Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychiatry, 09 January 2023
Sec. Psychological Therapy and Psychosomatics
This article is part of the Research Topic Bridging the Gap: an Interdisciplinary Perspective on Alexithymia View all 4 articles

Hormonal abnormalities in alexithymia

  • 1Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
  • 2Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine 10, Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Medical Faculty, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany

Alexithymia is a personality trait characterized by difficulties in emotion recognition and regulation that is associated with deficits in social cognition. High alexithymia levels are considered a transdiagnostic risk factor for a range of psychiatric and medical conditions, including depression, anxiety, and autism. Hormones are known to affect social–emotional cognition and behavior in humans, including the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin, the steroid hormones testosterone and estradiol, the stress hormone cortisol as well as thyroid hormones. However, few studies have investigated hormonal effects on alexithymia and on alexithymia-related impairments in emotion regulation and reactivity, stress response, and social cognition. Here, we provide a brief overview of the evidence linking alexithymia to abnormalities in hormone levels, particularly with regard to cortisol and oxytocin, for which most evidence exists, and to thyroid hormones. We address the current lack of research on the influence of sex hormones on alexithymia and alexithymia-related deficits, and lastly provide future directions for research on associations between hormonal abnormalities and deficits in emotion regulation and social cognition associated with alexithymia.

1. Introduction

Alexithymia is a personality construct characterized by difficulty understanding one’s own emotions, distinguishing them from bodily signals of arousal, and verbalizing one’s feelings to others. The term “alexithymic” was coined by Sifneos (1) to describe a variety of patients with psychosomatic diseases who showed a common deficit, a marked difficulty finding words to describe their feelings. The alexithymia concept captured the attention of specialists in psychosomatic medicine and became the main theme of the 11th European Conference on Psychosomatic Research (2). Despite several debates, alexithymia was conceptualized as a hypothetical construct that could be operationalized and empirically evaluated [for reviews on the history and debates surrounding the alexithymia construct, see (3, 4)]. While several theoretical models of the alexithymia construct exist (5), the dominant model encompasses (1) difficulty in identifying and describing feelings; (2) difficulty in distinguishing between feelings and bodily sensations of arousal; (3) restrained and limited imaginative processes; and (4) a cognitive style oriented toward the outside (6, 7). As a consequence of these difficulties, alexithymia is associated with reduced empathy for others, impulsive behaviors, and problems in interpersonal relationships, and individuals with high degrees of alexithymia are sometimes described as being unimaginative and boring, having a stiff and wooden posture, and to seem cold and distant in social interactions [e.g., (8, 9)].

Today, most researchers agree that alexithymia is a dimensional, normally distributed, subclinical personality trait [e.g., (4, 10, 11)]. While several methods exist to assess alexithymia, both interviews and self-report scales, the most widely used measure is the Toronto Alexithymia Scale [TAS-20; (12)]. The TAS-20 is a five-point Likert-type 20-item self-report scale that assesses the total alexithymia level as well as three factors: difficulties identifying feelings (DIF), difficulties describing feelings (DDF), and externally oriented thinking (EOT). The international cut-off for clinically relevant alexithymia is a TAS-20 total score > 61, based on which an alexithymia prevalence of 10% in the general population was observed (13, 14).

Alexithymia is considered a transdiagnostic risk factor for affective disorders (e.g., depression, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress disorder). Moreover, alexithymia is associated with medical conditions that include a psychosomatic component (i.e., pain syndromes, gastroenterological, and dermatological disease), but also with primarily medical conditions such as cancer, cardiological, and neurological diseases. Theories linking alexithymia and physical illness involve the physiological level [e.g., the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, chronic sympathetic hyperarousal, inflammation], but also the behavioral and the cognitive level [e.g., illness perception and behavior, somatic amplification; for recent reviews, see (15, 16)]. Despite five decades of research, the intricate relationship between alexithymia and mental and physical disease proneness is still far from being fully understood.

In 1985, the stress-alexithymia hypothesis was proposed, stating that alexithymic individuals have difficulties identifying the emotions that are linked to physiological arousal when experiencing a stressful situation (17). These difficulties are accompanied by increased somatic markers (e.g., increased heart rate and higher electrodermal activity), resulting in sustained autonomic hyperactivity, which in turn may lead to psychologically and physiologically harmful consequences. In their review, Panayiotou et al. (16) found that several studies reported hyperarousal at rest in alexithymia or during emotional stimulation, which may indicate poor regulation of emotional response intensity. However, many studies observed opposing findings of attenuated autonomic responses, indicating diminished automatic emotion processing or aberrant emotion regulation involving blunted emotional responses, emotion suppression, or avoidance.

A further important hypothesis to understand the relationship between alexithymia and altered physiological response is the decoupling hypothesis (18). This hypothesis suggests a dissociation between physiological arousal and subjective awareness of emotions in alexithymic individuals. A recent review identified decoupling between different emotion response systems as a consistent finding in the alexithymia literature, concluding that “the large majority of studies support the presence of decoupling in alexithymia, with evidence clearly in the direction of blunted physiological activation and normal or heightened reported affect” (16).

Despite the large body of literature on the physiological, behavioral and cognitive levels of alexithymia, relatively few studies investigated the role of potential hormonal imbalances contributing to alexithymia. Hormones such as oxytocin and cortisol are well known to affect emotion regulation and social cognition, which represent areas of major difficulty for individuals with high alexithymia levels, who struggle recognizing their own feelings and those of others, show reduced empathy and less altruistic behavior, and experience less distress watching others in pain [e.g., (19, 20); for a review on alexithymia and theory of mind, see (21); for a review on alexithymia and empathy, see (22)]. In the last two decades, the influence of hormones on human social–emotional behavior has received increasing scientific attention. A review on single hormone administration studies established the important roles of the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin as well as of the steroid hormones testosterone and estradiol on social–emotional behavior in humans (23).

Hormones and stress are inherently linked: A stressful experience can trigger a cascade of hormonal responses. Upon receiving a distress signal from the amygdala, the hypothalamus activates the sympathetic nervous system and the HPA axis, prompting enhanced secretion of glucocorticoids such as cortisol, catecholamines, growth hormone, and prolactin, which results in a burst of energy, a phenomenon known as the “fight-or-flight” response. The stress response is necessary to maintain homeostasis, but long-term activation of the stress system can be hazardous or even lethal (24). Affective disorders such as depression and anxiety are associated with abnormalities in the stress system [for a review, see (25)]: Stress promotes the occurrence of mood disorders, and these pathologies are related to modifications of HPA axis functioning (26). Psychosocial stress in childhood can program the HPA axis permanently into hyperactivity (27), rendering individuals vulnerable to environmental stress that can further activate the HPA axis (25). The stress-alexithymia hypothesis suggests the existence of a “viscous circle” between vulnerability to environmental stress in individuals with high alexithymia levels and failure to down-regulate emotional arousal, resulting in sustained autonomic hyperarousal. Indeed, some studies provided evidence for altered HPA axis functioning in alexithymia [for a review, see (15)].

In sum, alexithymia is a transdiagnostic risk factor for conditions such as depression, anxiety, and autism, all of which have been linked to hormonal abnormalities. Yet, very few studies have investigated associations between hormones and alexithymia. Here, we provide an overview of evidence on hormonal abnormalities in alexithymia, and explain why in our opinion future research would benefit from more thorough investigations into hormonal contributions to alexithymia and deficits in emotion regulation and social cognition as well altered physiological responses to stress associated with this personality trait.

2. Hormone studies in alexithymia

2.1. Cortisol

Cortisol, known as the stress hormone, is a glucocorticoid hormone produced in the adrenal gland. Studies demonstrated that cortisol promotes cognitive control of negative emotions (28, 29), that cortisol administration reduced phobic fears (30, 31) and that cortisol levels correlated with emotionality (32).

A potential association between the stress hormone cortisol and alexithymia was already tested in the early 90s (33). Based on observations that alexithymia can result from traumatic stress and on findings of high norepinephrine/cortisol ratios in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [for a review on the effects of traumatic stress on the brain, see (34)], Henry et al. (33) correlated norepinephrine and cortisol levels with alexithymia levels in a small group (N = 17) of formerly alcohol-dependent men. Norepinephrine/cortisol ratios correlated significantly with alexithymia, reminiscent of observations in PTSD. The authors suggested that higher alexithymia levels may be accompanied by an increasing functional separation between the sympatho-adrenal medullary axis and the HPA axis and a failure of the HPA axis to respond properly.

Härtwig et al. (35) investigated the cortisol awakening response (CAR; the rapid increase of cortisol levels directly after waking up in the morning) in individuals with low (N = 37) versus high (N = 41) alexithymia (35). The CAR measures basal activity of the HPA system, the stress hormone system that regulates stress responses and adaptation to environmental challenges. Chronic hyperactivation of the HPA-system is common in stress-related disorders (36). Härtwig et al. (35) found that high alexithymics had a significantly lower mean-CAR than low alexithymics, a result that remained stable after controlling for age and biological sex. Healthy individuals with high alexithymia levels thus seem to exhibit hypoactivity of the HPA system as measured by CAR. In addition, compared to low-alexithymics, high-alexithymics reported to experience higher levels of stress in interpersonal interactions and showed more extreme decoupling between physiological response and psychological perception.

Cortisol awakening response was also tested in female patients with bulimia nervosa with and without alexithymia (37). A significant reduction in CAR magnitude was observed in alexithymic compared to non-alexithymic patients, in line with the previous finding in healthy individuals. However, this study differentiated between the overall production of cortisol, measuring HPA basal activity, and overall cortisol increase, measuring HPA sensitivity. HPA basal activity, but not sensitivity, was decreased in alexithymic compared to non-alexithymic patients. This suggests intact HPA axis sensitivity, but reduced HPA basal activity, which may result in inadequate cortisol production and thus impaired stress response in alexithymia. However, an earlier study in patients with somatoform disorders found no association between alexithymia and HPA basal activity and sensitivity (38).

Kano et al. (39) investigated the effects of corticotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH), the central driver of the HPA axis, on adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) responses in relation to alexithymia in patients with irritable bowel syndrome compared to healthy controls. Across the sample, individuals with higher alexithymia levels showed significantly stronger ACTH responses to CRH injection, demonstrating a stronger endocrine stress response to CRH with higher alexithymia.

In sum, studies on cortisol suggest that alexithymia is linked to an altered function of the HPA axis, specifically reduced HPA basal activity, indexed by higher norepinephrine/cortisol ratios, reduced CAR, and an exaggerated ACTH stress response to CRH.

2.2. Thyroid hormones

Thyroid hormones play essential roles in mood regulation and cognition. The thyroid-stimulating hormone (TSH) is a hormone released by the pituitary gland. TSH stimulates the thyroid gland to produce thyroxine (T4), which regulates metabolism, mood, and body temperature. T4 is converted to triiodothyronine (T3), which is the active hormone that stimulates metabolism and oversees bone health. Free triiodothyronine (FT3) is elevated in hyperthyroidism and decreased in hypothyroidism. Autoimmune thyroiditis is linked to depression and anxiety disorders [for a systematic review and meta-analysis, see (4043)].

Only one study so far investigated the relationship between thyroid hormone levels and alexithymia, albeit in a postpartum setting (44). The authors measured serum thyrotropin (TSH), free thyroxine (FT4) and free triiodothyronine (FT3) hormone levels, and TAS-20 alexithymia levels in 74 healthy women on day 3 postpartum. Alexithymic versus non-alexithymic women were found to differ significantly in their thyroid hormone levels: The former had significantly lower FT4, higher FT3, and lower FT4:FT3 ratios than the latter, indicative of altered thyroid homeostasis in alexithymia. These relationships remained stable after controlling for postpartum depression. These findings suggest that alexithymia is linked to an imbalance between the amount of thyroid hormones needed by the body and the amount of thyroid hormones available, providing initial evidence for a link between alexithymia and thyroid dysfunction.

In a recent pilot study, Martino et al. (45) investigated the associations between alexithymia and depression, anxiety, and health-related quality of life in patients with Hashimoto’s thyroiditis. Patients with Hashimoto’s and control patients with general thyroid disease participated in this study. Across the sample, the majority (89.2%) of patients were alexithymic (TAS-20 score > 60) or potentially alexithymic (TAS-20 score 52–60). Although patients with Hashimoto’s also had mild depression and moderate to severe anxiety, TAS-20 sum scores did not correlate with the other scores in patients with Hashimoto’s (only DDF showed a moderate, inverse correlation with the cognitive component of depression). These results indicated an association between alexithymia and Hashimoto’s thyroiditis that was largely independent from comorbid anxiety and depression.

Hasegawa et al. (46) examined TAS-20 alexithymia scores in surgical patients with thyroid disease (Graves’ disease and papillary thyroid cancer) before and after thyroidectomy surgery. After surgery, all patients scored significantly higher on the DIF factor of the TAS-20 alexithymia scale, compared to pre-surgically. Although hormone levels indicative of thyroid function were not assessed in this study, these results suggest an increase in DIF alexithymia levels following thyroid removal. However, the authors provided no physiological mechanism potentially underlying these changes, and no follow-up was performed, leaving potential long-term effects unknown.

In a further study, an alexithymia prevalence of 51.9% in 162 patients with chronic autoimmune thyroiditis was reported (47). The authors suggested that “alexithymia is not only an indirect risk factor of the autoimmune thyroiditis development but also a predictor of the disease course.”

In sum, very few studies examined potential associations of alexithymia with thyroid hormones, providing initial evidence for lower FT4, higher FT3, and lower FT4:FT3 ratios in alexithymic individuals, suggesting thyroid dysfunction. In addition, patients with chronic thyroid disease showed a much higher alexithymia prevalence (51.9%) than in the normal population (10%), and patients with thyroid disease had significantly more DIF after thyroidectomy than before surgery.

2.3. Oxytocin and vasopressin

Oxytocin and Arginine Vasopressin (AVP) are peptide hormones synthesized in the hypothalamus. Across different mammalian species, including humans, it has been demonstrated that AVP regulates social behaviors such as affiliation (48), social recognition (49), and cooperative behavior (50). In addition, AVP administration enhanced encoding of social information (51, 52), and AVP deficiency in patients is linked to deficits in categorizing socio-affective stimuli (53).

Many studies investigated the role of oxytocin on emotion regulation and social cognition. Intranasal oxytocin application increased positive communication during interpersonal conflict (54), modulated visual attention toward social signals of positive approach and threat (5557), and improved social cognition (5860). In fact, oxytocin administration has been suggested as a treatment for a number of disorders related to emotional and social dysfunction, including depression, anxiety, and autism [for reviews, see (49, 61)].

Luminet et al. (62) investigated the effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on individuals of varying socio-emotional ability in a randomized controlled trial including 60 male students who either received oxytocin or a placebo before performing the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test. The performance of individuals with lower alexithymia levels was equally good in both oxytocin and placebo conditions, whereas the performance of individuals with high alexithymia levels improved under oxytocin compared to placebo. These results demonstrated that oxytocin can improve socio-emotional competence in individuals with high alexithymia levels.

Koh et al. (63) hypothesized that variations in the oxytocin receptor gene might contribute to social-emotional difficulties in alexithymia. They tested potential associations between oxytocin receptor gene single nucleotide polymorphisms or haplotypes and alexithymia in 355 patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Single-marker and haplotype association analyses were performed with eight single nucleotide polymorphisms. However, no significant associations could be detected, leaving a role of genetic variations in the oxytocin receptor gene in alexithymia unclear.

Schneider-Hassloff et al. (64) focused on the single nucleotide polymorphism rs53576 in the oxytocin receptor gene and its interaction with childhood attachment security in 195 healthy participants. A subgroup (N = 163) additionally underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), during which they played the prisoner’s dilemma game, an interactive mentalizing task. It was observed that rs53576 and childhood attachment security jointly modulated alexithymia levels. Specifically, alexithymia levels were significantly higher in rs53576 GG-homozygotes with insecure versus secure childhood attachment. Moreover, lower alexithymia levels were associated with greater volumes of the superior parietal lobule, part of the parieto-frontal human mirror system (65) that is important for mentalizing abilities. These findings suggest that a gene-environment interaction between the rs53576 variation in the oxytocin receptor gene and insecure childhood attachment contributes to deficits in emotional awareness, the core characteristic of alexithymia.

Baskaran et al. (66) correlated serum oxytocin levels obtained every 5 min over a period of 10 h with alexithymia levels (amongst other measures) in healthy men. They observed strong inverse correlations between TAS-20 alexithymia levels and the secretory dynamics of oxytocin, which were driven by DIF. Additionally, subjects with reduced pulsatile release of oxytocin showed a more avoidant attachment style and felt less supported. These results further corroborate the role of oxytocin as a key mediator of social-emotional functioning and provide additional evidence for impaired oxytocin release patterns in high-alexithymic individuals.

Another study tested the relationship between oxytocin levels and social-emotional functioning in 79 women with anorexia nervosa (67). TAS-20 sum scores and DIF correlated negatively with oxytocin levels across the whole sample. These relationships remained significant after controlling for body-mass index and estrogen levels, providing further evidence for low oxytocin levels contributing to deficits in social-emotional functioning associated with alexithymia.

Recently, the relationship between alexithymia and oxytocin was investigated in perpetrators of intimate partner violence and non-violent controls (68). Effects of an empathic induction task (videos evoking negative emotions) were tested on endogenous salivary oxytocin levels, mood state, and emotional perception. Perpetrators of intimate partner violence were found to be significantly more alexithymic than controls. Importantly, salivary oxytocin levels were linked to a greater mood state response, a more intense perception of the video character’s emotional state, and lower alexithymia levels across the whole sample. These findings suggest that in addition to lower alexithymia levels, higher oxytocin levels are associated with a greater understanding of one’s own emotions, a greater insight into the emotional state of others, and a more accurate emotional response to others emotional states.

In sum, several studies on oxytocin showed that alexithymia is linked to reduced oxytocin levels. Further, there is evidence for a gene-environment interaction contributing to alexithymia, with variation in the oxytocin receptor gene and insecure childhood attachment contributing to high alexithymia levels. Notably, a randomized controlled trial demonstrated that intranasal oxytocin administration can improve social-emotional functioning in high-alexithymic individuals. Despite these promising results and the known beneficial effects of oxytocin on social cognition and communication [e.g., (54, 58)], there have been no further investigations on potentially beneficial effects of oxytocin on social cognition problems or reduced empathy and mentalizing abilities associated with alexithymia.

2.4. Sex hormones

Besides the hormones discussed above, the sex hormones testosterone, progesterone, and estradiol have been associated with social cognition and emotion processing, abilities that are impaired in alexithymia. Testosterone administration, for instance, influenced anger levels (69, 70), was associated with changes in connectivity within the emotion regulation network (53, 71), and enhanced empathic concern in men (72). In women, testosterone application affected emotional functions (73), enhanced responsiveness to social threat (74), and diminished cognitive empathy (75). Moreover, Machiavellian trait patterns [which are significantly associated with alexithymia; e.g., (76)] interacted with the application of testosterone and AVP and modulated aggression levels (77).

In addition, changes in estradiol and progesterone levels during menstrual cycle phases are associated with emotion recognition accuracy and emotional memories [for a review, see review (78)], emotion recognition and perspective taking performance (7981), and fear recognition (82). Moreover, estradiol application modulated emotion regulation in women (83), and resulted in an increase in emotional reactivity in men (73). Five decades of research have demonstrated that alexithymia is linked to deficits in emotion recognition and regulation, emotional memory and perspective taking (84), yet differences in the sex hormones testosterone, progesterone, and estradiol in relation to alexithymia have received little scientific attention.

A study on 84 infertile men reported that alexithymia was negatively correlated with stress hormones but not with sex steroids (85). In climacteric (menopausal) women, alexithymia was associated with significantly higher estradiol levels (86). A further study observed that higher testosterone levels in combination with lower alexithymia levels predicted higher sexual desire in women (87), but direct relationships between alexithymia and testosterone levels were not reported. These findings provide initial evidence for an association between alexithymia and the sex hormones testosterone and estradiol, but much more research is needed to shed light on this potential link.

3. Conclusion and future directions

Alexithymia is associated with impaired stress tolerance and reduced emotion regulation abilities. Therefore, studies on its association with hormonal abnormalities are of particular importance. Several studies focused on the role of the HPA axis in relation to alexithymia, a key mediator of physiological and psychological stress responses. Hormones also represent a link between psychological stress and alterations of the immune system, given that acute and chronic stress can impair cellular stress responses and pro-inflammatory cytokines while activating anti-inflammatory agents of the immune system (15).

The existing evidence suggests that there is a significant link between alexithymia and differences in cortisol levels, evidenced by higher norepinephrine/cortisol ratios (reminiscent of those observed in PTSD), lower CAR, and stronger endocrine stress responses to corticotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH). These results suggest that alexithymia is indeed linked to an altered function of the HPA axis. HPA axis functioning is influenced by factors such as age, sex, and hereditary predisposition, in combination with early childhood experiences and personality characteristics (88), factors that are also associated with alexithymia (89). Since the HPA axis is a key mediator of psychological and physiological stress responses, more studies are needed to investigate the association between alexithymia and altered HPA axis functioning more thoroughly to find out whether this association represents a direct link or an indirect link via other factors. Future studies could apply stress tests in combination with cortisol measures to shed light on the associations between alexithymia and cortisol levels, ACTH, and CRH within the context of responses to acute stress.

Thyroid diseases are linked to mood disorders such as anxiety and depression, yet very little research has been devoted to investigating the relationship between alexithymia and thyroid hormones and thyroid diseases. The existing evidence suggests that alexithymia is indeed associated with altered thyroid hormone levels, revealing a pattern of lower FT4 and higher FT3, indicative of thyroid dysfunction. Such a pattern of lower FT4 and higher FT3 was also identified in a randomized controlled trial to be related to the recurrence of depressive episodes in patients with bipolar disorder (90), further corroborating the link between abnormalities in thyroid functioning and mood disorders. Moreover, there is initial evidence for a high prevalence of alexithymia in thyroid diseases as well as for significant changes in alexithymia after thyroidectomy. Given this sparse, but significant evidence, we think that it would be worthwhile to conduct future studies on the link between thyroid abnormalities and alexithymia. Thyroid hormones and sex hormones could be measured in combination with alexithymia levels in healthy individuals and in patients with thyroid and other autoimmune diseases to better understand the complex associations between alexithymia, hormonal abnormalities, and alterations of the immune system.

Several studies focused on the role of oxytocin, demonstrating that alexithymia is linked to reduced oxytocin levels and that intranasal oxytocin administration can improve socio-emotional performance specifically in high-alexithymic individuals. Unfortunately, no further studies have tested potentially beneficial effects of oxytocin on alexithymia-related problems such as difficulties in social cognition or reduced empathy. Also surprising is the fact that there are currently no systematic investigations into associations between alexithymia and AVP vasopressin, which is–like oxytocin–known to play an important role in social cognition and behavior [for reviews, see (25, 91)]. AVP strongly contributes to the endocrine and neural response to stress, and affective disorders seem to be related to excessive vasopressin function [for a review, see (26)]. However, to our knowledge, only two studies on AVP included alexithymia as a variable, but reported no analysis results on associations between AVP and alexithymia (92, 93). As concluded in Section “2.1 Cortisol,” alexithymia is related to an exaggerated ACTH stress response to CRH. AVP has been shown to strongly potentiate ACTH-releasing activity, and in acute stress, CRH is the main cause of increased ACTH release, whereas in chronic stress, there seems to be a switch from CRH to AVP stimulation of ACTH release (25). Given these relationships, we think that future investigations into potential effects of AVP on ACTH stress response to CRH in relation to alexithymia could be informative. Hormone administration studies could be conducted to investigate the effects of oxytocin and AVP vasopressin on alexithymia-related impairments in stress response, emotion regulation, and social cognition.

On a more general note, future research would benefit from testing links between alexithymia and hormonal imbalance in big data sets rather than small samples using correlational approaches. Age and biological sex should always be taken into account in such studies. Machine learning and unbiased cluster analyses could be performed in healthy individuals and in various disorders to test, for example, links between alexithymia and variations in cortisol and oxytocin receptor genes.

In conclusion, while the topic is still in the infant stage, there is accumulating evidence for significant links between alexithymia and hormonal dysregulation. Therefore, we consider it important to continue this research line to extend our understanding of the role alexithymia plays in the stress-hormone system. Future studies should investigate the mechanisms underlying alexithymia and individual vulnerability to stress, and the extent to which individual differences in alexithymia levels mediate susceptibility to affective disorders via hormonal abnormalities and the stress-hormone system.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

KG conceptualized the manuscript. KG and MV wrote sections of the manuscript and provided the first draft, contributed to the manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Nemiah JC, Sifneos PE. Psychosomatic illness: a problem in communication. Psychother Psychosom. (1970) 18:154–60.

Google Scholar

2. Cleghorn RA. Toward a theory of psychosomatic disorders: alexithymia, pensée opératoire, psychosomatisches phanomen. Am J Psychiatry. (1979) 136:871–871.

Google Scholar

3. López-Muñoz F, Pérez-Fernández F. A history of the alexithymia concept and its explanatory models: an epistemological perspective. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 10:1026. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.01026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. Examining proposed changes to the conceptualization of the alexithymia construct: the way forward tilts to the past. Psychother Psychosomat. (2021) 90:145–55. doi: 10.1159/000511988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Preece D, Becerra R, Allan A, Robinson K, Dandy J. Establishing the theoretical components of alexithymia via factor analysis: introduction and validation of the attention-appraisal model of alexithymia. Personal Individ Diff. (2017) 119:341–52. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Sifneos PE. The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. (1973) 22:255–62. doi: 10.1159/000286529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JD. The alexithymia construct: a potential paradigm for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatics. (1991) 32:153–64. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(91)72086-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JD. Disorders of affect regulation: alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (1999).

Google Scholar

9. Teixeira RJ, Bermond B, Moormann PP. Current Developments in Alexithymia—A Cognitive and Affective Deficit, Hauppauge. New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers (2018).

Google Scholar

10. Goerlich KS. The multifaceted nature of alexithymia–a neuroscientific perspective. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:1614. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Luminet O, Nielson KA, Ridout N. Cognitive-emotional processing in alexithymia: an integrative review. Cognit Emot. (2021) 35:449–87. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2021.1908231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bagby R, Parker J, Taylor G. The twenty-item toronto alexithymia scale–i. item selection and cross-va15lidation of the factor structure. J Psychosomatic Res. (1994) 38:23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Honkalampi K, Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Tanskanen A, Hintikka J, Lehtonen J, Viinamäki H. Why do alexithymic features appear to be stable? Psychother Psychosom. (2001) 70:247–53. doi: 10.1159/000056262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Franz M, Popp K, Schaefer R, Sitte W, Schneider C, Hardt J, et al. Alexithymia in the German general population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2008) 43:54–62. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0265-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kano M, Grabe HJ, Terock J. Genetic factors and endocrine and immune system functioning associated with alexithymia. In: O Luminet, R Bagby, G Taylor editors. Alexithymia: advances in research, theory, and clinical practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2018). p. 267–90. doi: 10.1017/9781108241595.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Panayiotou G, Panteli M, Vlemincx E. Processing emotions in alexithymia: a systematic review of physiological markers. In: O Luminet, RM Bagby, GJ Taylor editors. Alexithymia: advances in research, theory, and clinical practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2018). p. 291–320. doi: 10.1017/9781108241595.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Martin JB, Pihl RO. The stress-alexithymia hypothesis: theoretical and empirical considerations. Psychother Psychosom. (1985) 43:169–76. doi: 10.1159/000287876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Papciak AS, Feuerstein M, Spiegel JA. Stress reactivity in alexithymia: decoupling of physiological and cognitive responses. J Hum Stress. (1985) 11:135–42. doi: 10.1080/0097840X.1985.9936750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. FeldmanHall O, Dalgleish T, Mobbs D. Alexithymia decreases altruism in real social decisions. Cortex. (2013) 49:899–904. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Wang Z, Goerlich KS, Luo YJ, Xu P, Aleman A. Social-specific impairment of negative emotion perception in alexithymia. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2022) 17:387–97. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Pisani S, Murphy J, Conway J, Millgate E, Catmur C, Bird G. The relationship between alexithymia and theory of mind: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 131:497–524. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Valdespino A, Antezana L, Ghane M, Richey JA. Alexithymia as a transdiagnostic precursor to empathy abnormalities: the functional role of the insula. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:2234. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bos PA, Panksepp J, Bluthé RM, Van Honk J. Acute effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social–emotional behavior: a review of single administration studies. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2012) 33:17–35. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Selye H. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (1998) 10:230–1a. doi: 10.1176/jnp.10.2.230a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Bao AM, Swaab DF. The human hypothalamus in mood disorders: the HPA axis in the center. IBRO Rep. (2019) 6:45–53. doi: 10.1016/j.ibror.2018.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Surget A, Belzung C. Involvement of vasopressin in affective disorders. Eur J Pharmacol. (2008) 583:340–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.11.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lucassen PJ, Pruessner J, Sousa N, Almeida OF, Van Dam AM, Rajkowska G, et al. Neuropathology of stress. Acta Neuropathol. (2014) 127:109–35. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1223-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Langer K, Hagedorn B, Stock LM, Otto T, Wolf OT, Jentsch VL. Acute stress improves the effectivity of cognitive emotion regulation in men. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:11571. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68137-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Langer K, Jentsch VL, Wolf OT. Cortisol promotes the cognitive regulation of high intensive emotions independent of timing. Eur J Neurosci. (2022) 55:2684–98. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Soravia LM, Heinrichs M, Aerni A, Maroni C, Schelling G, Ehlert U, et al. Glucocorticoids reduce phobic fear in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2006) 103:5585–90. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509184103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Nakataki M, Soravia LM, Schwab S, Horn H, Dierks T, Strik W, et al. Glucocorticoid administration improves aberrant fear-processing networks in spider phobia. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2017) 42:485–94. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Joseph NT, Jiang Y, Zilioli S. Momentary emotions and salivary cortisol: a systematic review and meta-analysis of ecological momentary assessment studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 125:365–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Henry JP, Haviland MG, Cummings MA, Anderson DL, Nelson JC, MacMurray JP, et al. Shared neuroendocrine patterns of post-traumatic stress disorder and alexithymia. Psychosom Med. (1992) 54:407–15. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199207000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Bremner JD. Traumatic stress: effects on the brain. Dial Clin Neurosci. (2006) 8:445–61. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.4/jbremner

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Härtwig EA, Aust S, Heuser I. HPA system activity in alexithymia: a cortisol awakening response study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2013) 38:2121–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.03.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Fries E, Dettenborn L, Kirschbaum C. The cortisol awakening response (CAR): facts and future directions. Int J Psychophysiol. (2009) 72:67–73.

Google Scholar

37. Cascino G, Monteleone AM, Marciello F, Pellegrino F, Ruzzi V, Monteleone P. Alexithymia and cortisol awakening response in people with eating disorders. World J Biol Psychiatry. (2021) 22:546–51. doi: 10.1080/15622975.2020.1844291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Pedrosa GF, Bidlingmaier M, Ridout N, Scheidt CE, Caton S, Schoechlin C, et al. The relationship between alexithymia and salivary cortisol levels in somatoform disorders. Nordic J Psychiatry. (2008) 62:366–73. doi: 10.1080/08039480801983554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kano M, Muratsubaki T, Yagihashi M, Morishita J, Mugikura S, Dupont P, et al. Insula activity to visceral stimulation and endocrine stress responses as associated with alexithymia in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Psychosom Med. (2020) 82:29–38. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Bagnasco M, Bossert I, Pesce G. Stress and autoimmune thyroid diseases. Neuroimmunomodulation. (2006) 13:309–17. doi: 10.1159/000104859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Bauer M, Goetz T, Glenn T, Whybrow PC. The thyroid-brain interaction in thyroid disorders and mood disorders. J Neuroendocrinol. (2008) 20:1101–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01774.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Duval F. Thyroid hormone treatment of mood disorders. Curr Treat Opt Psychiatry. (2018) 5:363–76. doi: 10.1007/s40501-018-0155-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Siegmann EM, Müller HH, Luecke C, Philipsen A, Kornhuber J, Grömer TW. Association of depression and anxiety disorders with autoimmune thyroiditis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. (2018) 75:577–84. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Le Donne M, Settineri S, Benvenga S. Early postpartum alexithymia and risk for depression: relationship with serum thyrotropin, free thyroid hormones and thyroid autoantibodies. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2012) 37:519–33. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Martino G, Caputo A, Vicario CM, Feldt-Rasmussen U, Watt T, Quattropani MC, et al. Alexithymia, emotional distress, and perceived quality of life in patients with hashimoto’s thyroiditis. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:667237. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.667237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hasegawa C, Kawai T, Tanaka M, Fukao A. Psychological characteristics of surgical patients with thyroid disease. Psychologia. (2017) 60:217–32. doi: 10.2117/psysoc.2019-B006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Ivanova GP, Gorobets LN. Psychological aspects of systemic approach to diagnostics of mental and immunological disorders in diffuse-nodular form of hashimoto’s thyroiditis. Zhurnal nevrologii i psikhiatrii imeni SS Korsakova. (2011) 111:10–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

48. Insel TR. The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron. (2010) 65:768–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:524–38. doi: 10.1038/nrn3044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Brunnlieb C, Nave G, Camerer CF, Schosser S, Vogt B, Münte TF, et al. Vasopressin increases human risky cooperative behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2016) 113:2051–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1518825113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Thompson RR, George K, Walton JC, Orr SP, Benson J. Sex-specific influences of vasopressin on human social communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2006) 103:7889–94. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600406103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Guastella AJ, Kenyon AR, Alvares GA, Carson DS, Hickie IB. Intranasal arginine vasopressin enhances the encoding of happy and angry faces in humans. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 67:1220–2. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Votinov M, Wagels L, Hermes A, Wölfle E, Streetz-van der Werf C, Karges W, et al. P. 0896 Role of vasopressin deficiency in patients with central diabetes insipidus (CDI) on recognizing emotions in social situations. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2021) 53:S658. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2021.10.752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ditzen B, Schaer M, Gabriel B, Bodenmann G, Ehlert U, Heinrichs M. Intranasal oxytocin increases positive communication and reduces cortisol levels during couple conflict. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 65:728–31. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Domes G, Steiner A, Porges SW, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin differentially modulates eye gaze to naturalistic social signals of happiness and anger. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2013) 38:1198–202. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Domes G, Sibold M, Schulze L, Lischke A, Herpertz SC, Heinrichs M. Intranasal oxytocin increases covert attention to positive social cues. Psychol Med. (2013) 43:1747–53. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Tollenaar MS, Chatzimanoli M, van der Wee NJ, Putman P. Enhanced orienting of attention in response to emotional gaze cues after oxytocin administration in healthy young men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2013) 38:1797–802. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. MacDonald K, MacDonald TM, Brüne M, Lamb K, Wilson MP, Golshan S, et al. Oxytocin and psychotherapy: a pilot study of its physiological, behavioral and subjective effects in males with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2013) 38:2831–43. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A, Berger C, Herpertz SC. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol Psychiatry. (2007) 61:731–3. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature. (2005) 435:673–6. doi: 10.1038/nature03701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Insel TR. Translating oxytocin neuroscience to the clinic: a national institute of mental health perspective. Biol Psychiatry. (2016) 79:153–4.

Google Scholar

62. Luminet O, Grynberg D, Ruzette N, Mikolajczak M. Personality-dependent effects of oxytocin: greater social benefits for high alexithymia scorers. Biol Psychol. (2011) 87:401–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Koh MJ, Kim W, Kang JI, Namkoong K, Kim SJ. Lack of association between oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene polymorphisms and alexithymia: evidence from patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0143168. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Schneider-Hassloff H, Straube B, Jansen A, Nuscheler B, Wemken G, Witt SH, et al. Oxytocin receptor polymorphism and childhood social experiences shape adult personality, brain structure and neural correlates of mentalizing. NeuroImage. (2016) 134:671–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Cattaneo L, Rizzolatti G. The mirror neuron system. Arch Neurol. (2009) 66:557–60. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Baskaran C, Plessow F, Silva L, Asanza E, Marengi D, Eddy KT, et al. Oxytocin secretion is pulsatile in men and is related to social-emotional functioning. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2017) 85:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.07.486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Schmelkin C, Plessow F, Thomas JJ, Gray EK, Marengi DA, Pulumo R, et al. Low oxytocin levels are related to alexithymia in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. (2017) 50:1332–8. doi: 10.1002/eat.22784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Comes-Fayos J, Blanco-Gandía MC, Moreno IR, Rodríguez-Arias M, Lila M, Sarrate-Costa C, et al. Reduced salivary oxytocin after an empathic induction task in intimate partner violence perpetrators: importance of socio-affective functions and its impact on prosocial behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2022) 137:105644. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105644

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Wagels L, Votinov M, Kellermann T, Eisert A, Beyer C, Habel U. Exogenous testosterone enhances the reactivity to social provocation in males. Front Behav Neurosci. (2018) 12:37. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Panagiotidis D, Clemens B, Habel U, Schneider F, Schneider I, Wagels L, et al. Exogenous testosterone in a non-social provocation paradigm potentiates anger but not behavioral aggression. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2017) 27:1172–84. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Votinov M, Wagels L, Hoffstaedter F, Kellermann T, Goerlich KS, Eickhoff SB, et al. Effects of exogenous testosterone application on network connectivity within emotion regulation systems. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:2352. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59329-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Puiu AA, Votinov M, Habel U, Konrad K. Testosterone administration does not alter the brain activity supporting cognitive and affective empathy. Compr Psychoneuroendocrinol. (2022) 10:100134. doi: 10.1016/j.cpnec.2022.100134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Olsson A, Kopsida E, Sorjonen K, Savic I. Testosterone and estrogen impact social evaluations and vicarious emotions: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. Emotion. (2016) 16:515. doi: 10.1037/a0039765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hermans EJ, Ramsey NF, van Honk J. Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in humans. Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 63:263–70. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Van Honk J, Schutter DJ, Bos PA, Kruijt AW, Lentjes EG, Baron-Cohen S. Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2011) 108:3448–52. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011891108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Sheikhi S, Issazadegan A, Norozy M, Saboory E. Relationships between alexithymia and machiavellian personality beliefs among university students. Br J Guid Couns. (2017) 45:297–304. doi: 10.1080/03069885.2015.1072128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Akkoc Altinok DC, Votinov M, Henzelmann F, Jo H, Eisert A, Habel U, et al. A combined administration of testosterone and arginine vasopressin affects aggressive behavior in males. Brain Sci. (2021) 11:1623. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11121623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Sundström-Poromaa I. The menstrual cycle influences emotion but has limited effect on cognitive function. Vitamins Horm (2018) 107:349–76. doi: 10.1016/bs.vh.2018.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Derntl B, Kryspin-Exner I, Fernbach E, Moser E, Habel U. Emotion recognition accuracy in healthy young females is associated with cycle phase. Horm Behav. (2008) 53:90–5. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Derntl B, Hack RL, Kryspin-Exner I, Habel U. Association of menstrual cycle phase with the core components of empathy. Horm Behav. (2013) 63:97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Guapo VG, Graeff FG, Zani ACT, Labate CM, dos Reis RM, Del-Ben CM. Effects of sex hormonal levels and phases of the menstrual cycle in the processing of emotional faces. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2009) 34:1087–94. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Pearson R, Lewis MB. Fear recognition across the menstrual cycle. Horm Behav. (2005) 47:267–71. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Rehbein E, Kogler L, Hornung J, Morawetz C, Bayer J, Krylova M, et al. Estradiol administration modulates neural emotion regulation. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2021) 134:105425. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Luminet O, Bagby RM, Taylor GJ editors. Alexithymia: advances in research, theory, and clinical practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2018). doi: 10.1017/9781108241595

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Conrad R, Schilling G, Haidl G, Geiser F, Imbierowicz K, Liedtke R. Relationships between personality traits, seminal parameters and hormones in male infertility. Andrologia. (2002) 34:317–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0272.2002.tb02947.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Ushiroyama T, Ueki M, Orino I, Ikeda A. Alexithymia and undefined complaints in climacteric women: prevalence, correlates and personality traits. Res Commun Psychol Psychiatry Behav. (1994) 19:49–58.

Google Scholar

87. Costa RM, Oliveira G, Pestana J, Costa D, Oliveira RF. Do psychosocial factors moderate the relation between testosterone and female sexual desire? The role of interoception, alexithymia, defense mechanisms, and relationship status. Adapt Hum Behav Physiol. (2019) 5:13–30. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0102-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Guilliams TG, Edwards L. Chronic stress and the HPA axis. Standard. (2010) 9:1–12.

Google Scholar

89. Karukivi M, Saarijärvi S. Development of alexithymic personality features. World J Psychiatry. (2014) 4:91. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v4.i4.91

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Wang X, Luo J, Yuan XF, Chen X, Yu HY, Zhu XQ, et al. Relationship of free thyroxine and triiodothyronine on recurrence in maintenance therapy for bipolar depression. Asian J Psychiatry. (2022) 71:103086. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2022.103086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. McCall C, Singer T. The animal and human neuroendocrinology of social cognition, motivation and behavior. Nat Neurosci. (2012) 15:681–8. doi: 10.1038/nn.3084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Chen X, Xu Y, Li B, Wu X, Li T, Wang L, et al. Intranasal vasopressin modulates resting state brain activity across multiple neural systems: evidence from a brain imaging machine learning study. Neuropharmacology. (2021) 190:108561. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2021.108561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Straccia MA, Teed AR, Katzman PL, Tan KM, Parrish MH, Irwin MR, et al. Null results of oxytocin and vasopressin administration on mentalizing in a large fMRI sample: evidence from a randomized controlled trial. Psychol Med. (2021):1–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721004104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: alexithymia, social, emotional, cortisol, oxytocin, vasopressin, thyroid hormones, sex hormones

Citation: Goerlich KS and Votinov M (2023) Hormonal abnormalities in alexithymia. Front. Psychiatry 13:1070066. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1070066

Received: 14 October 2022; Accepted: 19 December 2022;
Published: 09 January 2023.

Edited by:

Kristen Paula Morie, Yale University, United States

Reviewed by:

Igor Riecansky, Slovak Academy of Sciences (SAS), Slovakia

Copyright © 2023 Goerlich and Votinov. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katharina S. Goerlich, www.frontiersin.org k.s.gorlich@umcg.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.