Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 23 June 2022
Sec. Social Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars in Social Cognition in Psychiatry View all 7 articles

Social Touch, Social Isolation, and Loneliness in Borderline Personality Disorder During the COVID-19 Pandemic

  • 1Department of Clinical Psychology, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany

Background: Interpersonal impairments in borderline personality disorder (BPD) are characterised by a lack in the sense of belonging and the fear of being excluded. One feature of interactions that can promote a sense of social belonging is interpersonal touch. While some studies suggest that individuals with BPD experience social touch as less pleasurable than healthy individuals (HCs), there are no studies that investigated whether this difference is associated with feeling less socially connected. This question is particularly important during the COVID-19 pandemic, since one central behavioural recommendation is “social distancing”. An increase in loneliness has been discussed as a consequence and it has been suggested that individuals with BPD may be particularly burdened. However, the primary goal of “social distancing” is not preventing social contacts, but physical proximity. In our study we investigated the interplay between feeling close to others, contact frequency and the appraisal of social touch in BPD. We were additionally interested in whether these factors contribute to the burden through “physical distancing”.

Methods: We assessed subjective and objective social isolation, the need, importance, and liking of social touch, as well as the burden through “physical distancing” policies in 130 women (61 BPD and 69 HCs).

Results: Participants of the BPD group reported higher loneliness, less social contacts and a lower need for, importance and liking of social touch compared to HCs. Larger social networks, higher frequency of in-person contacts and higher liking and importance of social touch were associated with lower levels of loneliness. Both groups did not differ regarding their burden through “physical distancing”. A higher need for and lower importance of social touch predicted a higher burden through “physical distancing”.

Conclusions: A positive appraisal of social touch was associated with less loneliness, independently of an individual's objective social isolation. In BPD, impairments of this fundamental facet of social interaction might hamper forming and strengthening of social bonds and contribute to the patients' interpersonal dysfunction. Changing the attitude towards social touch and in consequence its liking and importance in social interaction might provide one avenue to improve the sense of social connectedness in these patients.

Introduction

“Social distancing” is one of the main behavioural restrictions aiming to reduce the spread of the COVID-19 infections during the COVID-19 pandemic. The associated changes in every-day life may challenge one of the fundamental needs of human beings, that is, the need for social belonging (1). While this imposes a burden on most people, it has been suggested that this burden might be higher in individuals with a mental disorder (24). Individuals with Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) may be particularly affected: The threat to being socially connected imposed by “social distancing” might intensify the fear of being abandoned and the feeling of being disconnected from others, which are characteristic of BPD (59). Moreover, “social distancing” might also reduce the size and diversity of an individual's social network, potentially having a disproportionally strong effect on loneliness in BPD patients, who already have smaller and less diverse social networks (6, 7, 10, 11). In a first investigation of the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic and its mandated restrictions in a small sample of patients with mood disorders, especially individuals with BPD (n = 16) reported a deleterious impact on their symptomatology (12).

During the COVID-19 pandemic, especially when cases were peaking, governments recommend “social distancing” and even enforced these behavioural restrictions by mandated “lockdowns”. However, as the American Centres for Disease Control and Prevention state on their website “social distancing, …also called “physical distancing,” means keeping a safe space between yourself and other people who are not from your household” (13). Thus, the primary goal of “physical distancing” is not to prevent social contacts, but rather physical proximity in order to limit the spreading of the infection.

In line, one of the challenges people are faced with during the COVID-19 pandemic is to preserve social contacts and prevent the feeling of being lonely, while simultaneously reducing physical proximity. Beyond more traditional approaches such as writing letters or using phones, digital communication technologies like social media platforms and video calls allow people to stay in touch without meeting in person. The use of these technologies has already increased continuously before the pandemic (14). However, in non-pandemic times the use of virtual technologies has also been linked to higher levels of loneliness (15, 16). Hence, it is still up for debate whether their use is the cause or the consequence of feeling lonely, that is, being less socially connected than one would wish (17, 18). Moreover, their use does not seem to be particularly effective in counteracting loneliness, at least in the elderly (19).

So far, empirical data on the use of virtual technologies in individuals with BPD are missing. A recent study by Ooi, Michael (20) revealed that people high in BPD features ascribe a high importance to social media in their daily life. However, since communication via virtual channels restricts, at least partially, the availability of social cues such as body language, it might leave more room for ambiguity during social contacts. Since changes in social cognitive processing of ambiguous social cues are one feature of interpersonal impairments in BPD (2124), it is questionable whether individuals with BPD use virtual communication channels during “physical distancing” policies to a comparable extent as healthy individuals and whether this is comparably helpful in reducing the experience of loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic. On the other hand, social media offers people the possibility to present themselves online in a way they want to be seen. This could be helpful for people with BPD, as shame experience is elevated in this group (25). Thus, it is also conceivable that people with BPD use virtual communication channels as much as or even more often than healthy individuals.

Social contacts by virtual technologies reduce physical proximity during social encounters as intended by physical distancing policies. However, they also prevent social touch like hugging one another or linking arms, which is an important component of social interactions. Social touch might even promote prosocial behaviour among strangers; waiters, for example, receive greater tips when they touch customers (26). Receiving affectionate touch by someone close is positively associated with physical, social and psychological wellbeing in children, adolescents and adults (2729). Moreover, social touch promotes a sense of security, reduces subjective pain, and facilitates stress regulation (3033). In line with these findings, social touch has been shown to reduce feelings of loneliness (34). “Touch-hunger”, an increased need for social touch, has been discussed as a consequence of “physical distancing” policies (35, 36).

Despite touch being an important component of interpersonal encounters in general, the preference for physical proximity differs between people (37, 38). For example, individuals with BPD prefer a larger interpersonal distance (3941). One reason might be the high prevalence of adverse childhood experiences (ACE) in BPD (42, 43). Individuals with a history of ACE and a lack of caring touch in childhood are less sensitive to affective touch, reveal an attenuated neuronal response to affective touch, and prefer a greater interpersonal distance to strangers (4446). Particularly, ACE associated with physical neglect and abuse have been proposed as essential for the interpretation of touch as intrusive and less pleasant (47, 48).

Attachment styles developed in childhood are associated with different long-term effects following adversity in childhood (4952). Adults with insecure attachment styles experience gentle touch as less pleasant (53), report less positive feelings towards affectionate touch in romantic relationships (47, 48), show less comfort with close contact in interaction with their children (54), and discriminate less between affective and non-affective touch (55). Beyond these relationships between attachment style and alterations in the processing of social touch, an insecure attachment style is also linked to higher levels in the experience of loneliness (56, 57). Both, BPD and ACE have been associated with insecure attachment in adulthood (5861). In line with the aforementioned, individuals with BPD perceive social touch as less pleasant compared to healthy individuals (62). This suggests that the lack of physical proximity during “physical distancing” affects these individuals to a lower extent. However, these findings do not necessarily imply that the affected individuals also experience a lower need for physical closeness and that touch is less important during social contacts. So far, empirical studies on these different facets of social touch in BPD and ACE are missing as well as studies on their potentially differential impact on the experience of loneliness and the burden associated with “physical distancing” during the COVID-19 pandemic.

For many months the discussion about the consequences of “physical distancing” on mental health focussed on whether the reduction of in-person contacts result in an increase of loneliness and whether virtual communication channels can counteract this development. However, the extent to which “physical distancing” during the COVID-19 pandemic led to feelings of loneliness might not only be influenced by contact frequencies and channels, but also by the appraisal of physical proximity during social encounters. However, there is a lack of studies investigating whether the appraisal of interpersonal touch modulates the relationship between in-person contacts and virtual contacts with the feeling of being socially disconnected.

The present study aims to contribute to the understanding of the interplay between objective and subjectively experienced social isolation, the appraisal of social touch in social contacts, and the use of different communication channels in healthy individuals and individuals with BPD. Moreover, we were interested in whether a history of ACE affects the appraisal of interpersonal touch in BPD and whether an individual's attachment style modulates this relationship. We expected (1) that individuals with a history of BPD report smaller and less diverse social networks, a higher level of subjective social isolation and that they appraise social touch as less pleasant compared with healthy controls. Moreover, we hypothesised that (2) the frequency of social contacts partially predicts the feeling of social isolation and that (3) the appraisal of social touch moderates this relationship, depending on the used communication channels. In more detail, we expected that in participants who appraise social touch as more pleasant, the relationship of in-person contacts to feelings of social isolation is stronger and for the association of virtual contacts to feelings of social isolation attenuated. To further investigate the factors underlying inter-individual differences in the appraisal of social touch during social contacts, we analysed its relationship with both ACE and attachment style. We expected that (4) social touch is appraised as less pleasant in those with a history of ACE and that this association is particularly strong in those with a lower ability to feel close towards others, as one dimension of attachment. Beyond these research questions addressing interpersonal functioning independently of the COVID-19 pandemic, we additionally explored whether these facets of social interaction are related to the burden through “physical distancing” due to a lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic. In line with previous findings indicating preferences for a higher interpersonal distance and less pleasant appraisal of social touch in BPD patients, we expected that those individuals who appraise interpersonal touch as less pleasant, feel less burdened by physical distancing.

Materials and Methods

An online survey was conducted between February 13 and April 4, 2021. During this period, Germany had strict restrictions (“lockdown”) in place. Most public facilities were closed, face masks were mandatory on public transport, in supermarkets, and in places where it was not possible to maintain a 1.5 m distance to others. Regarding private gatherings, meetings were only allowed between one household and one extra person.

Participants

Participants were recruited from the database of the central project of the KFO 256, a Clinical Research Unit funded by the German Research Foundation dedicated to investigating mechanisms of disturbed emotion processing in BPD (63). The analyses in the present paper are based on survey data of 130 women of which 69 were healthy controls and 61 individuals who had met the Diagnostics and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) (64) diagnosis of BPD in the past, that is, met at least five of the nine DSM–IV criteria for BPD, as assessed by trained clinical psychologists using the International Personality Disorder Examination (IPDE) (65). For further details on the recruitment procedure, see Supplementary Material A. All of these individuals gave informed consent before participating in the survey. The study was approved by the Research Ethics Board II of the Medical Faculty Mannheim of Heidelberg University.

Sociodemographic features, BPD psychopathology and severity of childhood trauma for both groups are reported in Table 1. We measured the BPD symptom severity with the short version of the Borderline Symptom List (BSL-23) (66), the level of BPD features with the Borderline Scale from the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI-BOR) (67), German version VEI-BOR (68); and severity of depressive symptoms with the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II) (69), German version (70); range: 0–63; Cronbach's α in the current study were α = 0.96 for the total sample, α = 0.94 in the BPD and α = 0.87 in HC sample). The severity of childhood trauma was based on self-reports measured with the short form of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ-SF) (71), German version (72). For further details, see Supplementary Materials A, B.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

Both groups were balanced for age, education and relationship status (all ps > 0.1). However, individuals of the BPD group were more likely to live by themselves [χ2 (1) = 4.73, P = 0.030, ϕ = 0.19], worked less often full-time [χ2 (1) = 11.09, P = 0.001, ϕ = 0.29] and currently received psychotherapeutic, psychopharmacologic and/or psychiatric treatment to a higher percentage [χ2 (1) = 71.47, P < 0.001, ϕ = 0.74]. The BPD group reported a higher level of BPD symptoms (BSL-23), BPD features (VEI-BOR), depressive symptoms (BDI-II) and a higher severity of ACE (CTQ) compared with the HC group (see Table 1). According to the severity categories proposed by Kleindienst, Jungkunz (73), the mean BSL-23 scores obtained in our sample indicate a high level of BPD symptoms in the BPD group and none to low BPD symptoms in the HC group.

Measures

Loneliness

Loneliness, that is, the subjective experience of social isolation, was assessed using the Revised University of California Los Angeles Loneliness Scale (ULS-R) (74), German version (75).

The ULS-R consists of 20 items. Following the recommendations of the authors of the German validation study (75), items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale, instead of a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (not true at all) to 5 (completely true). Items were combined in a sum score (range 20–100) with higher scores indicating higher levels of loneliness. Internal consistency for the ULS-R was α = 0.96 (BPD: Cronbach's α = 0.91; HCs: Cronbach's α = 0.90).

To capture the subjective experience of social isolation in relation to an individual's actual social network, we additionally measured the feeling of closeness in the context of the social network index (see below).

Social Touch

We assessed three facets of the appraisal of social touch in social relationships: (a) an individual's general attitude towards interpersonal touch with a self-report questionnaire, (b) the liking of social touch in an experimental task, as well as (c) the importance of different kinds of touch towards the members of an individual's social network.

The need for interpersonal touch was assessed by the Need for Interpersonal Touch Scale (NFIPT) (76). The 20-item NFIPT measures an individual's general attitude towards interpersonal touch on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (exactly true), combined in a mean score (range 1–7) with higher scores indicating a higher need for interpersonal touch. In the present study internal consistency for the NFIPT Scale was α = 0.93 (BPD: Cronbach's α = 0.93; HCs: Cronbach's α = 0.91).

We assessed an individual's liking of social touch with an experimental task during which participants watched video-clips of socio-affective touch sequences and rated how pleasant they would experience the touch displayed on a 7-point-Likert scale ranging from −3 (very unpleasant) to +3 (very pleasant). Independent variables were the valence of the displayed touch (negative touch e.g., shaking or slapping; positive touch e.g., hugging or stroking) and the social context (non-social: touching an object; social: touching another person). Participants rated 60 video clips overall (15 clips for each of the experimental conditions of this 2 × 2 design). Video clips were selected from the socio-affective touch data base (77), Stimuli selected: positive: 2–6, 15–19, 28–32 (social), 40–44, 52–56, 64–69 (non-social), negative: 8,10–13, 21, 23–26, 34, 36–39 (social), 46,48–51, 58,60–63,70, 72–75 (non-social). The video clips were presented in six blocks. The sequence of the blocks as well as the videos within each block were presented in random order.

To measure the appraisal of interpersonal touch within an individual's actual social network, we additionally asked participants to judge the importance of interpersonal touch in the context of the social network index (see below).

Social Network and Use of Communication Channels

We measured participants' objective social isolation with the Social Network Index (SNI) (78). Based on 12 different social domains, the SNI quantifies the size of the network, that is, the number of people with whom an individual speaks at least once every 2 weeks (SNI-size), and the diversity of the network, that is, the number of social domains in which the respondent has regular contact with at least one person (SNI-diversity).

Since social touch is restricted to social contacts during which people meet in person, we additionally asked participants to assess in each of the social domains how often they used different communication channels for social contacts during the last 4 weeks of the lockdown. Beyond in-person contacts we asked how often participants used different digital communication channels (for further details see Supplementary Material C). Participants estimated the frequency of contacts on a 7-point Likert-scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (several times a day).

We additionally asked participants to rate the closeness and the importance of interpersonal touch towards others for each of the 12 social domains on a 7-point-Likert scale (range: 1–7, closeness: not at all to very close; importance of touch: not important at all to very important). Ratings of closeness (“How close to do you feel towards these persons?”) were averaged across the 12 social domains for further analyses. Participants rated the importance of touch for six different types of social encounters (adapted from) (79). For further details, see Supplementary Material D. Ratings of the importance of touch were averaged across the 12 social domains and the six social situations for further analyses.

Attachment

Attachment-related attitudes were assessed with the Adult Attachment Scale (AAS) (80), German version (81). The scale consists of 15 items, assessing the three subscales closeness (needing intimacy and being comfortable with it; BPD: Cronbach's α = 0.84; HC: Cronbach's α = 0.80), dependence (trust that others are available when needed; BPD: Cronbach's α = 0.76; HC: Cronbach's α = 0.76) and anxiety (fear of not being loved and abandoned; BPD: Cronbach's α = 0.61; HC: Cronbach's α = 0.56), thus measuring different dimensions of attachment. Participants were asked to indicate their level of agreement with these 15 statements on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not true at all) to 5 (completely right). Subscale scores range from 5 to 25.

Burden Through Physical Distancing Measures

Participants assessed how strongly they felt burdened by different physical distancing measures. Items were adapted from the Covid-19 Snapshot Monitoring Survey (82). All questions were rated on a 6-point Likert-Scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 6 (very). Since the perceived burden might be influenced by compliance to physical distancing measures, we additionally asked participants to judge how strongly they complied with the different behavioural recommendations. For further details, see Supplementary Material E.

Statistical Analysis

To examine differences in loneliness and social closeness, independent samples t-tests were applied. In order to examine differences in the frequencies of virtual contacts and in-person contacts to non-household members, 2 × 2 rm-ANOVA with the between-subjects factor “group” (HC, BPD) and within-subject factor “channel” (virtual, in person), was conducted. Need for touch and importance of touch in relationships towards members of participants' social network were compared between groups with independent samples t-test. Ratings of pleasantness of the videos depicting different types of touch were analysed with a 2 × 2 × 2 rm-ANOVA with the between-subjects factor “group” (HC, BPD) and within-subject factors “social” (social, non-social) and “valence” (negative, positive).

The role of the appraisal of social touch as a moderator in the relationship of contact frequencies to feelings of social isolation, respectively to the experienced burden through social distancing, was investigated with three multiple regression analyses across the whole sample. Additionally to the pre-registered analysis of the relationship between contact frequencies and the experience of closeness to members of the participants' social network (hypothesis 2), we analysed this relationship between objective and subjectively experienced social isolation on a more global level based on network size and loneliness measured with the ULS-R. In the first model, we used the frequency of in-person contacts and virtual contacts, the three facets of the appraisal of social touch, as well as their interactions with the frequencies of in-person contacts and virtual contacts to predict the closeness experienced within the participant's social network. In the second model, we predicted the ULS-R score with SNI size, the three facets of the appraisal of social touch, that is the NFTPS score, the liking of positive social touch (estimated as the difference between liking of positive social and non-social touch), and the importance of touch within the SN, as well as their interactions with the SNI size. In the third model, we repeated the first analysis exploratively with the experienced burden through physical distancing as dependent variable. Since the “physical distancing” policies aimed to reduce in-person contacts with persons from other households, we excluded ratings related to individuals living within the same household when calculating the mean scores of contact frequencies and closeness in model one and three.

Associations of ACE, the capacity to feel close to others as one dimension of attachment, and appraisal of social touch in BPD were examined with multivariate multiple regression. Hereby, we calculated a regression model with the CTQ score together with attachment closeness and its interaction with CTQ as predictors for the three facets of the appraisal of social touch.

All analyses were performed using SPSS software (version 25) and R (version 4.0.4). The level of significance was set to α = 0.05. For regression analyses, predictor variables were z-transformed before analyses. Participants exhibiting scores ± 2.5 SD from the mean were excluded from the respective analysis as outliers and can be found in see Supplementary Table S1. The observed power remained sufficient. We adjusted P-values according to Benjamini and Hochberg (83) for post-hoc tests (sub-designs of the rm-ANOVA, as well as pairwise comparisons). None of our participants exhibited processing times faster than 60% of the median and all stated that they filled out the query honestly and alone. Please note that we used the median instead of the mean of processing time to adjust for longer breaks in single participants during answering the survey.

Pre-registration

The main hypotheses were pre-registered together with the design and planned analyses (https://aspredicted.org/xs67c.pdf). In addition to our pre-registered hypotheses, we investigated the interplay of the contact frequencies, the appraisal of social touch and the experienced burden through social distancing. We did not fully achieve the sample size of 80 BPD and 80 HC participants planned in the pre-registration, for further details on the recruitment procedure, see Supplementary Material A.

Results

Loneliness and Closeness Within the Social Network

ULS-R scores revealed a higher level of loneliness in the BPD group compared to HCs [HC: M = 31.39, SD = 9.40, BPD: M = 60.17, SD = 14.29, t (99.52) = −13.30, P < 0.001, d = −2.41, Figure 1A]. When judging the closeness to members of their social network, individuals with BPD reported to feel less close than the HC group [HC: M = 4.24, SD = 0.66, BPD: M = 3.18, SD = 0.85, t (126) = 7.94, P < 0.001, d = 1.41, Figure 1B]. An exploratory analysis revealed that this was true for members of their social network with whom they do not live together, but not for members of the same household [non-household: HC: M = 3.96, SD = 0.73, BPD: M = 2.90, SD = 0.95, t (128) = 7.94, P < 0.001, d = 1.27; household: HC: M = 6.19, SD = 0.98,.BPD: M = 5.69, SD = 1.21, t (43.91) = 1.77, P = 0.083, d = 0.46]. For additional information on closeness in different social domains (Supplementary Table S2 and Supplementary Figure S1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Social connectedness and features of the social network in the HC and BPD groups. (A) ULS-R, (B) closeness within the network, (C) network size and (D) diversity, and (E) contact frequencies. Error bars represent 1 SE. ***P < 0.001.

Social Networks and Communication Channels

The social networks of the BPD group were smaller and less diverse than those of the HC group [size: HC: M = 16.45, SD = 5.68, BPD: M = 10.20, SD = 6.26, t (126) = 5.92, P < 0.001, d = 1.05; diversity: HC: M = 5.99, SD = 1.36, BPD: M = 4.45, SD = 1.72, t (127) = 5.66, P < 0.001, d = 1.00; Figures 1C,D].

When judging the frequency of contacts when using different communication channels, we restricted our analyses to members of the social network that do not live in the same household. The frequency of in-person contacts and virtual contacts did not differ significantly and this was true for both groups [Figure 1E, “channel:” F(1, 126) = 1.62, P = 0.205, ηp2 = 0.01; “group” × “channel”: F(1, 126) < 0.01, P = 0.997, ηp2 < 0.01; in person: HC: M = 1.14, SD = 0.38, BPD: M = 0.92, SD = 0.38, virtual: HC: M = 1.18, SD = 0.31, BPD: M = 0.96, SD = 0.34]. However, overall, contacts were less frequent in the BPD compared with the HC group [“group”: F(1, 126) = 17.25, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.12, HC: M = 1.16, SD = 0.29, BPD: M = 0.94, SD = 0.33]. For additional information for social domains, see Supplementary Tables S3, S4 and Supplementary Figures S2, S3.

Appraisal of Social Touch

Need for Touch

NFIPT scores indicate a lower need for social touch in the BPD group compared with the HC group [HC: M = 4.34, SD = 1.04, BPD: M = 3.64, SD = 1.26, t (116.88) = 3.49, P = 0.001, d = 0.61, Figure 2A].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Need for touch, (B) liking of touch, and (C) importance of touch within social network in HC and BPD. Error bars represent 1 SE. *P < 0.05. **P > 0.01. ***P < 0.001.

Liking of Social Touch

Compared to HCs, BPD patients judged the video clips as less pleasant [“group:” F(1, 118) = 100.05, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.16]. The strength of differences between groups was influenced by both experimental factors [“valence” × “social” × “group:” F(1, 118) = 12.09, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.09, Figure 2B, for summary of ANOVA results see Supplementary Table S6]. A 2 × 2 ANOVA sub-design for social scenes revealed that the pleasantness ratings in the BPD group was stronger reduced for the positive than the negative social scenes compared with HC [“group” × “valence”: F(1, 118) = 12.76, P = 0.001, pFDR = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.10; “valence”: F(1, 118) = 1,544.96, P < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.93; “group”: F(1, 118) = 28.25, P < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.19; comparison between groups: negative: HC: M = −2.25, SD = 0.44, BPD: M = −2.42, SD = 0.47, t (118) = 2.02, P = 0.046, pFDR = 0.046 d = 0.28; positive: HC: M = 2.03, SD = 0.80, BPD: M = 1.15, SD = 1.17, t (87.63) = 4.68, P < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001, d = 0.90]. In contrast, a 2 × 2 sub-design for non-social scenes revealed no statistically significant difference between groups in the effects of valence: all participants assessed negative scenes as less pleasant than positive scenes [“valence”: F(1, 118) = 174.02, P < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.60; “group” × “valence”: F(1, 118) =.01, P = 0.931, pFDR = 0.931, ηp2 < 0.0;1 “group”: F(1, 118) = 6.66, P = 0.011, pFDR = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.19] For summary of ANOVA results of sub-designs, see Supplementary Tables S7, S8.

Please note that, in general, videos of positive touch were rated as more pleasant than videos of negative touch [“valence”: F(1, 118) = 1029.19, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.90]. This difference was higher for social compared to non-social scenes [“valence” × “social”: F(1, 118) = 674.64, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.85], with negative touch being rated as even more negative and positive touches as even more positive [pairwise comparison non-social with social: negative: t (119) = 20.64, P < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001, dz = 1.88; positive: t (119) = −16.64, P < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001, dz = −1.52].

Importance of Touch in Social Relationships

Individuals with BPD reported a lower importance of touch in their relationships [HC: M = 3.77, SD = 1.01, BPD: M = 2.82, SD = 0.96, t (128) = 5.46, P < 0.001, d = 0.96, Figure 2C]. An exploratory analysis revealed that this applied only for social network members outside of their household but not household members [household: HC: M = 6.10, SD = 0.93, BPD: M = 5.62, SD = 1.33, t (41.51) = 1.63, P = 0.111, d = 0.44; outside: HC: M = 3.43, SD = 1.10, BPD: M = 2.58, SD = 1.00, t (128) = 4.61, P < 0.001, d = 0.81]. For additional information on the importance of touch in different social domains, see Supplementary Table S9 and Supplementary Figure S4.

Interpersonal Touch as a Moderator Between the Frequency of Social Contacts and Social Closeness Towards Members of the Social Network Living Outside of the Own Household

Multiple regression analyses revealed that contact frequencies and appraisal of social touch predicted 29.8% of the variance of closeness to members of the social network outside of the own household [F(11, 114) = 4.39, P < 0.001, adjusted R2 = 0.23, Table 2]. With increasing frequency of in-person contacts to non-household members, as well as a higher liking and importance of social touch in relationships to non-household members, the experienced closeness towards these members of the social network increased (Figure 3). None of the interactions of the frequency of in-person contacts and virtual contacts with the facets of the appraisal of social touch were significant predictors.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Prediction of social closeness by contact frequencies and appraisal of social touch.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Partial residual plots of regression model 1; the observed data is based on the values of (A) in-person contact frequency, (B) liking, and (C) importance of touch and the model error. The grey area represents the confidence interval, grey dots data of HC and red dots data of BPD.

Interpersonal Touch as a Moderator Between Social Network Size and Loneliness

Multiple regression analyses revealed that network size and appraisal of social touch predicted 35.4% of the variance of loneliness [F(7, 117) = 9.16, P < 0.001, adjusted R2 = 0.32, Table 3]. With an increase of the network size, as well as a stronger liking and importance of touch the ULS-R score decreased (Figure 4). None of the interactions of the social network size with the facets of the appraisal of social touch were significant predictors.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Prediction of loneliness (ULS-R) by social network size and appraisal of social touch.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Partial residual plots of regression model 2; the observed data is based on the values of (A) size of social network, (B) liking, and (C) importance of touch and the model error. The grey area represents the confidence interval, grey dots data of HC and red dots data of BPD.

Interpersonal Touch as a Moderator Between the Frequency of Social Contacts Towards Members of the Social Network Living Outside of the Own Household and Burden Through “Physical Distancing”

Multiple regression analysis revealed that contact frequencies and appraisal of social touch explained 19.0% of the variance of experienced burden through “physical distancing” [F(11, 113) = 2.40, P = 0.010, adjusted R2 = 0.11, Table 4]. Contact frequencies did not predict the experienced burden through “physical distancing” policies, but a higher need for touch and a lower importance of touch predicted a higher burden (Figure 5).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Prediction of burden through physical distancing policies by contact frequencies and appraisal of social touch.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Partial residual plots of regression model 3; the observed data is based on the values of (A) need for interpersonal touch scale and (B) importance of touch and the model error. The grey area represents the confidence interval, grey dots data of HC and red dots data of BPD.

Please note that both groups did neither differ in the burden induced by social distancing measures nor in their compliance with these behavioural recommendations [burden: HC: M = 3.93, SD = 1.27, BPD: M = 3.76, SD = 1.30, t (127) = 0.72, P = 0.474; compliance HC: M = 4.73, SD = 0.97, BPD: M = 4.87, SD = 0.85, t (123) = −0.84, P = 0.404].

ACE, Attachment Closeness and the Appraisal of Interpersonal Touch in BPD

To investigate whether the severity of ACE influences the appraisal of social touch in the BPD group, we calculated a multivariate linear regression model with the CTQ score and attachment closeness as well as its interaction with CTQ score as predictors and the need, liking and importance of social touch as dependent variables. The analyses revealed that the CTQ score predicted neither the need, liking nor the importance of social touch (all P > 0.1). A higher attachment closeness predicted a higher need for touch and a higher liking of touch, but not a higher importance of interpersonal touch towards the members of the social network. It did not moderate the relation between the severity of ACE and the appraisal of social touch. For further details, see Supplementary Tables S10, S11.

Discussion

In the present study, our findings revealed differences between individuals with BPD and HCs regarding their subjective feelings and their objective level of social isolation as well as their appraisal of social touch. Participants of the BPD group reported to feel more lonely and less close to those members of their social networks living without the same household. Moreover, their social networks were smaller and less diverse with a lower frequency of both in-person contacts and virtual contacts with members of their social network living outside their household. Social touch as one fundamental component of social relationships had an altered relevance in BPD in all three investigated facets: individuals with BPD reported a lower need for touch, a lower liking of particularly positive interpersonal touch and a lower importance of touch in the relationships at least towards those members of their social networks living outside their own household. Investigating the interplay between subjective and objective isolation and appraisal of social touch revealed that being objectively more isolated to others as well as a lower liking and lower importance of social touch during social interactions predicted feeling socially isolated. This was true for both, analysing these associations on a general level as well as when investigating this interplay for the participants' actual social networks. Similarly, the burden experienced during “physical distancing” policies was predicted by the evaluation of the role of social touch. However, in contrast to feeling lonely, both groups experienced a comparable burden and beyond the importance of social touch, it was particularly the need for touch instead of the liking of touch that predicted the severity of the burden people experience during the COVID-19 pandemic. When analysing the underlying mechanism, we did not find a history of ACE to predict the appraisal of social touch. In contrast, a higher capacity to feel close to others as one dimension of attachment was associated with a higher need for touch and stronger liking of positive social touch.

Objective and Subjectively Experienced Social Isolation

Compared to HCs, individuals with BPD reported both, feeling lonelier overall, but also feeling less close to members of their social network. This is in line with our hypotheses and confirms previous findings of increased levels of loneliness in BPD in general (57) as well as reduced feelings of being socially included in experimental paradigms (8486). Also, in line with our expectations and previous literature, compared to HCs, individuals with BPD reported smaller and less diverse social networks (6, 7, 10, 11). Our findings also emphasise the particular importance of meeting others in person. Only the frequency of in-person contacts, but not virtual contacts were shown to contribute to the feeling of closeness.

Appraisal of Social Touch

Individuals with a history of BPD differed from HCs in all three assessed facets of their appraisal of social touch. As predicted and in line with literature (62), our data revealed not only a lower liking of pleasant touch in the BPD group, but also identified positive social touch as the domain especially affected in BPD. This is in line with prior findings about a negative bias in decoding positive social cues in BPD (87, 88). Moreover, the BPD group stated that social touch is less important in their relationships towards members of their social network, which fits with previous findings that they prefer greater interpersonal distance (41). Beyond facets of physical proximity investigated in previous studies in BPD, our findings suggest that there is also a lower need for social touch in BPD. One might speculate whether a lower need and a lower importance of social touch are consequences of a lower liking of positively annotated social touch. However, the lower need for touch might alternatively be explained by an individual's denial of this need due to fears associated with physical proximity or shame (25). In this case, another speculative explanation could be that impulsive sexual behaviour seen in some patients with BPD constitutes a maladaptive attempt to satisfy an unmet need for social touch (89, 90). Liking and living affectionate touch to a lower extent than healthy individuals, might also constitute a source for misunderstandings in social relationships, particularly with people who value physical closeness and use touch to provide social support and form interpersonal bonds. Our findings emphasise the need for further studies in BPD investigating the interplay between these different facets of social touch, as well as their effects on impairments of forming, maintaining and benefitting from social relationships.

Appraisal of Social Touch and Social Isolation

A higher liking of positive touch and a higher importance of social touch during social interactions predicted lower levels of social isolation, supporting the role of touch for closeness. However, in contrast to our hypotheses, neither the need, liking nor the importance of touch moderated the relationship between objective and subjectively experienced social isolation. Together with the finding that the frequency of virtual contact was not a predictor of feeling socially isolated, this implies that in-person encounters are beneficial regardless of the possibility to satisfy the need for social touch. Possible reasons could be that meeting others in person allows in a larger extent for joined activities and might mostly, for example a walk, take longer and thus allow a deeper and potentially emotionally more supportive exchange. In addition to being able to see a person's head-to-toe body language and posture when meeting personally, touch could also be helpful as a further social cue to stabilise and deepen social relationships and thus be beneficial for the feeling of social closeness.

Appraisal of Social Touch and Burden Through “Physical Distancing”

In addition to the preregistered main analyses, we investigated whether contact frequencies and the appraisal of social touch are associated with the experience of social distancing policies during the COVID-19 pandemic. Contrary to the suggestion of Preti, Di Pierro (3), individuals with BPD did not differ from HCs in terms of burden through and compliance with social distancing policies. The impairments in interpersonal and emotional domains in BPD did not seem to affect them additionally in dealing with the challenges and consequences of social distancing. However, a high percentage of BPD patients in this study were currently receiving therapeutic treatment. This might be one possible reason for the finding that no extraordinary burden was induced by the “physical distancing” rules, if these individuals had already learnt strategies for coping with loneliness and also received therapeutic support during the pandemic. Another reason could be that due to BPD patients' smaller social networks they experience a smaller change in their social life. Contrary to our hypothesis, the burden through social distancing was not predicted by contact frequencies and this was not modulated by the appraisal of social touch. However, in line with our hypotheses, a higher need for touch constitutes a vulnerability factor during “physical distancing”. Higher need for touch was associated with higher burden, although independently from objective social isolation. In contrast, a higher burden was also predicted by a lower instead of a higher importance of touch. This finding suggests—together with the association of a higher importance of touch with higher levels of experienced closeness—that the existence of close relationships supported by social touch have a protective effect and are less challenged by a transient state during which in-person meetings are prohibited. Whether this holds true when “physical distancing” recommendations are in place for extended periods of time, has to be investigated during the further course of the COVID-19 pandemic.

Appraisal of Social Touch, ACE, and Attachment Style

Contrary to our hypotheses, the severity of childhood trauma did not predict the appraisal of social touch in the BPD group, that is, neither the need, liking nor importance of social touch. However, it can be assumed that the impact of childhood trauma on the appraisal of social touch depends on whether the trauma was related to a lack of physical contact or aversive experiences with social touch (47, 48). In the present study, individuals with BPD reported primarily emotional neglect and abuse as types of trauma. While our findings do not support the role of trauma in alterations of the appraisal of social touch in BPD, further studies are needed to investigate whether there might be associations found in those who suffered especially from trauma associated with social touch such as physical or sexual abuse or physical neglect. Independently from the severity of childhood trauma, attachment closeness predicted a higher need and liking of positive touch, but not a higher importance of touch in actual interpersonal relationships. These findings suggest that the need for and liking of touch correspond to the bodily expressions of the capacity to feel close to others. In contrast, the importance of touch in relationships with members of the social network might be influenced more strongly by other factors such as the social domain and the related importance of feeling close.

Limitations of the Current Study

The present study has some limitations. Some refer to the investigated sample. Due to the overrepresentation of female BPD patients in the health system, we included female participants only. Results can therefore not be generalised to men; we therefore emphasise the importance of replicating this study in a more gender-diverse sample. Moreover, individuals of the BPD group had been formally diagnosed with BPD in the past. BPD participants were included in the study based on the current severity of BPD features rated on PAI-BOR without reassessing their former BPD diagnosis. Due to this group differences might be larger than reported, since some of the BPD participants might not fulfil clinical criteria anymore. However, sample diversity might also resemble diverse BPD presentation. BPD is characterised by frequent alterations between recovery and the reoccurrence of symptoms, which is often linked to a persistence of low levels of social functioning and impairments in social cognition during remission and recovery (87, 9193). This shows the importance of identifying factors that might contribute to the fluctuation of severity in the course of the disorder. Finally, we did not include a healthy control group with exposure to childhood trauma. Therefore a differentiation of the effects of childhood trauma and borderline personality psychopathology was not possible.

Due to the COVID-19 pandemic, we collected data through online surveys. Hence, we focussed on self-report questionnaires. Please note that it cannot be ruled out that these self-report measures might be influenced differentially in both groups by a bias due to socially desirable answers. Further studies are needed extending the measures we used by experimental paradigms using actual touch in addition to rating video clips. While video clips may still be better at simulating the sensual aspect of touch compared to verbal descriptions of scenarios, it falls short of using actual (social) touch. Moreover, further studies are needed using experimental tasks combined with psychophysiological and neural correlates of processing to investigate the different functions of social touch (e.g., communicating social support in distressing situations or showing affection towards others) within different social domains (romantic partners, family, close friends or work colleague). In addition, analysis approaches like hierarchical linear models might be useful for future studies in order to investigate the findings of our ANOVA on the individual level in more detail.

When analysing the relationship of contact frequencies and the appraisal of touch to subjective social isolation and burden trough social distancing, we calculated the regression analyses across the entire sample to cover a broad range of evaluations of loneliness, the appraisal of touch and social isolation. Since both groups differ in these variables, our results might be confounded by group membership although the partial correlation plots suggest that the association are not only driven by differences between groups. Nevertheless, further studies with larger sample sizes are needed that allow to include a diagnosis with BPD as an additional factor in the analyses and take interactions between this factor and the other predictors into account. Moreover, future studies may investigate, whether these associations can be found across larger groups of healthy individuals covering the broad spectrum of loneliness and appraisal of social touch to investigate the interplay between these factors independently of BPD.

Conclusions

Our study contributes to the understanding of impairments of social belonging in BPD by identifying the appraisal of social touch beyond social isolation as a factor associated with the experience of loneliness and reduced closeness towards others. Our findings suggest that it is important to consider different facets of the appraisal of social touch when researching their effect on social isolation as well as perceived burden due to “physical distancing” measures.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because according to European law (GDPR), data containing potentially identifying or sensitive patient information are restricted; our data involving clinical participants are not freely available in the article, Supplementary Material, or in a public repository. Data access can be requested on reasonable demand via the corresponding author. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to anna.schulze@zi-mannheim.de.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Research Ethics Board II of the Medical Faculty Mannheim of Heidelberg University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AS, MBi, KA, and SL designed the study. AS and LW set up the experiment. FU, LW, and AS recruited the sample. AS conducted all statistical analyses and created the figures. SL, MBi, and MBo provided substantive and conceptual feedback on all draft. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation)—GRK2350/1–324164820.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Marie Hamm for her support during the recruitment of the participants, to Hannah Jussli for language editing and to Mona Morbitzer for final proofreading and formatting of the manuscript. We thank all participants involved in the study. For the publication fee we acknowledge financial support by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft within the funding programme Open Access Publikationskosten as well as by Heidelberg University.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.876413/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Baumeister RF, Leary MR. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol Bull. (1995) 117:497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Iasevoli F, Fornaro M, D'Urso G, Galletta D, Casella C, Paternoster M, et al. Psychological distress in patients with serious mental illness during the COVID-19 outbreak and 1-month mass quarantine in Italy. Psychol Med. (2021) 51:1054–6. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720001841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Preti E, Di Pierro R, Fanti E, Madeddu F, Calati R. Personality disorders in time of pandemic. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2020) 22:80. doi: 10.1007/s11920-020-01204-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Quittkat HL, Dusing R, Holtmann FJ, Buhlmann U, Svaldi J, Vocks S. Perceived impact of Covid-19 across different mental disorders: a study on disorder-specific symptoms, psychosocial stress and behaviour. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:586246. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hauschild S, Winter D, Thome J, Liebke L, Schmahl C, Bohus M, et al. Behavioural mimicry and loneliness in borderline personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry. (2018) 82:30–6. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Liebke L, Bungert M, Thome J, Hauschild S, Gescher DM, Schmahl C, et al. Loneliness, social networks, and social functioning in borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord. (2017) 8:349–56. doi: 10.1037/per0000208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Nenov-Matt T, Barton BB, Dewald-Kaufmann J, Goerigk S, Rek S, Zentz K, et al. Loneliness, social isolation and their difference: a cross-diagnostic study in persistent depressive disorder and borderline personality disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:608476. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.608476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bohus M, Stoffers-Winterling J, Sharp C, Krause-Utz A, Schmahl C, Lieb K. Borderline personality disorder. Lancet. (2021) 398:1528–40. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(21)00476-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Clifton A, Pilkonis PA, McCarty C. Social networks in borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord. (2007) 21:434–41. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2007.21.4.434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Lazarus SA, Cheavens JS. An examination of social network quality and composition in women with and without borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord. (2017) 8:340–8. doi: 10.1037/per0000201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. McLoughlin J, O'Grady MM, Hallahan B. Impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on patients with pre-existing mood disorders. Ir J Psychol Med. (2021) 1–10. doi: 10.1017/ipm.2021.38

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Centres for Disease Control Prevention How to Protect Yourself & Others. (2021). Available online at: https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/prevent-getting-sick/social-distancing.html (accessed June 20, 2021).

Google Scholar

14. Eurostat. Internet Activities. (2021). Available online at: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/databrowser/bookmark/d0a39f1a-532d-41fa-8d74-669 aa9aad51ce4e?lang=de (accessed June 20, 2021).

Google Scholar

15. Hunt MG, Marx R, Lipson C, Young J. No more FOMO: limiting social media decreases loneliness and depression. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2018) 37:751–68. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2018.37.10.751

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Moody EJ. Internet use and its relationship to loneliness. Cyberpsychol Behav. (2001) 4:393–401. doi: 10.1089/109493101300210303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Kim J, LaRose R, Peng W. Loneliness as the cause and the effect of problematic internet use: the relationship between internet use and psychological wellbeing. Cyberpsychol Behav. (2009) 12:451–5. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Song H, Zmyslinski-Seelig A, Kim J, Drent A, Victor A, Omori K, et al. Does facebook make you lonely? A meta analysis. Comput Hum Behav. (2014) 36:446–52. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Shah SGS, Nogueras D, van Woerden HC, Kiparoglou V. Evaluation of the effectiveness of digital technology interventions to reduce loneliness in older adults: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Med Internet Res. (2021) 23:e24712. doi: 10.2196/24712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Ooi J, Michael J, Lemola S, Butterfill S, Siew CSQ, Walasek L. Interpersonal functioning in borderline personality disorder traits: a social media perspective. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:1068. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-58001-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Fertuck EA, Grinband J, Stanley B. Facial trust appraisal negatively biassed in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2013) 207:195–202. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Sato M, Fonagy P, Luyten P. Rejection sensitivity and borderline personality disorder features: a mediation model of effortful control and intolerance of ambiguity. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 269:50–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Baer RA, Peters JR, Eisenlohr-Moul TA, Geiger PJ, Sauer SE. Emotion-related cognitive processes in borderline personality disorder: a review of the empirical literature. Clin Psychol Rev. (2012) 32:359–69. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Domes G, Schulze L, Herpertz SC. Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder-a review of the literature. J Pers Disord. (2009) 23:6–19. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2009.23.1.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Buchman-Wildbaum T, Unoka Z, Dudas R, Vizin G, Demetrovics Z, Richman MJ. Shame in borderline personality disorder: meta-analysis. J Pers Disord. (2021) 35(Suppl A):149–61. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2021_35_515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Crusco AH, Wetzel CG. The midas touch: the effects of interpersonal touch on restaurant tipping. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. (1984) 10:512–7. doi: 10.1177/0146167284104003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Cascio CJ, Moore D, McGlone F. Social touch and human development. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2019) 35:5–11. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Field T. Touch for socioemotional and physical wellbeing: a review. Dev Rev. (2010) 30:367–83. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2011.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Jakubiak BK, Feeney BC. Affectionate touch to promote relational, psychological, and physical wellbeing in adulthood: a theoretical model and review of the research. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. (2017) 21:228–52. doi: 10.1177/1088868316650307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Ditzen B, Germann J, Meuwly N, Bradbury TN, Bodenmann G, Heinrichs M. Intimacy as related to cortisol reactivity and recovery in couples undergoing psychosocial stress. Psychosom Med. (2019) 81:16–25. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Jakubiak BK, Feeney BC. A sense of security: touch promotes state attachment security. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. (2016) 7:745–53. doi: 10.1177/1948550616646427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Morrison I. Keep calm and cuddle on: social touch as a stress buffer. Adapt Hum Behav Physiol. (2016) 2:344–62. doi: 10.1007/s40750-016-0052-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. von Mohr M, Krahe C, Beck B, Fotopoulou A. The social buffering of pain by affective touch: a laser-evoked potential study in romantic couples. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2018) 13:1121–30. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Tejada AH, Dunbar RIM, Montero M. Physical contact and loneliness: being touched reduces perceptions of loneliness.Adapt Human Behav Physiol. (2020) 6:292–306. doi: 10.1007/s40750-020-00138-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Durkin J, Jackson D, Usher K. Touch in times of COVID-19: touch hunger hurts. J Clin Nurs. (2021) 30:e4–5. doi: 10.1111/jocn.15488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Golaya S. Touch-hunger: an unexplored consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic. Indian J Psychol Med. (2021) 43:362–3. doi: 10.1177/02537176211014469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Pedrazza M, Berlanda S, Trifiletti E, Minuzzo S. Variables of individual difference and the experience of touch in nursing. West J Nurs Res. (2018) 40:1614–37. doi: 10.1177/0193945917705621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Wilhelm FH, Kochar AS, Roth WT, Gross JJ. Social anxiety and response to touch: incongruence between self-evaluative and physiological reactions. Biol Psychol. (2001) 58:181–202. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00113-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Fineberg SK, Leavitt J, Landry CD, Neustadter ES, Lesser RE, Stahl DS, et al. Individuals with borderline personality disorder show larger preferred social distance in live dyadic interactions. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 260:384–90. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.11.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Schienle A, Wabnegger A, Schongassner F, Leutgeb V. Effects of personal space intrusion in affective contexts: an fMRI investigation with women suffering from borderline personality disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2015) 10:1424–8. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Abdevali M, Mazaheri MA, Besharat MA, Zabihzadeh A, Green JD. Borderline personality disorder and larger comfortable interpersonal distance in close relationships. Pers Individ Dif. (2021) 182:111067. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Battle CL, Shea MT, Johnson DM, Yen S, Zlotnick C, Zanarini MC, et al. Childhood maltreatment associated with adult personality disorders: findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. J Pers Disord. (2004) 18:193–211. doi: 10.1521/pedi.18.2.193.32777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Porter C, Palmier-Claus J, Branitsky A, Mansell W, Warwick H, Varese F. Childhood adversity and borderline personality disorder: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2020) 141:6–20. doi: 10.1111/acps.13118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Devine SL, Walker SC, Makdani A, Stockton ER, McFarquhar MJ, McGlone FP, et al. Childhood adversity and affective touch perception: a comparison of United Kingdom care leavers and non-care leavers. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:557171. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.557171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Trotter PD, McGlone F, Reniers R, Deakin JFW. Construction and validation of the touch experiences and attitudes questionnaire (TEAQ): a self-report measure to determine attitudes towards and experiences of positive touch. J Nonverbal Behav. (2018) 42:379–416. doi: 10.1007/s10919-018-0281-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Maier A, Gieling C, Heinen-Ludwig L, Stefan V, Schultz J, Gunturkun O, et al. Association of childhood maltreatment with interpersonal distance and social touch preferences in adulthood. Am J Psychiatry. (2020) 177:37–46. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19020212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Carmichael CL, Goldberg MH, Coyle MA. Security-based differences in touch behaviour and its relational benefits. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. (2020) 12:550–60. doi: 10.1177/1948550620929164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Chopik WJ, Edelstein RS, van Anders SM, Wardecker BM, Shipman EL, Samples-Steele CR. Too close for comfort? Adult attachment and cuddling in romantic and parent–child relationships. Pers Individ Differ. (2014) 69:212–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Corcoran M, McNulty M. Examining the role of attachment in the relationship between childhood adversity, psychological distress and subjective wellbeing. Child Abuse Negl. (2018) 76:297–309. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hinnen C, Sanderman R, Sprangers MA. Adult attachment as mediator between recollections of childhood and satisfaction with life. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2009) 16:10–21. doi: 10.1002/cpp.600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Lin HC, Yang Y, Elliott L, Green E. Individual differences in attachment anxiety shape the association between adverse childhood experiences and adult somatic symptoms. Child Abuse Negl. (2020) 101:104325. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. McCarthy G, Taylor A. Avoidant/ambivalent attachment style as a mediator between abusive childhood experiences and adult relationship difficulties. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1999) 40:465–77. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Spitoni GF, Zingaretti P, Giovanardi G, Antonucci G, Galati G, Lingiardi V, et al. Disorganised attachment pattern affects the perception of affective touch. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:9658. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-66606-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Selcuk E, Günaydin G, Sumer N, Harma M, Salman S, Hazan C, et al. Self-reported romantic attachment style predicts everyday maternal caregiving behaviour at home. J Res Pers. (2010) 44:544–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Krahe C, von Mohr M, Gentsch A, Guy L, Vari C, Nolte T, et al. Sensitivity to CT-optimal, affective touch depends on adult attachment style. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:14544. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32865-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Bernardon S, Babb KA, Hakim-Larson J, Gragg M. Loneliness, attachment, and the perception and use of social support in university students. Can J Beh Sci Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement. (2011) 43:40. doi: 10.1037/a0021199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Goossens L, Marcoen A, van Hees S, van de Woestijne O. Attachment style and loneliness in adolescence. Eur J Psychol Educ. (1998) 13:529–42. doi: 10.1007/BF03173103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Baer JC, Martinez CD. Child maltreatment and insecure attachment: a meta-analysis. J Reprod Infant Psychol. (2006) 24:187–97. doi: 10.1080/02646830600821231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Swanson B, Mallinckrodt B. Family environment, love withdrawal, childhood sexual abuse, and adult attachment. Psychother Res. (2001) 11:455–72. doi: 10.1080/713664062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Unger JAM, De Luca RV. The relationship between childhood physical abuse and adult attachment styles. J Fam Violence. (2014) 29:223–34. doi: 10.1007/s10896-014-9588-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Agrawal HR, Gunderson J, Holmes BM, Lyons-Ruth K. Attachment studies with borderline patients: a review. Harv Rev Psychiatry. (2004) 12:94–104. doi: 10.1080/10673220490447218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Löffler A, Kleindienst N, Neukel C, Bekrater-Bodmann R, Flor H. Pleasant touch perception in borderline personality disorder and its relationship with disturbed body representation. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. (2022) 9:1–16. doi: 10.1186/s40479-021-00176-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Schmahl C, Herpertz SC, Bertsch K, Ende G, Flor H, Kirsch P, et al. Mechanisms of disturbed emotion processing and social interaction in borderline personality disorder: state of knowledge and research agenda of the German clinical research unit. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. (2014) 1:1–17. doi: 10.1186/2051-6673-1-12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (1994).

Google Scholar

65. Loranger AW. International Personality Disorder Examination: IPDE; DSM-IV and ICD-10; Interviews. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (1999).

Google Scholar

66. Bohus M, Kleindienst N, Limberger MF, Stieglitz R-D, Domsalla M, Chapman AL, et al. The short version of the borderline symptom list (BSL-23): development and initial data on psychometric properties. Psychopathology. (2009) 42:32–9. doi: 10.1159/000173701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Morey LC. The Personality Assessment Inventory: P Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (1991). doi: 10.1037/t03903-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Groves JA, Engel RR. The German adaptation and standardisation of the personality assessment inventory (PAI). J Pers Assess. (2007) 88:49–56. doi: 10.1080/00223890709336834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (1996). doi: 10.1037/t00742-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Hautzinger M, Keller F, Kühner C. BDI-II Beck Depressions-Inventar. Frankfurt: Harcourt Test Services (2006).

Google Scholar

71. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the childhood trauma questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. (2003) 27:169–90. doi: 10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Klinitzke G, Romppel M, Hauser W, Brahler E, Glaesmer H. Die deutsche version des childhood trauma questionnaire (CTQ)—psychometrische Eigenschaften in einer bevölkerungsrepräsentativen Stichprobe [the German version of the childhood trauma questionnaire (CTQ): psychometric characteristics in a representative sample of the general population]. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. (2012) 62:47–51. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1295495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kleindienst N, Jungkunz M, Bohus M. A proposed severity classification of borderline symptoms using the borderline symptom list (BSL-23). Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. (2020) 7:11. doi: 10.1186/s40479-020-00126-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Russell D, Peplau LA, Cutrona CE. The revised UCLA loneliness scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1980) 39:472–80. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Döring N, Bortz J. Psychometrische einsamkeitsforschung: Deutsche neukonstruktion der UCLA loneliness scale. Diagnostica. (1993) 39:224–39.

Google Scholar

76. Nuszbaum M, Voss A, Klauer KC. Assessing individual differences in the need for interpersonal touch and need for touch. Soc Psychol. (2014) 45:31–40. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Lee Masson H, Op de Beeck H. Socio-affective touch expression database. PloS ONE. (2018) 13:e0190921. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0190921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Rabin BS, Gwaltney JM Jr. Social ties and susceptibility to the common cold. JAMA. (1997) 277:1940–4. doi: 10.1001/jama.1997.03540480040036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Suvilehto JT, Glerean E, Dunbar RI, Hari R, Nummenmaa L. Topography of social touching depends on emotional bonds between humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2015) 112:13811–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1519231112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Collins NL, Read SJ. Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1990) 58:644–63. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.4.644

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Schmidt S, Strauss B, Hoger D, Brahler E. The adult attachment scale (AAS)—psychometric evaluation and normation of the German version. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. (2004) 54:375–82. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-815000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Betsch C, Wieler L, Bosnjak M. COVID-19 Snapshot MOnitoring (COSMO): Monitoring Knowledge, Risk Perceptions, Preventive Behaviours, and Public Trust in the Current Coronavirus Outbreak. Psych Archives (2020). doi: 10.23668/PSYCHARCHIVES.2776

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol. (1995) 57:289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Bungert M, Koppe G, Niedtfeld I, Vollstadt-Klein S, Schmahl C, Lis S, et al. Pain processing after social exclusion and its relation to rejection sensitivity in borderline personality disorder. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0133693. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Liebke L, Koppe G, Bungert M, Thome J, Hauschild S, Defiebre N, et al. Difficulties with being socially accepted: an experimental study in borderline personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. (2018) 127:670–82. doi: 10.1037/abn0000373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Renneberg B, Herm K, Hahn A, Staebler K, Lammers CH, Roepke S. Perception of social participation in borderline personality disorder. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2012) 19:473–80. doi: 10.1002/cpp.772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Kleindienst N, Hauschild S, Liebke L, Thome J, Bertsch K, Hensel S, et al. A negative bias in decoding positive social cues characterises emotion processing in patients with symptom-remitted borderline personality disorder. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. (2019) 6:17. doi: 10.1186/s40479-019-0114-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Thome J, Liebke L, Bungert M, Schmahl C, Domes G, Bohus M, et al. Confidence in facial emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord. (2016) 7:159–68. doi: 10.1037/per0000142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Mangassarian S, Sumner L, O'Callaghan E. Sexual impulsivity in women diagnosed with borderline personality disorder: a review of the literature. Sex Addict Compulsivity. (2015) 22:195–206. doi: 10.1080/10720162.2015.1017781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Sansone RA, Sansone LA. Sexual behaviour in borderline personality: a review. Innov Clin Neurosci. (2011) 8:14–8.

Google Scholar

91. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Reich DB, Fitzmaurice G. Attainment and stability of sustained symptomatic remission and recovery among patients with borderline personality disorder and axis II comparison subjects: a 16-year prospective follow-up study. Am J Psychiatry. (2012) 169:476–83. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11101550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Reich DB, Fitzmaurice GM. Fluidity of the subsyndromal phenomenology of borderline personality disorder over 16 years of prospective follow-up. Am J Psychiatry. (2016) 173:688–94. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15081045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Schneider I, Bertsch K, Izurieta Hidalgo NA, Muller LE, Schmahl C, Herpertz SC. Remnants and changes in facial emotion processing in women with remitted borderline personality disorder: an EEG study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2018) 268:429–39. doi: 10.1007/s00406-017-0841-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social touch, loneliness, social distancing, Borderline Personality Disorder, COVID-19 pandemic

Citation: Schulze A, Biermann M, Atanasova K, Unterseher F, Winkler L, Bohus M and Lis S (2022) Social Touch, Social Isolation, and Loneliness in Borderline Personality Disorder During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Front. Psychiatry 13:876413. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.876413

Received: 15 February 2022; Accepted: 30 May 2022;
Published: 23 June 2022.

Edited by:

Macià Buades-Rotger, University of Lübeck, Germany

Reviewed by:

Dirk Scheele, University of Oldenburg, Germany
Oswald David Kothgassner, Medical University of Vienna, Austria

Copyright © 2022 Schulze, Biermann, Atanasova, Unterseher, Winkler, Bohus and Lis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anna Schulze, anna.schulze@zi-mannheim.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.