Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 10 January 2023
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Appropriateness and Safety of using Cannabinoid and Psychedelic Medicines as Treatments for Psychiatric Disorders View all 7 articles

The association between naturalistic use of psychedelics and co-occurring substance use disorders

\r\nJonina Rabinowitz*Jonina Rabinowitz1*Shaul Lev-Ran,,Shaul Lev-Ran2,3,4Raz Gross,,*Raz Gross1,3,5*
  • 1School of Public Health, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv-Yafo, Israel
  • 2Lev Hasharon Medical Center, Netanya, Israel
  • 3Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv-Yafo, Israel
  • 4Israel Center on Addictions, Netanya, Israel
  • 5Division of Psychiatry, Sheba Medical Center, Ramat Gan, Israel

Objective: Classic psychedelics (LSD, psilocybin, and peyote/mescaline) have been used to support addiction treatment in a variety of contexts ranging from ceremonial use to clinical trials. The aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that past naturalistic use of classic psychedelics would be associated with decreased prevalence of substance use disorder, when controlling for known confounders.

Methods: This cross-sectional study used 2017 NSDUH survey data to evaluate the association between past use of the classic psychedelics LSD, psilocybin and peyote/mescaline and past year substance dependence or abuse. We calculated adjusted odds ratios by multivariate logistic regression, controlling for a range of sociodemographic variables, use of non-psychedelic illicit drugs and mental health related variables.

Results: A total of 56,276 participants were included in this study. Past use of LSD and psilocybin were associated with increased odds of substance dependence or abuse compared to those who had never used psychedelics before, and this was more likely for those who had used LSD more recently. However, prior use of peyote or mescaline was associated with lower odds of past year substance dependence or abuse compared to people who had never used psychedelics before (aOR = 0.68, p < 0.001). Past use of classic psychedelics was not associated with nicotine dependence.

Conclusion: Past use of peyote/mescaline was associated with decreased odds of substance use disorder compared to people who had never used psychedelics before, while past use of LSD or psilocybin was not. It remains unclear whether this difference is due to pharmacological differences between these compounds or simply due to the context in which peyote/mescaline are traditionally taken. Future research should investigate why naturalistic use of different psychedelics is associated with different substance use disorder effects.

1. Introduction

Alcohol and drug addiction pose a major threat to public health (1). In 2010, illicit drug dependence accounted for 20 million disability adjusted life years (2). In 2015, alcohol and tobacco use cost the human population over a quarter billion disability adjusted life years and illicit drug use cost an additional tens of millions of disability adjusted life years (3, 4). The public health burden of these diseases is likely to grow as the global prevalence of substance use disorders continues to rise (4).

Substance use disorders (SUD) are complex conditions and treatment is not effective for many patients (5). Relapse rates for addictions are similar to those of chronic diseases like diabetes and asthma (6). Current treatment for SUD is multifaceted, with treatment outcomes dependent on the extent and nature of the patient’s problems, appropriateness of treatment used to address those problems, and the quality of interaction between the patient and treatment providers (5, 6).

In the first wave of psychedelic research in the 1950’s, LSD was investigated in the treatment of alcohol dependence (7). A recent meta-analysis of six randomized controlled trials from this period showed that a “single dose of LSD, in the context of various alcoholism treatment programs, is associated with a decrease in alcohol misuse” (8). In recent years there has been renewed interest in the use of classic psychedelics to aid in the treatment of SUD (9, 10).

Classic psychedelics are serotonin 2A (5-HT2A) receptor agonists, such as lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), psilocybin and mescaline that induce hallucinogenic and mystical-type experiences and have anti-depressive, anxiolytic, and antiaddictive effects (1013). Though the exact anti-addictive mechanism of action is unknown, there is substantial evidence that drugs of abuse modulate serotonin transmission in the 5-HT raphe nuclei and their forebrain projections. Furthermore, 5-HT receptors seem to play a role in impulsivity, a behavioral characteristic that contributes to an individual’s vulnerability to addiction (14). As such, effective pharmacological manipulation of the 5-HT system may be the mechanism which is contributing to successful recovery from the repeating cycle of addiction by alleviating some of the neurochemical abnormalities associated with drugs of abuse (14).

The 5-HT2A receptor agonism in frontal and limbic areas of the brain by classic psychedelics increase glutamatergic transmission and neuroplasticity (12), so much so that research has shown that just one or two doses can have enduring positive outcomes for individuals with SUD (10). Classic psychedelics both enable reorganization of disordered neural pathways in the default mode network and attenuate maladaptive signaling in the mesolimbic reward circuitry that plays a central role in addiction (10). Persistent changes in substance use behavior seen after psychedelic experiences may be due to these neurological processes, but also seem to be mediated by psychological responses to the highly meaningful and mystical experience from psychedelics (9), similar to brain changes seen after traumatic events (15). Such profound mystical experiences are associated with sudden and lasting behavioral change, such as long-term abstinence from alcohol (7, 1517).

Still, the plasticity-inducing effects of 5-HT2A receptor agonism make the psychedelic experience especially sensitive to context (18). This context is commonly referred to as “set and setting” (15, 19, 20). Set and setting theory proposes that psychedelics act as non-specific amplifiers of the contents of one’s consciousness (19). Therefore, one’s preparation, expectation, physical environment, and even cultural attitudes shape the effects of a psychedelic experience. Contemporary research has found that set and setting is so important for realizing therapeutic effect because psychedelics put the user in a state of enhanced suggestibility (21, 22). In fact, in studies in which set and setting were neglected, therapeutic outcomes were less positive (18).

It is for this reason that researchers believe that many indigenous groups have participated in sacramental ritual use of psychedelics for religious and pedagogical purposes since ancient times (11, 23). Examples of such sacramental use include religious consumption of ayahuasca (12, 23) and the religious sacrament performed by the Native American Church (NAC) in which peyote is consumed as part of an all-night communal prayer and song ceremony for the purpose of healing physical or spiritual imbalances that an individual or the community may be experiencing (15, 2426). Similarly, ibogaine is often used in ceremonial contexts and in clinical research for addiction treatment (27, 28). Use in these contexts has been associated with remission of psychopathologies such as depression, anxiety, and SUD (12).

Current research continues to show the efficacy of treating various addictive disorders with psychedelic assisted psychotherapy in the clinical context (9, 2933). In addition to the extraordinary effects of psychedelic assisted psychotherapy seen in clinical trials, large population-based studies of naturalistic (i.e., non-laboratory) use of psychedelics have shown the power of these compounds to affect mood, behavior, and even increase openness (15, 34).

One of the largest population studies on psychedelic use sought to evaluate the effect of naturalistic use of psychedelics on mental health. The results of this study showed that use of psychedelics was not only not an independent risk factor for mental health problems, but that psychedelic use was indeed associated with a lower rate of mental health problems (35). Further epidemiological studies have suggested that naturalistic use of classic psychedelics is associated with positive mental health outcomes (11, 3437), including smoking cessation (38) and decreased risk of opioid use disorder (39, 40). These studies of naturalistic use of psychedelics are particularly important for our understanding of psychedelics because of the central role played by set and setting in mediating therapeutic effects and because naturalistic psychedelic use happens outside of controlled settings.

While past epidemiological studies have shown significant association between past use of classic psychedelics and specific substance use disorders, there are no known large population-based studies which look at the association between naturalistic use of classic psychedelics categorically across several compounds and the broad category of substance use disorders. Therefore, the goal of this study is to validate the effect of naturalistic use of classic psychedelics seen in substance use disorders across classic psychedelic compounds. This large cross-sectional study compares the prevalence of substance use disorder among individuals who have and have not been exposed to the classic psychedelics LSD, psilocybin and peyote/mescaline as a class of substances and individually. Based on past research, we hypothesized that individuals exposed to any of these classic psychedelics would have lower rates of substance use disorder when controlling for demographic variables and for confounding by co-occurring mental illness.

2. Materials and methods

This cross-sectional study used data from the 2017 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). The 2017 NSDUH survey provides estimates of substance use and mental health indicators from a representative sample of the US civilian non-institutionalized population. The data included individuals 12 years of age and older.

2.1. Variable selection

2.1.1. Lifetime use of classic psychedelics

A new variable indicating lifetime use of classic psychedelics which was coded as “1” if the individual reported having ever used LSD, psilocybin, peyote, or mescaline. Each of the substances were also analyzed as individual substances and therefore each substance was represented with its own variable, except for peyote and mescaline which were collapsed into one variable since mescaline is the active compound in the peyote cactus.

2.1.2. LSD recency

Lysergic acid diethylamide was the only classic psychedelic in the 2017 survey that included questions on recency of use. This variable was included to investigate whether there were differences in substance dependence or abuse among individuals based on how long ago they had last used LSD in comparison to individuals who had never used LSD before.

2.1.3. Past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse

The DSM-4 had two diagnoses: substance dependence and substance abuse. In the DSM-5, these diagnoses were collapsed into one diagnosis–substance use disorder (mild to severe) (41). Hence, the variable “past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse” was chosen as the primary outcome variable as it captures the central components to the diagnosis of substance use disorder (SUD).

2.1.4. Nicotine dependence

This variable was added as a secondary dependent variable because, unlike past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse, this variable included information on substance cravings. While the DSM-5 diagnosis of SUD largely focuses on substance dependence and abuse criteria, it also includes criteria on substance cravings. The Nicotine Dependence Syndrome Scale (or NDSS) assesses dependence on nicotine and includes criteria on nicotine craving (42). As such, nicotine dependence based on NDSS score was included as an additional dependent variable to investigate whether past exposure to classic psychedelics was differentially associated with nicotine dependence.

2.1.5. Control variables

This study investigated the presence of substance dependence or abuse among individuals who have and have not been exposed to classic psychedelics. Because SUD so often co-occurs with other mental illness, (43, 44) a categorical variable on presence and severity of past year mental illness was included as a control variable. In addition, Krebs and Johansen paper (35), which was a study using NSDUH data from previous years, compared various mental health outcomes among psychedelic users and non-users and included the following control variables which were also included in this study: age at interview, sex, race/ethnicity, education, household income, marital status, likes to test self with risky behavior, and lifetime non-medical use of each of ten types of drugs: cannabis (marijuana), opiates (heroin, opiate pain relievers), cocaine, tranquilizers/sedatives (benzodiazepines, barbiturates), stimulants (amphetamine, methamphetamine, methylphenidate), MDMA (ecstasy), inhaled anesthetics (nitrous oxide, ether), alkyl nitrites (poppers), other inhalants (solvents, volatile chemicals), and PCP (phencyclidine). The Krebs and Johansen study (35) also included a control variable on lifetime exposure to an extremely stressful event. The 2017 NSDUH survey did not include any such variable and thus this control variable was not included in the study.

2.2. Data analysis

Regression analysis was done to find the ratio of the odds of having past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse among individuals who used classic psychedelics in their lifetime compared to those that had never used before. The binary logistic regression was first run to get the unadjusted odds ratio, and then another logistic regression was run which included control variables. The exposure variables (lifetime use of classic psychedelics) were then separated into three individual variables: lifetime use of LSD, lifetime use of psilocybin and lifetime use of peyote/mescaline and the same regression analysis was run, again using past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse as the dependent variable.

Another set of logistic regressions was run using nicotine dependence as the dependent variable, first by using the variable lifetime use of classic psychedelics as one binary variable and then by differentiating between the three categories of classic psychedelics. These regressions were also run first without and then with the control variables mentioned above. Lastly, a series of logistic regressions was run using recency of LSD use as the predictor variable in place of lifetime exposure to classic psychedelics. No power analysis was performed because the sample size was n = 56,276 and there were 21 variables included in the analysis, suggesting that power would be well above 90% even for detecting a small effect size (45).

All data analysis was done using SPSS version 27. Significance was set at the p < 0.05 level, adjusted using the Bonferroni correction when appropriate. The Bonferroni correction was applied for all regression analyses which differentiated between various psychedelics such that the adjusted significance level was considered at the p < 0.0167 level. All tests were two-sided.

3. Results

There were 56,276 individuals included in this study, 27,037 males (48%) and 29,239 females (52%). Lifetime exposure to at least one of the classic psychedelics was reported by 6,362 individuals (11.3%), 4,688 of those had been exposed to LSD at least once over the course of their lifetime (8.3%), 4,614 had been exposed to psilocybin (8.2%), and 1,285 had been exposed to either peyote or mescaline (2.3%). Further summary data can be found in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary demographic and mental health data of individuals who have and have not used any of the classic psychedelics included in this study.

When analyzing these data to see how use of psychedelics was associated with SUD without taking into account control variables, we saw an unadjusted OR = 6.03 (p < 0.001), indicating that lifetime exposure to classic psychedelics was associated with six times greater odds of past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse. This OR was considerably attenuated after adjusting for the above-mentioned control variables (aOR = 1.21, p < 0.001).

The unadjusted odds of past year drug or alcohol dependence or abuse among individuals exposed to LSD in their lifetimes was 5.73 (p < 0.001) times greater than among those who had never used LSD. When including control variables, however, we computed an aOR of 1.41 (p = 0.024). This result was not considered statistically significant considering the Bonferroni corrected significance level p < 0.0167. Similarly, the unadjusted OR = 6.02 (p < 0.001) for illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse among those with lifetime exposure to psilocybin substantially decreased when control variables were included in the model (aOR = 1.135, p = 0.031).

While the unadjusted analysis for illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse among individuals who had been exposed to peyote or mescaline in their lifetime showed a positive association (OR = 2.97, p < 0.001), the adjusted model suggests that lifetime exposure to peyote or mescaline was significantly associated with lower odds of illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse in the past year (aOR = 0.68, p < 0.001). The results from these logistic regressions, including the attenuating affect of control variables are summarized in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of logistic regression showing adjusted odds ratio for past year substance use disorder among users of any of the classic psychedelics in this study, as a class of substances and individually.

When looking at nicotine dependence as the outcome variable, the odds ratio for individuals exposed to any psychedelic was 4.76, p < 0.001. When differentiating between psychedelics, ORLSD = 4.97, ORpsilocybin = 4.42 and ORpeyote/mescaline = 3.67 and all were significant at the p < 0.001 level. When including control variables, we found no significant association between nicotine dependence and exposure to psychedelics. However, aORLSD = 1.17 (p = 0.0168) and aORpeyote/mescaline = 0.80 (p = 0.018) are suggestive of a trend that might be worth investigating in future studies, albeit not statistically significant when Bonferroni correction was applied. These results are summarized in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Odds ratios (OR) and adjusted odds ratios (aOR) of past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse and nicotine dependence among individuals exposed to classic psychedelics in comparison to individuals in the sample who have never used these psychedelics.

When differentiating the effect on substance use disorder by recency of LSD use, participants who were exposed to LSD within the past 30 days had 18.34 greater odds of past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse than those who had never used LSD, without controlling for other variables. Adjusting for control variables, aORpast 30 days = 1.972 for illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse. These same individuals had 4.35 times greater odds of nicotine dependence than people who had never used LSD, without controlling for other variables. All of these results were found at the p < 0.001 level.

Similarly, for individuals who had used LSD more than a month prior but in the past year, the odds of past year SUD were 15.65 times greater than those who had never used LSD, without controlling for other variables. When adjusting for control variables, aOR = 2.17. This group (those who had used LSD within the past year but more than 30 days prior) had 3.57 times greater odds of nicotine dependence than those who had never use LSD before, without including control variables. All these results were significant at the p < 0.001 level. In the absence of control variables, individuals who had last used LSD more than a year prior had 4.47 times greater odds of past year drug or alcohol dependence or abuse than those who had never used LSD before. This same group had 5.23 times greater odds of nicotine dependence without factoring in control variables. These were both found to be significant at the p < 0.001 level. When including control variables in the analysis, individuals whose last use of LSD was more than a year prior had just 1.14 times greater odds of nicotine dependence than those who had never used LSD before, p < 0.05. The adjusted odds ratio of drug or alcohol dependence for those who last used LSD more than a year prior was 0.94, though these results were not significant.

With the presence of control variables, LSD recency within the past year was significantly associated with greater odds of past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse (ORpast 30 days = 1.97, p < 0.001; ORpast 12 months = 2.17, p < 0.001). In addition, the odds of nicotine dependence were slightly greater among people who had used LSD more than a year prior to survey, compared to people who had never used LSD before (OR = 1.14, p < 0.05). No other associations were significant when controlling for other variables. These results are summarized in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Odds ratios (OR) and adjusted odds ratios (aOR) of past year illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse and nicotine dependence among individuals based on recency of last LSD use.

4. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate whether individuals who had used classic psychedelics in a naturalistic setting were less likely to have substance use disorder compared to those who had never used classic psychedelics. This study used a large sample of individuals who reported on their use of psychedelics, illicit drugs, alcohol, and nicotine, thereby illuminating several interesting facets of psychedelic use and its associated effects in a representative sample of the US population.

Without adjusting for control variables, the part of the population that had used psychedelics in their lifetime were indeed significantly and dramatically more likely to have substance dependence or abuse in the past year. This makes sense when considering the growing prevalence of psychedelic use among users of other drugs (26, 46, 47). Though prior psychedelic use was found to be associated with greater prevalence of substance dependence or abuse in the past year, adjusted analysis revealed a meaningful decrease in this effect. Furthermore, while lifetime exposure to classic psychedelics in general was associated with a slightly increased likelihood of past year substance dependence or abuse, differentiating between psychedelic substances in the regression analysis showed divergent effects between the different psychedelic compounds. Specifically, people who had used LSD or psilocybin in their lifetimes had slightly greater odds of substance dependence or abuse in the past year, while individuals who had used peyote or mescaline in their lifetimes had a significantly lower likelihood of substance dependence or abuse in the past year (aOR = 0.68, p < 0.001). This suggests that even naturalistic use of peyote/mescaline could bear a protective effect against illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse and may even serve as a protective factor against nicotine dependence.

Controlling for the confounding effects of age, sex, education level, race, past year mental illness severity, marital status, family income level, how often the respondent likes to test self with risky behavior, and having ever used any of the aforementioned drugs resulted in considerably adjusted odds ratios. Interestingly, the older an individual was at the time of interview seems to have resulted in lower odds of having an SUD. While this effect was not significant across the board, this does follow a trend which shows that age likely served as a confounding variable. Similarly, sex seemed like an obvious confounder as females have significantly lower odds of SUD and are similarly a smaller percentage of psychedelic users as shown in Table 1. Controlling for race revealed that some races seemed to have a significantly greater aOR for SUD compared to the non-Hispanic white population, which justifies including it as a control variable. However, future research should be done on these differences because, for example, while the Native American population had greater odds of having SUD, there are also traditions like the Native American Church which use psychedelics in a ceremonial context for healing (24). Past year mental illness and how often one likes to test oneself with risky behavior both showed clear, significant trends which also make it likely that these variables were confounders. In addition, marital status was a significant control variable, suggesting that it too was a confounding variable. As is expected, people who had used almost all other drugs which were included as a control variable had significantly greater odds of SUD, excluding users of PCP, inhaled anesthetics and amyl nitrite. People who had used inhaled anesthetics indeed had significantly lower odds of SUD which is a phenomenon worth investigating in future research as well. Lastly, the two socioeconomic status control variables, income and education level, did not seem to show any clear, significant trend. This perhaps suggests that the effect of psychedelic use on SUD is independent of socioeconomic status.

This was a cross-sectional study and so the effect of time was necessarily taken out of the equation. Nonetheless, by differentiating LSD users by how long ago they had last used LSD, we were able to see if there were immediate and/or long-term effects. Interestingly, there was a significant downward trend in unadjusted odds for SUD as time since last use of LSD passed, although no such trend was found for nicotine dependence or when adjusting for control variables.

4.1. Directions for future research

The difference between the various classic psychedelics studied are most obviously explained by two possibilities which should be investigated in further research: chemical/pharmacological differences between the different compounds and differences in the set and setting in which different psychedelics are used. One notable chemical difference is that mescaline is a phenylalkylamine whereas LSD and psilocybin are indoleamines (48, 49). While it is well established that classic psychedelics share a common attribute of agonist activity at the serotonin 5HT2A receptor (49, 50), there is also evidence that other receptor sites are involved in bringing about the psychopharmacological effects of classic psychedelics. One difference between phenylalkylamines and indoleamines that may be consequential in explaining the differences seen between the compounds studied in this work is that phenylalkylamines, like mescaline, are selective 5-HT2 receptor agonists, whereas indoleamines are non-selective for 5-HT receptors (49). While the research on this is still in its early stages, it seems that the non-addictive nature of classic psychedelics is at least partially associated with serotonin 5-HT2C receptor agonism (48).

Another possible explanation for why mescaline use in our study was associated with decreased SUD while LSD and psilocybin use were not, could be that mescaline is typically used in a more supportive context than LSD or psilocybin (26, 51). While the influence of set and setting was not accounted for in this study due to survey limitations, set and setting could have contributed to the different SUD outcomes seen in the LSD or psilocybin users vs. mescaline users in this study. The use of peyote/mescaline has a long history of being used in religious and therapeutic contexts and its illicit use is less common than illicit use of cocaine or cannabis (25, 51). Furthermore, LSD and psilocybin use is associated with use of other illicit substances (5254). While this is hardly evidence for different set and setting, the possibility that mescaline users are more likely to enjoy a therapeutic context for their mescaline use than most LSD and psilocybin users is worth investigating in future research, given the extensive evidence on contextual factors (set and setting) mediating the kinds of benefits realized by psychedelic users (18, 19, 21, 23).

It is important to note that these two possible explanations for why decreased substance dependence or abuse was observed for mescaline only and not LSD or psilocybin are not mutually exclusive. Nonetheless, future research could compare the effects of LSD, psilocybin, and mescaline use for treating SUD, using a unified set and setting protocol. This could provide greater evidence for or against the hypothesis that pharmacological differences between classic psychedelics mediate therapeutic effects for treating SUD.

Furthermore, the outcome measure for this cross-sectional study was the (adjusted) odds ratio. This measure is based on the prevalence of SUD among people who had used psychedelics and not the incidence of SUD among psychedelic users. However, a cohort study of people who have used other substances and which measures the relative risk of developing SUD among psychedelic users compared to the psychedelically naïve would show greater evidence for the idea that naturalistic use of classic psychedelics can be a protective factor against substance use disorders.

4.2. Limitations

There were several limitations in this study. Firstly, there was incomplete information on the outcome of substance use disorder and we therefore used two response variables–illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse and nicotine dependence. While illicit drug or alcohol dependence or abuse was a good variable for capturing dependence or abuse of a large variety of substances that people abuse or are dependent on, this variable did not include information on substance cravings which is a criterion that is included in the DSM-5 diagnosis of substance use disorder. While there was not a singular SUD outcome variable, significant and meaningful associations were found for both outcome variables.

Another limitation was that we were not able to include several control variables, such as set and setting, frequency, dose of psychedelics used and, unlike in Krebs and Johansen study (35), “lifetime exposure to an extremely stressful event.” These would have been helpful to control for, given that recent research has highlighted the importance of consuming a greater dose for eliciting the peak experience which enable greater therapeutic effect (15, 18), how common exposure to adverse childhood events and other lifetime stressful events are among people who are substance dependent and how much harder it is for these people to decrease their consumption (55, 56).

This study was a cross-sectional study and therefore, limited in that it was unable to show temporality or causality. Nonetheless, by using lifetime use of psychedelics as the primary exposure variable and past year drug or alcohol dependence or abuse and past year nicotine dependence as the outcome variables, much of the exposure likely preceded the outcomes. Furthermore, by using the recency of LSD use variable, a better picture emerged of how the outcome variables changed as time passed since last LSD use. Though no strong conclusions could be drawn from the inclusion of this variable, it does appear that there was a downward trend of SUD among people the more time had passed since their last LSD use which should be investigated further in future research.

4.3. Conclusion

The main hypothesis of this study was that naturalistic use of each of the classic psychedelics LSD, psilocybin, and peyote/mescaline would be associated with a lower likelihood of substance use disorder when controlling for demographic and mental health related variables. While this was not found for LSD or psilocybin users, it was confirmed for people who had used peyote/mescaline. This study suggests that naturalistic use of different psychedelics may have different effects and should thus be investigated and compared in future research, while taking into account other possible mediating factors.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://www.samhsa.gov/data/release/2017-national-survey-drug-use-and-health-nsduh-releases.

Author contributions

This work was a product of the JR’s Master’s thesis at Tel Aviv University. As such, JR performed the research, analysis, and writing of this manuscript under the supervision of her thesis advisors, SL-R and RG. SL-R and RG provided their expertise in psychiatry, reviewed and approved the statistical analysis, proceeding results, and contributed their oversight to the writing of the manuscript. In addition, SL-R advised on all matters related to addiction and substance use disorders. RG advised on all matters related to epidemiology and psychopharmacology, as well as his background in clinical use of psychedelics. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling editor declared a past collaboration with the author, SL-R.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Tenegra J, Leebold B. Substance abuse screening and treatment. Prim Care. (2016) 43:217–27. doi: 10.1016/j.pop.2016.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Degenhardt L, Whiteford H, Ferrari A, Baxter A, Charlson F, Hall W, et al. Global burden of disease attributable to illicit drug use and dependence: findings from the global burden of disease study 2010. Lancet. (2013) 382:1564–74. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61530-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Peacock A, Leung J, Larney S, Colledge S, Hickman M, Rehm J, et al. Global statistics on alcohol, tobacco and illicit drug use: 2017 status report. Addiction. (2018) 113:1905–26. doi: 10.1111/add.14234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. GBD 2016 Alcohol and Drug Use Collaborators. The global burden of disease attributable to alcohol and drug use in 195 countries and territories, 1990-2016: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2016. Lancet Psychiatry. (2018) 5:987–1012. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30337-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Volkow N, Boyle M. Neuroscience of addiction: relevance to prevention and treatment. Am J Psychiatry. (2018) 175:729–40. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17101174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. National Institute on Drug Abuse [NIDA]. Principles of drug addiction treatment: a research based guide. Maryland: National Institute of Health (2012).

Google Scholar

7. Bogenschutz M, Ross S. Therapeutic applications of classic hallucinogens. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2018) 36:361–91. doi: 10.1007/7854_2016_464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Krebs T, Johansen P. Lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) for alcoholism: meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Psychopharmacol. (2012) 26:994–1002. doi: 10.1177/0269881112439253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bogenschutz M, Johnson M. Classic hallucinogens in the treatment of addictions. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2016) 64:250–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. DiVito A, Leger R. Psychedelics as an emerging novel intervention in the treatment of substance use disorder: a review. Mol Biol Rep. (2020) 47:9791–9. doi: 10.1007/s11033-020-06009-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Johnson M, Hendricks P, Barrett F, Griffiths R. Classic psychedelics: an integrative review of epidemiology, therapeutics, mystical experience, and brain network function. Pharmacol Ther. (2019) 197:83–102. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2018.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Dos Santos R, Hallak J. Therapeutic use of serotoninergic hallucinogens: a review of the evidence and of the biological and psychological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 108:423–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Kyzar E, Nichols C, Gainetdinov R, Nichols D, Kalueff A. Psychedelic drugs in biomedicine. Trends Pharmacol Sci. (2017) 38:992–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2017.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kirby L, Zeeb F, Winstanley C. Contributions of serotonin in addiction vulnerability. Neuropharmacology. (2011) 61:421–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bogenschutz M, Pommy J. Therapeutic mechanisms of classic hallucinogens in the treatment of addictions: from indirect evidence to testable hypotheses. Drug Test Anal. (2012) 4:543–55. doi: 10.1002/dta.1376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Garcia-Romeu A, Griffiths R, Johnson M. Psilocybin-occasioned mystical experiences in the treatment of tobacco addiction. Curr Drug Abuse Rev. (2014) 7:157–64. doi: 10.2174/1874473708666150107121331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Griffiths R, Johnson M, Richards W, Richards B, McCann U, Jesse R. Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences: immediate and persisting dose-related effects. Psychopharmacology. (2011) 218:649–65. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2358-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Carhart-Harris R, Roseman L, Haijen E, Erritzoe D, Watts R, Branchi I, et al. Psychedelics and the essential importance of context. J Psychopharmacol. (2018) 32:725–31. doi: 10.1177/0269881118754710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Gukasyan N, Nayak S. Psychedelics, placebo effects, and set and setting: insights from common factors theory of psychotherapy. Transcult Psychiatry. (2021) 59:652–64. doi: 10.1177/1363461520983684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Nichols D. Psychedelics. Pharmacol Rev. (2016) 68:264–355. doi: 10.1124/pr.115.011478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Carhart-Harris R, Kaelen M, Whalley M, Bolstridge M, Feilding A, Nutt DJ. LSD enhances suggestibility in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology. (2015) 232:785–94. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3714-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Timmermann C, Kettner H, Letheby C, Roseman L, Rosas F, Carhart-Harris R. Psychedelics alter metaphysical beliefs. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:22166. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-01209-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Dupuis D. Psychedelics as tools for belief transmission. set, setting, suggestibility, and persuasion in the ritual use of hallucinogens. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:730031. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.730031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Jones P. The native american church, peyote, and health: expanding consciousness for healing purposes. Contemp Justice Rev. (2007) 10:411–25. doi: 10.1080/10282580701677477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Dinis-Oliveira R, Pereira C, da Silva D. Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic aspects of peyote and mescaline: clinical and forensic repercussions. Curr Mol Pharmacol. (2019) 12:184–94. doi: 10.2174/1874467211666181010154139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Prince M, O’Donnell M, Stanley L, Swaim R. Examination of recreational and spiritual peyote use among American Indian youth. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. (2019) 80:366–70. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2019.80.366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Mosca A, Chiappini S, Miuli A, Mancusi G, Santovito M, Carlo F, et al. Ibogaine/Noribogaine in the treatment of substance use disorders: a systematic review of the current literature. Curr Neuropharmacol. (2022). [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.2174/1570159X21666221017085612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Corkery J. Ibogaine as a treatment for substance misuse: potential benefits and practical dangers. Prog Brain Res. (2018) 242:217–57. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Reiff C, Richman E, Nemeroff C, Carpenter L, Widge A, Rodriguez C, et al. Psychedelics and psychedelic-assisted psychotherapy. Am J Psychiatry. (2020) 177:391–410. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19010035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Bogenschutz M, Forcehimes A, Pommy J, Wilcox C, Barbosa P, Strassman R. Psilocybin-assisted treatment for alcohol dependence: a proof-of-concept study. J Psychopharmacol. (2015) 29:289–99. doi: 10.1177/0269881114565144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ross S. Serotonergic hallucinogens and emerging targets for addiction pharmacotherapies. Psychiatr Clin North Am. (2012) 35:357–74. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2012.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bogenschutz M, Podrebarac S, Duane J, Amegadzie S, Malone T, Owens L, et al. Clinical interpretations of patient experience in a trial of psilocybin-assisted psychotherapy for alcohol use disorder. Front Pharmacol. (2018) 9:100. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Johnson M, Garcia-Romeu A, Cosimano M, Griffiths R. Pilot study of the 5-HT2AR agonist psilocybin in the treatment of tobacco addiction. J Psychopharmacol. (2014) 28:983–92. doi: 10.1177/0269881114548296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. MacLean K, Johnson M, Griffiths R. Mystical experiences occasioned by the hallucinogen psilocybin lead to increases in the personality domain of openness. J Psychopharmacol. (2011) 25:1453–61. doi: 10.1177/0269881111420188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Krebs T, Johansen P. Psychedelics and mental health: a population study. PLoS One. (2013) 8:e63972. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Sexton J, Nichols C, Hendricks P. Population survey data informing the therapeutic potential of classic and novel phenethylamine, tryptamine, and lysergamide psychedelics. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:896. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hendricks P, Thorne C, Clark C, Coombs D, Johnson M. Classic psychedelic use is associated with reduced psychological distress and suicidality in the United States adult population. J Psychopharmacol. (2015) 29:280–8. doi: 10.1177/0269881114565653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Johnson M, Garcia-Romeu A, Johnson P, Griffiths R. An online survey of tobacco smoking cessation associated with naturalistic psychedelic use. J Psychopharmacol. (2017) 31:841–50. doi: 10.1177/0269881116684335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Argento E, Socias M, Hayashi K, Choi J, Mackay L, Christie D, et al. Psychedelic use is associated with reduced daily opioid use among people who use illicit drugs in a Canadian setting. Int J Drug Policy. (2022) 100:103518. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2021.103518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Pisano V, Putnam N, Kramer H, Franciotti K, Halpern J, Holden S. The association of psychedelic use and opioid use disorders among illicit users in the United States. J Psychopharmacol. (2017) 31:606–13. doi: 10.1177/0269881117691453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Compton W, Dawson D, Goldstein R, Grant B. Crosswalk between DSM-IV dependence and DSM-5 substance use disorders for opioids, cannabis, cocaine and alcohol. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2013) 132:387–90. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.02.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Shiffman S, Sayette M. Validation of the nicotine dependence syndrome scale (NDSS): a criterion-group design contrasting chippers and regular smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2005) 79:45–52. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Priester M, Browne T, Iachini A, Clone S, DeHart D, Seay K. Treatment access barriers and disparities among individuals with co-occurring mental health and substance use disorders: an integrative literature review. J Subst Abuse Treat. (2016) 61:47–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2015.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Smith L, Yan F, Charles M, Mohiuddin K, Tyus D, Adekeye O, et al. Exploring the link between substance use and mental health status: what can we learn from the self-medication theory? J Health Care Poor Underserved. (2017) 28:113–31. doi: 10.1353/hpu.2017.0056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Green S. How many subjects does it take to do a regression analysis. Multivariate Behav Res. (1991) 26:499–510. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2603_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Palamar J, Le A. Trends in DMT and other tryptamine use among young adults in the United States. Am J Addict. (2018) 27:578–85. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Johnstad P. Who is the typical psychedelics user? Methodological challenges for research in psychedelics use and its consequences. Nordisk Alkohol Nark. (2021) 38:35–49. doi: 10.1177/1455072520963787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Canal C, Murnane K. The serotonin 5-HT(2C) receptor and the non-addictive nature of classic hallucinogens. J Psychopharmacol. (2017) 31:127–43. doi: 10.1177/0269881116677104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Halberstadt A, Geyer M. Multiple receptors contribute to the behavioral effects of indoleamine hallucinogens. Neuropharmacology. (2011) 61:364–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Nichols D. Chemistry and structure-activity relationships of psychedelics. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2018) 36:1–43. doi: 10.1007/7854_2017_475

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Uthaug M, Davis A, Haas T, Davis D, Dolan S, Lancelotta R, et al. The epidemiology of mescaline use: pattern of use, motivations for consumption, and perceived consequences, benefits, and acute and enduring subjective effects. J Psychopharmacol. (2021) 36:309–20. doi: 10.1177/02698811211013583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Krebs T, Johansen P. Over 30 million psychedelic users in the United States. F1000Res. (2013) 2:98. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.2-98.v1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Leonard J, Anderson B, Klein-Schwartz W. Does getting high hurt? Characterization of cases of LSD and psilocybin-containing mushroom exposures to national poison centers between 2000 and 2016. J Psychopharmacol. (2018) 32:1286–94. doi: 10.1177/0269881118793086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hallock R, Dean A, Knecht Z, Spencer J, Taverna ECA. survey of hallucinogenic mushroom use, factors related to usage, and perceptions of use among college students. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2013) 130:245–8. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Pilowsky D, Keyes K, Hasin D. Adverse childhood events and lifetime alcohol dependence. Am J Public Health. (2009) 99:258–63. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.139006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Bell S, Britton A. An exploration of the dynamic longitudinal relationship between mental health and alcohol consumption: a prospective cohort study. BMC Medicine. (2014) 12:91. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-12-91

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychedelic agents, mescaline, addiction, substance use disorder, population study

Citation: Rabinowitz J, Lev-Ran S and Gross R (2023) The association between naturalistic use of psychedelics and co-occurring substance use disorders. Front. Psychiatry 13:1066369. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1066369

Received: 10 October 2022; Accepted: 13 December 2022;
Published: 10 January 2023.

Edited by:

Ben H. Amit, Reuth Medical and Rehabilitation Center, Israel

Reviewed by:

Mauro Pettorruso, University of Studies G. d’Annunzio Chieti and Pescara, Italy
Caroline Zangani, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2023 Rabinowitz, Lev-Ran and Gross. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jonina Rabinowitz, www.frontiersin.org rabinowitz.nina@gmail.com; Raz Gross, www.frontiersin.org raz.gross@sheba.health.gov.il

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.