Erratum: Perceived Stress, Cortical GABA, and Functional Connectivity Correlates: A Hypothesis-Generating Preliminary Study
- 1Department of Psychiatry, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
- 2Division of Women's Health, Department of Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
- 3Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
- 4Department of Epidemiology, Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, MA, United States
- 5Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
Stress exposures and dysregulated responses to stress are implicated in psychiatric disorders of mood, anxiety, and cognition. Perceived stress, an individual's appraisal of experienced stress and ability for coping, relates to dysregulated functioning in resting state brain networks. Alterations in GABAergic function may underlie perceived stress-related functional dysregulation in resting state networks but this has not yet been explored. Therefore, the current study examined the association of perceived stress, via the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), with prefrontal GABA levels and corresponding resting state functional connectivity (RSFC) alterations. Twelve women and five men, ages 35–61, participated. MR spectroscopy was used to measure brain GABA levels in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC). Resting state functional scans acquired at 3 Tesla were used to measure RSFC within and between the default mode (DMN), salience (SN), and central executive networks (CEN), hippocampus and amygdala. We observed significant negative correlations between total PSS scores and left DLPFC GABA levels (r = −0.62, p = 0.023). However, PSS scores were not significantly correlated with RSFC measures (all p > 0.148). These preliminary results support a relationship between perceived stress and GABAergic functioning in DLPFC, a core node of the CEN, an intrinsic network thought to underlie goal-directed attentional processes. Our findings extend previous work suggesting that functioning in the CEN is related to perceived stress and may inform treatment strategies to improve outcomes in stress-related conditions.
Introduction
Stress exposures are risk factors for psychiatric disorders including disorders of mood (1–3), anxiety (3) and cognition (4). Dysregulated psychological and physiological responses to stress are also common in these disorders. Psychological perceived stress is an individual's appraisal of experienced stress and ability for coping (5). Thus, perceived stress reflects the interaction between an individual and their environment and involves feelings of uncontrollability and unpredictability (6). Higher perceived stress is associated with adverse health outcomes including depression (7) and cognitive decline (8). However, neural mechanisms underlying interindividual differences in perceived stress are not fully known.
Resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rfMRI) is a method that involves observing spontaneous brain activity in the absence of a task and has been translated to investigate group differences in brain functioning, develop biomarkers of disease (9), and identify resting state networks (RSNs) with distinguishable brain functions (10). A triple network model involving aberrant activity among the default mode (DMN), salience (SN), and central executive networks (CEN) has been proposed to describe dysfunction in psychiatric disorders (11). The triple network model proposes that weak salience mapping by regions of the SN leads to aberrant engagement of the CEN and deficits in disengagement of the DMN. The DMN is believed to subserve self-referential mental activity such as emotion processing, rumination and thoughts of the past and future (12–15) and is primarily deactivated when attention is shifted to the external environment (16). The SN is thought to be involved in assigning salience to emotional and sensory stimuli (17) and to mediate the shift between the internally directed cognition of the DMN to more external cognitive processes (i.e. working memory and goal-directed cognition) characteristic of the central executive network (CEN) (18, 19). Dysregulated function within these three intrinsic networks has been linked to higher perceived stress with greater functional connectivity within the DMN (20), disrupted functional connectivity within the CEN (21), and greater functional connectivity between the SN and limbic regions (e.g. amygdala) (22). Perceived stress has been shown to be related to greater functional connectivity between the amygdala and the ACC and following a stress-reducing mindfulness intervention this functional connectivity between the amygdala and the ACC in individuals with higher perceived stress has been shown to decrease (22).
One potential mechanism underlying perceived stress and related functional dysregulation in resting state networks and limbic regions is altered GABAergic function. GABA is the primary inhibitory neurotransmitter in the brain and pre-clinical and clinical studies suggest that GABA modulates hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA-axis) response to stress (23) which has been shown to be predicted, in part, by psychological perceived stress (24, 25). In addition, GABA has been shown to be decreased in the prefrontal cortex in depression (26) and increased in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and ACC in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) (27), two stress-related psychiatric disorders. The prefrontal cortex together with limbic structures (hippocampus and amygdala) is thought to form a regulatory network of the stress response by the HPA axis (28). However, the role of GABA in perceived stress and related network and limbic function has not yet been determined.
The current study aimed to preliminarily examine GABAergic functioning related to perceived stress and resting state functional connectivity (RSFC) with the goal of serving as a hypothesis generating study for future research investigating stress and GABAergic functioning. We employed proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS), a non-invasive method for detecting, identifying, and quantifying bulk GABA in the human brain, at 7T where increased spectral dispersion and signal to noise provide more accurate measurements (29). We utilized the Perceived Stress Scale [PSS: (30)], a well-validated and psychometrically strong instrument for assessing perceived stress, and tested relationships between perceived stress and functional connectivity in resting state networks and limbic regions (e.g. DMN, CEN, SN, amygdala, and hippocampus) along with relationships between perceived stress and in vivo measurements of GABA levels in the VMPFC, DLPFC, and ACC, nodes of the DMN, CEN and SN, respectively.
Methods and Materials
Participants
Seventeen participants (12 women and 5 men) were recruited at Brigham and Women's Hospital through online advertisements targeted toward healthy participants and participants with mood disorder in order to enrich the sample for a range of emotion regulation in line with the NIMH Research Domain Criteria (31). Inclusion criteria were: (1) 35–64 years old, (2) with or without a research diagnosis of a mood disorder [lifetime bipolar I disorder, lifetime bipolar II disorder, recurrent major depression from MINI for DSM-IV (32)], (3) affectively stable, [i.e. absence of current depressed or manic mood episode from the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) (33) and Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (34)] and without current symptomatic posttraumatic stress disorder, (4) native English speaker able to read. Exclusion criteria were: (1) alcohol or substance abuse or dependence during the past 12 months, (2) positive toxicology screen, (3) dementia or mild cognitive impairment [from the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (35)], (4) contraindication to an MRI scan, (5) history of head injury resulting in loss of consciousness, (6) medical condition that would interfere with the MRI protocol, (7) current pregnancy.
Assessment
After a phone screen, eligible participants were invited to participate in the study. Prior to the first study visit participants provided written informed consent. A psychiatrist or highly trained clinical MA-level coordinator administered the MINI to assess Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) psychiatric research diagnoses. The MADRS and YMRS were administered to assess current depressive and manic symptoms. Participants additionally completed the Hopkins Adult Reading Test (36) as an indicator of full-scale IQ and the MoCA to assess possible dementia or mild cognitive impairment. Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (37) was used to assess handedness.
Perceived Stress
Participants completed the PSS (30). The PSS is a 10 item self-report scale to measure individuals' perceptions of stress over the past month by assessing both experienced levels of stress and coping. The PSS has been validated in international samples (38–40) and has strong psychometric properties (41). Three participants did not complete the PSS and were not included in related analyses.
1H-MRS Acquisition and Processing
1H-MRS was performed on a clinical 7.0 Tesla MR scanner (Siemens Magnetom Terra) with a 32-channel receiver head coil. Prior to spectroscopy T1-weighted images were acquired with a 3D magnetization-prepared-rapid-gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence (TR = 2,290 ms, TE = 2.95 ms, voxel size = 0.7 × 0.7 × 0.7 mm3, acquisition matrix = 224 × 168, flip angle = 7°, slice thickness 0.70 mm, 240 total slices) and were used for localization of the voxel. Single-voxel MRS utilized a STEAM localization sequence (TR/TE/TM = 3,000/20 /10 ms, 128 averages) positioned in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC, voxel size = 40 × 20 × 20 mm3), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC, voxel size = 20 × 20 × 20 mm3), and left DLPFC (voxel size = 20 × 20 × 20 mm3) as shown in Supplementary Figure 1. Voxel locations were placed in three imaging planes by an experienced MRS technologist based on the following parameters. For ACC, the voxel is initially placed on a midline sagittal slice such that the anterior end of the voxel starts at the genu of the corpus callosum and the rotation of the voxel is such that it is parallel to the corpus callosum. Axial images are reviewed to ensure the voxel is midline and coronal images are reviewed to ensure that maximal gray matter is included. For VMPFC, the voxel is placed anterior to the genu of the corpus callosum such that the center of the voxel is along the bicommissural line. Axial and coronal images are reviewed to ensure that the voxel is in the midline and maximizes the amount of gray matter. For DLPFC, the voxel is placed on the coronal image between the superior and inferior frontal sulcus with a rotation that is parallel to the side of the skull. Axial and sagittal images are reviewed to ensure that the voxel does not contain the skull. Each voxel underwent automated optimization including 3-dimensional shimming, transmit gain, frequency adjustment, and water suppression. When the full width at half maximum (FWHM) of the water signal was ≥30 Hz, manual shimming was performed to optimize the magnetic field homogeneity of the selected spectroscopy volume of interest to a line width of <30 Hz FWHM of the absolute water signal.
OpenMRSLab was utilized for pre-processing including coil combination, frequency and phase correction as well as eddy current correction and residual water removal (42). LCModel software (43) was used for the 1H single voxel spectroscopy spectral fitting using simulated spectra of 19 metabolites as a customized basis set. All spectra were quality controlled by visual inspection of the spectra and according to the following criteria: (1) a FWHM linewidth of the unsuppressed water spectrum <30Hz, (2) signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) <5, and (3) Cramer-Rao Lower Bound (CRLB) of NAA >5. Spectra that did not meet these criteria were re-processed with eddy current and frequency shift correction. If issues remained after re-processing, those spectra were eliminated from further analyses. FWHM, SNR, and CRLB are described in Supplementary Table 3. GABA levels in institutional units (i.u.) were calculated in the LCModel output and standard correction for CSF was applied.
rfMRI Acquisition and Processing
On the same day as the scan for 1H-MRS a second scan for rfMRI was performed on a clinical 3.0 Tesla MR scanner (Siemens TIM Skyra, VD13) with a 20-channel receiver head coil. Two participants did not complete this scan and were not included in related analyses. Whole-brain functional imaging was performed using a gradient-echo EPI pulse sequence with z-shim gradients to compensate for magnetic susceptibility-induced signal loss at the base of the brain (44–46) with the following parameters: 21 contiguous oblique-axial slices, 5.0 mm slice thickness, TR/TE = 1,200/30ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 240 × 240 mm, voxel size = 3.8 × 3.8 × 5.0 mm, 250 total images. Duration of the resting state scan was 5:06 min and participants were instructed to lie still with their eyes closed and let their mind freely wander. Participants communicated with study staff immediately before and after the scan to verify they were awake. One participant reported falling asleep during the scan and was excluded from related analyses. T1-weighted images were acquired with a 3D magnetization-prepared-rapid-gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence (TR = 2,300 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, voxel size = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.1 mm, acquisition matrix = 248 × 256, flip angle = 9°, 176 sagittal slices) and used for co-registration.
We used the CONN Toolbox Version 19b (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn/) implemented through Matlab Version 2019b for processing structural and functional MRI data. We employed CONN's default preprocessing pipeline for volume-based analyses (direct normalization to MNI-space) which performs the following preprocessing steps via procedures in SPM12: functional realignment and unwarp; slice-timing correction; outlier identification; direct segmentation and normalization; and spatial smoothing. Specifically, functional data were coregistered and resampled to the first scan of each session using b-spline interpolation for the realignment and unwarping procedure. For slice-timing correction, functional data were time-shifted to correct for interleaved slice acquisition and resampled using sinc-interpolation to match the time in the middle of each acquisition time. Potential outlier scans were identified based on the observed global BOLD signal and the amount of subject-motion in the scanner using the “conservative” threshold settings in CONN of 0.5 mm for scan-to-scan frame-wise displacement and 3 s.d. away from the mean global brain activation across the entire volume for each timepoint. Functional and anatomical data were normalized into standard MNI space and segmented into gray matter, white matter, and CSF tissues classes with a resampling of functional data to 2 mm isotropic voxels and structural data to 1 mm isotropic voxels data using 4th order spline interpolation. Last, functional data were smoothed using a spatial convolution with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm FWHM.
To account for artifact and physiological noise in the data we implemented CONN's default denoising pipeline involving linear regression of potential confounding effects in the BOLD signal and temporal filtering. The default denoising pipeline implements an anatomical component-based noise correction procedure (aCompCor) that includes noise components from cerebral white matter and cerebrospinal areas and includes other noise components of estimated subject-motion parameters (12 components), scrubbing of outlier scans (components vary per subject), and first order linear session effects. In addition, based on recommendations that higher frequencies contain informative resting state signals (47) and based on processing pipelines implemented in other studies including the Human Connectome Project (48–50) we implemented a temporal high-pass filter of 0.01 Hz to minimize the influence of physiological, head-motion, and other noise sources while focusing on frequency fluctuations informative of resting-state networks.
Statistical Analysis
Relationships between continuous variables were tested using Pearson's correlations and t-tests were used to test relationships involving dichotomous variables. Given evidence of differential responses to stress by biological sex and age, we assessed these variables along with diagnosis, trauma exposure, symptom ratings, and handedness in relation to PSS scores.
MRS Analyses
We tested for correlations between continuous measures of GABA concentrations and total PSS scores using Pearson's correlations. In this hypothesis generating, preliminary analysis we did not correct for multiple testing. When a nominally significant correlation between PSS score and GABA was observed, the voxel was carried forward for functional connectivity analyses.
Functional Connectivity Analyses
Emergent nominally significant correlations between GABA and PSS scores were used to inform seed region selection for subsequent RSFC analyses. Masks of the MRS-determined voxel region were created for each subject and translated into MNI space for use as subject-specific seed ROIs in RSFC processing. We conducted seed-based correlations between the MRS-determined seed and network nodes in the DMN (posterior cingulate cortex [PCC], left and right lateral parietal [LP], medial prefrontal cortex [mPFC]), CEN (left and right lateral prefrontal cortex [LPFC] and left and right PPC), and SN (ACC, left and right insula, left and right supramarginal gyrus [SMG], left and right rostral prefrontal cortex [RPFC]) and with limbic areas (bilateral hippocampus, bilateral amygdala) using preconstructed ROIs from the Harvard-Oxford atlas in CONN. Network nodes were chosen based on predefined network assignment in CONN.
To examine functional connectivity between the seed region with each ROI we conducted ROI-to-ROI connectivity analyses which produce weighted connectivity matrices computed from a Weighted Least Squares (WLS) linear model to characterize functional connectivity strength. For second-level analyses CONN implements a General Linear Modeling (GLM) framework for testing between-subjects and between-conditions contrasts. We conducted a one-sample t-test to determine ROI-to-ROI connectivity from the seed region to each of our pre-defined ROIs. Results were thresholded at an analysis-level false discovery (FDR) rate of p < 0.05 to correct for the total number of connections tested from the seed ROI. Bivariate correlation coefficients were calculated between the average time-courses for the seed to each ROI in our pre-defined set and converted to Fisher-transformed z-scores. Z-scores were extracted for significant RSFC results from the ROI-to-ROI analyses for use in primary analyses where separate correlation analyses were used to test the relationship of RSFC results with PSS scores and with cortical GABA. Results were deemed significant at a nominal threshold of p < 0.05.
Results
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics
In total, of the 17 original subjects, 3 did not complete the PSS, 1 reported falling asleep during the 3T scanning session and was removed from related analyses, and in 3 participants GABA measurements did not meet our quality standards and were removed from their respective analyses (1 participant removed from DLPFC, 1 participant removed from ACC, and 1 participant removed from VMPFC analyses). Consequently, for comparisons involving perceived stress and GABA levels as well as comparisons involving perceived stress and RSFC there was complete data for 13 participants and for comparisons involving GABA levels and RSFC, 15 participants. In total 12 participants had complete data across all analyses. Demographic and clinical features of the participants are shown in Table 1. Three participants (17.6%) had an anxiety disorder. As expected, younger age (r = −0.61, p = 0.021) and mood disorder diagnosis (t12 = 3.68, p = 0.003) were significantly related to greater total PSS scores. None of the demographic or clinical variables tested were significantly associated with GABA levels (p > 0.054), but a trend was present for mood disorder diagnosis.
Table 1. Demographic, clinical characteristics, and relevant relationships with PSS scores and DLPFC GABA.
Perceived Stress and GABA Levels
We observed a significant negative correlation between PSS scores and left DLPFC GABA (r = −0.62, p = 0.023; Table 1; Figure 1) while VMPFC GABA (r = −0.46, p = 0.086) and ACC GABA (r = 0.19, p = 0.479) were not significantly correlated with PSS scores (Supplementary Table 1).
Figure 1. Perceived stress and DLPFC GABA. (Left) DLPFC voxel placement and example of spectrum. (Right) Negative correlation between perceived stress scale (PSS) scores and left DLPFC GABA. DLPFC GABA measurements are in institutional units. There were two participants (cases 11 and 12) with similar values for both DLPFC GABA and PSS score. These data points are labeled with the case number to identify the overlapping data points.
Perceived Stress and RSFC
Having observed a significant nominal correlation between left DLPFC GABA and PSS scores, subject-specific left DLPFC voxel masks were implemented as the seed ROIs in the seed-based ROI-to-ROI analyses. We extracted the Fisher-transformed Z-scores for all significant connections and carried them forward to test for relationships with PSS scores and GABA. Significant connections with the left DLPFC voxel carried forward included: bilateral LPFC, bilateral PPC, bilateral LP, bilateral RPFC and the ACC (Supplementary Figures 2, 3; Supplementary Table 2). In exploratory analyses we also extracted the Fisher-transformed z-scores for non-significant connections and tested the relationships with PSS as all a priori ROIs were of interest.
No significant correlations emerged between PSS scores and any of the primary RSFC connections tested (all p > 0.148; Table 2). For exploratory RSFC analyses with PSS scores we observed a trend toward a negative association between PSS scores and RSFC from the left DLPFC voxel to the left hippocampus (r = −0.52, p = 0.068; Supplementary Table 4).
Table 2. Correlations of PSS scores and DLPFC GABA with significant RSFC from the DLPFC voxel seeds.
RSFC and DLPFC GABA
As expected, We observed a significant negative correlation between left DLPFC GABA with FC between our left DLPFC voxel and the left LPFC ROI from the Harvard-Oxford atlas in CONN, (r = −0.67, p = 0.006; Table 2). No significant associations were observed in the main analyses, or in exploratory analyses testing DLPFC GABA and nonsignificant RSFC (all p > 0.074; Supplementary Table 4). However, a trend in positive association emerged between left DLPFC GABA with left DLPFC voxel-MPFC FC (r = 0.47, p = 0.077), and a trend in negative association emerged between left DLPFC GABA and FC between the left DLPFC voxel and the left SMG, a node of the SN (r = −0.47, p = 0.074; Supplementary Table 4). Given the small sample size, formal tests of mediation and moderation were not feasible so we restricted our analyses to testing correlations between GABA and RSFC.
Discussion
In this preliminary study we examined GABAergic function as a potential mechanism related to perceived stress and stress-related functional network dysregulation. We tested the association of self-reported perceived stress with brain GABA levels in prefrontal control regions along with corresponding alterations in RSFC in intrinsic networks and limbic regions implicated in the regulation of the stress response. We found that measures of perceived stress were negatively related to GABA levels in the left DLPFC. However, we did not observe a significant relationship between perceived stress and RSFC.
The DLPFC is thought to play an inhibitory role in the physiological response to stress via regulation of amygdala projections to hypothalamus, a key region of the HPA-axis known to be modulated by GABAergic activity in response to stress (23, 51, 52). The DLPFC has been targeted in neurostimulation treatment aimed at improving stress-related cognitive impairments and physiological responses. DLPFC stimulation by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been shown to increase GABA levels in the DLPFC and striatum, respectively (53, 54). Moreover, tDCS of the DLPFC has been shown to prevent acute stress-induced working memory deficits (55) and high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (HF-rTMS) over the left DLPFC applied before a social stress task blunted cortisol response (52). Our result showing an inverse relationship between perceived stress and left DLPFC GABA levels is consistent with these findings. However, we did not observe a relationship between PSS scores and GABA levels in the VMPFC or ACC, despite evidence implicating altered GABA levels in these regions in the etiology of chronic stress-related conditions such as major depressive disorder (MDD) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (56).
Task-based functional neuroimaging studies suggest the left DLPFC and hippocampus along with the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), dorsal ACC, and left insula make up a stress adaptation response network (57). RSFC studies have shown that FC between the hippocampus and the DLPFC increases in response to a stressor and predict feeling less subjective stress and lower subjective arousal in response to a stressor (58). In addition, decreased hippocampal FC to the precuneus and increase FC to the middle frontal gyrus are associated with greater perceived stress. Our finding of a trend in negative association between perceived stress and RSFC from the DLPFC voxel to the left hippocampus extends prior work indicating that increased FC between these regions predicts lower subject arousal to and feelings of stress and suggests a possible role for DLPFC-hippocampus FC in the stress adaption response.
Although we found perceived stress to be related to GABA levels in the left DLPFC but not to RSFC with the left DLPFC voxel seed, we did observe a significant negative relationship between GABA levels in the left DLPFC and RSFC between the left DLPFC voxel and the left LPFC node of the CEN. The LPFC includes the DLPFC, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) and frontal eye fields (59) and GABA+ levels in the DLPFC and VLPFC have been shown to be positively correlated (60); thus, it would be expected to find an association of left DLPFC GABA with connectivity between the left DLPFC and the left LPFC node. Previous research has shown that GABA concentrations in the mPFC are associated with between-network anticorrelations of the DMN and CEN but that DLPFC GABA concentrations did not play a significant role in between-network interactions (61). In contrast, our result of a trend in positive association between DLPFC GABA and DLPFC voxel-MPFC FC implicates DLPFC GABA in between-network interactions of the CEN and DMN but in terms of increased connectivity rather than anticorrelation between the DLPFC and MPFC nodes. Additionally, the CEN is typically more active during cognitive tasks requiring externally directed thought (19) and anti-correlated with the DMN at rest (16). We also observed a trend in negative association between DLPFC GABA levels and FC from DLPFC voxel to left SMG, a node in the SN. Functional imaging studies have revealed decreased DLPFC activity and increased SMG activity in response to stress induction (62) and increased SMG activity has also been shown to correlate with stress-induced cortisol measures (63). Taken together these results trends suggest that it is possible that the association of DLPFC GABA levels with PSS may be related to aberrant CEN-DMN and CEN-SN interactions. However, studies involving larger samples sizes are need to better elucidate these results.
Findings of the current study should be evaluated with consideration of several limitations. The sample size in this study is small, limiting our ability to detect small effects and to test for effects of potential confounding, mediating and moderating variables. However, this preliminary study aimed to generate hypotheses to be tested in future studies and provides valuable initial data related to the role of GABAergic functioning in perceived stress that warrant further investigation. This study did not include biological measurements of HPA-axis activity or autonomic nervous system function response to stress. The collection of such measures would capture additional facets underlying stress and the perception of stress that may additionally be related to GABAergic functioning and RSFC. Given the large voxel sizes utilized in MRS, biologically important heterogeneity within each voxel may not be captured. For rsfMRI in this preliminary study, we implemented a high-pass filter only in the pre-processing of the data. Though others have recommended this decision the use of band-pass filtering instead is still debated in rsfMRI research. We therefore note that our analyses may be less generalizable to studies using band-pass filtering in the processing pipeline. In addition, we limited our analyses to a priori atlas-defined ROI and thus do not report on whole-brain seed-to-voxel analyses that might have identified additional regions that are biologically relevant to perceived stress. Such analyses would be of interest in future studies in larger samples, as would formal statistical examination of GABA as a potential modifier or mediator of the relationship between perceived stress and RSFC together with the impact of diagnosis, age, sex, and biological measurements of HPA-axis activity, all of which were beyond the scope of this preliminary study.
Conclusions
In the present study, we conducted a preliminary investigation regarding the relationship of perceived stress, brain GABA levels, and RSFC in stress-related networks and limbic regions. Our preliminary results support an association between perceived stress and dysregulated GABAergic functioning in DLPFC, a core node of the CEN, an intrinsic network thought to underlie goal-directed attentional processes. This line of work has potential to shed light on GABAergic and other neurobiological mechanisms that may contribute to increased perception of stress experiences and coping which may in turn aid in the development of prevention and treatment strategies to improve outcomes in stress-related conditions.
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.
Ethics Statement
The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Mass General Brigham Human Subjects Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
PM, AL, JT, and LH contributed to the study design. EC, WZ, and JB performed data analysis. HL and WZ contributed to data collection. All authors contributed to manuscript preparation.
Funding
This work was supported by grants from the Brigham and Women's Hospital Connors Center for Women's Health and Gender Biology WHISPR Pilot Award (PM) and from the National Institutes of Health R01MH110979 (PM), U54AG062322 (PM) and T32MH017119 (JB).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.802449/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Calabrese F, Molteni R, Racagni G, Riva MA. Neuronal plasticity: a link between stress and mood disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2009) 34:S208–16. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.014
2. Kino T. Stress, glucocorticoid hormones, and hippocampal neural progenitor cells: implications to mood disorders. Front Physiol. (2015) 6:230. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2015.00230
3. Nugent NR, Tyrka AR, Carpenter LL, Price LH. Gene–environment interactions: early life stress and risk for depressive and anxiety disorders. Psychopharmacology. (2011) 214:175–96. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2151-x
4. Marin M-F, Lord C, Andrews J, Juster R-P, Sindi S, Arsenault-Lapierre G, et al. Chronic stress, cognitive functioning and mental health. Neurobiol Learn Mem. (2011) 96:583–95. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.02.016
5. Folkman S, Lazarus RS. Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer Publishing Company. (1984).
6. Todrank Heth J, Somer E. Characterizing stress tolerance: “controllability awareness” and its relationship to perceived stress and reported health. Pers Individ Dif. (2002) 33:883–95. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00198-2
7. Hewitt PL, Flett GL, Mosher SW. The perceived stress scale: factor structure and relation to depression symptoms in a psychiatric sample. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (1992) 14:247–57. doi: 10.1007/BF00962631
8. Turner AD, James BD, Capuano AW, Aggarwal NT, Barnes LL. Perceived stress and cognitive decline in different cognitive domains in a cohort of older African Americans. Am J Geriatr Psychiat. (2017) 25:25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.10.003
9. Fox MD, Greicius M. Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front Syst Neurosci. (2010) 4:19. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00019
10. Smitha K, Akhil Raja K, Arun K, Rajesh P, Thomas B, Kapilamoorthy T, et al. Resting state fMRI: a review on methods in resting state connectivity analysis and resting state networks. Neuroradiol J. (2017) 30:305–17. doi: 10.1177/1971400917697342
11. Menon V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn Sci. (2011) 15:483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003
12. Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Huang C, Buckner RL. Evidence for the default network's role in spontaneous cognition. J Neurophysiol. (2010) 104:322–35. doi: 10.1152/jn.00830.2009
13. Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2001) 98:4259–64. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071043098
14. Raichle ME. The brain's default mode network. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2015) 38:433–47. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030
15. Zhou H-X, Chen X, Shen Y-Q, Li L, Chen N-X, Zhu Z-C, et al. Rumination and the default mode network: Meta-analysis of brain imaging studies and implications for depression. Neuroimage. (2020) 206:116287. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116287
16. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain's default network. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2008) 1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011
17. Uddin LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2015) 16:55–61. doi: 10.1038/nrn3857
18. Goulden N, Khusnulina A, Davis NJ, Bracewell RM, Bokde AL, McNulty JP, et al. The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: replication from DCM. Neuroimage. (2014) 99:180–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.052
19. Uddin LQ, Yeo BTT, Spreng RN. Towards a universal taxonomy of macro-scale functional human brain networks. Brain Topogr. (2019) 32:926–42. doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00744-6
20. Soares JM, Sampaio A, Ferreira LM, Santos NC, Marques P, Marques F, et al. Stress impact on resting state brain networks. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e66500. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066500
21. Liston C, McEwen BS, Casey BJ. Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2009) 106:912–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0807041106
22. Taren AA, Gianaros PJ, Greco CM, Lindsay EK, Fairgrieve A, Brown KW, et al. Mindfulness meditation training alters stress-related amygdala resting state functional connectivity: a randomized controlled trial. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2015) 10:1758–68. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv066
23. Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:397–409. doi: 10.1038/nrn2647
24. Farag NH, Moore WE, Lovallo WR, Mills PJ, Khandrika S, Eichner JE. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function: relative contributions of perceived stress and obesity in women. J Women's Health. (2008) 17:1647–55. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2008.0866
25. Goldman N, Glei DA, Seplaki C, Liu I-W, Weinstein M. Perceived stress and physiological dysregulation in older adults. Stress. (2005) 8:95–105. doi: 10.1080/10253890500141905
26. Ghosal S, Hare B, Duman RS. Prefrontal cortex GABAergic deficits and circuit dysfunction in the pathophysiology and treatment of chronic stress and depression. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2017) 14:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.09.012
27. Michels L, Schulte-Vels T, Schick M, O'Gorman RL, Zeffiro T, Hasler G, et al. Prefrontal GABA and glutathione imbalance in posttraumatic stress disorder: preliminary findings. Psychiat Res Neuroimag. (2014) 224:288–95. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.09.007
28. Dedovic K, D'Aguiar C, Pruessner JC. What stress does to your brain: a review of neuroimaging studies. Canad J Psychiat. (2009) 54:6–15. doi: 10.1177/070674370905400104
29. Wijtenburg SA, Rowland LM, Edden RAE, Barker PB. Reproducibility of brain spectroscopy at 7T using conventional localization and spectral editing techniques. J Magn Reson Imag. (2013) 38:460–7. doi: 10.1002/jmri.23997
30. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. (1983) 24:385–96. doi: 10.2307/2136404
31. Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, et al. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry. (2010) 167:748–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379
32. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiat. (1998) 59:22–33. doi: 10.1037/t18597-000
33. Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiat. (1979) 134:382–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.134.4.382
34. Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiat. (1978) 133:429–35. doi: 10.1192/bjp.133.5.429
35. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2005) 53:695–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x
36. Schretlen DJ, Winicki JM, Meyer SM, Testa SM, Pearlson GD, Gordon B. Development, psychometric properties, and validity of the hopkins adult reading test (HART). Clin Neuropsychol. (2009) 23:926–43. doi: 10.1080/13854040802603684
37. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. (1971) 9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
38. Andreou E, Alexopoulos EC, Lionis C, Varvogli L, Gnardellis C, Chrousos GP, et al. Perceived stress scale: reliability and validity study in greece. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2011) 8:3287–98. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8083287
39. Baik SH, Fox RS, Mills SD, Roesch SC, Sadler GR, Klonoff EA, et al. Reliability and validity of the Perceived Stress Scale-10 in Hispanic Americans with English or Spanish language preference. J Health Psychol. (2019) 24:628–39. doi: 10.1177/1359105316684938
40. Roberti JW, Harrington LN, Storch EA. Further psychometric support for the 10-item version of the perceived stress scale. J College Counsel. (2006) 9:135–47. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-1882.2006.tb00100.x
41. Lee E-H. Review of the psychometric evidence of the perceived stress scale. Asian Nurs Res. (2012) 6:121–7. doi: 10.1016/j.anr.2012.08.004
42. Rowland B, Mariano L, Irvine JM, Lin AP. OpenMRSLab: An open-source softward repository for Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy data analysis tools. In: International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine MR Spectroscopy Workshop. (2016).
43. Provencher SW. Estimation of metabolite concentrations from localized in vivo proton NMR spectra. Magn Reson Med. (1993) 30:672–9. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910300604
44. Gu H, Feng H, Zhan W, Xu S, Silbersweig DA, Stern E, et al. Single-shot interleaved Z-Shim EPI with optimized compensation for signal losses due to susceptibility-induced field inhomogeneity at 3 T. Neuroimage. (2002) 17:1358–64. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1274
45. Hoge WS, Kraft RA, Stern E, Pan H. A method for z-shim compensated EPI-bold imaging in a single shot. In: 2013 IEEE 10th International Symposium on Biomedical Imaging. (2013). p. 338–41. doi: 10.1109/ISBI.2013.6556481
46. Stern E, Silbersweig DA. System and method for Z-Shim compensated echo-planar magnetic resonance imaging (Patent No. 61,871,103). (2014).
47. Bijsterbosch J, Smith S, Beckman C. Introduction to Resting State fMRI Functional Connectivity. Oxford Univ. Press. (2017).
48. Andoh J, Ferreira M, Leppert IR, Matsushita R, Pike B, Zatorre RJ. How restful is it with all that noise? Comparison of Interleaved silent steady state (ISSS) and conventional imaging in resting-state fMRI. Neuroimage. (2017) 147:726–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.065
49. Pruim RHR, Mennes M, Buitelaar JK, Beckmann CF. Evaluation of ICA-AROMA and alternative strategies for motion artifact removal in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. (2015) 112:278–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.063
50. Smith SM, Beckmann CF, Andersson J, Auerbach EJ, Bijsterbosch J, Douaud G, et al. Resting-state fMRI in the human connectome project. Neuroimage. (2013) 80:144–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.039
51. Chen F, Ke J, Qi R, Xu Q, Zhong Y, Liu T, et al. Increased inhibition of the amygdala by the mPFC may reflect a resilience factor in post-traumatic stress disorder: a resting-state fMRI granger causality analysis. Front Psychiatry. (2018) 9:516. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00516
52. Pulopulos MM, Schmausser M, De Smet S, Vanderhasselt M-A, Baliyan S, Venero C, et al. The effect of HF-rTMS over the left DLPFC on stress regulation as measured by cortisol and heart rate variability. Horm Behav. (2020) 124:104803. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104803
53. Bunai T, Hirosawa T, Kikuchi M, Fukai M, Yokokura M, Ito S, et al. tDCS-induced modulation of GABA concentration and dopamine release in the human brain: A combination study of magnetic resonance spectroscopy and positron emission tomography. Brain Stimul. (2021) 14:154–60. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2020.12.010
54. Levitt JG, Kalender G, O'Neill J, Diaz JP, Cook IA, Ginder N, et al. Dorsolateral prefrontal γ-aminobutyric acid in patients with treatment-resistant depression after transcranial magnetic stimulation measured with magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J Psychiat Neurosci. (2019) 44:386–94. doi: 10.1503/jpn.180230
55. Bogdanov M, Schwabe L. Transcranial stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex prevents stress-induced working memory deficits. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:1429–37. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3687-15.2016
56. Schür RR, Draisma LWR, Wijnen JP, Boks MP, Koevoets MGJC, Joëls M, et al. Brain GABA levels across psychiatric disorders: a systematic literature review and meta-analysis of 1H-MRS studies. Hum Brain Mapp. (2016) 37:3337–52. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23244
57. Sinha R, Lacadie CM, Constable RT, Seo D. Dynamic neural activity during stress signals resilient coping. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2016) 113:8837–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1600965113
58. Goldfarb EV, Rosenberg MD, Seo D, Constable RT, Sinha R. Hippocampal seed connectome-based modeling predicts the feeling of stress. Nat Commun. (2020) 11:2650. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-16492-2
59. Petrides M. Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2005) 360:781–95. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1631
60. Wang K, Smolker HR, Brown MS, Snyder HR, Hankin BL, Banich MT. Association of γ-aminobutyric acid and glutamate/glutamine in the lateral prefrontal cortex with patterns of intrinsic functional connectivity in adults. Brain Struct Funct. (2020) 225:1903–19. doi: 10.1007/s00429-020-02084-9
61. Chen X, Fan X, Hu Y, Zuo C, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Holt D, et al. Regional GABA concentrations modulate inter-network resting-state functional connectivity. Cerebral Cortex. (2019) 29:1607–18. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy059
62. Dedovic K, Rexroth M, Wolff E, Duchesne A, Scherling C, Beaudry T, et al. Neural correlates of processing stressful information: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Res Stress Cop Dis. (2009) 1293:49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.06.044
Keywords: perceived stress, gamma-aminobutyric acid, magnetic resonance spectroscopy, prefrontal cortex, rfMRI
Citation: Busler JN, Coello E, Liao H, Taylor J, Zhao W, Holsen LM, Lin AP and Mahon PB (2022) Perceived Stress, Cortical GABA, and Functional Connectivity Correlates: A Hypothesis-Generating Preliminary Study. Front. Psychiatry 13:802449. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.802449
Received: 26 October 2021; Accepted: 24 January 2022;
Published: 08 March 2022.
Edited by:
Ali Saffet Gonul, Ege University, TurkeyReviewed by:
Gabriele Ende, University of Heidelberg, GermanyGregory Fonzo, The University of Texas at Austin, United States
Julian Chase Motzkin, University of California, San Francisco, United States
Copyright © 2022 Busler, Coello, Liao, Taylor, Zhao, Holsen, Lin and Mahon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Pamela B. Mahon, pmahon@bwh.harvard.edu