernoff MA, von Ranson KM, Culbert KM, Larson CL, Burt SA, Klump KL. An examination of the representativeness assumption for twin studies of eating pathology and internalizing symptoms. Behav Genet. (2013) 43:427–35. doi: 10.1007/s10519-013-9603-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Bolger N, Laurenceau JP. Analyzing diary and intensive longitudinal data from dyads. In: Mehl M, Conner TS editors. Handbook of Research Methods for Studying Daily Life. (New York, NY: Guildford) (2011).

Google Scholar

81. Kirtley OJ, Rodham K, Crane C. Understanding suicidal ideation and behaviour in individuals with chronic pain: a review of the role of novel transdiagnostic psychological factors. Lancet Psychiatry. (2020) 7:282–90. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(19)30288-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Chang OD, Batra MM, Premkumar V, Chang EC, Hirsch JK. Future orientation, depression, suicidality, and interpersonal needs in primary care outpatients. Death Stud. (2020) 44:98–104. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2018.1522389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Franklin JC, Ribeiro JD, Fox KR, Bentley KH, Kleiman EM, Huang X, et al. Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: a meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Psychol Bull. (2017) 143:187–232. doi: 10.1037/bul0000084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Glenn CR, Nock MK. Improving the short-term prediction of suicidal behavior. Am J Prev Med. (2014) 47:S176–80. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Myin-Germeys I, Klippel A, Steinhart H, Reininghaus U. Ecological momentary interventions in psychiatry. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2016) 29:258–63. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Coppersmith DD, Dempsey W, Kleiman E, Bentley K, Murphy S, Nock M. Just-in-time adaptive intervetions for suicide prevention: promise, challenges, and future directions. PsyArXiv. (2021) [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/eg9fx

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. van Beek W, Kerkhof A, Beekman A. Future oriented group training for suicidal patients: a randomized clinical trial. BMC Psychiatry. (2009) 9:65. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-9-65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Habak S, Bennett J, Davies A, Davies M, Christensen H, Boydell KM. Edge of the present: a virtual reality tool to cultivate future thinking, positive mood and wellbeing. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 18:140. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18010140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. United Nations. Global Issues: Youth. New York, NY: United Nations. (accessed July 14, 2022).

Google Scholar

90. Sawyer SM, Azzopardi PS, Wickremarathne D, Patton GC. The age of adolescence. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. (2018) 2:223–8. doi: 10.1016/s2352-4642(18)30022-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: suicidal ideation, future thinking, experience sampling method, youth, general population

Citation: Kirtley OJ, Lafit G, Vaessen T, Decoster J, Derom C, Gülöksüz S, De Hert M, Jacobs N, Menne-Lothmann C, Rutten BPF, Thiery E, van Os J, van Winkel R, Wichers M and Myin-Germeys I (2022) The relationship between daily positive future thinking and past-week suicidal ideation in youth: An experience sampling study. Front. Psychiatry 13:915007. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.915007

Received: 07 April 2022; Accepted: 11 August 2022;
Published: 29 September 2022.

Edited by:

Eve Griffin, University College Cork, Ireland

Reviewed by:

Yuan Cao, Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong SAR, China
Philip Batterham, Australian National University, Australia

Copyright © 2022 Kirtley, Lafit, Vaessen, Decoster, Derom, Gülöksüz, De Hert, Jacobs, Menne-Lothmann, Rutten, Thiery, van Os, van Winkel, Wichers and Myin-Germeys. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Olivia J. Kirtley, b2xpdmlhLmtpcnRsZXlAa3VsZXV2ZW4uYmU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.