Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 08 April 2022
Sec. Computational Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic The Development of Biomarkers in Psychiatry View all 11 articles

Predictability of Seasonal Mood Fluctuations Based on Self-Report Questionnaires and EEG Biomarkers in a Non-clinical Sample

\nYvonne Hller
Yvonne Höller1*Maeva Marlene UrbschatMaeva Marlene Urbschat2Gísli Kort KristferssonGísli Kort Kristófersson2Ragnar Ptur 
lafssonRagnar Pétur Ólafsson3
  • 1Faculty of Psychology, University of Akureyri, Akureyri, Iceland
  • 2School of Health Sciences, University of Akureyri, Akureyri, Iceland
  • 3Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland, Reykjavík, Iceland

Induced by decreasing light, people affected by seasonal mood fluctuations may suffer from low energy, have low interest in activities, experience changes in weight, insomnia, difficulties in concentration, depression, and suicidal thoughts. Few studies have been conducted in search for biological predictors of seasonal mood fluctuations in the brain, such as EEG oscillations. A sample of 64 participants was examined with questionnaires and electroencephalography in summer. In winter, a follow-up survey was recorded and participants were grouped into those with at least mild (N = 18) and at least moderate (N = 11) mood decline and those without self-reported depressive symptoms both in summer and in winter (N = 46). A support vector machine was trained to predict mood decline by either EEG biomarkers alone, questionnaire data from baseline alone, or a combination of the two. Leave-one-out-cross validation with lasso regularization was used with logistic regression to fit a model. The accuracy for classification for at least mild/moderate mood decline was 77/82% for questionnaire data, 72/82% for EEG alone, and 81/86% for EEG combined with questionnaire data. Self-report data was more conclusive than EEG biomarkers recorded in summer for prediction of worsening of depressive symptoms in winter but it is advantageous to combine EEG with psychological assessment to boost predictive performance.

1. Introduction

Winter depression is the most common form of seasonal affective disorder (SAD), characterized by depressive symptoms in winter and remission in spring (1, 2). As compared to major depressive disorder, patients with SAD exhibit atypical depression symptoms, especially hyperphagia and hypersomnia, but scoring lower in interpersonal sensitivity and rejection avoidance (3). The condition has been reported in many regions of the world, with 1–3% of adults being affected in temperate climates (4), and being highly relevant in nordic countries with prevalence rates over 12%, e.g., in Alaska, Denmark, Norway, and Siberia (58). The disorder was reported to be occurring over many years for most patients, with full remission within about 9 years being found in 14% of cases, only (9), although a later study suggests higher remission rates (10). Several reports criticize the defined borders between SAD, major depression, and the DSM criteria (11, 12). However, SAD is usually not as severe as major depression but still has socioeconomic implications as it negatively impacts quality of life and was suggested to increase the probability of unemployment (13). Because of the relatively short period of SAD compared to the typical duration of psychotherapy and the long time it takes for serotonine-selective reuptake inhibitors to show an effect it might be wise to start prevention at least two months before onset of symptoms. In turn, this requires early identification of people at-risk to develop SAD. Therefore, the search for characteristics and biomarkers with a high predictive value as well as a better understanding of vulnerabilities for SAD is highly warranted. If we could identify cognitive vulnerabilities for SAD, specific designs for psychotherapy could be developed. Both, the early estimation of the risk for SAD, and suggestions toward an effective psychotherapeutic intervention would be a tremendous improvement of mental health care.

Several attempts have been undertaken to predict sad mood in winter based on psychological examinations or biomarkers measured in summer. The best indicator for a likely occurrence of depression in winter is the individual's report on prior experience of seasonal symptoms, e.g., according to the seasonal pattern assessment questionnaire (SPAQ, 14). The SPAQ is still the most used instrument for estimating subjective experience of seasonal occurrence of depression symptoms. Furthermore, psychological research has considered cognitive vulnerabilities. In patients with SAD, there is a bias toward remembering words of negative valence more likely in the winter than in the summer (15). In addition to remembering negative words more likely, patients with depression also create more false memories than healthy controls and perceive even positive items with a less positive, i.e., more negative valence (16). Individuals with SAD estimate future negative events as more likely to happen (17), and demonstrate a high level of automatic thoughts and dysfunctional assumptions (18) as well as negative attributions (19). Such psychological features, i.e., cognitive-behavioral factors such as increased rumination, automatic thoughts and dysfunctional attitudes were shown to be not only indicative (20) but even predictive for SAD (21, 22). A ruminative response style as measured in fall predicts symptom severity in winter (21, 22) which indicates a predisposition for ruminative processes being mediators for SAD symptomatology. When examining rumination, it is crucial to distinguish between trait and state rumination (23), as induced state rumination predicts negative affect, independent of the extent of trait rumination (24). Recent theoretical accounts (25) and empirical evidence also suggest that increases in state negative affect may subsequently set of increases in state ruminative thinking in an automatic and habit-like way, and are associated with symptoms of depression and depression status (26, 27). Emotional responses were also combined with attention demands in an emotional Stroop task to predict subsequent levels of symptomatology with tests in winter and follow-up in summer (28). Performance in the Stroop task relies on cognitive flexibility, and cognitive flexibility is impaired in depression (29, 30).

Biomarkers have mainly been derived from major biological hypotheses regarding circadian rhythms, neurotransmitters, and molecular genetics (31). Circadian rhythms were suggested to play a role in SAD, where according to the phase-delay hypothesis the patient's circadian rhythm is delayed relative to the daily routine of sleeping/resting and waking/activity (4). Well in line with this hypothesis, SAD is especially common in younger subsamples who are often evening chronotypes, and in general in people with evening chronotype (32). Moreover, in patients with SAD, depressive symptoms are typically worse in the morning (33).

Another approach to identify vulnerability to SAD and, thus, find predictive biomarkers is based on neuroimaging. Since the brainstem is affected by photoperiodic changes, a large study used magnetic resonance imaging to determine a relation between brainstem volume and low mood (34). In this study, a relationship between photoperiod, volume of whole brainstem, pons and medulla, and low mood and anhedonia was found only in women, but not in men. Women with the short 5-HTTLPR genotype who suffered from SAD showed higher 5-HTT levels compared to those who did not suffer from SAD in the ventral striatum, right orbitofrontal cortex, middle frontal gyrus, left supramarginal gyrus, left precentral gyrus, and left postcentral gyrus and this difference was most pronounced during winter (35).

However, as neuroimaging and genetic testing is not widely available, the most convenient approach to predict SAD is a psychological examination. In order to boost accuracy of prediction a physiological marker could be added that is easily obtained at low cost. The electroencephalogram (EEG) is a method that is traditionally used in clinical and research settings, but commercial products for brain computer interfacing, e.g., in the gaming industry raise the hope that soon there will be easy-to-use systems available that can combine the lightweight design of devices used in non-professional settings with the accuracy needed for clinical and research questions. Indeed, it was demonstrated by a limited number of EEG studies in northern countries that EEG-biomarkers correlate with the absence of daylight and with midnight sun, and factors such as responsiveness of the brain to lighting conditions but also sleep were discussed to be the source of this variance (36, 37). The EEG is also indicative for depression and variants of it (38, 39). The earlier mentioned valence effects of memorized visual stimuli are detectable in the EEG (40), further suggesting top-down attentional modulation of emotional memory bias. Broadband lower absolute EEG-power was found in patients with major depression disorder (41), but especially in the theta (42, 43) and alpha frequency band and especially in the frontal cortex (41, 4447). The prefrontal cortex is also involved in rumination (4850). Interestingly, beta and alpha power varies with seasons (51, 52), and so does frontal alpha asymmetry (53). Abnormalities in beta and alpha power as well as frontal alpha asymmetry are also specific for SAD (5458).

In this study, we aim to identify biomarkers in the EEG which, when measured in summer, allow prediction of increased depressive symptoms in winter. We aim to combine these biomarkers with personal characteristics, depressive symptoms, emotional reactivity (mood induction in an experimental task), and cognitive vulnerabilities such as rumination tendencies, thoughts and beliefs, and habitual characteristics of negative thinking to answer the question whether it is beneficial to add EEG biomarkers to prediction models for depressive mood in winter.

2. Methods

2.1. Ethics

We obtained prior approval from the Icelandic National Bioethics Committee on May 28th 2019 (study number 19-090-V1). All investigators signed a non-disclosure contract and written informed consent was obtained prior to inclusion from all participants.

2.2. Research Setting

The study was carried out as a collaboration between the University of Akureyri and the University of Iceland. The baseline assessment was performed between July and September 2019 in the EEG laboratory of the Faculty of Psychology at the University of Akureyri. Follow-up assessments were conducted in October, January, and April 2020 via online questionnaires and telephonic reminders conducted by the team at the Faculty of Psychology, University of Iceland. For the purpose of the present manuscript, only data from the follow-up in January was analyzed.

2.3. Recruitment

Participants were recruited via email to students at the University of Akureyri, as well as via advertisement in social media, directing interested individuals to a webform. Inclusion criteria were the minimum age of 18 years, proficiency in Icelandic, and the ability to give informed consent for participation. For completion of all follow-ups participants were remunerated with a voucher of 4,000 ISK for a local shop.

2.4. Procedure

Baseline assessment took about 120 min. After participants completed informed consent, a digital questionnaire, consisting of 72 custom made questions and the questionnaires as listed in Section 2.5 were answered by the participants. While participants answered the questions, the EEG was mounted with electrolyte containing a mild abrasive in order to achieve impedances below 10 kΩ. Before recordings began participants were shown the effect of muscle movement on the EEG and consequently instructed to keep movements to a minimum and asked to refrain from talking during the recordings.

The first two conditions were resting state measurements which lasted for 3 min, with eyes open and eyes closed respectively, and with the computer screen turned off. The other tasks were presented on a screen based on the Psychtoolbox in Matlab. First, in the emotional picture learning task participants were shown 60 pictures from the OASIS database (59), balanced for negative, neutral, and positive valence and low, medium, and high arousal. Participants were informed that in the subsequent task they would be asked to recall the pictures shown. The task required to indicate whether each picture represented spring, summer, fall, or winter by pressing a corresponding key on the keyboard with the right hand to ensure attention and to prime seasonal concepts. Pictures were shown with an inter-trial interval of 1 s and a variance of 0–10 screen flip intervals during which a fixation cross was presented. All pictures were shown for at least 2 s and otherwise until participants responded via key press. In the following the picture recall task participants were asked to freely recall and name pictures seen in the previous task. Their answers were noted by the experimenter. Subsequently, a recognition condition involved presentation of the pictures from the picture learning task but randomly intermixed with 60 new images, again balanced for valence and arousal. Participants were asked to indicate via keys on the keyboard whether each picture was new or previously seen. Timing of the presentation was the same as in the learning condition. The next condition was a Stroop task where participants were asked to indicate the font color of words displayed on the stimulus computer by pressing a correspondingly colored key on a keyboard. There were 105 congruent trials and 210 incongruent trials, presented in a randomized order, with an inter-trial interval of 1 s and a variance of 0–10 screen flip intervals during which a central fixation cross was presented.

In the final condition, the rumination task, participants received a printed three part form containing questions about their current emotional state and the brief state of rumination inventory (BSRI, 23). All instructions were given verbally through headphones and partly additionally on a screen. Firstly, participants completed part A on the form containing one question on their current emotional state and the 8-item BSRI. Next, an 8 min musical piece was played in order to evoke temporary sadness or dysphoria, and participants asked to freely experience any emotions they might feel. We used a musical excerpt from Prokofiev's “Russia Under the Mongolian Yoke,” remastered at half speed. Prior research has shown that this approach can effectively cause a transient dysphoric mood (6062).

Immediately after the song had finished, participants answered the forms' part B containing one question on their current emotional state. They were then instructed to wait in silence for 5 min for a challenging cognitive task. However, no cognitive task followed but the waiting period served as a free contemplation time in anticipation of a task. In the third and final part of the rumination task participants answered an 8-item BRSI and one question on their current emotional state.

On the day following the EEG recording, participants began the baseline measurement of the studies follow-up phase which consisted of a 4 day long measurement period using the mobile application ExperienceSampler (63) with which mood fluctuations over the course of a day along with activity level, fatigue, and rumination was assessed in questionnaire form. Five measurements were taken at random times each day between 9:00 a.m. and 21:00 p.m.

The three subsequent follow-up intervals, conducted in October, January, and April, had the same form as the baseline measurement with the addition of a 48 question internet survey. The internet survey consisted of the following questionnaires: Patients Health Questionnaire, Rumination Responses Scale-short form, Perceived Stress Scale, and Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (see Section 2.5 for more details). In addition, the survey included questions on recent traveling and use of any depression treatments.

2.5. Questionnaires

We assessed subjective perception of mood and behavioral change with seasons with the seasonal pattern assessment questionnaire (SPAQ, 1). It includes 8-items regarding seasonal change in mood and behavior, pattern of seasonal change, reactivity to different climatic and atmospheric conditions and whether and to what extent those changes affect the individual (64). We used the Icelandic version, which performed compared to a diagnostic clinical interview with a sensitivity of 94%, a specificity of 73%, and a combined positive predictive value of 45% for SAD and subsyndromal SAD (65). The questionnaire is acknowledged as an effective screening tool for SAD, with an internal consistency of α=0.74–0.81 and a test-retest reliability of 0.76 at an interval of 2 months.

As mentioned in Section 2.4, we examined state rumination before and after mood induction with the 8-item BSRI (23). The Ruminative Responses Scale-short form (RRS, 66) was used to measure the degree of trait rumination.

The habit index of negative thinking (HINT, 67) measures in 12 items habitual characteristics of negative thoughts (i.e., automaticity, lack of intent and awareness, difficult to control). In addition, we measured mood with the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ, 68), sleep problems with the Bergen Insomnia Scale (BIS, 69), depression, anxiety, and stress with the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS, 70), positive attitudes toward ruminative thinking with the Positive Beliefs in Rumination Scale (PBRS, 71), chronotype by using the Morningness Eveningness Questionnaire—Revised (MEQ-R, 72), and to what extent people were following habits with the Creature of Habit Scale (COHS, 73). Participants were also asked about their age, gender, handedness, first language, body weight, and height from which we calculated the body mass index (BMI).

Furthermore, we asked about optimism, nutrition, mental or neurological diseases, regularly taken medication, current tiredness, bed- and waketime the night before the experiment, exercise, phase of menstrual cycle in women, and weather, but we did not include the respective data into the present manuscript.

All questionnaires and written materials used in this study were in Icelandic, therefore, only participants who were fluent in Icelandic were recruited for the study. This was ensured by having all recruitment material in Icelandic. In Iceland, 96% of the population speaks Icelandic, with 12.4% of the population being foreign citizens in 2019 according to Statistics Iceland (www.statice.is).

2.6. EEG Recording and Preprocessing

EEG data was recorded with software and hardware from Brain Products GmbH (Gilching, Germany) at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz with an EasyCap in an extended 10-20 system, including 32 electrodes (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, O2, F7, F8, T7, T8, P7, P8, Fz, Cz, Pz, FC1, FC2, CP1, CP2, FC5, FC6, CP5, CP6, FT9, FT10, TP9, TP10) referenced to FCz and grounded at AFz. In addition, lower vertical electrooculogram was recorded.

EEG-data was pre-processed with BrainVision Analyzer (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). First, band-pass filters from 0.5 to 48 Hz with zero-phase shift Butterworth filters were applied. Then, data was re-referenced to common average. Next, an independent component analysis (ICA) was used such that in the backtransform the signals that include eye-blink artifacts would be removed (infomax restricted algorithm). Finally, remaining artifacts were identified and excluded automatically by the following standard thresholds: check gradient (maximal allowed voltage step: 50 microvolt/ms), check difference (maximal allowed difference of values in intervals of 200 ms: 200 microvolt), lowest activity allowed in 100 ms intervals: 0.5 microvolts. The artifacts that were identified in this way were excluded with a time-range of ±200 ms.

The EEG recorded during 3 min of rest with eyes open, 3 min rest with eyes closed, the whole recall session, and the last 3 min out of 5 min of sad mood induction was segmented into equal-sized epochs of 2 s. From the learning and recall conditions 1 s starting at stimulus presentation were extracted, and they were processed separately for negative, neutral, and positive pictures for learning, and in addition for old and new pictures for recognition. From the Stroop task 500 ms from stimulus onset, and congruent and incongruent conditions were processed separately. Thus, in total, there were 15 conditions extracted from the EEG experiment that were submitted to feature extraction.

2.7. Feature Extraction

For all of these conditions and each segment we extracted features based on the multivariate autoregressive model with the functions mvfreqz.m and mvar.m from the BioSig toolbox (74) with model order 10, and partial correlation estimation with unbiased covariance estimates (75), which is an accurate estimation method (76). The multivariate parameters in the frequency domain that can be derived from these transfer functions were computed for 1 Hz frequency steps between 1 and 48 Hz. Only data from electrodes F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, O2, F7, F8, T7, T8, P7, P8, Fz, Cz, Pz was used. The measures that were extracted were the following:

Spectrum: The auto- and the cross-spectrum, which is the Fourier transform of the cross-covariance function (77).

Direct causality: Direct causality as developed by (78); this measure is not computed for each frequency.

Transfer function: Related to the non-normalized directed transfer function (79).

Transfer function polynomial: Frequency transform of a polynomial describing the transfer function. It is related to coherence as the absolute of the squared transfer function polynomial represents the non-normalized partial directed coherence (79).

Real valued coherence: The real part of the complex-valued coherence (80) is an ordinary coherence (74).

Complex coherence: The imaginary part of the complex-valued coherence (80).

Partial coherence: Designed by (81) it's concept is that one channel drives the other channels if the first channel explains or accounts for the linear relation between the other two.

Partial directed coherence: An extended concept of partialized coherence, measuring the relative strength of the direct interaction between pairs of signals (82).

Partial directed coherence factor: An intermediate step between partial coherence and partial directed coherence by adding directionality to partial coherence and including instantaneous causality (82).

Generalized partial directed coherence: In contrast to partial directed coherence, generalized partial directed coherence is invariant against scaling differences between signals (83, 84).

Directed transfer function: Represents information that flows from one region to another over many possible alternative pathways (85).

Direct directed transfer function: Extends directed transfer function by separating direct from indirect causal relations of signals (86).

Full frequency directed transfer function: In contrast to directed transfer function, the full frequency directed transfer function is normalized with respect to all the frequencies in the predefined frequency interval (86).

Geweke's Granger Causality: A bivariate version (87) of Geweke's Granger Causality (88).

Finally, we also included the power-spectral density as a feature, representing band-pass power in 1 Hz frequency steps from 1 to 48 Hz.

2.8. Features and Feature Combinations for Machine Learning

For classification, we considered three situations:

• EEG features only; each EEG feature was used individually, i.e., we conducted for each of the 15 conditions classification with each of the 16 feature vectors as described in Section 2.7.

• Questionnaire data only; We classified participants by a feature vector including their total scores in PBRS, the three mood measurements in the mood induction task individually and also the difference between the first two and the latter two, and the two rumination measurements with the BSRS in the mood induction task, SPAQ global seasonality score, HINT, DASS stress and anxiety, RSS brooding and reflection, sex, age, education, body mass index (BMI) calculated by the participants indication of height and weight, MEQ, BIS, and COHS.

• A combination of each of the EEG features and conditions with the questionnaire feature vector.

2.9. Grouping for Prediction Modeling

For machine learning we divided the sample into the group experiencing depressive symptoms in winter and a control group. For defining the borders between these groups we used the diagnostic criteria according to the DASS-21 subscale for no depression, mild depression, and moderate depression (70). The control group included participants which were not depressed at baseline in summer as well as at winter follow up in January, i.e., showing <10 points on the DASS-21 depression scale at both timepoints. For prediction we did two analyses, for mild and moderate decline of mood, i.e., increase of depressive symptoms. Both groups showed no depression at baseline (DASS-21 depression scale <10). The group with at least mild increase of depressive symptoms comprised those participants who would show a depression score on the DASS-21 of at least 10 points at follow up in January. The group with moderate increase of depressive symptoms comprised those participants who would show a depression score on the DASS-21 of at least 14 points at follow up. Thus, the group with at least mild increase of depressive symptoms overlapped with the group of at least moderate increase of depressive symptoms.

2.10. Machine Learning and Statistical Analysis

We used leave-one-out cross-validation, thus, a model was fitted for each participant to all participants but the left-out participant using the Matlab function fitclinear using a logistic regression as learner. The fitting procedure was performed with a regularization term strength λ of 10−11.We used lasso (L1) penalty for the composition of the objective function for minimization from the sum of the average loss function, with sparse reconstruction by Separable Approximation (SpaRSA) as objective function minimization technique and 10−8 as gradient tolerance. The initial linear coefficient estimates were set to zeros as initial values and the learning rate was constant.

Lasso regularization reduces the number of predictors, identifies important predictors and selects among redundant predictors, which is important in the high-dimensional feature space of EEG biomarkers extracted with the multivariate autoregressive model. As λ increases, the number of nonzero components of β increases. Intuitively, the predictor coefficients β are therefore indicative for each feature's importance to the model and were therefore reported graphically with the results to demonstrate which brain regions/frequency range contributed most to the prediction of worsening of depressive symptoms in winter.

Machine learning results were gathered overall as accuracy (% of correctly classified individuals overall), specificity (% of correctly classified individuals who did not show depressive symptoms in winter, i.e., correctly classified controls), sensitivity (% of correctly classified individuals with depressive symptoms in winter), positive predictive value (PPV; % of predicted cases actually developing depressive symptoms in winter), and negative predictive value (NPV, % of not predicted actually not developing depressive symptoms in winter).

For psychological self-report questionnaires we calculated means and standard deviations separately for controls and the overall group of people experiencing mild or moderate decline of mood in winter, as well as Mann–Whitney U-tests comparing these two groups at baseline. A non-parametric test was chosen because the questionnaire data is ordinal, so parametric tests should not be used.

3. Results

3.1. Sample

A total of 119 participants were recruited for this study and participated in the baseline assessment in summer 2019. Among them, 89 participated in the second follow-up in winter (January 2020). After exclusion of missing data in four participants, 18 participants showed no depression at baseline and mild worsening from baseline to follow up, and 11 among those showed moderate worsening of depression from baseline to follow-up. The control group of 46 participants was free of depression at baseline as well as at follow up.

The control group sample consisted of 40 women and 6 men, while the group of participants with at least mild increase of depressive symptoms consisted of 16 women and 2 men. The odds ratio for gender to suffer from SAD is 1.8 according to (89) justifying an overall overrepresentation of female participants in our sample.

In the sample of controls/participants with at least mild worsening of depressive symptoms in winter, 8.70/0% had completed primary education, only 47.83/50% had higher education entrance qualification, 2.17/11.11% had learned a trade, 28.26/22.22% had completed undergraduate education at a university, and 10.87/11.11% had completed master or doctoral level education at a university. The native language was Icelandic in 95% of the sample, however, all participants were fluent in Icelandic.

Descriptive statistics for the self-report questionnaires, separately for the two groups as well as results from Mann-Whitney U-tests comparing the two samples are shown in Table 1. For some measures there is evidence that the groups are very inhomogenous. For chronotype, as measured by the MEQ, in the group experiencing at least mild depressive symptoms, the standard deviation is twice the mean, whereas for symptoms of insomnia, as measured by the BIS, the standard deviation is even three times as large as the mean.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Self-reported characteristics of the control group and group with worsening of depressive symptoms in winter at baseline.

3.2. Classification Results

For questionnaire data, only, accuracy was 76.56 for prediction of mild and 82.46 for moderate depressive symptoms in winter, with a specificity (accuracy to classify control group participants correctly) of 78.26 and 84.78 and sensitivity (accuracy to identify participants who experience depressive symptoms in winter) of 72.22 and 72.73. Positive predictive value (PPV) for at least mild depressive symptoms in winter was 56.52 and for moderate depressive symptoms it was 53.33. Negative predictive value (NPV) for at least mild depressive symptoms in winter was 87.80 and 92.86 for moderate depressive symptoms in winter. Figure 1 shows the predictor coefficients β for each feature's importance to the model for the prediction of worsening of depressive symptoms in winter for the two classifications. Relative importance of features were highly consistent for the two prediction models. Predictive for worsening of depressive symptoms in winter were low quality of sleep, low BMI, anxiety, stress, habits of negative thinking, eveningness, and a low degree of positive believes in rumination, measured in summer.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Predictor coefficients β for each questionnaires' importance to the model for the prediction of worsening of depressive symptoms in winter to at least mild (DASS depression score ≥10; blue) and moderate (DASS depression score ≥14; red) extent.

When using EEG data, this resulted in 15 times 16 classifications for each condition and each feature used. EEG alone for prediction of mild depressive symptoms in winter yielded best classification accuracy for partial directed coherence factor extracted during the Stroop task's matching condition (accuracy: 71.88, specificity: 80.43, sensitivity: 50). Because sensitivity was at guessing level it is safe to not interpret these results any further. PPV for at least mild depressive symptoms in winter was 50, NPV was 80.43.

EEG alone for prediction of moderate depressive symptoms in winter yielded best classification accuracy for directed transfer function during recognition of previously seen positive pictures (accuracy: 82.46, specificity: 97.83, sensitivity: 73.33%), PPV for at least moderate depressive symptoms in winter was 88.99, NPV was 93.88. Predictor coefficients β shown in Figure 2, showing which frequencies and brain connections were most predictive. The directed transfer function showed a broader network with interhemispheric frontal connections in the delta to theta range, frontocentral connections in the alpha-gamma range, and temporo- and fronto-occipital connections in the alpha-gamma range. Autocorrelations were also identified to be predictive right frontal, central, bilateral temporal, and right occipital in the alpha-gamma range.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Predictor coefficients β for directed transfer function (DTF) extracted during recognition of positive pictures. Coefficients indicate importance of the feature to the model for prediction of at least moderate depressive symptoms in winter. (A) Delta 1–4 Hz, (B) Theta 5–7 Hz, (C) Alpha 8–12 Hz, (D) Beta 13–20 Hz, (E) Beta 2 21–30 Hz, (F) Gamma 31–48 Hz.

EEG combined with questionnaire data yielded best classification accuracy for prediction of at least mild depressive symptoms in winter with an accuracy of 81.25 (specificity: 82.61; sensitivity: 77.78; PPV: 63.64; NPV: 90.48). This result was obtained by most biomarkers (i.e., partial directed coherence, coherence, directed transfer function, direct directed transfer function, full frequency directed transfer function, partial coherence, partial directed coherence factor, generalized partial directed coherence, and power spectral density) and most conditions (i.e., EEG data recorded during eyes closed or open, learning of neutral or positive images, recognition of recognition of new positive pictures, recognition of previously seen neutral or positive pictures, rumination, Stroop match and non-match condition). Predictor coefficients β for directed transfer function are given in Figure 3 extracted during rest with eyes open. Again, frontal and central regions were strongly involved, across all frequency ranges. Most of the frontal involvement was autocorrelative, except for a frontocentral correlation in the higher beta and gamma range. Central correlation was highly pronounced for central-left area to all other areas in all frequency bands.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Predictor coefficients β for questionnaire data combined with directed transfer function (DTF) extracted during rest with eyes open. Coefficients indicate importance of the feature to the model for prediction of at least mild depressive symptoms in winter. (A) Delta 1–4 Hz, (B) Theta 5–7 Hz, (C) Alpha 8–12 Hz, (D) Beta 13–20 Hz, (E) Beta 2 21–30 Hz, (F) Gamma 31–48 Hz.

EEG combined with questionnaire data yielded best classification accuracy for prediction of at least moderate depressive symptoms in winter with an accuracy of 85.96 (specificity: 86.96; sensitivity: 81.82; PPV: 60.01; NPV: 95.24). This result was obtained by several biomarkers (i.e., spectrum, partial directed coherence, directed transfer function, partial coherence, generalized partial directed coherence) and most conditions (i.e., EEG data recorded during eyes open, learning of negative, neutral or positive images, recognition of new positive pictures, rumination, Stroop match, and non-match condition). Predictor coefficients β for directed transfer function are given in Figure 4, showing where in the brain oscillatory activity in a certain frequency band was most predictive for directed transfer function during rumination. Most areas were highly involved in this prediction, but fronto-occipital and temporo occipital connections on the left hemisphere dominated the higher beta-gamma range.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Predictor coefficients β for questionnaire data combined with directed transfer function (DTF) extracted during rumination. Coefficients indicate importance of the feature to the model for the prediction of at least moderate worsening of depressive symptoms in winter. (A) Delta 1–4 Hz, (B) Theta 5–7 Hz, (C) Alpha 8–12 Hz, (D) Beta 13–20 Hz, (E) Beta 2 21–30 Hz, (F) Gamma 31–48 Hz.

4. Discussion

In this study we aimed to identify biomarkers in the EEG and self-reported characteristics which, when measured in summer, can be used for accurate prediction of whether an individual will suffer from worsening of depressive symptoms in winter. We found that indeed, a combination of cognitive aspects and EEG biomarkers allows for a better prediction than the cognitive aspects or EEG biomarkers alone. Prediction accuracy was better for prediction of at least moderate depressive symptoms as compared to mild depressive symptoms, which could be due to the clearer distinction of the group with at least moderate depressive symptoms from the control group. However, since these two groups overlap, and since the statistical power is limited for the group with at least moderate depressive symptoms we limit the discussion to the sample of at least mild depressive symptoms in winter.

4.1. Self-Reported Characteristics and Cognitive Vulnerabilities

We found that low quality of sleep, low BMI, anxiety, stress, habits of negative thinking, eveningness, and a low degree of positive believes in rumination, measured in summer predicted worsening of depressive symptoms in winter.

Insomnia or, more generally, sleep problems were related to SAD in earlier studies (5, 32, 9096). In our sample, the variance of symptoms of insomnia was very high in the group who would develop at least mild depressive symptoms in winter, suggesting that there might be different subgroups, reflecting different paths of vulnerability that is based on the different factors causing insomnia, being either physiological or cognitive-behavioral in nature. On the one hand, increased rumination is linked to insomnia (97), but on the other hand, insomnia might be the symptom of a physiological vulnerability. It has been suggested that in some cases, insomnia could be treated by administration of melatonin (98), and there have also been attempts to treat SAD by melatonin (99, 100). Further evidence links melatonin to the serotonergic system, which in turn, is linked to depression, anxiety, and stress (101). Melatonin treatment alters the expression of genes of serotonergic neurotransmission in a mouse model of SAD (102). More evidence points to involvement of major monoamine neurotransmitters serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine in SAD (103). Therefore, biochemical markers such as cortisol awakening response as a marker for hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function (104) and serotonin-transporter binding (105) have been suggested. In line with the daily rhythm of cortisol, eveningness was found previously to be strongly related to seasonality and SAD (32, 95, 96). Being a serotonin-transporter-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) short allele carrier was found to be a risk factor for developing SAD (1). A genotype-dependent increase in winter of serotonin transporter binding was found to be specific for patients with winter depression (105). However, although the level of serotonin transporter binding is comparable between healthy controls and patients with winter depression during summer, the patient group shows a lower increase from summer to winter as compared to controls (105). The link to the serotonergic system is supported by predictability of relapse during winter based on depressive symptoms during tryptophan depletion in summer (106). Possibly related to neurotransmitter systems, low vitamin D3 levels where also suggested to predict depressive symptoms increase from fall to winter (107).

The fact that melatonin was suggested as a treatment for SAD (99, 100) is closely linked to the finding that melatonin levels underly a strict circadian rhythm. Administrating melatonin at a specific time of the day triggers a change in sleeping time, which can help to ameliorate the so-called social jetlag of the evening chronotype (108). As chronotype changes with age toward more morningness, the vulnerability for anxiety and depression also decreases (109). Also in our sample, the late chronotype was predictive for depressive symptoms in winter, although the group showed a large variance in the respective score. This detail could point toward considerable inhomogeneity of the group in terms of their chronotype. Although being an evening chronotype is a risk factor for developing depressive symptoms in winter, also individuals reporting to be morning or neutral chronotypes in summer might suffer from mood decline during the dark season. Conclusions to be drawn on this end are limited, but it was also reported that chronotype self-reports do vary with the season especially in individuals with winter depression more morningness was measured in the summer than in the winter (110).

Although controls and individuals who would develop at least mild depressive symptoms in winter did not differ significantly by BMI in summer, the highly negative beta values as indicated in Figure 1 suggest that a low BMI in summer can serve as a predictor for worsening of depressive symptoms in winter. The similarity in BMI between the two groups in summer indicates that the relationship between BMI and vulnerability to seasonal mood fluctuations is not too large. The relation between body weight and SAD has been investigated previously. A higher BMI at baseline was found to predict treatment outcome of 6 weeks light treatment (111). As emotional eating and weight gain are associated with SAD (112), our finding seems to be unexpected. However, it was shown recently that the self reported seasonal changes in weight is related to lower plasma adiponectin levels, an indicator for metabolic dysregulation (113). Therefore, it might be that the difference in weight between summer and winter is more relevant, and a lower BMI in summer might be indicative for a larger weight gain. However, this is pure speculation and needs to be addressed in future studies.

Habits of negative thinking were assessed with the HINT, where negative thoughts are characterized by automaticity, lack of intent and awareness, and difficulty to control them (67). HINT scores predicted mood worsening in winter, whereas trait ruminative brooding and reflective pondering did not. This may suggest that people experiencing their self-focused negative thoughts as being triggered more automatically and without intent, awareness, or control, are specifically vulnerable to experience upward shifts in symptoms of depression during winter.

Individuals with SAD also seem to estimate future negative events as more likely to happen (17), which is a concept closely related to the habit of negative thinking as well as the optimism-pessimism as assessed in our study. Endorsement of emotional adjectives and a negative attributional style are elevated in patients with SAD (114). However, in contrast to our data it was previously reported that these cognitive aspects could not be used to predict later symptom levels (114). Also, countering our expectations, a lower score in summer on the PBRS indicated a higher risk for worsening of depressive symptoms in winter. Prior research showed that rumination is linked to depression (115) and that positive beliefs about rumination are associated with ruminative thinking, mediating further a negative association with positive affect (116). While prior research suggests that increased rumination are predictive for SAD (21), we could not confirm this relationship. In our data, a negative predictor coefficient for the PBRS suggested that positive beliefs in rumination would rather protect from worsening of depressive symptoms in winter. This finding is difficult to explain but warrants further investigation. The first four items of the Icelandic translation (e.g., “I need to consider things to realize how I feel.”) of the scale could have been rather interpreted as being indicative for a positive attitude toward being considerate, which might indeed be a protective factor instead of a risk factor.

Furthermore, patients with prior experience of SAD show depressive affect in response to low light intensity stimuli (21), another indicator for emotional responses to darkness. However, as this result is based on prior experience of SAD, it might rather be due to reactivated memories of sad mood during the dark period rather than an indicator for emotional response style. In line with the potential role of memory mechanisms, autobiographical memory style was examined in winter in individuals with SAD (117). It was found that the number of overly general memories that were generated in response to positive cues was related to symptom levels measured during remission in summer (117).

4.2. EEG Biomarkers

Best results for EEG alone were of guessing level for sensitivity for the prediction of mild depressive symptoms in winter, which might indicate that a mild increase of depressive symptoms can not be predicted by EEG features.

EEG alone yielded sensitivity at guessing level for prediction of mild depressive symptoms in winter, which should, therefore, not be interpreted any further.

The frontal involvement and EEG-results in general were more reliable when EEG was combined with self-report questionnaires, emphasizing further the superiority of self-report data over EEG biomarkers. Specifically, the high stability across biomarkers and conditions indicates that EEG combined with self-report data can contribute reliable additional information, but the consideration of the self-report data is crucial.

EEG studies have identified some likely structural and activational irregularities being candidates for the neurological mechanisms involved in depressive tendencies and depressive mechanisms such as rumination. For example, lowered alpha activity in the prefrontal cortex is thought to predict higher tendency to ruminate (50). Inefficient information transfer from the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to the temporal lobe structures might be critical for trait rumination (48). The involvement of the frontal cortex as well as the alpha frequency range points to the role of cognitive control over negative thoughts. High alpha power is acknowledged to reflect active inhibition (118). Therefore, the involvement of alpha activity in the left hemisphere can be interpreted as reduced cortical activity. It was theorized that hypoactivation of the left frontal area leads to ruminative tendencies and consequently to negative emotional interpretation (119). The frontal cortex is also involved in cognitive flexibility (120), which has been reported to be impaired in individuals with depression (29). Specifically, in negative emotional contexts individuals with major depressive disorders were suggested to exhibit ruminative and negative automatic thoughts because they lack cognitive flexibility (30).

An abnormal activation in the lower left frontal cortex has been found to be critical regarding depressed individuals' tendency to pay greater attention to adverse stimuli (41, 47). Abnormalities in the activation or structure of the circuitry of emotion, which includes the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, hippocampus and amygdala have been suggested to underlie depressive disorders (121). In addition to the alpha abnormalities, abnormal synchronization of theta and beta oscillations was suggested to reflect unstable states of cognitive processing, specifically of working memory in individuals with depression (122). Analysing EEG band power beyond the alpha frequency range provided evidence which suggests that decreased theta power might be important during rumination (42), and lower power in the theta range, as well as alpha frequency band has been noted during mind wandering (43). Moreover, increases in the delta band are generally related to pathology such as mental slowing in dementia (123), as well as psychopathology (124).

4.3. Limitations

It was recently shown that machine learning performance in neuroimaging studies of depression overestimate the classification accuracy in small sample sizes (125). This is a well-known phenomenon when the number of features describing the samples exceeds the size of the sample and is not limited to neuroimaging but any modality where the feature vector is long. Certainly, our sample size is very small, as well, especially for the prediction or moderate depressive symptoms in winter. Therefore, we have chosen lasso regularization as an approach to address those problems of high-dimensional feature spaces. The prediction of moderate depressive symptoms might lead to better results because the distinction of the sample is clearer, but it might also be related to the small sample size. Therefore, we chose to not interpret those results any further. However, we also have to question whether the sample composition is representative as women outnumbered men. Although SAD is also more common among women, we had an even higher proportion of women participating in the study, which limits generalizability of the results to men. Future studies need to recruit a significant proportion of men in order to allow for interpretation of gender-specific results.

Another restriction of the study is that it was performed in Iceland, where lighting conditions might not be representative for regions with a lower latitude.

We also need to re-emphasize that this study included the GSS score as a measure for seasonality and other self-assessment questionnaires to measure depressive symptoms, while a clinical interview was not part of the study to ascertain diagnosis of SAD. Therefore, we limit our conclusions to results from a non-clinical sample with mild or moderate depressive symptoms in winter.

4.4. Future Directions

The use of psychological characteristics to predict seasonal affective occurrence can be extended to prediction of treatment response. Negative attributional style predicted poor response to pharmacotherapy in nonseasonal depression but not in seasonal affective disorder (19). It was also reported that psychic anxiety was related to response to light therapy while somatic anxiety was rather related to a negative outcome (126), and that atypical symptoms of depression predict responsiveness to light therapy (127). There have also been attempts to predict treatment outcome in order to determine which patients might respond to light therapy (128), or which patients respond better to light therapy, cognitive-behavioral therapy (129), or a combination of the two (130). When patients exhibit cognitive vulnerabilities, the use of cognitive behavior therapy might be crucial (130). In a later study cognitive vulnerability could not be replicated as prognostic or prescriptive predictor of outcome of light- vs. cognitive behavior therapy, but greater morningness was associated with less severe post-treatment depression in both treatment approaches (129). It is possible that EEG-biomarkers could add to the planning of personalized treatment of patients with SAD, both by helping to select the most appropriate therapy alongside with the consideration of cognitive vulnerabilities, as well as by identifying individuals at risk in order to initiate preventative treatment in a timely manner.

5. Conclusions

Depressive symptoms in winter may be predicted by self-report questionnaire data better than by EEG measures collected in summer, but the combination of features from both domains is advantageous and leads to higher prediction accuracy.

Our findings on relevant EEG biomarkers emphasize the importance of frontal brain regions in the vulnerability for depressive symptoms in winter as well as a broad frequency range.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

We obtained prior approval from the Icelandic National Bioethics Committee on May 28th 2019 (study number 19-090-V1). All investigators signed a non-disclosure contract and written informed consent was obtained prior to inclusion from all participants. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

YH and RÓ: conceptualization. YH (for EEG): methodology. RÓ (for psychological tests): methodology. YH: software, validation, formal analysis, investigation, data curation, visualization, project administration, and funding acquisition. YH, GK, RÓ, and MU: writing—original draft preparation and writing—review and editing. YH and GK: supervision. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The study was supported by the Research Fund of the University of Akureyri (RHA, R1916).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank the BS-students Anna Hjálmeig Hannesdóttir, Elísa Huld Jensdóttir, Máni Snær Hafdísarson, Sara Teresa Jónsdóttir, Sigrún María Óskarsdóttir, and Silja Hlín Magnúsdóttir at the Faculty of Psychology of the University of Akureyri for recruitment and data collection. Also many thanks to the BS-students of the Faculties of Psychology at the University of Iceland, Anton Nikolaisson Haydarly, Elena Arngrímsdóttir, Erla Ástrós Jónsdóttir, Inga Valdís Tómasdóttir, María Lovísa Breiðdal, and Ólöf Traustadóttir to sample the data in the online part of the study.

References

1. Rosenthal NE, Sack DA, Gillin JC, Lewy AJ, Goodwin FK, Davenport Y, et al. Seasonal affective disorder. A description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1984) 41:72–80. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790120076010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Magnússon A, Partonen T. The diagnosis, symptomatology, and epidemiology of seasonal affective disorder. CNS Spectr. (2005) 10:625–34. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900019593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Tam EM, Lam RW, Robertson HA, Stewart JN, Yatham LN, Zis AP. Atypical depressive symptoms in seasonal and non-seasonal mood disorders. J Affect Disord. (1997) 44:39–44. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(97)01447-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Magnússon A, Boivin D. Seasonal affective disorder: an overview. Chronobiol Int. (2003) 20:189–207. doi: 10.1081/CBI-120019310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Booker JM, Hellekson CJ, Putilov AA, Danilenko KV. Seasonal depression and sleep disturbances in Alaska and Siberia: a pilot study. Arctic Med Res. (1991) (Suppl.):281–4.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

6. Levine ME. Seasonal symptoms in the sub-Arctic. Mil Med. (1995) 160:110–4. doi: 10.1093/milmed/160.3.110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Dam H, Jakobsen K, Mellerup E. Prevalence of winter depression in Denmark. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (1998) 97:1–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1998.tb09954.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Magnússon A. An overview of epidemiological studies on seasonal affective disorder. Acta Psychiat Scand. (2000) 101:176–84. doi: 10.1046/j.0902-4441.2000.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Schwartz PJ, Brown C, Wehr TA, Rosenthal NE. Winter seasonal affective disorder: a follow-up study of the first 59 patients of the National Institute of Mental Health Seasonal Studies Program. Am J Psychiatry. (1996) 153:1028–36. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.8.1028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Cléry-Melin ML, Gorwood P, Friedman S, Even C. Stability of the diagnosis of seasonal affective disorder in a long-term prospective study. J Affect Disord. (2018) 227:353–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Dittmann V, Elster K, Graw P, Wirz-Justice A. Seasonal affective disorder: are the DSM-III-R criteria valid? Psychopathology. (1994) 27:291–7. doi: 10.1159/000284886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Thompson C, Raheja SK, King EA. A follow-up study of seasonal affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry. (1995) 167:380–4. doi: 10.1192/bjp.167.3.380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Tefft N. Mental health and employment: the SAD story. Econ Hum Biol. (2012) 10:242–55. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2011.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Murray G, Allen NB, Trinder J. A longitudinal investigation of seasonal variation in mood. Chronobiol Int. (2001) 18:875–91. doi: 10.1081/CBI-100107522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Jensen CG, Hjordt LV, Stenbæk DS, Andersen E, Back SK, Lansner J, et al. Development and psychometric validation of the verbal affective memory test. Memory. (2016) 24:1208–23. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1087573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Yeh ZT, Hua MS. Effects of depressive disorder on false memory for emotional information. Depress Anxiety. (2009) 26:456–63. doi: 10.1002/da.20453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Dalgleish T, Golden AMJ, Yiend J, Dunn BD. Differential predictions about future negative events in seasonal and non-seasonal depression. Psychol Med. (2010) 40:459–65. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Hodges S, Marks M. Cognitive characteristics of seasonal affective disorder: a preliminary investigation. J Affect Disord. (1998) 50:59–64. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(98)00034-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Levitan RD, Rector NA, Bagby RM. Negative attributional style in seasonal and nonseasonal depression. Am J Psychiatry. (1998) 155:428–30. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.3.428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Rohan KJ, Nillni YI, Mahon JN, Roecklein KA, Sitnikov L, Haaga DAF. Cognitive vulnerability in moderate, mild, and low seasonality. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2011) 199:961–70. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182392948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Rohan K, Sigmon S, Dorhofer D. Cognitive-behavioral factors in seasonal affective disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2003) 71:22–30. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.1.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Enggasser JL, Young MA. Cognitive vulnerability to depression in seasonal affective disorder: predicting mood and cognitive symptoms in individuals with seasonal vegetative changes. Cogn Ther Res. (2007) 31:3–21. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9076-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Marchetti I, Mor N, Chiorri C, Koster EHW. The Brief State Rumination Inventory (BSRI): validation and psychometric evaluation. Cogn Ther Res. (2018) 42:447–60. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9901-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Moberly N, Watkins E. Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: an experience sampling study. J Abnorm Psychol. (2008) 117:314–23. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Watkins ER, Nolen-Hoeksema S. A habit-goal framework of depressive rumination. J Abnorm Psychol. (2014) 123:24–34. doi: 10.1037/a0035540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hjartarson KH, Snorrason I, Bringmann LF, Ögmundsson BE, Ólafsson RP. Do daily mood fluctuations activate ruminative thoughts as a mental habit? Results from an ecological momentary assessment study. Behav Res Ther. (2021) 140:103832. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2021.103832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hjartarson KH, Snorrason I, Bringmann LF, Ólafsson RP. Automaticity as a Vulnerability to Depression: Daily Mood-Reactive Rumination and Early-Life Stress in People With- and Without Depression History. (2021). Available online at: doi: 10.31234/osf.io/n3dah

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Spinks H, Dalgleish T. Attentional processing and levels of symptomatology in Seasonal Affective Disorder (SAD): a preliminary longitudinal study. J Affect Disord. (2001) 62:229–32. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00155-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Murphy FC, Michael A, Sahakian BJ. Emotion modulates cognitive flexibility in patients with major depression. Psychol Med. (2012) 42:1373–82. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Deveney CM, Deldin PJ. A preliminary investigation of cognitive flexibility for emotional information in major depressive disorder and non-psychiatric controls. Emotion. (2006) 6:429–37. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Sohn CH, Lam RW. Update on the biology of seasonal affective disorder. CNS Spectr. (2005) 10:635–46. doi: 10.1017/S109285290001960X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Höller Y, Gudjónsdottir BE, Valgeirsdóttir SK, Heimisson GT. The effect of age and chronotype on seasonality, sleep problems, and mood. Psychiatry Res. (2021) 297:113722. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Graw P, Kräuchi K, Wirz-Justice A, Pöldinger W. Diurnal variation of symptoms in seasonal affective disorder. Psychiatry Res. (1991) 37:105–11. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(91)90110-B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Majrashi NA, Ahearn TS, Waiter GD. Brainstem volume mediates seasonal variation in depressive symptoms: a cross sectional study in the UK Biobank cohort. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:3592. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-60620-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Nørgaard M, Ganz M, Svarer C, Fisher PM, Churchill NW, Beliveau V, et al. Brain networks implicated in seasonal affective disorder: a neuroimaging pet study of the serotonin transporter. Front Neurosci. (2017) 11:614. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Soroko SI, Rozhkov VP, Bekshaev SS. Features of seasonal reorganizations of the central mechanisms of regulation in children northerners with different levels of social risk. Ross Fiziol Zh Im I M Sechenova. (2013) 99:1435–49.

Google Scholar

37. Demin DB, Poskotinova LV, Krivonogova EV. Comparison of electroencephalogram changes at cardiovascular training in adolescents of subpolar and polar northern territories. Ross Fiziol Zh Im I M Sechenova. (2014) 100:128–38.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

38. de Freitas SB, Marques AA, Bevilaqua MC, de Carvalho MR, Ribeiro P, Palmer S, et al. Electroencephalographic findings in patients with major depressive disorder during cognitive or emotional tasks: a systematic review. Braz J Psychiatry. (2016) 38:338–46. doi: 10.1590/1516-4446-2015-1834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Höller Y, Uhl A, Bathke A, Thomschewski A, Butz K, Nardone R, et al. Reliability of EEG measures of interaction: a paradigm shift is needed to fight the reproducibility crisis. Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11:441. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Kuchinke L, Fritsch N, Müller CJ. Evaluative conditioning of positive and negative valence affects P1 and N1 in verbal processing. Brain Res. (2015) 1624:405–13. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ding X, Yue X, Zheng R, Bi C, Li D, Yao G. Classifying major depression patients and healthy controls using EEG, eye tracking and galvanic skin response data. J Affect Disord. (2019) 251:156–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.03.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Shim M, Im CH, Lee SH. Disrupted cortical brain network in post-traumatic stress disorder patients: a resting-state electroencephalographic study. Transl Psychiatry. (2017) 7:e1231. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Atchley R, Klee D, Oken B. EEG frequency changes prior to making errors in an easy stroop task. Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11:521. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Gollan JK, Hoxha D, Chihade D, Pflieger ME, Rosebrock L, Cacioppo J. Frontal alpha EEG asymmetry before and after behavioral activation treatment for depression. Biol Psychol. (2014) 99:198–208. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kaiser AK, Gnjezda MT, Knasmüller S, Aichhorn W. Electroencephalogram alpha asymmetry in patients with depressive disorders: current perspectives. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2018) 14:1493–504. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S137776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Park Y, Jung W, Kim S, Jeon H, Lee SH. Frontal alpha asymmetry correlates with suicidal behavior in major depressive disorder. Clin Psychopharm Neurosci. (2019) 17:377–87. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2019.17.3.377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Cisler JM, Koster EHW. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2010) 30:203–16. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ferdek MA, van Rijn CM, Wyczesany M. Depressive rumination and the emotional control circuit: an EEG localization and effective connectivity study. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2016) 16:1099–113. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0456-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Rosenbaum D, Thomas M, Hilsendegen P, Metzger F, Häußinger F, Nuerk HC, et al. Stress-related dysfunction of the right inferior frontal cortex in high ruminators: an fNIRS study. Neuroimage Clin. (2018) 18:510–7. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Putnam K, McSweeney L. Depressive symptoms and baseline prefrontal EEG alpha activity: a study utilizing ecological momentary assessment. Biol Psychol. (2008) 77:237–40. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Machleidt W, Gutjahr L. Ultradian periodicity, diurnal and circannual rhythms in the electroencephalogram. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr. (1984) 52:135–45. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-1002011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Barbato G, Cirace F, Monteforte E, Costanzo A. Seasonal variation of spontaneous blink rate and beta EEG activity. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 270:126–33. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Velo JR, Stewart JL, Hasler BP, Towers DN, Allen JJB. Should it matter when we record? Time of year and time of day as factors influencing frontal EEG asymmetry. Biol Psychol. (2012) 91:283–91. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Allen JJ, Iacono WG, Depue RA, Arbisi P. Regional electroencephalographic asymmetries in bipolar seasonal affective disorder before and after exposure to bright light. Biol Psychiatry. (1993) 33:642–6. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90104-L

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Volf NV, Senkova NI, Danilenko KV, Putilov AA. Hemispheric language lateralization in seasonal affective disorder and light treatment. Psychiatr Res. (1993) 47:99–108. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(93)90059-P

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Teicher MH, Glod K, Ito Y. Hemispheric asymmetry of EEG and T2 relaxation time in seasonal affective disorder (SAD) pre- and post-light therapy. In: SLTBR: Abstracts of the Annual Meeting of the Society for Light Treatment and Biological Rhythms. Wheat Tidge, CO (1996). p. 9.

Google Scholar

57. Passynkova N, Volf N. Seasonal affective disorder: Spatial organization of EEG power and coherence in the depressive state and in light-induced and summer remission. Psychiatry Res. (2001) 108:169–85. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(01)00122-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Volf NV, Passynkova NR. EEG mapping in seasonal affective disorder. J Affect Disord. (2002) 72:61–9. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00425-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kurdi B, Lozano S, Banaji MR. Introducing the open affective standardized image set (OASIS). Behav Res Methods. (2017) 49:457–70. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0715-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Jarrett RB, Minhajuddin A, Borman PD, Dunlap L, Segal ZV, Kidner CL, et al. Cognitive reactivity, dysfunctional attitudes, and depressive relapse and recurrence in cognitive therapy responders. Behav Res Ther. (2012) 50:280–6. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Lau MA, Segal ZV, Williams JMG. Teasdale's differential activation hypothesis: implications for mechanisms of depressive relapse and suicidal behaviour. Behav Res Ther. (2004) 42:1001–17. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Ólafsson RP, Gudmundsdóttir SJ, Björnsdóttir TD, Snorrason I. A Test of the habit-goal framework of depressive rumination and its relevance to cognitive reactivity. Behav Ther. (2020) 51:474–87. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2019.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Thai S, Page-Gould E. ExperienceSampler: an open-source scaffold for building smartphone apps for experience sampling. Psychol Methods. (2017) 23:729–39. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/gxv5b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Murray G. The Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire as a measure of mood seasonality: a prospective validation study. Psychiatr Res. (2003) 120:53–9. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00147-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Magnússon A. Validation of the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire (SPAQ). J Affect Disord. (1996) 40:121–9. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(96)00036-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Treynor W, Gonzalez R, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cogn Ther Res. (2003) 27:247–59. doi: 10.1023/A:1023910315561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Verplanken B, Friborg O, Wang CE, Trafimow D, Woolf K. Mental habits: metacognitive reflection on negative self-thinking. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2007) 92:526–41. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. (2001) 16:606–13. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Pallesen S, Bjorvatn B, Sivertsen NBIH, Hjornevik M, Morin CM. A new scale for measuring insomnia: the Bergen Insomnia Scale. Percept Motor Skills. (2008) 107:691–706. doi: 10.2466/pms.107.3.691-706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther. (1995) 33:335–43. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Watkins E, Moulds M. Positive beliefs about rumination in depression—A replication and extension. Pers Indiv Differ. (2005) 39:73–82. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. (1976) 4:97–110. doi: 10.1037/t02254-000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Ersche KD, Lim TV, E WLH, Robins TW, Stochl J. Creature of habit: a self-report measure of habitual routines and automatic tendencies in everyday life. Pers Indiv Differ. (2017) 116:73–85. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Schlögl A, Brunner C. BioSig: a free and open source software library for BCI research. Computer. (2008) 41:44–50. doi: 10.1109/MC.2008.407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Marple S. Digital Spectral Analysis With Applications. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall (1987).

Google Scholar

76. Schlögl A. A comparison of multivariate autoregressive estimators. Signal Process. (2006) 86:2426–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sigpro.2005.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Murthy V. Estimation of the cross-spectrum. Ann Math Stat. (1963) 34:1012–21. doi: 10.1214/aoms/1177704024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Kaminski M, Ding M, Truccolo W, Bressler S. Evaluating causal relations in neural systems: granger causality, directed transfer function and statistical assessment of significance. Biol Cybern. (2001) 85:145–57. doi: 10.1007/s004220000235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Eichler M. On the evaluation of information flow in multivariate systems by the directed transfer function. Biol Cybern. (2006) 94:469–82. doi: 10.1007/s00422-006-0062-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Nolte G, Bai O, Wheaton L, Mari Z, Vorbach S, Hallett M. Identifying true brain interaction from EEG data using the imaginary part of coherency. Clin Neurophysiol. (2004) 115:2292–307. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.04.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Gersch W, Goddard G. Epileptic focus location: spectral analysis method. Science. (1970) 169:701–2. doi: 10.1126/science.169.3946.701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Baccalá L, Sameshima K. Partial directed coherence: a new concept in neural structure determination. Biol Cybern. (2001) 84:463–74. doi: 10.1007/PL00007990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Baccalá L, Takahashi D, Sameshima K. Generalized partial directed coherence. In: Sanei S, Chambers J, McWhirter J, Hicks Y, Constantinides A, editors. Proceedings of the 15th International Conference on Digital Signal Processing (DSP). Wales; New York, NY: IEEE (2007). p. 162–6. doi: 10.1109/ICDSP.2007.4288544

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Taxidis J, Coomber B, Mason R, Owen M. Assessing cortico-hippocampal functional connectivity under anesthesia and kainic acid using generalized partial directed coherence. Biol Cybern. (2010) 102:327–40. doi: 10.1007/s00422-010-0370-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Kaminskí M, Blinowska K. A new method of the description of the information flow in the brain structures. Biol Cybern. (1991) 65:203–10. doi: 10.1007/BF00198091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Korzeniewska A, Maczak M, Kaminskí M, Blinowska K, Kasicki S. Determination of information flow direction among brain structures by a modified directed transfer function (dDTF) method. J Neurosci Methods. (2003) 125:195–207. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(03)00052-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Bressler S, Richter C, Chen Y, Ding M. Cortical functional network organization from autoregressive modeling of local field potential oscillations. Stat Med. (2007) 26:3875–85. doi: 10.1002/sim.2935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Geweke J. Measures of conditional linear dependence and feedback between time series. J Am Stat Assoc. (1982) 77:304–13. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1982.10477803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Magnússon A, Stefánsson JG. Prevalence of seasonal affective disorder in Iceland. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1993) 50:941–6. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240025002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Albert PS, Rosen LN, Alexander JRJ, Rosenthal NE. Effect of daily variation in weather and sleep on seasonal affective disorder. Psychiatry Res. (1991) 36:51–63. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(91)90117-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Anderson JL, Rosen LN, Mendelson WB, Jacobsen FM, Skwerer RG, Joseph-Vanderpool JR, et al. Sleep in fall/winter seasonal affective disorder: effects of light and changing seasons. J Psychosom Res. (1994) 38:323–37. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90037-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Koorengevel KM, Beersma DGM, Den Boer JA, Van Den Hoofdakker RH. Sleep in seasonal affective disorder patients in forced desynchrony: an explorative study. J Sleep Res. (2002) 11:347–56. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00319.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Johnsen MT, Wynn R, Bratlid T. Is there a negative impact of winter on mental distress and sleeping problems in the subarctic: the Tromso study. BMC Psychiatry. (2012) 12:225. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Tonetti L, Fabbri M, Erbacci A, Martoni M, Natale V. Association between seasonal affective disorder and subjective quality of the sleep/wake cycle in adolescents. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 215:624–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Borisenkov MF, Petrova NB, Timonin VD, Fradkova LI, Kolomeichuk SN, Kosova AL, et al. Sleep characteristics, chronotype and winter depression in 10-20-year-olds in northern European Russia. J Sleep Res. (2015) 24:288–95. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Sandman N, Merikanto I, Määttänen H, Valli K, Kronholm E, Laatikainen T, et al. Winter is coming: nightmares and sleep problems during seasonal affective disorder. J Sleep Res. (2016) 25:612–9. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Ballesio A, Ottaviani C, Lombardo C. Poor cognitive inhibition predicts rumination about insomnia in a clinical sample. Behav Sleep Med. (2019) 17:672–81. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2018.1461103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Low TL, Choo FN, Tan SM. The efficacy of melatonin and melatonin agonists in insomnia - an umbrella review. J Psychiatr Res. (2020) 121:10–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2019.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Danilenko KV, Putilov AA. Melatonin treatment of winter depression following total sleep deprivation: waking EEG and mood correlates. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2005) 30:1345–52. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Nussbaumer-Streit B, Greenblatt A, Kaminski-Hartenthaler A, Van Noord MG, Forneris CA, Morgan LC, et al. Melatonin and agomelatine for preventing seasonal affective disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2019) 6:CD011271. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011271.pub3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Graeff FG, Guimar aes FS, De Andrade TG, Deakin JF. Role of 5-HT in stress, anxiety, and depression. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (1996) 54:129–41. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02135-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Nagy AD, Iwamoto A, Kawai M, Goda R, Matsuo H, Otsuka T, et al. Melatonin adjusts the expression pattern of clock genes in the suprachiasmatic nucleus and induces antidepressant-like effect in a mouse model of seasonal affective disorder. Chronobiol Int. (2015) 32:447–57. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2014.992525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Levitan RD. The chronobiology and neurobiology of winter seasonal affective disorder. Dialog Clin Neurosci. (2007) 9:315–24. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2007.9.3/rlevitan

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Agustini B, Bocharova M, Walker AJ, Berk M, Young AH, Juruena MF. Has the sun set for seasonal affective disorder and HPA axis studies? A systematic review and future prospects. J Affect Disord. (2019) 256:584–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.06.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Mc Mahon B, Andersen SB, Madsen MK, Hjordt LV, Hageman I, Dam H, et al. Seasonal difference in brain serotonin transporter binding predicts symptom severity in patients with seasonal affective disorder. Brain. (2016) 139(Pt 5):1605–14. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Neumeister A, Habeler A, Praschak-Rieder N, Willeit M, Kasper S. Tryptophan depletion: a predictor of future depressive episodes in seasonal affective disorder? Int Clin Psychopharmacol. (1999) 14:313–5. doi: 10.1097/00004850-199909000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Kerr DCR, Zava DT, Piper WT, Saturn SR, Frei B, Gombart AF. Associations between vitamin D levels and depressive symptoms in healthy young adult women. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 227:46–51. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms. (2003) 18:80–90. doi: 10.1177/0748730402239679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Druiven SJM, Hovenkamp-Hermelink JHM, Knapen SE, Kamphuis J, Haarman BCM, Penninx BWJH, et al. Stability of chronotype over a 7-year follow-up period and its association with severity of depressive and anxiety symptoms. Depress Anxiety. (2020) 37:466–74. doi: 10.1002/da.22995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Putilov AA. State- and trait-like variation in morning and evening components of morningness-eveningness in winter depression. Nord J Psychiatry. (2017) 71:561–9. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2017.1353642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Dimitrova TD, Reeves GM, Snitker S, Lapidus M, Sleemi AR, Balis TG, et al. Prediction of outcome of bright light treatment in patients with seasonal affective disorder: discarding the early response, confirming a higher atypical balance, and uncovering a higher body mass index at baseline as predictors of endpoint outcome. J Affect Disord. (2017) 222:126–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.06.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Kräuchi K, Reich S, Wirz-Justice A. Eating style in seasonal affective disorder: who will gain weight in winter? Compr Psychiatry. (1997) 38:80–7. doi: 10.1016/S0010-440X(97)90085-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Akram F, Gragnoli C, Raheja UK, Snitker S, Lowry CA, Stearns-Yoder KA, et al. Seasonal affective disorder and seasonal changes in weight and sleep duration are inversely associated with plasma adiponectin levels. J Psychiatr Res. (2020) 122:97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2019.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Dalgleish T, Spinks H, Golden AM, du Toit P. Processing of emotional information in seasonal depression across different cognitive measures. J Abnorm Psychol. (2004) 113:116–26. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Takano K, Van Grieken J, Raes F. Difficulty in updating positive beliefs about negative cognition is associated with increased depressed mood. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2019) 64:22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Kubiak T, Zahn D, Siewert K, Jonas C, Weber H. Positive beliefs about rumination are associated with ruminative thinking and affect in daily life: evidence for a metacognitive view on depression. Behav Cogn Psychother. (2014) 42:568–76. doi: 10.1017/S1352465813000325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Dalgeish T, Spinks H, Yiend J, Kuyken W. Autobiographical memory style in seasonal affective disorder and its relationship to future symptom remission. J Abnorm Psychol. (2001) 110:335–40. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Klimesch W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res Rev. (1999) 29:169–95. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Disner SG, Beevers CG, Haigh EA, Beck AT. Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:467–77. doi: 10.1038/nrn3027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Kim C, Johnson NF, Cilles SE, Gold BT. Common and distinct mechanisms of cognitive flexibility in prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. (2011) 31:4771–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5923-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Davidson RJ, Pizzagalli D, Nitschke JB, Putnam K. Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Annu Rev Psychol. (2002) 53:545–74. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Li Y, Kang C, Wei Z, Qu X, Liu T, Zhou Y, et al. Beta oscillations in major depression -signalling a new cortical circuit for central executive function. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:18021. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18306-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Cassani R, Estarellas M, San-Martin R, Fraga FJ, Falk TH. Systematic review on resting-state EEG for Alzheimer's disease diagnosis and progression assessment. Dis Mark. (2018) 2018:5174815. doi: 10.1155/2018/5174815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Newson JJ, Thiagarajan TC. EEG frequency bands in psychiatric disorders: a review of resting state studies. Front Hum Neurosci. (2019) 12:521. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Flint C, Cearns M, Opel N, Redlich R, Mehler DMA, Emden D, et al. Systematic misestimation of machine learning performance in neuroimaging studies of depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2021) 46:1510–7. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01020-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. MacKenzie B, Levitan RD. Psychic and somatic anxiety differentially predict response to light therapy in women with seasonal affective disorder. J Affect Disord. (2005) 88:163–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Nagayama H, Sasaki M, Ichii S, Hanada K, Okawa M, Ohta T, et al. Atypical depressive symptoms possibly predict responsiveness to phototherapy in seasonal affective disorder. J Affect Disord. (1991) 23:185–9. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(91)90099-E

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Terman M, Amira L, Terman JS, Ross DC. Predictors of response and nonresponse to light treatment for winter depression. Am J Psychiatry. (1996) 153:1423–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.11.1423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Camuso JA, Rohan KJ. Cognitive vulnerabilities as prognostic predictors of acute and follow-up outcomes in seasonal affective disorder treatment with light therapy or cognitive-behavioral therapy. Cogn Ther Res. (2020) 44:468–82. doi: 10.1007/s10608-020-10086-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Sitnikov L, Rohan KJ, Evans M, Mahon JN, Nillni YI. Cognitive predictors and moderators of winter depression treatment outcomes in cognitive-behavioral therapy vs. light therapy. Behav Res Ther. (2013) 51:872–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: seasonal mood fluctuations, EEG biomarkers, cognitive vulnerabilities, prediction, machine learning, seasonal affective disorder winter depression

Citation: Höller Y, Urbschat MM, Kristófersson GK and Ólafsson RP (2022) Predictability of Seasonal Mood Fluctuations Based on Self-Report Questionnaires and EEG Biomarkers in a Non-clinical Sample. Front. Psychiatry 13:870079. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.870079

Received: 05 February 2022; Accepted: 14 March 2022;
Published: 08 April 2022.

Edited by:

Masahiro Takamura, Shimane University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Peter Watson, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom
Axel Steiger, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Höller, Urbschat, Kristófersson and Ólafsson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yvonne Höller, eXZvbm5lJiN4MDAwNDA7dW5hay5pcw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.