Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 25 April 2022
Sec. Computational Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic New Advancement in Network and Path-analysis Approaches for the Study of Disorders within the Impulse-Compulsive Spectrum Disorders View all 7 articles

The Role of Impulsivity and Reward Deficiency in “Liking” and “Wanting” of Potentially Problematic Behaviors and Substance Uses

  • 1Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
  • 2Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 3Wigner Research Centre for Physics, Budapest, Hungary
  • 4Centre of Excellence in Responsible Gaming, University of Gibraltar, Gibraltar, Gibraltar

A few studies have examined the changes in substance- and behavior-related “wanting” and “liking” of human subjects, the key properties of Incentive Sensitization Theory (IST). The aim of this study was to examine the dissociation between “wanting” and “liking” as a function of usage frequency, intensity, and subjective severity in individuals across four substances (alcohol, nicotine, cannabis, and other drugs) and ten behaviors (gambling, overeating, gaming, pornography use, sex, social media use, Internet use, TV-series watching, shopping, and work). Also, the potential roles of impulsivity and reward deficiency were investigated in “wanting,” “liking,” and wellbeing. The sex differences between “wanting” and “liking” were also examined. Based on our findings using structural equation modeling with 749 participants (503 women, Mage = 35.7 years, SD = 11.84), who completed self-report questionnaires, “wanting” increased with the severity, frequency, and intensity of potentially problematic use, while “liking” did not change. Impulsivity positively predicted “wanting,” and “wanting” positively predicted problem uses/behaviors. Reward deficiency positively predicted problem uses/behaviors, and both impulsivity and problem uses/behaviors negatively predicted wellbeing. Finally, women showed higher levels of “wanting,” compared to men. These findings demonstrate the potential roles of incentive sensitization in both potentially problematic substance uses and behaviors.

Introduction

Psychologists and neuroscientists have long strived to understand how addictions develop and what mechanisms maintain the usage, despite inevitable adverse effects. Many people use recreational drugs regularly, including alcohol, and for the vast majority, it does not raise serious concerns (1). Similarly, many people engage in potentially addictive behaviors without developing adverse consequences [e.g., (2)]. However, in some cases, casual use may result in compulsive behavior, which often persists even after the negative consequences predominate. The consequences of substance addictions are well-known, including medical problems, problems with employment, criminal behavior, and family relations (3). Also, both substance addictions and non-substance-related addictive behaviors negatively impact subjective wellbeing [e.g., (4, 5)].

The Incentive Sensitization Theory [IST (6); see also (710)] of addiction accounts for the psychological and neurobiological basis of drug craving, leading to substance use disorder and relapse. According to this theory, pleasure activates mechanisms of associative learning that normally functions to attribute incentive salience to reward cues, the process by which stimuli become “wanted.” Under normal circumstances, this mechanism is adaptive, promoting behavior to obtain fundamental rewards, such as food or sex (11). However, repeated and intermittent drug-taking behavior might lead to chronic neuroadaptational changes in the mesolimbic dopamine system, rendering this brain system hyper-sensitized to the drug and to the drug-associated cues, manifested in the increasing feeling of “want” toward the drug of abuse (12). Importantly, distinct neural structures (opioid, endocannabinoid, and GABA benzodiazepine neurotransmitter systems) are assumed to be responsible for the hedonic impact, i.e., “liking” of the substance, and they are not subject to sensitization (13), but to tolerance (6). Although the “wanting” and “liking” systems work in sync under normal circumstances, repeated drug taking can upset their balance—due to the different tendency to sensitization—resulting in increasing “wanting” with constant or reduced “liking.” This imbalance between the motivational and affective system is assumed to be responsible for the paradoxical situation, where the addict craves a substance, from which he/she does not expect pleasant feelings (9). The sensitized “wanting” system is then not specific to the subject of abuse, but spills over in a more general way, resulting in the “wanting” of multiple rewards (13), known as cross-sensitization; e.g., sensitization to a specific drug enhances the sensitivity to gambling-related stimuli (14). The sensitization progresses faster in the case of female rats, who show a more rapid and greater increase in motivation compared to males (15), which might contribute to the observation that women progress more rapidly from initial use to addiction (16). Importantly, the sensitization process is not restricted to substance abuse, but growing evidence suggests that it is involved in the development and maintenance of problematic behaviors, such as Internet-use disorder (17) or gambling, gaming, buying-shopping, and compulsive sexual behavior disorders (18).

Although most of the evidence for IST stems from studies on animals, more recently, the dissociation between “wanting” and “liking” has been examined in humans as well with contradictory results (19). This discrepancy may derive from the inconsistencies in the operationalization of the concepts of “wanting” and “liking.” While in animal experiments, the amount of work invested for reward (i.e. the number of lever presses) is a good indictator of “wanting”, and “liking” is well reflected in orofacial expressions (i.e. the rhythmic protrusion of the tongue) (13), human facial expressions are easily faked. Thus, the original research paradigm is not suitable to investigate the processes in question in humans (20). As Pool et al.'s (19) comprehensive review shows, there is no consensual paradigm investigating the IST in humans. From the 84 publications included in their review, 54% used physiological (e.g., mobilized effort, electromyography, food or drug administration), 31% used neurobiological (e.g., fMRI, PET, EEG, brain lesions), 11% used behavioral, and 5% used survey methods.

The low number of survey studies is due to the assumption that both “wanting” and “liking” work outside of conscious awareness—at least partly—(13), thus, explicit self-reports might not be suitable to differentiate between them. Robinson and Berridge (6) argue that humans may not be able to subjectively tell the difference between the two psychological processes of “wanting” vs. “liking,” and a person might mistake a change in incentive salience for a change in pleasure (“If I don't want it, then I must not like it”). Also, explicit incentive processes are relatively immune to manipulations of mesolimbic dopamine systems that change “wanting” (21). However, according to Berridge et al. (13), vivid imagery of reward cues can trigger measurable “wanting” properties; thus, an imagination situation might be sufficient to measure “wanting” and “liking” without the presence of the actual stimuli.

Despite the methodological concerns, a few survey methods were developed. Goldstein et al. (22) introduced the Sensitivity to Reinforcement of Addictive and other Primary Rewards (STRAP-R), which successfully differentiated between “wanting” and “liking” of the drug, food (22), and alcohol (23). The “strong desires to use” and “positive reinforcement from using” scales of Desires for Alcohol (DAQ) (24) and Speed Questionnaire (DSQ) (25) were used to measure alcohol- and amphetamine-related “wanting” and “liking” in the study by Willner et al. (26). They found that “wanting” and “liking” increased as a function of dependence on amphetamine or level of consumption in the case of alcohol, which partially supported IST.

An advantage of the questionnaire method is that it allows one to access larger samples and investigate the relationship of IST with other psychological constructs. For example, impulsivity is a significant marker for substance use disorders (27, 28) or non-substance-related potentially addictive behaviors, such as sexual behaviors [e.g., (29)], or Facebook use, or TV-series watching (30). However, the link between trait impulsivity and incentive sensitization has not yet been investigated. Impulsivity is a heterogeneous personality and behavioral construct (31), well described by the term “disinhibition,” referring to the not appropriate top-down control mechanisms supposed to suppress automatic or reward-driven responses (32). State and trait impulsivity can be distinguished. State impulsivity varies across time and is most often assessed via a neuropsychological test, while trait impulsivity is relatively constant and refers to an overall degree of impulsive behavior in an individual assessed by self-report questionnaires (33). According to the multidimensional UPPS-P model of trait impulsivity (34), five dimensions of impulsivity are distinct: positive/negative urgency, a tendency to act rashly in response to extreme positive/negative emotions, (lack of) premeditation, a tendency to consider the possible consequences of an act before engaging in it, perseverance, the ability to remain focused on a task, and sensation seeking, a tendency to enjoy and pursue stimulating activities (35). On the behavioral level, it is manifested in sudden actions in an unplanned manner to satisfy desires, such as acting on the spur of the moment or not considering the potential outcomes of an action before carrying it out (36). Impulsivity may manifest in maladaptive behaviors, such as aggressive or self-injuring behaviors, domestic violence, and suicide attempts (37). It is also associated with psychiatric disorders, such as antisocial personality disorder, borderline personality disorder, or bipolar disorder (38). In general, elevated trait impulsivity has been found in the patients with different substance use disorders, such as alcohol (39), cocaine (40), or tobacco (41). Also, problematic behaviors were linked to high impulsivity, such as problematic smartphone use (42), Internet-pornography-use disorder (43), problematic use of pornography (29), disordered use of social media (44), sexual addiction (45), cybersex addiction (46) or Internet addiction (47). The multidimensional approach (UPPS-P, see above) was suitable to examine more specific relationships between separate impulsivity-related constructs and addictions (48). Urgency was found to be an important predictor of the development of addictions. Positive urgency was associated with the quantity of alcohol students consumed on a single occasion and the negative outcomes experienced (49)— nicotine dependence and smoking status (50), while negative urgency was highly related to alcohol dependence (48), food addiction (51), and the severity of gambling disorder (52). The lack of perseverance also predicts the drinking quantity (48), smoking status (50), problematic use of pornography, binge eating, and use of drugs other than cannabis (53). However, it is important to note that impulsivity occurs on a continuum, thus, impulsivity per se is not an indicator of pathology (36). Another concept linked to various substance misuses is the reward deficiency syndrome (RDS), which is described as insufficiency of usual feelings of satisfaction, caused by a genetic defect (54). As natural rewards do not adequately stimulate the reward system of individuals with reward deficiency, they are at a greater risk of developing substance use disorder (55). Blum et al. (55) investigated the relationship between RDS and “wanting-liking,” and concluded that the two concepts are related. The mesolimbic dopamine dysregulation observed in RDS predisposes the individual to seek substances and behaviors, which is manifested in the “wanting” of those.

Current Study

The aim of this study was to test the dissociation between “wanting” and “liking” across four substances (alcohol, nicotine, cannabis, other drugs) and ten potentially addictive behaviors (gambling, overeating, gaming, pornography use, sex, social media use, Internet use, TV-series watching, shopping, work). We hypothesized that “wanting” would increase with more frequent and intense use, while “liking” would stay steady or even decrease. We hypothesized that “wanting” of the substance/behavior which shows the greatest imbalance between the motivational and rewarding systems (i.e., the greatest difference between “wanting” and “liking”) would positively predict a general problem use construct, which involves all problem uses/behaviors specific to the individual. We hypothesized that “wanting” would be higher among women than men. Also, we hypothesized that impulsivity would positively predict “wanting” and the general problem use construct. We hypothesized that RDS would negatively predict “liking” and positively predict the general problem use construct. Finally, we hypothesized that indicators of problem uses/behaviors mediated by “wanting” would negatively predict wellbeing. The substances and behaviors of interest were based on Schulter et al.'s study (56), with the modification that cocaine was replaced by “other drugs,” and four additional behaviors were investigated (pornography use, social media use, TV-series watching, and Internet use), given the recent calls for further investigations [e.g., (18, 57, 58)].

Methods

Procedure and Participants

Data collection took place on social media sites and a popular Hungarian news portal via an online survey from June to September 2020. The study was advertised as a research project about the psychological factors of intense engagement in different behaviors. The survey completion took ~20–25 min. The study was conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Joint Committee of Ethics of the Psychology Institutes, Hungary (Number 2020/258). The participants were informed about the aims of the study. Informed consent was obtained from the participants before data collection, and the participants were ensured of their anonymity. No personal information, that might have allowed identification, was asked, and a secure online platform (Qualtrics Research Suite; Qualtrics, Provo, UT) was used for data collection.

Overall, 749 participants (503 women, 67.2%) aged between 18 and 74 years (Mage = 35.7 years, SD = 11.8) completed the questionnaire. As for the level of education, 16 participants had a primary level of education or less (2.1%), 46 had a vocational degree (6%), 105 had a high-school degree (13.8%), 553 had a college or university degree (72.8%), and 39 (5.1%) participants did not want to answer this question. Regarding the relationship status, 233 were single (30.7%), 463 were in any kind of romantic relationship (i.e., being in a romantic relationship or married) (61%), 25 chose the “other” option (3.2%), and 38 did not want to respond (5%).

Measures

Screener for Substance and Behavioral Addictions

One item from the Screener for Substance and Behavioral Addictions (SSBA) (i.e., “I did it too much in the past 12 months.”) was used to measure potentially problematic behaviors. The participants were asked to indicate whether they did too much of any of the substances/behaviors of interest (alcohol, nicotine, cannabis, other drugs, gambling, overeating, gaming, pornography use, sex, social media use, Internet use, TV-series, shopping, work) in the past 12 months, with four answer options: Totally disagree, Partly disagree, Partly agree, and Totally agree. This measure has demonstrated good psychometric properties (56, 59) and allowed us to measure potentially addictive behaviors in a short time. Only substances/behaviors were assessed for “wanting,” “liking,” and frequency, for which “Partly agree” or “Totally agree” responses were indicated.

Imaginative “Wanting” and “Liking” Questionnaire

Imaginative “Wanting” and “Liking” Questionnaire (IWLQ) was designed to measure substance- and behavior-related “wanting” and “liking.” According to Berridge et al. (13), vivid imagery of reward cues may be sufficient to trigger measurable “wanting” properties, without the presence of the actual stimuli. Based on this, IWLQ contains micro scenarios in which subjects are asked to imagine themselves in certain substance- or behavior-related situations. For example: “Imagine yourself sitting in front of your favorite alcoholic drink, in the right time and place.” After the imagery call, participants have to indicate their (1) expected feelings on a ruler (−100: very bad, 0: neutral, 100: very good) before, during, and after use (e.g., “How would you feel before the first sip of your drink?,” “How would you feel during the drinking session?,” “How would you feel after the effects of the drinks are over?,” respectively). (2) The expected willpower they would need (0: nothing, 100: enormous) to resist/stop to participate in the behavior before, during, and after use (e.g., “How much willpower would you need in order to not drink from your drink?,” “How much willpower would you need in order to stop drinking after the first few sips?,” “How much willpower would you need in order to not drink in the next 24 h after you consumed the desired quantity?,” respectively). (3) Frequency of use (e.g., “How often do you consume alcohol?”. Answers: Weekly or less, 2–3 times a week, 4–5 times a week, every day, or nearly every day), and (4) intensity (for substances, e.g., “On days you consume alcohol, in general how much drinks do you have?”. Answers: 1, 2–3, 3–4, 5–6, 7, or more; for behaviors, e.g., “On days you overwork, in general how many extra hours?”. Answers: 1 hour or less, 2–3 h, 4–5 h, 6 h, or more).

We considered “willpower” as a good indicator of “wanting,” as it is the folk notion of self-control, the capacity to override one's impulses and habitual responses (60). The survey was created in a way that the IWLQ items of a substance/behavior were only shown to the participants if they previously indicated a substance or behavior as problematic (i.e., “Partly agree,” “Totally agree”).

Personal Well-Being Index—Adult

The Personal Well-Being Index—Adult (PWI-A) was used to measure subjective personal wellbeing (see the (61)). PWI-A measures seven domains of wellbeing—the standard of living, personal health, achieving in life, personal relationships, personal safety, community-connectedness, future security—and the participants indicated their answers on a ruler (0: not at all, 100: absolutely). The scale showed good reliability in the present sample (α = 0.82).

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-Revised

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-Revised (BIS-R-21) is a self-report measure designed to assess cognitive impulsivity, behavioral impulsivity, and impatience/restlessness (62). It includes 21 items (answer options: 1: rarely/never, 2: occasionally, 3: often, 4: almost always/ always). Since factor loadings of impatience/restlessness were low in our sample (mean 0.37 ± 0.09), and three items (items 2, 4, and 5) showed a floor effect (skewness >1), only cognitive impulsivity (e.g., “I plan tasks carefully”) and behavioral impulsivity (e.g., “I do things without thinking”) were included (14 items) in the present study. The scale showed good reliability in the present sample (α = 0.78).

Reward Deficiency Syndrome Questionnaire

Reward Deficiency Syndrome Questionnaire (RDSQ-29) was used to measure reward deficiency via five domains with 29 items: lack of sexual satisfaction (three items, e.g., “I can never get enough sex.”), activity (five items, e.g., “I cannot stand inactivity.”), social concerns (two items, e.g., “My friends and family often worry about my lifestyle.”), risk-seeking behavior (five items, e.g., “Extreme sports stimulate me.”), and additional items (14 items, e.g., “I like to be always active.”). The Participants indicated their answers using a four-point scale (1: totally disagree, 2: partly disagree, 3: partly agree, 4: totally agree) (63). The scale showed good reliability in the present sample (α = 0.91).

Statistical Analyses

The 679 participants indicated a total of 2,770 uses/behaviors as problematic. For each problem behavior, weekly (F1) and daily (F2) frequency of activity and for each problem use, weekly frequency (F1) and quantity of substance per use (F2), were assessed. An intensity index was formed as the multiplication of F1 and F2. Separate multiple linear regression models were computed on standardized predictor and outcome variables in the three time points (before, during, and after) to analyze the relationship between “liking” or “wanting” and the intensity of the problem, as a function of problem type (behavior or substance use) and sex (female or male). Regression analyses were performed in R (4.0.2), package stats (64). Figures were plotted with ggplot2 (65).

Prior to the main analyses, we conducted separate factor analyses for RDS and BIS to ascertain their psychometric properties. Given the complexity of the hypothesized model, we opted to save these preliminary measurement models as factor scores and use them as input for the main analyses. Factor scores have the advantage over manifest scale scores of providing partial control for measurement error by allocating more weight to the items with lower error variances (66). The use of this approach is becoming increasingly popular (57, 67), further supporting our decision. For every participant, only one problem use/behavior was selected—the one with the highest difference between “wanting” and “liking” scores (76% behavioral). Normality was assessed by the investigation of skewness and kurtosis. Hae-Young Kim (68)—for sample sizes greater than 300—recommended the absolute values of 2.0 for skewness and 7.0 for kurtosis, which could be interpreted as thresholds for acceptability.

Next, structural equation modeling (SEM) with latent variables was conducted with diagonally weighted least squares estimation to examine the relationship pattern between impulsivity, reward deficiency, “wanting,” “liking,” wellbeing, and a general measure of problem use. The latent variable problem uses were inferred from the summed frequency (weekly use × regular amount of use) and summed subjective severity (SSBA responses) for all substances/behaviors indicated problematic, and the number of substances/behaviors indicated problematic (SSBA response partly agree/totally agree). Note, that while “wanting” and “liking” reflects one problem use—the one in which the difference between “wanting” and “liking” was the greatest, i.e., the most problematic according to IST—, the latent variable “problematic use” was constructed from all the substances/behaviors reported as problematic. SEM was performed in R (4.0.2), package Lavaan (69) with diagonally weighted least squares estimation. When assessing the models, multiple goodness-of-fit indices were observed (70) with good or acceptable values based on the following thresholds (71, 72). Regarding the comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), values higher than 0.95 indicated that a model had a good fit, whereas values higher than 0.90 indicated that a model had an acceptable fit. Regarding the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) with its 90% confidence interval (90% CI), a model can be considered good if its RMSEA value is below 0.06, whereas it can be considered acceptable if this value is below 0.08. In addition, following Schellenberg and Bailis's (73) suggestions, to examine the significance of indirect pathways in the mediation model, 95% bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals (CIs) with 5,000 resamples were computed.

The missing values were replaced by a semi-random value, pooled from a set of numbers following the distribution of the particular variable.

Results

Multiple linear regression was used to test if the intensity of usage (weekly frequency × daily frequency), problem type (substance, behavior), and sex (male, female), significantly predicted “wanting” in the three assessed time points (before, during, and after usage/activity) (see Table 1). The overall regressions were statistically significant. The explained variances were low in the case of before and during and moderate in the case of after use/activity. According to the predictions of IST, the intensity positively predicted “wanting” in all three time points, being the highest for “wanting-after.” In the case of problem behaviors, the slope coefficient of “wanting-after” was lower (see Figure 1A). According to the predictions of IST, “wanting” was lower in the male participants in all three time points and a Type × Sex interaction indicated, that “wanting-during” in the case of problem behaviors was even lower for the male participants (see Figure 1B).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Relationship between “wanting,” ”liking,” and intensity as a function of problem type and sex.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scatter plots and linear regression lines of “wanting” and “liking” before, during, and after use/activity (A) difference between substance uses and behaviors, (B) difference between females and males. On the left side, the y-axis represents “wanting” (0: no willpower is needed to resist use/behavior; 1: enormous willpower is needed to resist use/behavior). On the right side, the y-axis represents “liking” (0: negative emotions; 0.5: neutral emotions; 1: positive emotions). The x-axis represents intensity (weekly frequency × daily frequency/amount of use).

Multiple linear regressions were used to test the intensity of usage (weekly frequency × daily frequency), problem type (substance, behavior), and sex (male, female) (see Table 1). Although the explained varianve was low, Intensity negatively predicted “liking” before and during the use/activity. In the case of problem behaviors, for “liking-before” the slope coefficient was higher, while for “liking” during and after was lower compared to substances. The male subjects indicated higher “liking” during the uses/activities, and the Intensity × Type × Sex interaction indicated, that the male subjects indicated higher “liking” as a function of intensity in the case of problem behaviors.

In the mediation model, the role of impulsiveness, reward deficiency, and problem uses was investigated regarding wellbeing through “wanting” and “liking” (Figure 2). Normality was examined and did not violate the thresholds of Kim (68), neither for skewness (ranging from −1.28 to 0.61), nor for kurtosis (ranging from 2.02 to 4.26). The model showed a good fit to the data (CFI: 0.964; TLI: 0.947; RMSEA: 0.047).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The role of impulsiveness (BIS), reward deficiency (RDS), “wanting” and “liking” on the wellbeing is mediated by problem use. The coefficients represent standardized regression weights. The gray arrows represent non-significant paths. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

The trimmed model also had a good fit to the data (CFI: 0.986; TLI: 0.980; RMSEA: 0.040), the results are reported in Table 2. The analyses showed that “wanting” was positively and weakly predicted by impulsivity, and “wanting” and reward deficiency had direct positive associations with problem uses (moderate and strong, respectively). Also, both problem uses and impulsivity negatively and weakly predicted wellbeing (Figure 3). Overall, the model explained 26% of the variance of problem uses, and 4% of wellbeing.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mediation analyses including direct and indirect effects for the trimmed model.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The final trimmed model of impulsivity, reward deficiency, “wanting,” problem uses, and wellbeing. The coefficients represent standardized regression weights. ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

To date, relatively few studies have investigated the changes in substance- and behavior-related “wanting” and “liking” of individuals, especially in relation to other psychological factors. The aim of this study was to contribute to the scientific literature about IST by examining (1) the role of impulsivity, and reward deficiency as predictors of “wanting” and “liking,” and their associations with potentially problematic substance use/behaviors and wellbeing, and the (2) differences between “wanting” and “liking” as a function of usage frequency, severity, and sex differences.

Our first findings complement the prior studies in the field of IST studies showing that “wanting” increased with the self-reported severity, frequency, and intensity of potentially problematic use/behavior, while “liking” showed a slight decrease. These results support the accessibility of IST to individuals with potentially problematic substance use and behaviors in line with others [e.g., (12, 22, 23, 7476)]. Also, the results suggest that the sensitization of the underlying processes of “wanting” is gradual (9), increasing with the frequency of usage.

Contrary to our expectations, self-estimated “wanting” followed by the use/behavior (i.e., “wanting-after”) showed the greatest increment as a function of usage intensity. Since “wanting” is linked to reward cues (77) triggering the urge to use (7), it is reasonable to expect the highest cue effect on “wanting-before.” A possible explanation is a methodological shortcoming that the question regarding “wanting-after” assessed the estimated willpower for abstinence of a well-defined time range (24 h), while no such time range was present before and during use/activity. It is possible that in the case of intense use/behavior, the presence of the time range amplified “wanting,” while no such effect was present for low/moderate intensity uses/behaviors, as reflected in low “wanting-after.” Another possible explanation is that moderate users are able to satisfy their “wanting” with use, but not intense users, supported by previous studies (12, 78) finding that implicit “wanting” scores of smokers were unaffected by nicotine deprivation (12 or 10 h of deprivation vs. immediately after smoke).

“Liking” slightly decreased as a function of usage intensity, but the explained variance was very low (1–3%), supporting that “liking” is largely unaffected by sensitization (7).

An important finding of the current study was that “wanting” showed similar tendencies to potentially problematic substance use and behaviors, which supports the applicability of IST to problematic behaviors, corroborating the findings of prior studies [e.g., (1, 79)]. Since the assessment of intensity for substance use (amount) and behaviors (time spent) was different, direct comparisons might be misleading and are out of the scope of the current study. Although the same restriction applies to the interpretation of “liking,” the tendency of “liking-after” for problem uses and problem behaviors was markedly different. Slightly positive effects were present for moderate behaviors, which decreased with intensity, being neutral for intense behaviors. In the case of substances, “liking-after” indicated slightly negative effects for moderate uses, which increased with intensity, being slightly positive for intense uses. We have no clear explanation for this unexpected pattern. A possible factor might be withdrawal relief [e.g., (80, 81)], but supplementary questions (e.g., about craving, guilt, relief, and euphoria) must be addressed to define the factors behind “liking,” preferably alongside substance-/behavior-specific questionnaires, to define the severity of the problem with higher accuracy and in more detail.

Sex differences in addiction research received increased attention in the past two decades (82), reporting that females generally exceed males in drug use (83) and problem behaviors, such as Internet addiction (84), overeating (85), gambling (86), or social media use (87). Expanding Kawa and Robinson's (15) findings in female rats, women reported a higher level of “wanting” in the present study, which was the most pronounced for problem behaviors, during the activity. This is in line with the findings that women showed increased reactivity to internal (emotional) and external (drug-associated) cues, leading to a higher propensity to drug relapse (88). Also, women reported slightly lower “liking” during the use/activity, which is in line with the previous studies indicating that women with addiction tend to display a higher level of negative emotions [e.g., (85, 89)]. As “wanting” defines the motivational aspects toward substances and behaviors, while decreased “liking” might be connected to coping with related motives, it is reasonable to assume a relationship with complex behaviors, contributing to other sex differences, such as vulnerability to substance use disorder (9092), entering treatment (93), or motives of relapse (94, 95). Since this study is restricted regarding data from substance- and behavior-specific scales, more focused studies are needed.

An important finding of our study was that impulsivity positively predicted “wanting” of the substance/behavior, which showed the greatest imbalance between the motivational and rewarding systems (i.e., the greatest difference between “wanting” and “liking”). Although impulsivity is a significant marker for substance use disorders and problematic behaviors (27, 28), direct associations between impulsivity and “wanting” were not described in terms of IST. A transition from heightened impulsivity to heightened compulsivity might reflect the formation of problem behavior (29, 30, 9698), as strong motivational urges can transform into automatic, compulsive actions (99). The I-PACE model (18), thus considers impulsivity as a general predisposing variable for addictive behaviors. Our results indicate that impulsivity might not be directly related to problem use but via the sensitization of the motivation system (“wanting”), resulting in compulsive behaviors. Previous studies indicated that high-trait impulsive individuals show higher cue reactivity for addiction-related cues [e.g., (100, 101)] and interaction between impulsivity and premorbid alternations in inhibitory processes were reported (102). This might explain the current results assuming that high-trait impulsivity increases cue reactivity, which triggers “wanting,” leading to decreased inhibitory and self-control abilities, which contributes to the development of problem substance use/behaviors. Considering that “wanting” and craving are closely related (103), this is in line with the findings of Meule and Blechert (104), who reported no direct effect of impulsivity on the body mass index, but higher impulsivity predicted more frequent and intense food cravings, which in turn predicted lower perceived self-regulatory success in eating, and that in turn predicted a higher body mass index. Also, in another study, higher rash impulsiveness (the tendency for approach despite potential negative consequences) did not directly predict the risk of relapse but was found to increase vulnerability to the craving, which in turn, increased the risk of relapse (105). However, the lack of direct association between impulsivity and problem behaviors is in contrast with a large literature [for a review see (106)], thus, more focused studies would be necessary to better understand the role of impulsivity-related constructs on incentive sensitization. Since “wanting” was derived from one behavior in our model, and problem use reflected all problematic behaviors, our results extend Berridge et al.'s (13) findings to humans that a sensitized dopamine system might not be specific to the subject of abuse, but spill over in a more general way. This observation might be potentially useful in understanding the mechanisms underlying the co-occurrence of substance addictions and problematic behaviors.

Corroborating previous findings [e.g., (54, 55)], reward deficiency positively predicted problem use in our study. According to the RDS hypothesis, individuals with higher levels of reward deficiency might be at greater risk of developing substance use disorder as natural rewards do not adequately stimulate their reward system (55). However, contrary to our expectations, reward deficiency did not predict “wanting” or “liking.” A potential explanation is that the positive effects related to short-term rewards might not be affected by reward deficiency, or even if they are, our tool was not able to capture it, since only subjective evaluation of the effects related to such rewards were assessed. However, this possible explanation warrants further investigation.

A great difficulty in human IST research is the operationalization of the “wanting” and “liking” concepts (107). In animal studies “wanting” is reflected by the work (e.g., paddle pushes) the animal is willing to invest in to get the substance (8). For people, determining the amount of work to be invested in would require serious calculations and in many cases (e.g., problematic behaviors such as social media use) would be difficult to interpret. Since cognitive wanting and liking are often used interchangeably (7), asking the person if she/he wants the substance might result in a “no” (although “wanting” might be high). For example, an individual recovering from alcohol-use problems might not want to drink alcohol at a social occasion, but still needs plenty of willpower to successfully resist. We argued that assessing “wanting” in an indirect way throughout the perceived cognitive resources of refusal might be a more accurate measure as it is not/less mixed with the affective aspects. For that reason, “wanting” was conceptualized in willpower, the folk notion of self-control, a conscious and effortful form of self-regulation, the capacity to override one's impulses, and automatic or habitual responses (i.e., “wanting”) (60). Since “wanting” and “liking” were assessed in an imagined ideal situation, it is assumable that the imagery triggered motivational/emotional signals (13) potentially lead to more accurate self-reports. Also, assessing an ideal scenario rather than at the given moment (i.e., “now”), might help to dissociate from the circumstances of the study participation (e.g., one might enjoy smoking cannabis after work, but not when completing the survey in the office). Also, the implicit nature of “wanting” and “liking” might be captured more adequately using a ruler for responses, and not categorical responses that may require a thoughtful elaboration of answers. Finally, in the IWLQ, “wanting” and “liking” were assessed in an imagined scenario before, during, and after use, allowing us to examine the dynamics of the two underlying systems over time. Yet, future studies are needed to examine the reliability and validity of the scale in diverse populations.

Several limitations warrant consideration. We used self-report scales in a self-selected sample that may introduce biases (e.g., overreporting or underreporting). Given the study's cross-sectional nature, causality cannot be inferred. This study did not test the dissociation explicitly across individuals with the clinical diagnosis of substance use or problematic engagement in a given behavior and non-problematic users. Rather, potentially problematic use was classified as “mild” and “severe” based on one item from the SSBA (56). To investigate the relationship between substance/behavioral addictions and the studied psychological constructs, further studies are needed, using substance- and behavior-specific scales. A weakness of the study is that we did not measure the duration/onset of the substance use/problem behavior. Such a variable would have allowed us to examine the sensitization process and the development of addictions in more detail. Also, from the 749 subjects, 70 did not indicate any substance/behavior as problematic, from whom we had no data on “wanting” and “liking.” In a future study, it would be beneficial to collect data from such participants to achieve a better understanding of the development of the early stages of problem behaviors and uses. Also, further studies should investigate the relation of positive (reward/pleasure) and negative (relief/satiation) reinforcement to “wanting” and “liking.” Considering that positive reinforcement may involve more elements of impulsivity, while negative reinforcement may involve more elements of compulsivity (108), it is reasonable to assume that such distinction would reveal important correlations between “wanting”-“liking” and impulsivity and reward deficiency. Also, since negative reinforcement can initiate and maintain self-medication behaviors (109), the dynamics of initial “wanting” and “liking” and their correlation to the motivation of usage should be investigated to test whether the willpower-based conceptualization of “wanting” is suitable to explain negative reinforcement-based motivational factors. Further, since “wanting” was conceptualized in the willpower to resist the use/activity, the effect of the underlying cause of restriction [i.e., internal (e.g., quitting attempt) or external (e.g., lack of resources)] should be investigated in the future, as it presumably has an effect on “liking.”

In sum, the present study largely supports the role of incentive sensitization in both potentially problematic substance use and behaviors. The results corroborate the notion that survey methods might be suitable for investigating IST. Moreover, the findings also suggest that impulsivity might not be directly associated with problematic engagement in substance use and different problematic behaviors (e.g., problematic pornography use) (29), but via “wanting” providing targets for future interventions.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by ELTE PPK Ethical Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DF: conceptualization, methodology, visualization, writing—original draft, and formal analysis. BF: conceptualization, methodology, and writing—review and editing. ZD: conceptualization, funding acquisition, methodology, supervision, writing—review and editing. BB: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, writing—review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The study was supported by the National Research, Development and Innovation Office (K126835, K131635, and PD138976). BB was supported by the Merit Scholarship Program for Foreign Students (PBEEE) awarded by the Ministère de l'Éducation et de l'Enseignement Supérieur (MEES) and by a postdoctoral fellowship from the SCOUP Team—Sexuality and Couples—Fonds de recherche du Québec, Société et Culture.

Conflict of Interest

ELTE Eötvös Loránd University receives funding from the Szerencsejáték Ltd. to maintain a telephone helpline service for problematic gambling. ZD has also been involved in research on responsible gambling funded by Szerencsejáték Ltd. and the Gambling Supervision Board and provided educational materials for the Szerencsejáték Ltd's responsible gambling program.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Thomsen K, Fjorback LO, Møller A, Lou HC. Applying incentive sensitization models to behavioral addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 45:343–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bothe B, Tóth-Király I, Potenza MN, Orosz G, Demetrovics Z. High-frequency pornography use may not always be problematic. J Sex Med. (2020) 17:793–811. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2020.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Stein MD, Mulvey KP, Plough A, Smaet JH. The functioning and well being of persons who seek treatment for drug and alcohol use. J Subst Abuse. (1998) 10:75–84. doi: 10.1016/S0899-3289(99)80142-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Wittmann M, Paulus M, Roenneberg T. Decreased psychological well-being in late 'chronotypes' is mediated by smoking and alcohol consumption. Substance Use Misuse. (2010) 45:15–30. doi: 10.3109/10826080903498952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Caplan SE. Preference for online social interaction: a theory of problematic internet use and psychosocial well-being. Communic Res. (2003) 30:625–48. doi: 10.1177/0093650203257842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev. (1993) 18:247–91. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Berridge KC, Robinson T. Liking, wanting and the incentive salience theory of addiction. Am Psychol. (2016) 71:670–9. doi: 10.1037/amp0000059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci. (2003) 26:507–13. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Incentive-sensitization and addiction. Addiction. (2001) 96:103–14. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.9611038.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. (2008) 363:3137–46. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC. The functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Discov Med. (2010) 9:579–87. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199570706.013.0018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Grigutsch LA, Lewe G, Rothermund K, Koranyi N. Implicit “wanting” without implicit “liking”: A test of incentive-sensitization-theory in the context of smoking addiction using the wanting-implicit-association-test (W-IAT). J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2019) 64:9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Berridge KC, Robinson TE, Aldridge JW. Dissecting components of reward: “liking”, “wanting”, and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol. (2009) 9:65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Hellberg SN, Russell TI, Robinson M. Cued for risk: evidence for an incentive sensitization framework to explain the interplay between stress and anxiety, substance abuse, and reward uncertainty in disordered gambling behavior. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2019) 19:737–58. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-00662-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kawa AB, Robinson TE. Sex differences in incentive-sensitization produced by intermittent access cocaine self-administration. Psychopharmacology. (2019) 236:625–39. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-5091-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Algallal H, Allain F, Ndiaye NA, Samaha AN. Sex differences in cocaine self-administration behaviour under long access versus intermittent access conditions. Addictionbiology. (2020) 25:e12809. doi: 10.1111/adb.12809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Brand M, Young KS, Laier C, Wölfling K, Potenza MN. Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: An Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 71:252–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Brand M, Wegmann E, Stark R, Müller A, Wölfling K, Robbins TW, et al. The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 104:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Pool E, Sennwald V, Delplanque S, Brosch T, Sander D. Measuring wanting and liking from animals to humans: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 63:124–42., 0.2016.01.006 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Tibboel H, De Houwer J, Spruyt A, Field M, Kemps E, Crombez G. Testing the validity of implicit measures of wanting and liking. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2011) 42:284–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Dickinson A, Smith J, Mirenowicz J. Dissociation of Pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning under dopamine antagonists. Behav Neurosci. (2000) 114:468–83. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.3.468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Goldstein RZ, Woicik PA, Moeller SJ, Telang F, Jayne M, Wong C, et al. Liking and wanting of drug and non-drug rewards in active cocaine users: the STRAP-R questionnaire. J Psychopharmacol. (2010) 24:257–66. doi: 10.1177/0269881108096982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Arulkadacham LJ, Richardson B, Staiger PK, Kambouropoulos N, O'Donnell RL, Ling M. Dissociation between wanting and liking for alcohol and caffeine: a test of the Incentive Sensitisation Theory. J Psychopharmacol. (2017) 31:927–33. doi: 10.1177/0269881117711711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Love A, James D, Willner P. A comparison of two alcohol craving questionnaires. Addiction. (1998) 93:1091–102. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.937109113.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. James D, Davies G, Willner P. The development and initial validation of a questionnaire to measure craving for amphetamine. Addiction. (2004) 2004:1181–1188. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00819.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Willner P, James D, Morgan M. Excessive alcohol consumption and dependence on amphetamine are associated with parallel increases in subjective ratings of both ‘wanting’ and ‘liking.’ Addiction. (2005) 100:1487–1495. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01222.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Conway KP, Kane RJ, Ball SA, Poling JC, Rounsaville BJ. Personality, substance of choice, and polysubstance involvement among substance dependent patients. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2003) 71:65–75. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(03)00068-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Verdejo-García A, Lawrence AJ, Clark L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2008) 32:777–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bothe B, Toth-Kiraly I, Potenza MN, Griffiths MD, Orosz G, Demetrovics Z. Revisiting the role of impulsivity and compulsivity in problematic sexual behaviors. J Sex Res. (2019) 56:166–79. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2018.1480744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Orosz G, Bothe B, Tóth-Király I. The development of the problematic series WatchingScale (PSWS). J Behav Addict. (2016) 5:144–50. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Evenden JL. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology. (1999) 146:348–61. doi: 10.1007/PL00005481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Aron AR. The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist. (2007) 13:214–28. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Khemiri L, Brynte C, Konstenius M, Guterstam J, Rosendahl I, Franck J, et al. Self-rated impulsivity in healthy individuals, substance use disorder and ADHD: psychometric properties of the Swedish Barratt impulsiveness scale. BMC Psychiatry. (2021) 21:458. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03462-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Lynam D, Smith GT, Cyders MA, Fischer S, Whiteside SA. The UPPS-P: a multidimensional measure of risk for impulsive behavior. Unpublished technical report (2007).

35. Billieux J, Rochat L, Ceschi G, Carré A, Offerlin-Meyer I, Defeldre A-C, et al. Validation of a short French version of the UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale. Compr Psychiatry. (2012) 53:609–15. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Kreek MJ, Nielsen DA, Butelman ER, LaForge KS. Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat Neurosci. (2005) 8:1450–7. doi: 10.1038/nn1583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Del Carlo A, Benvenuti M, Fornaro M, Toni C, Rizzato S, Swann AC, et al. Different measures of impulsivity in patients with anxiety disorders: a case control study. Psychiatry Res. (2012) 197:231–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry. (2001) 158:1783–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Bjork JM, Hommer DW, Grant SJ, Danube C. Impulsivity in abstinent alcoholdependent patients: relation to control subjects and type 1–/type 2-like traits. Alcohol. (2004) 34:133–50. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2004.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Fischer CJ, Dougherty DM, Reilly EL, Mathias CW, et al. P300 event-related potential amplitude and impulsivity in cocainedependent subjects. Neuropsychobiology. (2004) 50:167–73. doi: 10.1159/000079110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ozga-Hess JE, Romm KF, Felicione NJ, Dino G, Blank MD, Turiano NA. Personality and impulsivity as predictors of tobacco use among emerging adults: a latent class analysis. Personal Indiv Diff. (2020) 163:110076, doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Grant JE, Lust K, Chamberlain SR. Problematic smartphone use associated with greater alcohol consumption, mental health issues, poorer academic performance, and impulsivity. J Behav Addict. (2019) 8:335–42. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.32

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Antons S, Brand M. Trait and state impulsivity in males with tendency towards Internet pornography-use disorder. Addict Behav. (2018) 79:171–7. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.12.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Sindermann C, Elhai JD, Montag C. Predicting tendencies towards the disordered use of Facebook's social media platforms: on the role of personality, impulsivity, and social anxiety. Psychiatry Res. (2020). 285, 112793. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Levi G, Cohen C, Kaliche S, Sharaabi S, Cohen K, Tzur-Bitan D, et al. Sexual addiction, compulsivity, and impulsivity among a predominantly female sample of adults who use the internet for sex. J Behav Addict. (2020). 9, 83–92. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Wang J, Dai B. Event-related potentials in a two-choice oddball task of impaired behavioral inhibitory control among males with tendencies towards cybersex addiction. J Behav Addict. (2020) 9:785–96. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Cao F, Su L, Liu T, Gao X. The relationship between impulsivity and Internet addiction in a sample of Chinese adolescents. Eur Psychiatry. (2007) 22:466–71. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Coskunpinar A, Dir AL, Cyders MA. Multidimensionality in impulsivity and alcohol use: a meta-analysis using the UPPS model of impulsivity. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2013) 37:1441–50. doi: 10.1111/acer.12131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Cyders MA, Flory K, Rainer S, Smith GT. The role of personality dispositions to risky behavior in predicting first-year college drinking. Addiction. (2009) 104:193–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02434.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kale D, Stautz K, Cooper A. Impulsivity related personality traits and cigarette smoking in adults: A meta-analysis using the UPPS-P model of impulsivity and reward sensitivity. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2018) 185:149–67. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Wolz I, Hilker I, Granero R, Jiménez-Murcia S, Gearhardt AN, Dieguez C, et al. “Food addiction” in patients with eating disorders is associated with negative urgency and difficulties to focus on long-term goals. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:61. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Savvidou LG, Fagundo AB, Fernández-Aranda F, Granero R, Claes L, Mallorquí-Baqué N, et al. Is gambling disorder associated with impulsivity traits measured by the UPPS-P and is this association moderated by sex and age?. Compreh Psychiatry. (2017) 72:106–13. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Rømer Thomsen K, Callesen MB, Hesse M, Kvamme TL, Pedersen MM, Pedersen MU, et al. Impulsivity traits and addiction-related behaviors in youth. J Behav Addict. (2018) 7:317–30. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Blum K, Sheridan PJ, Wood RC, Braverman ER, Chen TJ, Cull JG, et al. The D2 dopamine receptor gene as a determinant of reward deficiency syndrome. J R Soc Med. (1996) 89:396–400. doi: 10.1177/014107689608900711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Blum K, Gardner E, Oscar-Berman M, Gold M. “Liking” and “wanting” linked to Reward Deficiency Syndrome (RDS): hypothesizing differential responsivity in brain reward circuitry. Curr Pharm Des. (2012) 18:113–8. doi: 10.2174/138161212798919110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Schluter MG, Hodgins DC, Wolfe J, Wild TC. Can one simple questionnaire assess substance-related and behavioural addiction problems? Results of a proposed new screener for community epidemiology. Addiction. (2018) 113:1528–37. doi: 10.1111/add.14166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Tóth-Király I, Vallerand RJ, Bothe B, Rigó A, Orosz G. Examining sexual motivation profiles and their correlates using latent profile analysis. Pers Individ Dif. (2019) 146:76–86. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Grubbs JB, Kraus SW. Pornography use and psychological science: A call for consideration. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2021) 30:68–75. doi: 10.1177/0963721420979594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Schulter MG, Hodgins DC, Thege BK, Wild TC. Predictive utility of the brief Screener for Substance and Behavioral Addictions for identifying self-attributed problems. J Behav Addict. (2020) 3:709–22. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Gailliot MT, Baumeister RF. The physiology of willpower: linking blood glucose to self-control. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. (2007) 11:303–27. doi: 10.1177/1088868307303030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. International Wellbeing Group. Personal Wellbeing Index. 5th ed. Melbourne: Australian Centre on Quality of Life; Deakin University (2013).

Google Scholar

62. Kapitány-Fövény M, Urbán R, Varga G, Potenza MN, Griffiths MD, Szekely A, et al. The 21-item Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Revised (BIS-R-21): an alternative three-factor model. J Behav Addict. (2020) 9:225–46. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.0003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Kótyuk E, Urbán R, Hende B, Richman M, Magi A, Király O, et al. Development and validation of the Reward Deficiency Syndrome Questionnaire (RDSQ-29). J Psychopharmacol. (2022) 36:409–22. doi: 10.1177/02698811211069102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2020). Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/. (accessed April 06, 2022).

Google Scholar

65. Wickham H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer (2016).

Google Scholar

66. Skrondal A, Laake P. Regression among factor scores. Psychometrika. (2001) 66:563–75. doi: 10.1007/BF02296196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Gillet N, Morin AJS, Cougot B, Gagné M. Workaholism profiles: associations with determinants, correlates, and outcomes. J Occup Organ Psych. (2017) 90:559–86. doi: 10.1111/joop.12185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Kim HY. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restorat Dentist Endodontics. (2013) 38:52. doi: 10.5395/rde.2013.38.1.52

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Rossel Y. lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. (2011) 48:472–8. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Brown TA. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. 2nd ed. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (2015).

Google Scholar

71. Hu L-T, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Eqn Modeling. (1999) 6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Marsh HW, Hau KT, Grayson D. Goodness of Fit in Structural Equation Models. (2005).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

73. Schellenberg BJI, Bailis DS, Mosewich AD. You have passion, but do you have self-compassion? Harmonious passion, obsessive passion, and responses to passion-related failure. Personal Individ Diff. (2016) 99:278–85. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hobbs M, Remington B, Glautier S. Dissociation of wanting and liking for alcohol in humans: a test of the incentive-sensitisation theory. Psychopharmacology. (2005) 178:493–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2026-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Koranyi N, Brückner E, Jäckel A, Grigutsch LA, Rothermund K. Dissociation between wanting and liking for coffee in heavy drinkers. J Psychopharmacol. (2020) 34:1350–6. doi: 10.1177/0269881120922960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Lambert NM, McLeod M, Schenk S. Subjective responses to initial experience with cocaine: An exploration of the incentive-sensitization theory of drug abuse. Addiction. (2006) 101:713–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01408.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Anderson BA, Yantis S. Persistence of value-driven attentional capture. J Exp Psychol Human Percept Perform. (2013) 39:6–9. doi: 10.1037/a0030860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Tibboel H, De Houwer J, Van Bockstaele B. Implicit measures of “wanting” and “liking” in humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 57(January 2019):350–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Polk SE, Schulte EM, Furman CR, Gearhardt AN. Wanting and liking: separable components in problematic eating behavior? Appetite. (2017). 115:45–53. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Hughes JR. Distinguishing withdrawal relief and direct effects of smoking. Psychopharmacology. (1991) 104:409–10. 46044 doi: 10.1007/BF02246044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Heinz A, Löber S, Georgi A, Wrase J, Hermann D, Rey ER, et al. Reward craving and withdrawal relief craving: assessment of different motivational pathways to alcohol intake. Alcohol Alcohol. (2003) 38:35–9. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agg005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Carroll ME, Smethells JR. Sex differences in behavioral dyscontrol: role in drug addiction and novel treatments. Front Psychiatry. (2016) 6:175. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Anker JJ, Carroll ME. The role of progestins in the behavioral effects of cocaine and other drugs of abuse: human and animal research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2010) 35:315–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Shen Y, Wang L, Huang C, Guo J, De Leon SA, Lu J, et al. Sex differences in prevalence, risk factors and clinical correlates of internet addiction among chinese college students. J Affect Disord. (2021) 279:680–686. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Anversa RG, Muthmainah M, Sketriene D, Gogos A, Sumithran P, Brown RM. A review of sex differences in the mechanisms and drivers of overeating. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2021) 63:100941. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2021.100941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Ohtsuka K, Bruton E, Deluca L, Borg V. Sex differences in pathological gambling using gaming machines. Psychol Rep. (1997) 80:1051–7. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1997.80.3.1051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Chae D, Kim H, Kim YA. Sex differences in the factors influencing korean college students' addictive tendency toward social networking sites. Int J Ment Health Addict. (2018) 16:339–50. doi: 10.1007/s11469-017-9778-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Hudson A, Stamp JA. Ovarian hormones and propensity to drug relapse: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:427–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Grant JE, Kim SW, Odlaug BL, Buchanan SN, Potenza MN. Late-onset pathological gambling: clinical correlates and gender differences. J Psychiatr Res. (2009) 43:380–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Lukas SE, Sholar M, Lundahl LH, Lamas X, Kouri E, Wines JD, et al. Sex differences in plasma cocaine levels and subjective effects after acute cocaine administration in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology. (1996) 125:346–54. 46017 doi: 10.1007/BF02246017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Guxens M, Nebot M, Ariza C. Age and sex differences in factors associated with the onset of cannabis use: a cohort study. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2007) 88:234–43. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Perkins KA. Nicotine discrimination in men and women. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (1999) 64:295–9. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(99)00085-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Hser YI, Anglin MD, Booth MW. Sex differences in addict careers. 3. Addiction. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. (1987) 13:231–51. doi: 10.3109/00952998709001512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Rubin AMY, Stout RL. Gender differences in relapse situations. Addiction. (1996). 91: 111–120 doi: 10.1080/09652149638845

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. McHugh RK, Geyer RB, Chase AR, Griffin ML, Bogunovic O, Weiss RD. Sex differences in benzodiazepine misuse among adults with substance use disorders. Addict Behav. (2021) 112:106608. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron. (2011) 69:680–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Izquierdo A, Jentsch JD. Reversal learning as a measure of impulsive and compulsive behavior in addictions. Psychopharmacology. (2012) 219:607–20. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2579-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Bargeon AH, Hormes JM. Psychosocial correlates of internet gaming disorder: psychopathology, life satisfaction, and impulsivity. Comput Human Behav. (2017) 68:388–94. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Jentsch JD, Ashenhurst JR, Cervantes MC, Groman SM, James AS, Pennington ZT. Dissecting impulsivity and its relationships to drug addictions. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2014) 1327:1–26. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Doran N, McChargue D, Spring B. Effect of impulsivity on cardiovascular and subjective reactivity to smoking cues. Addict Behav. (2008) 33:167–72. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Papachristou H, Nederkoorn C, Havermans R, van der Horst M, Jansen A. Can't stop the craving: the effect of impulsivity on cue-elicited craving for alcohol in heavy and light social drinkers. Psychopharmacology. (2012) 219:511–8. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2240-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Murray JE, Dilleen R, Pelloux Y, Economidou D, Dalley JW, Belin D, et al. Increased impulsivity retards the transition to dorsolateral striatal dopamine control of cocaine seeking. Biol Psychiatry. (2014) 76:15–22. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Berridge KC. From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation. Eur J Neurosci. (2012) 35:1124–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.07990.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Meule A, Blechert J. Indirect effects of trait impulsivity on body mass. Eating Behav. 26:66–9. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2017.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Coates JM, Gullo MJ, Feeney G, McD Young R, Dingle GA, Clark PJ, et al. Craving mediates the effect of impulsivity on lapse-risk during alcohol use disorder treatment. Addict Behav. (2020) 105:106286. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Lee RSC, Hoppenbrouwers S, Franken I. A systematic meta-review of impulsivity and compulsivity in addictive behaviors. Neuropsychol Rev. (2019) 29:14–26. doi: 10.1007/s11065-019-09402-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Korb S, Götzendorfer SJ, Massaccesi C, Sezen P, Graf I, Willeit M, et al. Dopaminergic and opioidergic regulation during anticipation and consumption of social and nonsocial rewards. Elife. (2020) 9:e55797. doi: 10.7554/eLife.55797.sa2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Koob GF. Negative reinforcement in drug addiction: the darkness within. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2013) 23:559–63. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Blume AW, Chmaling KB, Marlatt GA. Revisiting the self-medication hypothesis from a behavioral perspective. Cogn Behav Pract. (2000) 7:379–84. doi: 10.1016/S1077-7229(00)80048-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: incentive sensitization, impulsivity, reward deficiency, problem behavior, substance misuse

Citation: File D, Bőthe B, File B and Demetrovics Z (2022) The Role of Impulsivity and Reward Deficiency in “Liking” and “Wanting” of Potentially Problematic Behaviors and Substance Uses. Front. Psychiatry 13:820836. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.820836

Received: 23 November 2021; Accepted: 21 March 2022;
Published: 25 April 2022.

Edited by:

Isabel Krug, The University of Melbourne, Australia

Reviewed by:

Martin Zack, University of Toronto, Canada
Kristine Rømer Thomsen, Aarhus University, Denmark

Copyright © 2022 File, Bőthe, File and Demetrovics. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Domonkos File, file.domonkos@ppk.elte.hu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.