- 1Neuroscience and Behavioral Disorders Program, Duke-NUS Medical School, Singapore, Singapore
- 2School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
- 3Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore, Singapore
- 4Department of Psychiatry, Singapore General Hospital, Singapore, Singapore
Social dysfunction is a key characteristic of autism. Determining and treating autism-related social deficits have been challenging. The medical model views interpersonal difficulties in autism as a localized set of deficits to be managed, whereas the neurodiversity movement calls for the accommodation of differences by the larger community. One common assumption underlying these perspectives is a misalignment in social behaviors between autistic individuals and neurotypicals. This paper reviews and interrogates current perspectives on social functioning in autism to uncover the intricacies of such a notion. Even though extant literature has alluded to a misalignment in social behaviors between autistic and neurotypical individuals, it is uncertain where this disparity lies. Implications for future research and practice are discussed.
Introduction
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a heterogenous group of neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by social dysfunction and restricted, stereotyped behaviors [Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5); (1)]. Social deficits range from lack of social-emotional reciprocity and poor nonverbal communication to difficulties in developing and maintaining relationships. Comorbidities such as intellectual disability and anxiety disorders are common (2–5). Worldwide prevalence estimates of ASD are highly variable and rising, i.e., 0.08 – 9.3% (6). About one in 54 children in the United States were identified with ASD, with ASD being 4.3 times more common in boys than in girls (7). Overall prevalence rate of ASD is estimated at 0.36% across Asia (8).
Individuals living with ASD have substantial mental healthcare needs. Psychiatric service utilization and use of psychotropic medications to manage comorbid psychiatric conditions are high (9). Notably, there is no cure at present for core ASD symptoms of social deficits and stereotyped behaviors. Early structured and targeted behavioral interventions may be helpful only to reduce these symptoms and maximize capacities for daily functioning (10). Besides, ASD has been associated with significant healthcare burden. Several studies have reported on the high direct and indirect costs of managing the disorder (11–14). Disability-adjusted life-years for ASD have increased steadily from 1990 to 2016 (15), indicating a growing burden and poorer quality of life among sufferers.
Issues regarding ASD are exacerbated by an evolving nosology that remains fraught with longstanding difficulties. Problems with characterizing ASD have been largely attributed to the lack of a single known etiological pathway, a behavioral focus in diagnosis, and heterogenous phenotypic or behavioral manifestations. At present, ASD is understood to involve multiple possible factors that lead to an arguably similar behavioral outcome (16). Still, no one individual with ASD behaves the same way as another. If no single set of etiological factors or behavioral outcomes defines ASD, what makes an individual autistic? How does the clinician conclude based on reported or observed behaviors that one has ASD? Existing measurement tools are inadequate in discriminating marginal cases or translating quantitative outcomes to clinical diagnosis (17). Accurate diagnoses stay hampered by variability in clinical characteristics and limited access to multidisciplinary assessments (18). Besides, differential prognoses affect the utility of an ASD diagnosis (19). Even though theories and models have been proposed to account for its etiology (20, 21), they remain unintegrated, contentious, or subject to further validation.
Notwithstanding the enduring backdrop of challenges, interpersonal communication difficulties remain a central feature of ASD. Recently, the neurodiversity movement, which opposes the deficit-based medical model and propounds the view that ASD and other neurological impairments are normal human differences, has gained traction. This approach argues for a shift from treatment to an acceptance of characteristic differences between ASD and neurotypicals. Gillespie-Lynch et al. (22) conducted a survey involving both autistic and non-autistic persons. They found that autistic individuals perceived less importance in finding a cure and were inclined to view ASD as a biological difference rather than a deficit. However, the distinction between ASD-as-difference and ASD-as-deficit appears to be more complex: autistic and non-autistic persons did not differ in terms of negative emotions about and having support for ASD (23). Consequently, a deficit-as-difference model that acknowledges overlaps between the medical paradigm and neurodiverse view has been proposed.
While the medical model and neurodiversity movement seem to contend with how ASD should be addressed, a closer look suggests that these two approaches operate at disparate societal levels. The medical model responds to ASD as a localized set of deficits to be cured or managed, whereas the neurodiversity movement calls for ASD to be accommodated by the larger community. Regardless, one common assumption is a misalignment in social behaviors between autistic individuals and neurotypicals. This paper reviews and interrogates current perspectives on social functioning in ASD to uncover the intricacies of such a notion.
Perspectives From Neuroscience and Biology
Biological and genetics research in ASD have centered primarily on establishing genetic links to or risk factors for ASD and related traits. Genome-wide association studies found ASD to significantly correlate with deleterious de novo mutations (24, 25). Heritability has been indicated in family studies investigating unaffected relatives' genetic liability to ASD-related social characteristics, such as language irregularities, aloofness, rigidity, hypersensitivity to criticism, and reduced number and quality of friendships (26–28). Animal models help dissect the specific roles of genetic and environmental factors in ASD pathogenesis, untangle the relationships between social behavior and altered genes, genetic expression, or brain anatomy and function, as well as provide a means to test effects of pharmacotherapy (29–31). A range of animal models including mouse, flies, and primates has been used to improve translational outcomes (32, 33). While they remain highly integral to establishing links between ASD phenotypes and genetic, epigenetic, environmental, and neurobiochemical factors, these studies have assumed ASD a priori or investigated aspects of social functioning on proxy measures or species. Applications of drug discoveries through animal models are challenged by small and heterogeneous human clinical samples (34). How findings translate to the actual interpersonal functioning of individuals with ASD are subject to cautious interpretations.
Neuroimaging and behavioral studies complement animal studies and reveal added insights into the interpersonal deficits or functioning among autistic persons. In general, such efforts support the view that core social deficits are associated with differences in the brain or cognitive function. These include structural and functional abnormalities in “social brain” correlates such as the cerebellum, amygdala, inferior frontal gyrus, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (35–37). In the examination of higher order social cognitive processes, ASD social deficits have also been linked to variabilities in joint attention, social orienting, theory of mind, empathy, and eye gaze behavior (38–44). Critically, the “social brain” that supports these social cognitive processes is more complex than what has been described so far. It implicates a distributed collection of neural networks and a highly intricate, multilevel neurobiochemical process that influence perception, emotion, motivation, and executive function (45). Abnormalities can occur at any point or neural circuit in this process, ranging from neuroanatomical and genetic aberrations to deficits in the oxytocin or dopaminergic systems, yet contribute to a similar ASD phenotypic profile (46). The “social brain” largely overlaps with the brain reward circuit, but isolating the neural circuitry or variants relevant to ASD-related social deficits has been challenging.
Overall evidence in neuroimaging and behavioral studies are mixed. Guillon et al. (47) conducted a review of eye-tracking studies and failed to find consistent support for deficits in social orienting or eye gaze in ASD. Neuroimaging outcomes of ASD have also not been replicated, as ASD plausibly implicates “several large-scale neurocognitive networks” that are yet unknown (48). Intranasal oxytocin that has been widely advocated to modulate neural mechanisms and thus improve social behaviors in ASD is found ineffective alone and is likely only effective when implemented in appropriate contexts, such as alongside behavioral therapy (49). Furthermore, clinical neuroimaging has only been performed in high functioning ASD patients, for which FMRI scanning was acceptable. Heterogeneity in age, phenotypic expression of ASD, psychotropic medications, and/ or participation in behavioral therapy programs constitute another major source of bias in these neuroimaging studies.
Advances in neuroscience and biology have shown that ASD is not a result of single gene or a fixed constellation of genetic or neurological differences. Constantino (50) articulated the complexities involving genetic bases and phenotypic expression of ASD, positing that ASD is an aggregation of multiple early behavioral susceptibilities with a genetic basis. Neuroimaging and behavioral studies also found evidence for the heterogenous nature of brain anatomy and cognitive function in ASD across the developmental lifespan (51, 52). Curiously, interactions between cognition and socially relevant factors are intricate and variable. For instance, executive function accounted for theory of mind but not verbal communication in children with ASD (53). Mazefsky et al. (54) examined first-degree relatives of individuals with high functioning ASD and found that family history of shyness and depression predicted autistic persons' adaptive and socialization behavior. Determining ASD-specific or non-specific social behavior, including whether social functioning in ASD is poor, requires a sophisticated analysis of dynamic multilevel factors.
Research on the broad autism phenotype show that core ASD features lie on a continuum and can be observed in neurotypicals (55, 56). Accordingly, even if social deficits can be defined, they are neither a sufficient nor an exclusive feature of ASD. It is worth noting that other psychiatric diagnoses also entail difficulties in social interaction (57–60). What constitutes social dysfunction? What part of it makes individuals autistic? Extant neuroscientific evidence reveals the convolutedness, not the essence, of social functioning in ASD. They have worked from the premise, rather than show, that social functioning in autistic persons is misaligned or problematic.
Perspectives From Intervention Research
Broadly, treatment for ASD builds upon the neurobiological underpinnings of ASD. Pharmacological treatments for ASD have been proposed based on functional hypotheses or the repurposing of existing compounds through an empirical approach. Given the prevailing lack of agreement in etiology, there is no pharmacotherapy that effectively addresses core ASD challenges. Medicine for ASD is often for commonly co-occurring symptoms apart from social functioning. For example, risperidone and aripiprazole have been used to mitigate agitation and irritability in ASD (61, 62). Even though these symptoms likely interfere with positive social interactions, suppressing challenging behaviors does not necessarily equate to improved interpersonal functioning. Little is known about the relationships between aberrant behaviors and interpersonal difficulties. Where core ASD deficits were examined, there is little to no evidence to suggest that pharmacotherapy alleviates difficulties in social interactions (63, 64). Unlike behavioral symptoms like agitation, social functioning is conceivably more than a single behavioral problem to be tackled. Social challenges have also been assumed a priori. Until the neural substrates of ASD-related social deficits are identified, it remains uncertain whether pharmacotherapy targeting these substrates would be efficient.
Perhaps unsurprisingly then, behavioral therapies have been propounded as the mainstay treatment to minimize ASD-specific social challenges for better independent living. These therapies target functional aspects of social life, including self-expression, emotional awareness, and social problem-solving. Applied behavior analysis and cognitive behavior therapy have shown to improve socially relevant behaviors through reinforcement and/ or explicit skills training (65–67). While interventions have brought forth favorable changes in social functioning, it is unclear which strategies or mechanisms were responsible (68). Neuroimaging studies that investigated the neural mechanisms implicated in treatment response demonstrate variable neural responses to pivotal response treatment [i.e., increased activation in the reward system for those exhibiting hypoactivation vs. decreased activation in subcortical regions for those exhibiting hyperactivation; (69)], as well as variable improvements in EEG activity toward social vis-à-vis nonsocial stimuli between the Early Start Denver Model and a community intervention (70).
While studies that complement intervention evaluation with neuroimaging methods are promising (71), behavioral interventions encompass complex exchanges among participants, therapists, setting, expectations, strategies, intellectual capacity, and actual interactions, which have been difficult to tease apart. Where interventions were targeted, improvements on pertinent behaviors were mixed (72, 73). Interventions may have been recommended to encourage adaptive interpersonal behaviors in ASD, but methodological constraints limit inferences that can be drawn regarding improving social dynamics outside of therapy. To this end, novel early intervention programs in more “naturalistic” conditions, such as parent-implemented programs (74, 75), have been developed to facilitate generalization of outcomes to everyday life.
Although behavioral interventions approach ASD as prima facie set of social deficits to be addressed, not all methods appear to locate social dysfunction within the autistic individual. Many studies have recognized the importance of involving those closely associated with autistic persons in intervention (76–78), suggesting that social dysfunction in ASD implicates unaffected immediate others. This intersubjective locus of social functioning corroborates with how therapist-client relationships affect ASD treatment outcomes (79, 80). It is also consistent with findings on animal-assisted occupational therapies that indicate differential interactional preferences among individuals with ASD (81–83). Contrary to conventional belief regarding social behavior in ASD, autistic persons were found able to interact positively with animals. It is thus conceivable that social dysfunction is not an insular characteristic of ASD but a situated and dynamic inter-person problem implicating both autistic and non-autistic individuals.
The situated perspective of social functioning is underscored in implementation of school-based programs for ASD. ASD is often diagnosed early in children, and schools are children's major social participation apart from familial homes. In an elaborate five-year social skills training project, Crawford et al. (84) found success involving autistic youths, their typically developing peers, parents, and teachers. This School/Community/Home intervention model purports that ASD social behaviors can only be normalized by surrounding them with neurotypical peers in the same context wherein the behaviors were to be enacted. Therefore, intervention efforts have focused on inculcating autistic persons with adaptive interpersonal skills and expanding their networks with non-autistic peers in the school environment. School-based interventions have also been delivered to younger children, albeit highly individualized with video models of neurotypical peers (85).
While outcomes on social initiation, response, and interaction have been laudable (86), studies involving school-based interventions evaluated small sample sizes, were typically led by researchers, and were variable in components and degree of peer engagement. Participants with ASD were also high functioning or without cognitive deficits. When implications of intellectual capacity were examined, cognitive ability and not ASD influenced adaptive outcomes in real-life settings (87). These limit generalizability of findings to all autistic individuals as well as inferences that can be made on the eventual effectiveness of peer-mediated or school-based programs run by staff. In addition, adaptive social functioning in ASD appears to require sustained relational engagement, adequate intellectual capacity, and supportive interpersonal contexts. Explicating how these factors align social dynamics or preferences seems crucial to understanding social dysfunction in ASD, as well as to developing effective ASD interventions.
The ambiguity surrounding ASD etiology, diagnostics, and treatment have inadvertently supported an exploration of alternative treatment modalities. These include, but are not limited to, play and creative arts approaches (88–92) and technology-mediated interventions (93–97). Such interventions are premised on being developmentally appropriate (e.g., play, visual artmaking) or innovative and culturally relevant (e.g., digital games). Unfortunately, findings on alternative modalities are likewise constrained by methodological limitations and inconsistent results on social outcomes (98–100). Nevertheless, the expansion of intervention research into these diverse domains reflects a growing view that addressing ASD social dysfunction requires more than targeting localized social deficits. It alludes to a tacit understanding of materiality, an engagement of the perpetual dialectic between autistic individuals and their situated environments, which warrants empirical clarification.
Perspectives From Quality-of-Life, Cross-Cultural, and Patient-Outcome Studies
Quality-of-life (QoL) studies inquire autistic individuals' well-being, including social functioning, in everyday life and are less concerned with diagnostics and maladaptive traits. QoL measures may be domain-specific or -diverse and are often quantitative and based on parental or caregiver reports. Positive QoL in ASD has been associated with regular meaningful engagement (101) and adaptive social behaviors (102). Kuhlthau et al. found poorer social functioning but not school functioning in children with ASD. Differential findings could point to the influence of socializing opportunities and environmental adjustments made to cater to ASD needs. Parents of children with ASD have reported issues of lower school attendance, religious participation, and other organized activities (103). Yet, findings are limited to parent-reports, and it is uncertain whether these children perceived their reduced social participation poorly. Although bullying has also been reported, bullying is a societal issue that is confounded by factors beyond ASD-specific social deficits (104–106). Regardless, QoL studies help quantify or delineate some areas of real-life social functioning pertinent to ASD. While they are inherently bound by the measures used, variables investigated, and context under inquiry, QoL studies provide insights into the effects of ASD on actual non-clinical interpersonal situations.
Cross-cultural studies seek to reveal cultural impacts on ASD characterization or functioning. By implication, the dissimilarities and universal characteristics of ASD across different cultures or countries can be illuminated. Autistic children from Britain, Israel, and Sweden showed similar deficits in emotion recognition compared to neurotypical counterparts (107). Sipes et al. (108) found greater positive social skills, but no differences in hostile or inappropriate social behavior, in autistic children from the United States as compared to those from the United Kingdom. In another study, Taiwanese with ASD had more limited social participation than the Australian counterparts, with social participation being affected by gender, ASD symptom severity, and social anxiety (109). Sotgiu et al. (110) compared multiple psychosocial variables between Cuban and Italian children with and without ASD. They showed that Italian children had a wider social network, less frequent contact within network, and fewer multifunctional figures. No differences were found on mother-child attachment and cognitive or emotional competence. Apparently, most cross-cultural research has suspended the diagnostic assignment of valence to social functioning by establishing relationships among socially or culturally relevant factors.
Such studies often bring forth more questions than answers, as sociocultural factors are circumscribed and fail to serve as decisive explanations for variable ASD characteristics. Indeed, culture is a broad construct to be operationalized by selected measurable factors; overlaps among ASD-specific social behaviors, adaptive social functioning, and culturally situated social practices also remain fundamentally indistinguishable. There is some indication that ASD-specific social behaviors are normal in certain cultures. In Korea, cultural factors such as mainstream school structure may be befitting for autistic children, and ASD-related characteristics are attributable to mother-child interactions (111). ASD appears to be embedded within cultures, especially since cultures that emphasize communality regard autistic challenges as a collective problem of difference rather than disability (112). This normative perspective is further undergirded by social stigma research. Stigma has been found to predict greater camouflaging by autistic individuals to gain social acceptance and at the expense of the autistic person's psychological well-being (113–115). Depending on how social functioning is characterized, social functioning may be maladaptive from the perspective of the non-autistic world but not from that of the autistic individual.
Patient-outcome studies clarify the autistic individual's perspective on social functioning. Conclusions are mixed as individuals with ASD have reported both satisfactory and unsatisfactory social lives (116, 117). Consistent with stigma research, people living with ASD indicated poor social lives due to lack of others' understanding (118). In this interpretive phenomenological study, autistic individuals expressed that they valued familial-based or one-to-one support, desired more real-world practice, and did not find social communication interventions helpful. These subjective reports provide some indications of misalignment between autistic persons and others. However, misalignment has been indicated in terms of perceived individual challenges or intervention approaches rather than actual social behaviors. Parental stress has been attributed not to social interaction or communication deficits but to inadequate formal support, poor parental coping skills, autistic child's hyperactivity, challenging externalizing behaviors, and regulatory issues (119–121). Moreover, patient-outcome studies have been largely anecdotal in nature. Sample sizes are small, and participants evaluated are high functioning. It would be interesting to explore the lived experience involving a larger and more representative sample of both autistic and neurotypical individuals. By examining multiple stakeholders, the enduring perception of misalignment in social behaviors between autistic individuals and others can be better expounded.
Discussion
The present interrogation reveals intricate perspectives on social functioning in ASD. Neuroscience and biology suggest that ASD is associated with structural or functional differences that could adversely impact social functioning and other clinical symptoms; yet interventions that target these anomalies yield mixed or modest interpersonal outcomes. Interventions are also highly variable in component and principle. Patient-outcome studies demonstrate how individuals with ASD find existing interventions unremarkable and could benefit from greater understanding among society. In addition, cross-cultural and QoL research purport a nuanced and pragmatic view of social functioning in ASD, indicating a need to examine ASD within its idiosyncratic sociocultural context. Even though the current literature alludes to a misalignment in social behaviors between autistic individuals and their community or society, it is uncertain where this disparity lies and how it is problematic. Consequently, evidence is not definitive to inform intervention. Research regarding ASD has come far, and much more is required.
Expanded View in Basic Research
Neuroscience and biology continue to be key to informing ASD etiology, diagnosis, and treatment. Even though methodologies inquiring lived experiences seem compelling, participating individuals are often limited to those able to communicate their perspectives. This excludes many others on the spectrum with low intellectual and communicative abilities. Jaarsma and Welin (122) noted the inherent bias within the neurodiversity movement and argued for a narrow conception of neurodiversity. Specifically, high functioning ASD can be considered a difference that deserve equal rights and respect, but low functioning ASD remains a disability to warrant help. Therefore, basic research is essential for an inclusive understanding of the entire autism spectrum.
Investigation of intersubjective misalignment between autistic individuals and others will be critical insofar as ASD is characterized primarily by social deficits. As shown, basic research has assumed “social deficits” a priori, operationalizing them using singular proxy measures (e.g., joint attention as marker for social deficit) or a predetermined constellation of theoretically derived variables (e.g., social brain correlates). This implicit notion belies any difficulty related to social misalignment between autistic individuals and others. Is there a misalignment? If so, what is this misalignment and how does it produce interpersonal challenges? Thus, a more fundamental question appears to lie in defining and determining social dysfunction, rather than establishing a link between ASD and social dysfunction. This endeavor is challenging but not impossible. Measurement of discrepancy between parents' and autistic individuals' perceived social functioning has shown to be systematically meaningful (123). To expand beyond “calculating heritability estimates or conducting time-locked correlational analyses” of ASD deficits, Meek et al. (124) articulated the viability of a conceptual model that examines gene-environment correlations and their multiplier effect on social trajectories. Here, social functioning is viewed as process- and time-dependent, and its mechanisms studied through combined analyses of gene-environment correlations, social behaviors over time, as well as genetic, parenting, and school environment factors. Such inter-person, dynamic views could help discriminate misaligned social behaviors in context, whose impacts may or may not require intervention.
Advancing Intervention Studies Through People-First Research
An expanded perspective is likewise necessary for the advancement of intervention research. Current treatment approaches are inadequate in improving socially relevant aspects of ASD and in appealing to autistic individuals. Therapeutic needs also differ depending on developmental stages or parental and familial goals (125). Therefore, multiple stakeholders should be considered in treatment design to address core interpersonal challenges. Malinverni et al. (126) explored a participatory design approach involving not only clinical experts but also children with ASD. Though preliminary, their approach was feasible, and intervention showed indications of improved social initiation in autistic children.
The neurodiversity movement recognizes affected individuals as experts of their own diagnoses and experiences. It also involves a perspective change from positivist (“what are social deficits”) to constructivist (“who judges what adaptive social functioning is”), which has fueled considerable debate (127). In Autism in Translation (128), Weisner discussed the applicability of psychological and medical anthropology research in integrating neurobiological and sociocultural perspectives of ASD. Nonetheless, research in this area is nascent. Interpellation of individuals with ASD will continue to rely on historically based medical models of codification that likely keep on evolving. Diagnoses remain crucial at present to safeguard interventions that support ASD needs. Importantly, people-first approaches need not preclude a positivist or objective examination of ASD features that would warrant clinical help. Even as a paradigm shift toward societal acceptance is apparent, a paradigm expansion seems more appropriate for current research and practice purposes.
Our review did not elucidate how restricted, repetitive behaviors central to ASD might contribute to social functioning. Ideally, these must be examined alongside “social deficits” to present a comprehensive view. We also did not exhaust anthropological findings that shed light on the phenomenological and meaning construction of ASD. Although we questioned the a priori treatment of social dysfunction in ASD across studies, this does not mean that respective conclusions are suspect. In fact, such a presumption is essential to render scientific inquiry possible. Ultimately, our review brings to fore a longstanding assumption associated with ASD research and practice. It is our intention that future research would explicate this presumption through greater interdisciplinary efforts or enabling an intersubjective locus of social functioning involving both autistic individuals and the non-autistic world.
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author Contributions
XBL ideated, performed the literature search, and drafted the manuscript. CGL provided clinical expertise. TSL reviewed and critically revised the work. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
2. Cervantes PE, Matson JL. Comorbid symptomology in adults with autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:3961–70. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2553-z
3. Maddox BB, White SW. Comorbid social anxiety disorder in adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:3949–60. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2531-5
4. Steensel FJA, Bögels SM, Bruin EI. Psychiatric comorbidity in children with autism spectrum disorders: A comparison with children with ADHD. J Child Fam Stud. (2012) 22:368–76. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9587-z
5. Turygin N, Matson JL, Adams H. Prevalence of co-occurring disorders in a sample of adults with mild and moderate intellectual disabilities who reside in a residential treatment setting. Res Dev Disabil. (2014) 35:1802–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.01.027
6. Chiarotti F, Venerosi A. Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders: A review of worldwide prevalence estimates since 2014. MDPI AG. (2020). doi: 10.3390/brainsci10050274
7. Maenner MJ, Shaw KA, Baio J, Washington A, Patrick M, DiRienzo M, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 Years - Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Sites, United States, 2016. MMWR Surveillance Summaries. (2020) 69:1–12. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6904a1
8. Qiu S, Lu Y, Li Y, Shi J, Cui H, Gu Y, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder in Asia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 284:112679–112679. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112679
9. Ames JL, Massolo ML, Davignon MN, Qian Y, Croen LA. Healthcare service utilization and cost among transition-age youth with autism spectrum disorder and other special healthcare needs. Autism. (2021) 25:705–18. doi: 10.1177/1362361320931268
10. Helt M, Kelley E, Kinsbourne M, Pandey J, Boorstein H, Herbert M, et al. Can children with autism recover? If so, how? Neuropsychol Rev. (2008) 18:339–66. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9075-9
11. Buescher AVS, Cidav Z, Knapp M, Mandell DS. Costs of autism spectrum disorders in the United Kingdom and the United States. JAMA Pediatr. (2014) 168:721–8. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.210
12. Lavelle TA, Weinstein MC, Newhouse JP, Munir K, Kuhlthau KA, Prosser LA. Economic burden of childhood autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics. (2014) 133:e520–9. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-0763
13. Leslie DL, Martin A. Health care expenditures associated with autism spectrum disorders. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2007) 161:350–5. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.4.350
14. Zuvekas SH, Grosse SD, Lavelle TA, Maenner MJ, Dietz P, Ji X. Healthcare costs of pediatric autism spectrum disorder in the United States, 2003–2015. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04704-z
15. Global Burden of Disease Study. Global, regional, and national disability-adjusted life-years (DALYs) for 333 diseases and injuries and healthy life expectancy (HALE) for 195 countries and territories, 1990-2016: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. The Lancet. (2017) 390:1260–344.
16. Muotri AR. Autism spectrum disorders: challenges and perspectives. Dev Neurobiol. (2018) 78:431–3. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22586
17. Charman T, Gotham K. Measurement issues: screening and diagnostic instruments for autism spectrum disorders - lessons from research and practise. Child Adolesc Ment Health. (2013) 18:52–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2012.00664.x
18. Randall M, Albein-Urios N, Brignell A, Gulenc A, Hennel S, Coates C, et al. Diagnosing autism: Australian paediatric research network surveys. J Paediatr Child Health. (2016) 52:11–7. doi: 10.1111/jpc.13029
19. Zwaigenbaum L, Penner M. Autism spectrum disorder: Advances in diagnosis and evaluation. BMJ (Online). (2018) 361:k1674–k1674. doi: 10.1136/bmj.k1674
20. Baron-Cohen S., Lombardo M. V., Auyeung B., Ashwin E., Chakrabarti B., Knickmeyer R. (2011). Why are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in males? PLoS Biology, 9(6). doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001081
21. Inui T, Kumagaya S, Myowa-Yamakoshi M. Neurodevelopmental hypothesis about the etiology of autism spectrum disorders. Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00354
22. Gillespie-Lynch K, Kapp SK, Brooks PJ, Pickens J, Schwartzman B. Whose expertise is it? Evidence for autistic adults as critical autism experts. Front Psychol. (2017) 8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00438
23. Kapp SK, Gillespie-Lynch K, Sherman LE, Hutman T. Deficit, difference, or both? Autism and neurodiversity. Dev Psychol. (2013) 49:59–71. doi: 10.1037/a0028353
24. Itsara A, Wu H, Smith JD, Nickerson DA, Romieu I, London SJ, et al. De novo rates and selection of large copy number variation. Genome Res. (2010) 20:1469–81. doi: 10.1101/gr.107680.110
25. Moreno-De-Luca D, Sanders SJ, Willsey AJ, Mulle JG, Lowe JK, Geschwind DH, et al. Using large clinical data sets to infer pathogenicity for rare copy number variants in autism cohorts. Mol Psychiatry. (2013) 18:1090–5. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.138
26. Losh M, Martin GE, Lee M, Klusek J, Sideris J, Barron S, et al. Developmental markers of genetic liability to autism in parents: a longitudinal, multigenerational study. J Autism Dev Disord. (2017) 47:834–45. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2996-x
27. Piven J. Genetic liability for autism: The behavioural expression in relatives. Int Rev Psychiatry. (1999) 11:299–308. doi: 10.1080/09540269974186
28. Piven J, Palmer P, Landa R, Santangelo S, Jacobi D, Childress D. Personality and language characteristics in parents from multiple-incidence autism families. Am J Med Genet. (1997) 74:398–411. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19970725)74:4<398::AID-AJMG11>3.0.CO;2-D
29. Homberg JR, Kyzar EJ, Nguyen M, Norton WH, Pittman J, Poudel MK, et al. Understanding autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders through experimental translational neurobehavioral models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 65:292–312. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.013
30. Hui K, Katayama Y, Nakayama KI, Nomura J, Sakurai T. Characterizing vulnerable brain areas and circuits in mouse models of autism: Towards understanding pathogenesis and new therapeutic approaches. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 110:77–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.001
31. Servadio M, Vanderschuren LJMJ, Trezza V. Modeling autism-relevant behavioral phenotypes in rats and mice: Do 'autistic' rodents exist? Behav Pharmacol. (2015) 26:522–40. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000163
32. Delling JP, Boeckers TM. Comparison of SHANK3 deficiency in animal models: Phenotypes, treatment strategies, and translational implications. J Neurodev Disord. (2021) 13. doi: 10.1186/s11689-021-09397-8
33. Watson KK, Platt ML. Of mice and monkeys: Using non-human primate models to bridge mouse- and human-based investigations of autism spectrum disorders. J Neurodev Disord. (2012) 4:21. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-21
34. Chadman KK. Animal models for autism in 2017 and the consequential implications to drug discovery. Expert Opin Drug Discov. (2017) 12:1187–94. doi: 10.1080/17460441.2017.1383982
35. Ecker C, Spooren W, Murphy DGM. Translational approaches to the biology of Autism: False dawn or a new era? Mol Psychiatry. (2013) 18:435–42. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.102
36. Ha S, Sohn I-J, Kim N, Sim HJ, Cheon K-A. Characteristics of brains in autism spectrum disorder: Structure, function and connectivity across the lifespan. Exp Neurobiol. (2015) 24:273–84. doi: 10.5607/en.2015.24.4.273
37. Klapwijk ET, Aghajani M, Colins OF, Marijnissen GM, Popma A, Van Lang NDJ, et al. Different brain responses during empathy in autism spectrum disorders versus conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2016) 57:737–47. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12498
38. Best CS, Moffat VJ, Power MJ, Owens DGC, Johnstone EC. The boundaries of the cognitive phenotype of autism: Theory of mind, central coherence and ambiguous figure perception in young people with autistic traits. J Autism Dev Disord. (2008) 38:840–7. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0451-8
39. Colombi C, Liebal K, Tomasello M, Young G, Warneken F, Rogers SJ. Examining correlates of cooperation in autism: Imitation, joint attention, and understanding intentions. (2009) 13:143–63. doi: 10.1177/1362361308098514
40. Dawson G, Toth K, Abbott R, Osterling J, Munson J, Estes A, et al. Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Dev Psychol. (2004) 40:271–83. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271
41. Mundy P, Sullivan L, Mastergeorge AM. A parallel and distributed-processing model of joint attention, social cognition and autism. Autism Res. (2009) 2:2–21. doi: 10.1002/aur.61
42. Swanson MR, Siller M. Patterns of gaze behavior during an eye-tracking measure of joint attention in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2013) 7:1087–96. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.05.007
43. Thorup E, Kleberg JL, Falck-Ytter T. Gaze following in children with autism: Do high interest objects boost performance? J Autism Dev Disord. (2017) 47:626–35. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2955-6
44. Trimmer E, McDonald S, Rushby JA. Not knowing what I feel: Emotional empathy in autism spectrum disorders. Autism. (2017) 21:450–7. doi: 10.1177/1362361316648520
45. Fernandez M, Mollinedo-Gajate I, Penagarikano O. Neural circuits for social cognition: Implications for autism. Neuroscience. (2018) 370:148–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.013
46. Müller R-A, Fishman I. Brain connectivity and neuroimaging of social networks in autism. Trends Cogn Sci. (2018) 22:1103–16. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.09.008
47. Guillon Q, Hadjikhani N, Baduel S, Rogé B. Visual social attention in autism spectrum disorder: Insights from eye tracking studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 42:279–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.013
48. Ecker C, Bookheimer SY, Murphy DGM. Neuroimaging in autism spectrum disorder: Brain structure and function across the lifespan. Lancet Neurol. (2015) 14:1121–34. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00050-2
49. Ford CL, Young LJ. Refining oxytocin therapy for autism: context is key. Nature Reviews Neurology. (2022) 18:67–8. doi: 10.1038/s41582-021-00602-9
50. Constantino JN. Early Behavioral Indices of Inherited Liability to Autism. Nature Publishing Group. (2019). p. 127–133. doi: 10.1038/s41390-018-0217-3
51. Martinez-Murcia FJ, Lai MC, Górriz JM, Ramírez J, Young AMH, Deoni SCL, et al. On the brain structure heterogeneity of autism: Parsing out acquisition site effects with significance-weighted principal component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. (2017) 38:1208–23. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23449
52. Matthews NL, Goldberg WA, Lukowski AF, Osann K, Abdullah MM, Ly AR, et al. Does theory of mind performance differ in children with early-onset and regressive autism? Dev Sci. (2012) 15:25–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01094.x
53. Kouklari EC, Tsermentseli S, Auyeung B. Executive function predicts theory of mind but not social verbal communication in school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2018) 76:12–24. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.02.015
54. Mazefsky CA, Williams DL, Minshew NJ. Variability in adaptive behavior in autism: evidence for the importance of family history. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2008) 36:591–9. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9202-8
55. De Groot K, Van Strien JW. Evidence for a broad autism phenotype. Adv Neurodev Disord. (2017) 1:129–40. doi: 10.1007/s41252-017-0021-9
56. Rubenstein E, Chawla D. Broader autism phenotype in parents of children with autism: A systematic review of percentage estimates. J Child Fam Stud. (2018) 27:1705–20. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1026-3
57. Collip D, Wigman JTW, Lin A, Nelson B, Oorschot M, Vollebergh WAM, et al. Dynamic association between interpersonal functioning and positive symptom dimensions of psychosis over time: A longitudinal study of healthy adolescents. Schizophr Bull. (2013) 39:179–85. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr115
58. Driessen TM, Eisinger BE, Zhao C, Stevenson SA, Saul MC, Gammie SC. Genes showing altered expression in the medial preoptic area in the highly social maternal phenotype are related to autism and other disorders with social deficits. BMC Neurosci. (2014) 15. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-15-11
59. Hengartner MP, Müller M, Rodgers S, Rössler W, Ajdacic-Gross V. Interpersonal functioning deficits in association with DSM-IV personality disorder dimensions. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2014) 49:317–25. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0707-x
60. Romm KL, Melle I, Thoresen C, Andreassen OA, Rossberg JI. Severe social anxiety in early psychosis is associated with poor premorbid functioning, depression, and reduced quality of life. Compr Psychiatry. (2012) 53:434–40. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.06.002
61. Kent JM, Kushner S, Ning X, Karcher K, Ness S, Aman M, et al. Risperidone dosing in children and adolescents with autistic disorder: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:1773–83. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1723-5
62. Owen R, Sikich L, Marcus RN, Corey-Lisle P, Manos G, McQuade RD, et al. Aripiprazole in the treatment of irritability in children and adolescents with autistic disorder. Pediatrics. (2009) 124:1533–40. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3782
63. McDougle CJ, Scahill L, Aman MG, McCracken JT, Tierney E, Davies M, et al. Risperidone for the core symptom domains of autism: Results from the study by the autism network of the research units on pediatric psychopharmacology. Am J Psychiatry. (2005) 162:1142–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1142
64. Tural Hesapcioglu S, Ceylan MF, Kasak M, Sen CP. Olanzapine, risperidone, and aripiprazole use in children and adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2020) 72:101520–101520. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2020.101520
65. Bauminger N. The facilitation of social-emotional understanding and social interaction in high-functioning children with autism: Intervention outcomes. J Autism Dev Disord. (2002) 32:283–98. doi: 10.1023/A:1016378718278
66. Beaumont R, Sofronoff K. A multi-component social skills intervention for children with Asperger syndrome: the junior detective training program. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2008) 49:743–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01920.x
67. Williams BF, Williams RL. Effective Programs for Treating Autism Spectrum Disorder: Applied Behavior Analysis Models. Routledge Taylor and Francis Group. (2010). doi: 10.4324/9780203855034
68. Danial JT, Wood JJ. Cognitive behavioral therapy for children with autism: Review and considerations for future research. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2013) 34:702–15. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31829f676c
69. Ventola P, Yang DYJ, Friedman HE, Oosting D, Wolf J, Sukhodolsky DG, et al. Heterogeneity of neural mechanisms of response to pivotal response treatment. Brain Imaging Behav. (2015) 9:74–88. doi: 10.1007/s11682-014-9331-y
70. Dawson G, Jones EJH, Merkle K, Venema K, Lowy R, Faja S, et al. Early behavioral intervention is associated with normalized brain activity in young children with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2012) 51:1150–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.018
71. Calderoni S, Billeci L, Narzisi A, Brambilla P, Retico A, Muratori F. Rehabilitative interventions and brain plasticity in autism spectrum disorders: focus on MRI-based studies. Front Neurosci. (2016) 10. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00139
72. Adams C, Lockton E, Freed J, Gaile J, Earl G, McBean K, et al. The Social Communication Intervention Project: A randomized controlled trial of the effectiveness of speech and language therapy for school-age children who have pragmatic and social communication problems with or without autism spectrum disorder. Int J Lang Commun. (2012) 47:233–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00146.x
73. Marraffa C, Araba B. Social communication in autism spectrum disorder not improved by theory of mind interventions: autism and theory of mind. J Paediatr Child Health. (2016) 52:461–3. doi: 10.1111/jpc.13178
74. Waddington H, van der Meer L, Sigafoos J. Supporting parents in the use of the early start Denver model as an intervention program for their young children with autism spectrum disorder. Int J Dev Disabil. (2021) 67:23–36. doi: 10.1080/20473869.2019.1585694
75. Wetherby AM, Guthrie W, Woods J, Schatschneider C, Holland RD, Morgan L, et al. Parent-implemented social intervention for toddlers with autism: An RCT. Pediatrics. (2014) 134:1084–93. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-0757
76. Benson P, Karlof KL, Siperstein GN. Maternal involvement in the education of young children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. (2008) 12:47–63. doi: 10.1177/1362361307085269
77. Flippin M, Hahs-Vaughn DL. Parent couples' participation in speech-language therapy for school-age children with autism spectrum disorder in the United States. Autism. (2020) 24:321–37. doi: 10.1177/1362361319862113
78. Hastings RP. Behavioral adjustment of siblings of children with autism engaged in applied behavior analysis early intervention programs: the moderating role of social support. J Autism Dev Disord. (2003) 33:141–50.
79. Kang E, Gioia A, Pugliese CE, Islam NY, Martinez-Pedraza FL, et al. (2021). Alliance-outcome associations in a community-based social skills intervention for youth with autism spectrum disorder. Behav Ther. 52:324–37. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2020.04.006
80. Klebanoff SM, Rosenau KA, Wood JJ. The therapeutic alliance in cognitive-behavioral therapy for school-aged children with autism and clinical anxiety. Autism. (2019) 23:2031–42. doi: 10.1177/1362361319841197
81. Hill JR, Ziviani J, Driscoll C. Canine-assisted occupational therapy for children on the autism spectrum: Parents' perspectives. Aust Occup Ther J. (2020) 67:427–36. doi: 10.1111/1440-1630.12659
82. Llambias C, Magill-Evans J, Smith V, Warren S. Equine-assisted occupational therapy: Increasing engagement for children with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Occupat Ther. (2016) 70. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2016.020701
83. Solomon O. “But-He'll Fall!”: Children with autism, interspecies intersubjectivity, and the problem of ‘being social'. Cult Med Psychiatry. (2015) 39:323–44. doi: 10.1007/s11013-015-9446-7
84. Crawford ME, Gray C, Woolhiser J. Design and delivery of a public school social skills training program for youth with autism spectrum disorders: A five year retrospective of the school/community/home (SCH) model of social skills school development. Annu Therap Recreat. (2012). 20:17–35. Available online at: https://www.thefreelibrary.com/Design+and+delivery+of+a+public+school+social+skills+training+program...-a0292087289
85. Radley KC, Hanglein J, Arak M. School-based social skills training for preschool-age children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. (2016) 20:938–51. doi: 10.1177/1362361315617361
86. Whalon KJ, Conroy MA, Martinez JR, Werch BL. School-based peer-related social competence interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis and descriptive review of single case research design studies. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:1513–31. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2373-1
87. Wyman J, Claro A. The UCLA PEERS school-based program: Treatment outcomes for improving social functioning in adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorder and those with cognitive deficits. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50:1907–21. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03943-z
88. Charlop MH, Lang R, Rispoli M. Play and Social Skills for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Cham: Springer. (2018). doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-72500-0
89. D'Amico M, Lalonde C. The effectiveness of art therapy for teaching social skills to children with autism spectrum disorder. Art Therapy. (2017) 34:176–82. doi: 10.1080/07421656.2017.1384678
90. Epp KM. Outcome-based evaluation of a social skills program using art therapy and group therapy for children on the autism spectrum. Children and Schools. (2008) 30:27–36. doi: 10.1093/cs/30.1.27
91. LaGasse AB. Effects of a music therapy group intervention on enhancing social skills in children with autism. J Music Ther. (2014) 51:250–75. doi: 10.1093/jmt/thu012
92. Pater M, Spreen M, van Yperen T. The developmental progress in social behavior of children with autism spectrum disorder getting music therapy. A multiple case study. Children and Youth Services Review. (2021) 120:105767–105767. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105767
93. Beaumont R, Walker H, Weiss J, Sofronoff K. Randomized controlled trial of a video gaming-based social skills program for children on the autism spectrum. J Autism Dev Disord. (2021) 2021:1–14. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04801-z
94. Chung US, Han DH, Shin YJ, Renshaw PF. A prosocial online game for social cognition training in adolescents with high-functioning autism: An fMRI study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2016) 12:651–60. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S94669
95. Friedrich EVC, Sivanathan A, Lim T, Suttie N, Louchart S, Pillen S, et al. An effective neurofeedback intervention to improve social interactions in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:4084–100. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2523-5
96. Wainer AL, Ingersoll BR. The use of innovative computer technology for teaching social communication to individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2011) 5:96–107. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.08.002
97. Wang X, Xing W, Laffey JM. Autistic youth in 3D game-based collaborative virtual learning: Associating avatar interaction patterns with embodied social presence. Br J Educ Technol. (2018) 49:742–60. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12646
98. Jiménez-Muñoz L, Peñuelas-Calvo I, Calvo-Rivera P, Díaz-Oliván I, Moreno M, Baca-García E, et al. Video games for the treatment of autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. J Autism Dev Disord. (2021) 1–20. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-04934-9
99. Thompson GA, McFerran KS, Gold C. Family-centred music therapy to promote social engagement in young children with severe autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled study. Child Care Health Dev. (2014) 40:840–52. doi: 10.1111/cch.12121
100. van Hoogdalem LE, Feijs HME, Bramer WM, Ismail SY, van Dongen JDM. The effectiveness of neurofeedback therapy as an alternative treatment for autism spectrum disorders in children: A systematic review. J Psychophysiol. (2021) 35:102–15. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000265
101. Billstedt E, Gillberg IC, Gillberg C. Aspects of quality of life in adults diagnosed with autism in childhood A population-based study. Autism. (2011) 15:1362–3613. doi: 10.1177/1362361309346066
102. Kuhlthau K, Orlich F, Hall TA, Sikora D, Kovacs EA, Delahaye J, et al. Health-Related Quality of Life in children with autism spectrum disorders: Results from the autism treatment network. J Autism Dev Disord. (2010) 40:721–9. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0921-2
103. Lee LC, Harrington RA, Louie BB, Newschaffer CJ. Children with autism: Quality of life and parental concerns. J Autism Dev Disord. (2008) 38:1147–60. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0491-0
104. Cappadocia MC, Weiss JA, Pepler D. Bullying experiences among children and youth with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2011) 42:266–77. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1241-x
105. Nansel TR, Overpeck M, Pilla RS, Ruan WJ, Simons-Morton B, Scheidt P. Bullying behaviors among US youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. JAMA. (2001) 285:2094–100. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.16.2094
106. Reijntjes A, Vermande M, Goossens FA, Olthof T, van de Schoot R, Aleva L, et al. Developmental trajectories of bullying and social dominance in youth. Child Abuse and Neglect. (2013) 37:224–34. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.12.004
107. Fridenson-Hayo S, Berggren S, Lassalle A, Tal S, Pigat D, Bölte S, et al. Basic and complex emotion recognition in children with autism: cross-cultural findings. Mol Autism. (2016). doi: 10.1186/s13229-016-0113-9
108. Sipes M, Furniss F, Matson JL, Hattier M. A multinational study examining the cross cultural differences in social skills of children with autism spectrum disorders: A comparison between the United Kingdom and the United States of America. J Dev Phys Disabil. (2012) 24:145–54. doi: 10.1007/s10882-011-9261-1
109. Chen YW, Bundy AC, Cordier R, Chien YL, Einfeld SL. A cross-cultural exploration of the everyday social participation of individuals with autism spectrum disorders in Australia and Taiwan: An experience sampling study. Autism. (2017) 21:231–41. doi: 10.1177/1362361316636756
110. Sotgiu I, Galati D, Manzano M, Gandione M, Gómez K, Romero Y, et al. Parental attitudes, attachment styles, social networks, and psychological processes in autism spectrum disorders: A cross-cultural perspective. J Genet Psychol. (2011) 172:353–75. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2010.544342
111. Kang-Yi CD, Grinker RR, Mandell DS. Korean culture and autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:503–20. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1570-4
112. Kim HU. Autism across cultures: Rethinking autism. Disability and Society. (2012) 27:535–45. doi: 10.1080/09687599.2012.659463
113. Cage E, Troxell-Whitman Z. Understanding the reasons, contexts and costs of camouflaging for autistic adults. J Autism Dev Disord. (2019) 49:1899–911. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x
114. Mandy W. Social camouflaging in autism: Is it time to lose the mask? Autism. (2019) 23:1879–81. doi: 10.1177/1362361319878559
115. Perry E, Mandy W, Hull L, Cage E. Understanding camouflaging as a response to autism-related stigma: A social identity theory approach. J Autism Dev Disord. (2021) 2021:1–11. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/7w2pe
116. Kelly R, O'Malley MP, Antonijevic S. ‘Just trying to talk to people … It's the hardest': Perspectives of adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder on their social communication skills. Child Lang Teach Ther. (2018) 34:319–34. doi: 10.1177/0265659018806754
117. Vine Foggo RS, Webster AA. Understanding the social experiences of adolescent females on the autism spectrum. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2017) 35:74–85. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2016.11.006
118. Santhanam S, Hewitt LE. Perspectives of adults with autism on social communication intervention. Commun Disord Q. (2021) 42:156–65. doi: 10.1177/1525740120905501
119. Davis NO, Carter AS. Parenting stress in mothers and fathers of toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: Associations with child characteristics. J Autism Dev Disord. (2008) 38:1278–91. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0512-z
120. McStay RL, Dissanayake C, Scheeren A, Koot HM, Begeer S. Parenting stress and autism: The role of age, autism severity, quality of life and problem behaviour of children and adolescents with autism. Autism. (2014) 18:502–10. doi: 10.1177/1362361313485163
121. Zablotsky B, Bradshaw CP, Stuart EA. The association between mental health, stress, and coping supports in mothers of children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2012) 43:1380–93. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1693-7
122. Jaarsma P, Welin S. Autism as a natural human variation: Reflections on the claims of the neurodiversity movement. Health Care Analysis. (2011) 20:20–30. doi: 10.1007/s10728-011-0169-9
123. Lerner MD, Calhoun CD, Amori MY, De A, Reyes L. Understanding parent-child social informant discrepancy in youth with high functioning autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2012) 42:2680–92. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1525-9
124. Meek SE, Lemery-Chalfant K, Jahromi LB, Valiente C. A review of gene-environment correlations and their implications for autism: a conceptual model. Psychol Rev. (2013) 120:497–521. doi: 10.1037/a0033139
125. Lord C, Elsabbagh M, Baird G, Veenstra-Vanderweele J. Autism spectrum disorder. The Lancet (British Edition). (2018) 392:508–20. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31129-2
126. Malinverni L, Mora-Guiard J, Padillo V, Valero L, Hervás A, Pares N. An inclusive design approach for developing video games for children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Comput Human Behav. (2017) 71:535–49. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.01.018
127. Baron-Cohen S. Editorial Perspective: Neurodiversity – a revolutionary concept for autism and psychiatry. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. (2017) 58:744–7. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12703
Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, social functioning, review, neurodiversity, neurodevelopmental conditions
Citation: Lin XB, Lim CG and Lee T-S (2022) Social Deficits or Interactional Differences? Interrogating Perspectives on Social Functioning in Autism. Front. Psychiatry 13:823736. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.823736
Received: 28 November 2021; Accepted: 30 March 2022;
Published: 25 April 2022.
Edited by:
Antonio Narzisi, Stella Maris Foundation (IRCCS), ItalyReviewed by:
Julie Le Merrer, Université de Tours, Inserm, FranceCopyright © 2022 Lin, Lim and Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Xiangting Bernice Lin, bGlueDAwNjEmI3gwMDA0MDtlLm50dS5lZHUuc2c=