Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 03 November 2022
Sec. Anxiety and Stress Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Monitoring Depression and Anxiety Symptoms: Scales and Measurements View all 17 articles

Revision and psychometric properties of the negative cognitive processing bias scale

\r\nKuan MiaoKuan Miao1Xuerong LiuXuerong Liu1Xiaoling ZhangXiaoling Zhang1Yuanyuan LiYuanyuan Li2Xingya LiaoXingya Liao1Rui ZhangRui Zhang1Zhengzhi Feng*Zhengzhi Feng1*Zhiyi Chen,*Zhiyi Chen1,3*
  • 1School of Psychology, Army Medical University, Chongqing, China
  • 2Western Medical Branch of PLA General Hospital, Beijing, China
  • 3School of Psychology, Experimental Research Center for Medical and Psychological Science (ERC-MPS), Army Medical University, Chongqing, China

Negative cognitive processing bias (NCPB) is a cognitive trait that makes individuals more inclined to prioritize negative external stimuli (cues) when processing information. Cognitive biases have long been observed in mood and anxiety disorders, improving validation of tools to measure this phenomenon will aid us to determine whether there is a robust relationship between NCPB and major depressive disorder, anxiety disorders and other clinical disorders. Despite the development of an initial measure of this trait, that is, the negative cognitive processing bias questionnaire (NCPBQ), the lack of psychometric examinations and applications in large-scale samples hinders the determination of its reliability and validity and further limits our understanding of how to measure the NCPB traits of individuals accurately. To address these issues, the current study evaluated the psychometric properties of the NCPBQ in a large-scale sample (n = 6,069), which was divided into two subsamples (Subsample 1, n = 3,035, serving as the exploratory subsample, and Subsample 2, n = 3,034, serving as the validation subsample), and further revised it into a standardized scale, that is the negative cognitive processing bias scale (NCPBS), based on psychometric constructs. The results show that NCPBS possesses good construct reliability, internally consistent reliability, and test-retest reliability. Furthermore, by removing two original items from NCPBQ, NCPBS was found to have good criterion-related validity. In conclusion, the present study provides a reliable and valid scale for assessing negative cognitive processing bias of individuals.

Introduction

Negative cognitive processing bias (NCPB) is a cognitive trait which not only directs attention to negative internal or external stimuli but also leads to misinterpretation of this information in a more negative way (1). Thus, the negative impacts that NCPB may have on psychological health and psychiatric conditions have sparked great interest in the scientific community. For instance, a study has demonstrated the prominent predictive role of NCPB on short-sighted judgments and decisions (2). Furthermore, NCPB has been revealed as one of the most common phenomena of major depressive disorder (MDD) and anxiety disorders (3, 4). Moreover, NCPB has been established to not only play an important role in the onset of depression, but also maintain depressed mood states (5). To reveal why a close association between NCPB and these mood disorders exists, Beck (6) explained that hypersensitivity in processing negative information (i.e., NCPB) in daily life is the key cognitive pedestal for depression symptom development and maintenance (6). Numerous studies have shown a robust link between NCPB and cognitive-related mental health problems (7, 8). Although NCPB is important for mental health, reliable and valid tools to measure this trait accurately are still scarce.

Different aspects of NCPB in depression and anxiety have been examined, including attention bias (9), memory bias (10), and interpretation bias (11, 12). Negative attention bias, which acts as the first filter for information selection, shows an attentional preference for negative stimuli and deviation from positive stimuli (13). Negative memory bias is the inclination to recall negative materials more often than positive materials. A supporting evidence is that patients with MDD show poorer recall performance for positive stimuli in memory tests than healthy controls (14). Moreover, interpretation bias involves prominent preferences for interpreting information or materials in negative ways (15). Recently, the response styles theory (RST) proposed a new framework for explaining NCPB for repetitive rethinking of negative memories and information that predominates transdiagnostic hallmark across mood disorders (16). Thus, predominating negative information in repetitive rethinking – that is, rumination – has been increasingly indicated to be an additional profile for NCPB (17).

Despite the lack of a reliable and valid scale to measure NCPB in terms of the conceptual structure mentioned above, several tools have been developed to partly assess cognitive bias. For example, the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (DAS) (18) was built to assess individuals’ maladaptive attitudes and beliefs about life and contains three main facets: perfectionism, utilitarianism, and criticism, which are not fully equal to one’s cognitive traits (i.e., NCPB). In addition, the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ) (19) was developed to investigate the frequency of negative automatic thoughts in self-statements. However, it focuses on individuals’ automatic thoughts with multiple cognitive components rather than cognitive traits. Furthermore, the Cognitive Bias Questionnaire (CBQ) (20) and Cognitive Style Questionnaire (CSQ) (21) were developed to measure one’s cognitive processing styles, such as negative self-evaluation, cognitive distortion, and outcome expectation. However, both questionnaires aim to measure one’s daily life behaviors and quantify the NCPB trait indirectly. Recently, the Negative Cognitive Processing Bias Questionnaire (NCPBQ) (22) was initially proposed to measure NCPB directly following four subdimensions. However, due to the lack of psychometric examinations and limited applications in small-scale samples, such as military personnel (23) and elderly individuals (24), the reliability and validity of the NCPBQ remain unclear. Furthermore, disparities in the construct structure of NCPBQ were found in previous studies (23, 24). Thus, it is necessary to examine the psychometric properties of NCPBQ and revise it into a standardized scale.

To address these issues, we recruited a large-scale sample (n = 6,069) across mainland China. In Subsample 1 (n = 3,035), we examined the reliability of the original version of NCPBQ using internal consistency analysis and test-retest analysis. In addition, validity was examined using criterion-related analysis and exploratory factor analysis (EFA) model. Furthermore, the NCPBQ was revised using standardized pipelines for building the NCPB scale. Finally, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used for construct validity examination in Subsample 2 (n = 3,034).

Materials and methods

Participants

A large-scale sample (n = 6,069) was recruited using a hierarchical random sampling method (45.18% females and a mean age of 31.44 years, SD = 8.99, range = 18–65 years). Participants were recruited based on the provincial population distribution in mainland China, with a larger number of participants recruited in provinces with a larger population (e.g., Guangdong, Shandong and Henan). This sample pool covered the vast majority of occupations in mainland China (e.g., students, farmers, and businessmen). The sociodemographic features and results of statistical test are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the participants (n = 6,069).

All the participants were instructed to complete an online survey via a webpage and received payment for their participation. All the participants provided written informed consent preceding access to the online questionnaire. In addition, items for lie detection were included in the survey for quality control. A third-party platform (WJX Platform, Ran-Xin Technique Co. Ltd., Changsha, China) was involved in the sampling, data acquisition and quality control. This study was approved by the IRB of Army Medical University (China).

Measures

Sociodemographic characteristics

To ensure sample representativeness, a sociodemographic investigation was conducted. This part included items as follows: gender, age, educational attainment, and occupation.

Negative cognitive processing bias questionnaire

The NCPBQ was initially developed by Yan et al. (22) to assess cognitive processing traits. The original version of the NCPBQ contained 23 self-reported items rated on a 4-point Likert-scale style (“1” for “never”; “4” for “always”) in four dimensions: negative attention bias (NAB, e.g., My attention is easily drawn to the tragic images on TV and is difficult to shift.), negative memory bias (NMB, e.g., I can easily remember the negative comments people make about me.), negative interpretation bias (NIB, e.g., If a new leader or teacher is hard on me, I think it is because he sees me in a bad light and wants to get me in trouble.), and negative rumination bias (NRB, e.g., I often think about why I am so sad). Each dimension included five items, except these, there are three lie detection items in the original measure (items 4, 16, and 23. e.g., I have never told a lie.).

Dysfunctional attitude scale

The DAS consists of 40 items evaluating respondents’ attitudes toward daily life, such as “undesirable life attitudes or beliefs,” “black-and-white attitudes for moral judgment” and “perfectionism.” This measure uses a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “completely disagree” to “completely agree,” with higher scores indicating more maladaptive attitudes. This scale has been found to have high reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.93) in psychometric examinations (25).

Beck depression inventory II

Developed and revised by Beck (26), Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II) contains 21 self-reported items and has long been acknowledged as one of the most broadly certified tools for assessing the severity of MDD (27). This inventory has been validated for good reliability and validity in clinical practice (28). The internal and test-retest reliability of the BDI-II of the Chinese version was found to be good (Cronbach’s α = 0.94; rtest–retest = 0.55) (29).

Statistical analyses

The full sample was divided into two subsamples, with one serving as an exploratory subsample (Subsample 1, n = 3,035) and the other serving as the validation subsample (Subsample 2, n = 3,034). Descriptive statistics were first reported for both subsamples. Item analysis was performed to examine the items’ suitability. Furthermore, to reveal the factor structures of the original version of the NCPBQ, EFA was conducted with principal component analysis (PCA), dimension reduction and varimax rotation in Subsample 1. By visual inspection of the scree plot and psychometric criteria (factor eigenvalues > 1.0), the number of factor structures was determined. Moreover, to revise the original version of the NCPBQ for a better factor structure, items with loadings under 0.50 were removed (30). Finally, CFA was carried out on the revised negative cognitive processing bias scale (NCPBS) in Subsample 2 to validate the factor structure. Seven metrics assessing goodness-of-fit were drawn to evaluate this model, including root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), goodness-of-fit index (GFI), comparative fit index (CFI), normed fit index (NFI), incremental fit index (IFI), and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI). An RMSEA and SRMR of <0.05; a GFI and CFI > 0.95; and a NFI, IFI, and TLI > 0.90, together, would suggest a good model fit (31).

The reliability was evaluated mainly via internal consistency reliability and test-retest reliability. Cronbach’s alpha (α) and McDonald’s omega (ω) were used to measure the internal consistency reliability. Since there is an argument regarding which is the best measure for assessing internal consistency reliability (32), α and ω were both calculated, with values of 0.70 or higher considered acceptable (33). In addition to internal consistency reliability, the 2-month test-retest reliability was also evaluated. A Pearson r value >0.50 indicated good test-retest reliability for a given scale (34).

The criterion-related validity of the revised NCPBS was estimated by the Pearson bivariate correlations across the NCPBS, BDI-II, and DAS, with significantly positive correlations between the NCPBS and both the BDI-II and DAS for high validity.

To gain further insights into the validity of this revised scale, between-group differences were examined for demographic features, which were compared by using independent sample t-tests or one-way ANOVA (Bonferroni correction for post hoc test), including gender, age, and education. The participants were classified into three age groups: early-adult group (aged 18–30), mid-adult group (aged 31–45), and old-adult group (aged 46–65). Additionally, education level was divided into two group: well-educated group for educational experiences >13 years and a less-educated group for educational experiences <13 years.

The data were analyzed by IBM SPSS Statistics 26.0 (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.), Amos 21.0 programs, and JASP 0.16.2.1

Results

Descriptive statistics

The descriptive statistics for each item and subdimensions were tabulated (see Table 2), including the mean value, standard deviation and normality (estimated by Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, skewness, and kurtosis). Although, the results showed each item failed the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, we found no prominent irregular skewness or kurtosis for this sample (The distribution of item scores was listed in Supplementary Figures). Furthermore, a similar pattern was found in each dimension, including negative attention bias (NAB, 11.47 ± 3.06 for items 1, 6, 13, 17, and 21), negative memory bias (NMB, 10.60 ± 2.67, for items 2, 8, 11, 15, and 20), negative interpretation bias (NIB, 11.21 ± 2.87, for item 3, 5, 9, 12, and 18), and negative rumination bias (NRB, 8.66 ± 2.71, for items 7, 10, 14, 19, and 22).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and the normality of data.

Item analysis

To examine the validity of each item, critical ratio (CR) method, Pearson correlation method and a homogeneity test were applied to Subsample 1 for item analysis. The results revealed significant score differences between the high-total-score (top 27%) and low-total-score groups (last 27%) for all items, irrespective of CR values, indicating that all the items possessed high discrimination power (see Table 3). Further analysis also illustrated statistically significant item-total correlations. Finally, the results of the homogeneity test found acceptable communalities and factor loadings, except for item 8 (see Table 3, communalities = 0.178, factor loading = 0.422). As a result, item 8 (I always remember my mistakes clearly) was removed in this step.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of the item analysis.

Exploratory factor analysis

The results demonstrated the suitability of EFA for the current dataset with an acceptable KMO coefficient (=0.93) and significant skewness from a spherical distribution (χ2 = 15,492.751, p < 0.001). Furthermore, principal axis factoring (PAF) was adopted for factor extraction and loading estimation, with <0.5 used as the exclusion criterion. The results indicated a four-dimensional structure for the NCPBQ, in which four common factors with eigenvalues >1 in orthogonal rotation from the maximum variance method), accounting for 52.87% of the total variance (see Table 4). Although the four-facet construct structure was validated here, item 7 was found to be unacceptable, as its factor loading was less than 0.50 (factor loading = 0.492). Finally, item 7 (I often think about why I am always inferior to others) was removed in this step.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Standardized factor loading of the negative cognitive processing bias scale.

On balance, the original version of the NCPBQ has been revised by removing two items (item 7 and item 8) based on the above results.

Validity analysis

Confirmatory factor analysis

To estimate the construct validity for the revised version, CFA was carried on this four-dimensional structure in independent Subsample 2. The results revealed good goodness-of-fit metrics for the revised NCPBS (RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.03, and GFI = 0.97, more details in Table 5). In addition, all items were found to have acceptable factor loadings (β = 0.52–0.74, see Figure 1).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Confirmatory factor analysis model fit indexes.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The four-factor confirmatory factor analysis model of the negative cognitive processing bias scale for the validation set (n = 3,034). Each number alongside the lines indicates standardized factor loading. NAB, negative attention bias; NMB, negative memory bias; NIB, negative interpretation bias; NRB, negative rumination bias.

Criterion-related validity

To test the validity of the revised NCPBS, the criterion-related validity was estimated by correlating its scores to those of the DAS and BDI-II. The results showed that the scores for both DAS (r = 0.551, p < 0.001) and BDI-II (r = 0.447, p < 0.001) were significantly correlated with the total score of the revised NCPBS and even its subdimensions (see Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Pearson’s correlations between the negative cognitive processing bias scale and the criterion measures.

Reliability analysis

Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s ω were calculated to estimate the internal consistency of the revised NCPBS. The results showed good internal consistency reliability for this revised version, including the whole scale and its subdimensions (both α and ω = 0.866, see Table 7). Furthermore, significant correlations were found in the 2-month test-retest consistency analysis, demonstrating good test-retest consistency reliability for the revised NCPBS (r = 0.943 for overall scale scores, more results can be found in Table 7).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Cronbach’s α, McDonald’s ω, and test-retest reliability of the negative cognitive processing bias scale.

Differential analysis for sociodemographic features

To examine whether the revised NCPBS was valid, differential analysis was conducted for sociodemographic features in the whole sample. The results revealed significant differences between genders for NCPBS scores (total for males: 41.34 ± 8.84, total for females: 42.67 ± 8.62, t = –5.936, p < 0.001; BF10 = 1.18 × 106 at JSY Cauchy distribution, see Table 8). Furthermore, we also found difference between educational levels in the NCPBS scores, with low scores for low educational level (total for less-educated group: 39.80 ± 8.49, total for well-educated group: 43.10 ± 8.70, t = –14.258, p < 0.001; BF10 = 6.26 × 1041 at JSY Cauchy distribution, see Table 9). Finally, the NCPBS scores varied between age-related groups (total for early-adult group: 42.83 ± 8.96, total for mid-adult group: 40.97 ± 8.43, total for old-adult group: 40.53 ± 8.32, F = 38.082, p < 0.001, see Table 10).

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Results for gender differences in negative cognitive processing bias scale scores.

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Results for education level differences in negative cognitive processing bias scale scores.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Results for age differences in negative cognitive processing bias scale scores.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the psychometric properties of the original version of the NCPBQ in a large sample and to revise it into a reliable and valid scale. As we expected, the results showed good four-dimensional construct validity and reliability for the revised NCPBS. Furthermore, small differences in NCPBS scores across sociodemographic features, including gender, educational level, and age, were found, with old less-educated males exhibiting low NCPB. On balance, the current study revealed the psychometric properties of the initial NCPBQ and further revised it into a reliable and valid scale for measuring individuals’ cognitive trait in negative information processing.

Here, a weakly skewed distribution was found in the item analysis for NCPBS, which seemed to align with general prevalence of cognition-biased mood disorder (e.g., MDD). The lifetime prevalence of these psychiatric conditions that reported in previous literature, such as depressive disorders (6.8%) and anxiety disorders (7.6%), were low in China (35, 36). Accordingly, we found no prominent irregular skewness or kurtosis for this sample by canonical criteria (i.e., skewness < 3; kurtosis < 8) (37). In this vein, this finding indicated that the NCPBS may be a valid tool for measuring cognitive processing trait.

Furthermore, the present study addressed a long-standing debate over the factor structure of NCPB. Both EFA and CFA revealed that the four-factor structure possessed good construct validity for the revised NCPBS, which strongly supports RST (16). The conventional theoretical basis for NCPB frames individuals’ cognitive biases in terms of fundamental cognitive components, such as attention, memory, and interpretation (38). However, it should be borne in mind that these cognitive faculties bias individual’s behaviors through “processing.” Thus, as a typical processing style, rumination functions to boost cognitive biases based on these cognitive components, which may determine the extent to which NCPB increases the risk of mood disorders (39). Thus, this study clarified the potential structure of NCPB by psychometric methods. In addition, NCPBS showed better reliability and validity than the previous version. Thus, the major goal of the current study was to provide a standardized NCPBS to accurately measure individuals’ negative cognitive trait.

In addition to revising the NCPBS, some between-group differences were also found. Although the differences were very small, the exploratory explanations would be inferred here. Firstly, a small gender difference in the NCPBS scores was observed, with slightly higher scores in females. This result may be supported by both theoretical and empirical evidence, as gender-related environment susceptibility theory proposes that females detect more subtle negative cues from daily life events and the environment due to genetic imprinting (4042). In addition, previous studies have validated this theoretical model, showing increased neural activity and behavioral reactions to negative information in females compared with that of males (43, 44). Thus, in the current study, these indirect evidence may imply higher negative information susceptibility in females compared to males. Furthermore, we observed slightly lower NCPB for individuals with a less educational level, which is consistent with previous evidence. Daraei and Ghaderi (45) documented the association of a low education level with optimism and well-being (45). Besides, compared to elders, young adults exhibited a little higher NCPB as measured by the revised NCPBS. This could be explained well by the differences in their emotional regulation ability. Existing studies have revealed that, as predicted by emotional regulation ability (including regulation resource and regulation strategy), older adults exhibit better decision-making ability in both positive and negative emotional conditions than young adults (4648). Thus, we inferred that such age-related effects in emotional processing may cause different NCPB for distinct age groups. Together, these evidences suggest that the current study may provide a valid and reliable measure to quantify individuals’ cognitive trait, with potentials for application in psychological and psychiatric domains.

Limitations

Several limitations in the current study should be borne in mind before applying the NCPBS. Despite claiming it to be a robust predictor of depression, little is known about whether this scale can be used in clinical practice because no depression patients were recruited in the present study. Thus, a cohort study for investigating the association between depression and NCPB in clinical practice is needed in the future. In addition, this large-scale sample was taken only from the Chinese population, which hampers the cross-cultural generalizability of this scale. To address this issue, future studies should examine the reliability and validity of the scale by using a broad sample.

Conclusion

The current study recruited a large-scale sample to validate the psychometric properties of the NCPBQ, and followed a standardized pipeline to revise the scale. The results show that NCPBS has better reliability and validity than the original version, with higher internal consistency reliability and construct validity. Furthermore, we found the statistical differences in NCPB across sociodemographic features by using NCPBS, which provided further evidence of external validity of this scale. Taken together, this study provides a reliable and valid measure to estimate individuals’ cognitive inclinations toward negative content accurately and advanced our understanding of the core components of NCPB.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://osf.io/ucmw4/.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the IRB of Army Medical University (China). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

KM: writing – original draft and review, and editing and data curation. XUL and XZ: writing – review and editing. YL, XIL, and RZ: data curation. ZF and ZC: conceptualization. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (NSFC 81971278).

Acknowledgments

We hereby appreciated Professor Qin Dai (Army Medical University) for her advice with regards to conceptional construction and statistical model, Professor Xi Luo (Army Medical University) and Xingmei Gu (Army Medical University) for their language editing, and Chang Liu (Army Medical University) for her assistance in preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1013108/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://jasp-stats.org/

References

1. Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S, Gollan JK. The negativity bias: conceptualization, quantification, and individual differences. Behav Brain Sci. (2014) 37:309–10. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13002537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Pronin E. Perception and misperception of bias in human judgment. Trends Cogn Sci. (2007) 11:37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Beevers CG, Mullarkey MC, Dainer-Best J, Stewart RA, Labrada J, Allen JJB, et al. Association between negative cognitive bias and depression: a symptom-level approach. J Abnorm Psychol. (2019) 128:212–27. doi: 10.1037/abn0000405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Clark DA, Beck AT. Cognitive theory and therapy of anxiety and depression: convergence with neurobiological findings. Trends Cogn Sci. (2010) 14:418–24. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Browning M, Blackwell SE, Holmes EA. The use of cognitive bias modification and imagery in the understanding and treatment of depression. Berlin: Springer Press (2013). p. 243–60.

Google Scholar

6. Beck AT. Cognitive models of depression. J Cogn Psychother. (1987) 1:5–37.

Google Scholar

7. Smith EM, Reynolds S, Orchard F, Whalley HC, Chan SWY. Cognitive biases predict symptoms of depression, anxiety and wellbeing above and beyond neuroticism in adolescence. J Affect Disord. (2018) 241:446–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Tairi T, Adams B, Zilikis N. Cognitive errors in greek adolescents: the linkages between negative cognitive errors and anxious and depressive symptoms. Int J Cogn Ther. (2016) 9:261–78. doi: 10.1521/ijct_2016_09_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Keller AS, Leikauf JE, Holt-Gosselin B, Staveland BR, Williams LM. Paying attention to attention in depression. Transl Psychiatry. (2019) 9:279. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0616-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Burt DB, Zembar MJ, Niederehe G. Depression and memory impairment: a meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychol Bull. (1995) 117:285–305. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Hirsch CR, Meeten F, Krahé C, Reeder C. Resolving ambiguity in emotional disorders: the nature and role of interpretation biases. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2016) 12:281–305. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Everaert J, Podina IR, Koster EH. A comprehensive meta-analysis of interpretation biases in depression. Clin Psychol Rev. (2017) 58:33–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gotlib IH, Joormann J. Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2010) 6:285–312. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Matt GE, Vázquez C, Campbell WK. Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (1992) 12:227–55. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(92)90116-P

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Beck AT, Haigh EAP. Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: the generic cognitive model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2014) 10:1–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Nolen-Hoeksema S. The response styles theory. In: Papageorgiou C, Wells A editors. Depressive rumination: nature, theory and treatment. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley (2003). p. 105–23. doi: 10.1002/9780470713853.ch6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S. Rethinking rumination. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2008) 3:400–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Weissman AN, Beck AT. Development and Validation of the dysfunctional attitude scale: a preliminary investigation. In: Paper presented at the meeting of the American educational research association. Toronto, ON (1978).

Google Scholar

19. Hollon SD, Kendall PC. Cognitive self-statements in depression: development of an automatic thoughts questionnaire. Cognit Ther Res. (1980) 4:383–95. doi: 10.1007/BF01178214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Krantz S, Hammen CL. Assessment of cognitive bias in depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (1979) 88:611–9. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.88.6.611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Abramson LY, Metalsky GI, Alloy LB. Hopelessness depression: a theory-based subtype of depression. Psychol Rev. (1989) 96:358–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Yan X, Zhang R, Feng Z. Development of negative cognitive processing bias questionnaire. J Third Mil Med Univ. (2017) 39:2329–34.

Google Scholar

23. Yan X, Feng Z. Assessment of reliability and validity of negative cognitive processing bias scale in military personnel. J Third Mil Med Univ. (2018) 40:1619–23.

Google Scholar

24. Zhou Y, Li Z. Reliability and validity of negative cognitive processing bias questionnaire in older adults. Chin Nurs Res. (2021) 31:44–7.

Google Scholar

25. Rogers GM, Park J-H, Essex MJ, Klein MH, Silva SG, Hoyle RH, et al. The dysfunctional attitudes scale: psychometric properties in depressed adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2009) 38:781–9. doi: 10.1080/15374410903259007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R. Comparison of beck depression inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess. (1996) 67:588–97.

Google Scholar

27. Kind P. Measuring health: a guide to rating scales and questionnaires. Public Health. (2008) 122:217. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2007.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wang YP, Gorenstein C. Psychometric properties of the beck depression inventory-II: a comprehensive review. Braz J Psychiatry. (2013) 35:416–31. doi: 10.1590/1516-4446-2012-1048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Wang Z, Yuan C, Huang J, Li Z, Chen Y, Zhang H, et al. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of beck depression inventory-II among depression patients. Chin Mental Health J. (2011) 25:476–80.

Google Scholar

30. Costello AB, Osborne J. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Pract Assess Res Eval. (2005) 10:1–9. doi: 10.7275/JYJ1-4868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Marsh HW, Hau K-T, Wen Z. In search of golden rules: comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Struct Equ Model. (2004) 11:320–41. doi: 10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hayes AF, Coutts JJ. Use Omega rather than Cronbach’s Alpha for estimating reliability. Commun Methods Meas. (2020) 14:1–24. doi: 10.1080/19312458.2020.1718629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. George D, Mallery P. SPSS for windows step by step: a simple guide and reference, 11.0 update. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon (2003).

Google Scholar

34. Portney LG, Watkins MP. Foundations of clinical research: applications to practice. 3rd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall (2008).

Google Scholar

35. Lu J, Xu X, Huang Y, Li T, Ma C, Xu G, et al. Prevalence of depressive disorders and treatment in China: a cross-sectional epidemiological study. Lancet Psychiatry. (2021) 8:981–90. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00251-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Huang Y, Wang Y, Wang H, Liu Z, Yu X, Yu Y, et al. Prevalence of mental disorders in China: a cross-sectional epidemiological study. Lancet Psychiatry. (2019) 3:211–24. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30511-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 4th ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2016).

Google Scholar

38. Beck AT. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatry. (2008) 165:969–77. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Nolen-Hoeksema S. The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. J Abnorm Psychol. (2000) 109:504–11. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Belsky J. Variation in susceptibility to environmental influence: an evolutionary argument. Psychol Inq. (1997) 8:182–6. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0803_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. For better and for worse: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2007) 16:300–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Hentges RF, Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML. Domain specificity of differential susceptibility: testing an evolutionary theory of temperament in early childhood. Dev Psychopathol. (2022):1–14. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1017/S0954579422000256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Yuan J, Luo Y, Yan JH, Meng X, Yu F, Li H. Neural correlates of the females’ susceptibility to negative emotions: an insight into gender-related prevalence of affective disturbances. Hum Brain Mapp. (2009) 30:3676–86. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Levkovich I, Shinan-Altman S. The impact of gender on emotional reactions, perceived susceptibility and perceived knowledge about COVID-19 among the Israeli public. Int Health. (2021) 13:555–61. doi: 10.1093/inthealth/ihaa101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Daraei M, Ghaderi AR. Impact of education on optimism/pessimism. J Indian Acad Appl Psychol. (2012) 38:339–43.

Google Scholar

46. Urry HL, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation in older age. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2010) 19:352–7. doi: 10.1177/0963721410388395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. You XQ, Ju CT, Wang M, Zhang BS, Liu P. Age differences in the influence of induced negative emotion on decision-making: the role of emotion regulation. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2019) 74:796–805. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbx137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Chow S-M, Hamagani F, Nesselroade JR. Age differences in dynamical emotion-cognition linkages. Psychol Aging. (2007) 22:765–80. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: negative cognitive processing bias, mental health, depression, scale revision, psychometric properties, measurement

Citation: Miao K, Liu X, Zhang X, Li Y, Liao X, Zhang R, Feng Z and Chen Z (2022) Revision and psychometric properties of the negative cognitive processing bias scale. Front. Psychiatry 13:1013108. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1013108

Received: 06 August 2022; Accepted: 12 October 2022;
Published: 03 November 2022.

Edited by:

Michael Noll-Hussong, Saarland University, Germany

Reviewed by:

Tamara J. Sussman, Stony Brook University, United States
Jing Wang, Fudan University, China

Copyright © 2022 Miao, Liu, Zhang, Li, Liao, Zhang, Feng and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Zhengzhi Feng, fzz@tmmu.edu.cn; Zhiyi Chen, chenzhiyi@email.swu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.