Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 23 November 2021
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Neural Bases of Neurological and Psychiatric Disorders and Their Neuromodulation Treatments View all 36 articles

A Review on P300 in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

  • 1Unit of Neurology, G.B. Morgagni – L. Pierantoni Hospital, Forlì, Italy
  • 2Department of Surgery and Medical-Surgical Specialties, University of Catania, Catania, Italy
  • 3Clinical Neurophysiology Research Unit, Oasi Research Institute - Istituto di Ricerca e Cura a Cattarere Scientifico (IRCCS), Troina, Italy

Neuropsychological studies indicate the presence of cognitive changes in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Indeed, OCD may be included among the dysfunctions of the frontal lobes and their connections with the limbic system, associative cortex, and basal ganglia. P300 is a positive component of the human event-related potential (ERP); it is associated with processes of encoding, identification, and categorization constituting, as a whole, the superior cortical function of information processing. Thus, P300 explores several areas that are implicated in OCD pathophysiology. Our aim is to review all relevant studies on the P300 component of the human ERP in order to recognize any significant central nervous system (CNS) correlate of cognitive dysfunction in OCD. A PubMed-based literature search resulted in 35 articles assessing P300 in OCD and reporting neurophysiological correlates of response inhibition, cortical hyperarousal, and over-focused attention. A decreased P300 amplitude was reported in both adult and pediatric patients, with a trend toward normalization after pharmacological treatment. Source localization studies disclosed an association between P300 abnormalities and the functioning of brain regions involved in the pathophysiology of OCD. Moreover, studies converge on the evidence of neurophysiological dysfunction in the frontal areas with impairment of the normal inhibitory processes in OCD. At least some of these electrophysiological correlates might reflect the obsessive thoughts and compulsions that characterize this disorder. These findings may also support cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) approaches on over-focused attention and inflexibility of compulsive behaviors, which should be associated to pharmacological treatment in these patients.

Introduction

Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) affects approximately 2–3% of people at some point of their life (1). In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders— Fifth Edition (2), OCD was moved from the group of anxiety disorders to a new category, named Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders (1). Obsessive-compulsive disorder usually first appears during adolescence or in early adults, although treatment may not be sought until the middle age. The two sexes are equally affected. The onset of OCD is typically gradual and cannot be accurately dated, although, in some cases, it is triggered by a particular event in the patient's life. In most instances, OCD is engrafted into a personality in which rigidity and lack of adaptability are prominent. These traits are manifest in the individual's punctuality and in his/her dependability in the activities of everyday life. Additionally, there is always a prevailing undercurrent sense of insecurity (36). Clinically, the main symptoms of OCD are intrusive thoughts or images (obsessions), which increase anxiety, and repetitive and ritualistic actions (compulsions), which typically decrease anxiety (7, 8). In all of these obsessions and compulsions, patients suffer from a feeling of insufficiency in being unable to reject their troublesome thoughts (7, 8). This insight into the psychopathological experience and the struggle against it distinguish obsessions from delusions. Therefore, the majority of OCD patients are tense, irritable, and apprehensive. They may complain of anxiety attacks and become depressed with fatigue and lack of interest (7, 8). Education and pharmacotherapy with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) are first-line treatments, along with behavior therapy (7).

Neuropsychological studies indicate that patients with OCD may present some cognitive changes mainly involving executive functioning, information coding, organization strategy, set-shifting, motor and cognitive inhibition, visual-constructive and controlled fluency, verbal memory, and processing speed (911). Because of its neurobiological features, OCD may be included within the wide spectrum of dysfunctions of the frontal lobes and their connections with the limbic system, the associative cortex, and the basal ganglia which, in turn, influence the ability of abstract decisions needed to create more efficient and controlled behavioral judgment or action (12). In a previous study of patients who developed elements of obsessiveness and compulsive behavior after focal brain lesions, the authors found changes in the cingulate, frontal, and temporal cortices, as well as in the basal ganglia (13). On the other hand, the surgical disconnection of the orbito-frontal regions from limbic, thalamic, and striatal structures in severely affected patients improves symptoms of OCD (14). Moreover, neuroimaging studies in OCD demonstrate the involvement of the cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical loop, anterior cingulate cortex, prefrontal cortex, cerebellum, and hippocampus (1518).

Based on this theoretical background, in this review we aimed to assess the utility of the P300 in the identification of any central nervous system (CNS) alteration, possibly correlated with cognitive dysfunction, in OCD patients. In particular, we focused on the electrophysiological correlates of some behaviors of individuals with OCD, such as the tendency of these patients to become aroused and exhibit strong defensive reactions to minimal stimulation (19, 20). Another aim was to assess the possible contribution of the P300 to the characterization of different OCD expressions, e.g., structural vs. functional brain abnormalities, within the clinical spectrum of a disease with a likely multifactorial basis.

As known, P300 is a positive component of the human event-related potential (ERP). It is associated with processes of encoding, identification, and categorization, that constitute, as a whole, the superior cortical function of information processing, and it is influenced by the effects of natural (i.e., circadian, ultradian, seasonal) and environmental state variables, such as fatigue (2125). P300 is generated along a widely distributed network, rather than by a specific region, including the hippocampus and the medial temporal lobe, the temporo-parietal junction, and parietal areas, although the inferior and middle frontal, the orbito-frontal, and the cingulate cortices also contribute to it (26). For this reason, P300 involves several areas that seem to be implicated in the OCD pathophysiology (1218).

P300 is most commonly elicited with an oddball paradigm in which a subject detects an occasional target stimulus within a regular train of standard stimuli (23, 25). Another paradigm to elicit P300 is the so-called “Go/No-Go” task, in which subjects are required to respond to one of the choices but must withhold a response to the other alternative (27). The test is passed only when the “Go” condition is met and the “No-Go” condition fails (25, 27). In psychophysiology, Go/No-Go tests are used to measure the participant's capacity to sustain attention and to control responses during the electroencephalographic (EEG) recording and the acquisition of ERP waves for each answer (25, 27, 28). For instance, a “Go/No-Go” test can require a participant to perform an action given certain stimuli (e.g., press a button: “Go”) and to inhibit the same action under a different set of stimuli (e.g., not press the same button: “No-Go”) (25, 27).

P300, also referred to as P3b, is better recorded with a maximum peak over the Pz scalp location (according to the EEG international 10–20 system) if the subject is actively engaged in the task of detecting the target (25, 29). Otherwise, a distractor tone (novelty) usually elicits a P3a wave, that has a frontal/central maximum amplitude distribution and frontal sources (30, 31). The P3b amplitude is a function of some psychological variables, such as attention, expectation of the event, and attribution to the event of a significance and complexity of the task (22, 23). Wave latency provides an indirect measure of the duration of the processes involved in stimulus discrimination, and ranges approximately from 300 ms (for simple dual tone discrimination tasks) to 750 ms (for much more complex processes) (22).

The P300 has already been applied in depicting, better than neuropsychological tests, even subtle cognitive deficits in some neuropsychiatric diseases, both in adults and children, such as sporadic amyotrophic lateral sclerosis without dementia, narcolepsy, obstructive sleep apnoea syndrome, and migraine (3236). Evoked potentials occurring after stimulus presentation and preceding (e.g., P100, N100, P200, N200) or following (slow wave) the P300 wave are all components reflecting the time course of task-related neural information-processing (25, 3740). In detail, N200 is elicited by both expected and ignored rare stimuli; it is followed by P300 when the subject is engaged in a particular stimulus of recognition (40). N200 seems to be an automatic process independent of control; it is similar to P300 in terms of sensitivity to attention and stimulus infrequency, and its latency correlates with reaction time (37). Finally, it is worth reminding that P300 is not seen in the raw EEG recording and can only be detected by averaging (41).

In summary, P300 seems to be a tool to investigate possible and different information processing changes in patients with OCD. The number of studies published on this topic deserves a timely re-examination of the available literature. The conclusion arising from this review may lead to further insights into the research agenda of OCD and, translationally, into clinical applications in other psychiatric and neuropsychiatric disorders, in terms of diagnostic work-up, follow-up assessment, and pharmacological and/or behavioral treatment index of response.

Methods

This review included all relevant original articles published in peer-reviewed journals, indexed in the National Institutes of Health—National Library of Medicine (PubMed) literature search system, from database inception to July 2021. Search terms were “obsessive-compulsive disorder” and “P300.” The main inclusion criterion required that all the original research articles measured the P300 wave in humans; conversely, all the studies that did not explicitly report data concerning the evaluation of P300 in OCD were excluded. Non-English written articles, book chapters, monographs, commentaries, reviews, case studies, dissertations, abstracts, and letters to editor were also excluded, as well as any other article that did not fit the primary goal of the present review. Titles and abstracts of the retrieved studies were independently reviewed by two authors (G.L. and R.F.) based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Additional articles in the reference list of the papers identified by the search were also evaluated for inclusion in the review.

Results

The PubMed-based search originally produced a group of 34 studies. Three of them were excluded because they were not written in English; another article was excluded because it was a review. Five additional papers were retrieved from the reference list of the selected articles. Therefore, a final group of 35 studies (summarized in Table 1) investigating P300 in patients with OCD was included in this review (19, 20, 28, 4273). Figure 1 shows the flow of information through the different phases of the review process.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies investigating P300 in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and P300 parameters at Pz (usually the lead with the largest peak).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow of information through the different phases of the review process.

Since the studies eventually included in this review deal with different aspects and methods, we will analyze their content in the following separate paragraphs.

P300 Amplitude

Brain volume changes have been reported in OCD by neuroimaging (17). Therefore, we first consider here the studies reporting a decreased P300 amplitude, such as in neurodegenerative disorders (74, 75) or schizophrenia (76). In these conditions, a reduced amplitude of P300 is considered as a somewhat robust finding, possibly suggesting a genetic endophenotype (75, 76).

Overall, studies reporting a P300 amplitude reduction in OCD are thirteen (20, 28, 45, 51, 54, 55, 59, 64, 65, 67, 68, 70, 72), including the result of a P300 alteration at the F7 location in patients with functional constipation within a probable OCD (65). Conversely, we counted eight studies reporting enhanced P300 amplitude in OCD patients compared to normal controls (50, 52, 58, 6163, 66, 69), a finding that was considered as an electrophysiological correlate of an OCD trait and indicating an increased propensity to be aroused (50, 77). Finally, one article reported that future responders to treatment had significantly enhanced P300 amplitude, compared to future non-responders; this finding is an example of the possible prognostic value of psychophysiological measures (47).

Clinical observations, neuropsychological testing, and pioneering neurophysiological investigations suggest deficits in set-shifting, impaired early-filtering selective attention, loss of normal inhibitory processes, and altered motor and cognitive inhibition in individuals with OCD (1, 7, 10, 11, 19, 20, 41). Accordingly, the “Go/No-Go” is a task in which subjects with impaired frontal lobe abilities are known to fail (78). Indeed, all the five studies adopting a “Go/No-Go” visual task in OCD highlighted a frontal dysfunction pattern (28, 53, 57, 65, 73), including a study in adolescent patients (73) and one in subjects with functional constipation and probable OCD (65). Therefore, the No-Go-N200 seems to be an accurate response inhibition measurement for patients affected by OCD.

A significant relationship between ERP abnormalities and severity of OCD symptoms was found in 13 studies (44, 4653, 55, 57, 59, 64, 6870, 72). Thus, ERP abnormalities might be considered as a sensitive tool for measuring the biological substrate of OCD severity. However, P300 characteristics were not associated with symptom severity in 11 studies on patients with OCD (19, 48, 51, 52, 54, 58, 6163, 66, 67), thus allowing to hypothesize that P300 abnormalities in patients with OCD might constitute a trait, rather than a state, feature.

P300 Latency

The shortening in P300 latency may reflect the trait of obsessionals to increase the response speed to task-dependent processes in a context of enhanced cortical responsiveness, probably due to a low level of inhibitory activity (20, 47). Studies reporting a decreased P300 latency in OCD are nine (20, 4249, 52, 55). For instance, some authors using a two-tone auditory paradigm (55) described a shorter P300 duration in obsessional patients, compared to normal controls, calculated as the time difference between the N200 peak and the beginning of the slow wave. As stated, N200 is similar to P300 in terms of sensitivity to attention and stimulus infrequency, and its latency correlates with the reaction time (37). There are also several articles reporting N200 latency reduction (19, 4244, 47, 48). Regarding the effect of treatment, one study only reported a longer P300 latency at baseline in drug-free OCD patients compared to controls, along with no modification at follow-up (51).

Source Localization

A detailed knowledge of P300 generators is crucial for an appropriate understanding of its cognitive significance and clinical utility (79). However, this remains a challenging issue, especially when translating it to a practical level (e.g., the high number of EEG electrodes to be used) and when considering that a very large number of different generator assemblies can produce the same potential field on the scalp (80). As such, the localization of a limited number of equivalent dipoles is the most typical approach (81, 82) also used in OCD research.

A preliminary study was set up in order to perform a dipole source analysis for the discrimination between P3a and P3b subcomponents elicited by an auditory oddball paradigm. Obsessive-compulsive disorder patients showed a larger P3b amplitude and a shorter P3b latency, as well as a shorter reaction time to target tones, compared to healthy controls. The P3b abnormalities found in these patients might be viewed as the electrophysiological correlate of overfocussed attention and faster cognitive processes, possibly due to a higher arousal (52). However, it cannot be excluded that the abnormalities in P3b amplitude and latency may reflect structural or functional disturbances in temporo-parietal or temporo-basal areas of OCD patients. This view is supported by neuroimaging research showing hyperperfusion or activation in the medial temporal lobe of subjects with OCD (83). Larger amplitude and shorter latency of the P3a subcomponent, which mainly reflects the frontal hyperactivity (42, 45, 54), were not confirmed in this study, probably because the authors found no difference in the P3a between patients and controls. It is worth to note that this apparent negative finding may also shows some limits of the technique.

The orbito-frontal cortex, the activity of which was found to be impaired in OCD patients (17, 84), is located deeply in the ventromedial anterior part of the brain. Given that neuronal activity of the orbito-frontal cortex cannot probably be recorded by scalp electrodes, the temporo-superior dipole (P3a), which is calculated for the scalp data, was unable to reflect activity from this brain area. Subsequently, other authors confirmed the previous findings on the P3b (58), whereas a more recent study on 30 psychiatric patients (but only two with OCD) was carried out with a visual oddball paradigm followed by a source localization analysis using a 256-channel EEG dense array (71). The inferior frontal gyrus was found to be the most active brain area in the few OCD patients (71). Two additional studies (62, 69) involving a higher number of subjects supported the concept that P300 is generated along a widely distributed network involving several brain areas implicated in OCD, as recently confirmed by some neuroimaging research (17, 18), with both pharmacological treatment (62) and emotional drive (60) able to attenuate these functional changes.

Moreover, topographic ERP mapping revealed significantly smaller P300 magnitude in the rostral frontal areas during the “No-Go” condition in individuals with OCD (28, 53). In a subsequent study (57), patients manifested reduced No-Go-N200 and Go-N200 amplitudes at the frontocentral electrode sites, compared to healthy controls, although the two groups were comparable with regard to Go-P300 and No-Go-P300 amplitudes and latencies.

Novelty P3a

The P3a originates from stimulus-driven frontal attention mechanisms during task processing (30). To obtain this evoked response, novel stimuli are presented infrequently within a background of frequently occurring standard stimuli and infrequently occurring distractor stimuli, while the subject is not required to respond mentally or physically to any stimulus (31). In an ad hoc study, novelty P3a amplitude was found to be increased in OCD patients compared to healthy controls, thus possibly representing a physiological index of enhanced cortical orienting response and implicating a facilitation of involuntary shifts of attention occurring in this condition (61).

Laterality Pattern

A re-examination of positron emission tomography (PET), EEG, and single case studies previously performed by Flor-Henry (85) suggests that a lateralized dysregulation of the left fronto-caudate network is the major cerebral determinant of the obsessive-compulsive state. The concept is even more interesting when considering the recognition of features of OCD in patients with schizophrenia (86) and the hypothesized hemispheric imbalance in psychosis (87). The laterality pattern proposed by Flor-Henry (85) was partially confirmed by some N200/P300 studies and topographical mapping in OCD (20, 42, 44, 47, 49, 62, 65). Indeed, in a dipole source analysis, patients with OCD showed P300 impairment only in the right hemisphere (52). Moreover, both abstinent heroin addicts and individuals with OCD show a P300 amplitude reduction over the right frontal area, compared to healthy subjects (56).

P300 and Treatment

We found six articles on the relationship between P300 and treatment in OCD (47, 49, 51, 62, 67, 68), one of them (68) was a pediatric survey. One study, and its replication with additional patients, showed that future responders to 1-year treatment (fluoxetine, fluvoxamine, clomipramine) had significantly reduced N200 and enhanced P300 amplitude compared to future non-responders (47, 49). Patients were considered non-responders if they failed to to respond to separate treatments lasting for at least 8 weeks in total, and at the maximum antidepressant dose for at least 5 weeks. The authors suggested that ERPs might constitute psychophysiological profiles in individuals with OCD, thus implying potential clinical and pharmacological implications (47, 49). Another article reported that P300 had a lower baseline amplitude and a longer latency in drug-free OCD patients compared to healthy controls; subsequently, P300 exhibited a trend toward an amplitude increase after treatment (clomipramine), without modification in latency (51). A few patients were treated when included in the study and initially recorded; they stopped taking medication, and after at least 1 month, when the acute symptomatology appeared again, they underwent the second interview and the ERP study. This allowed to minimize the learning effect on P300 obtained under treatment.

Some results by other authors (62) have already been described in the paragraph on source localization; the same authors also aimed to assess the effects of 10 ± 1 weeks of treatment with sertraline on P300 brain activity patterns. In the patients retested after treatment, a reduction of P300, in both amplitude and latency, was observed which, however, did not reach statistical significance, as also found by other researchers (51). On the contrary, another investigation demonstrated that individuals with OCD had decreased P300 amplitude at baseline, which significantly increased at Fz, Cz, C3, and C4, indicating normalization, after 1 year of behavioral and pharmacological treatment (fluvoxamine, paroxetine, sertraline, clomipramine); the same group achieved similar results on the utility of SSRIs in pediatric OCD patients (68). Compared to controls, P300 amplitudes were smaller in the OCD group at Fz, Cz, Pz, C3, and C4. Approximately 3 years after the start of SSRI treatment (unspecified molecules), P300 amplitude significantly increased at Fz and C4, along with clinical improvement.

P300 in OCD Overlapping With Other Disorders

Since OCD can affect patients with other conditions, we also found some studies on OCD overlapping with other diseases, such as schizophrenia (60), Tourette syndrome (43, 59), functional constipation (65), mild depressive disorder, chronic motor or vocal tic disorder, social anxiety disorder of childhood, adjustment disorder, attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), social phobia, and expressive language disorder (73).

Comorbid schizophrenia-OCD (schizo-OCD) is characterized by the concurrent presentation of psychotic and obsessive-compulsive symptoms (86, 8890), that may require high antipsychotic dosages for its acute exacerbations and for the maintenance of reduction of the severity of psychosis (91). Schizo-OCD sufferers show a distinct ERP pattern, with abnormally increased target activation similar to that described in OCD (20) but different from that usually observed in schizophrenia (76). These patients were reported to have also reduced P300 amplitude, similarly to schizophrenia (76, 92), but different from other results in individuals with OCD (50, 52, 58, 6163, 66, 69). Therefore, schizo-OCD may be not only a clinical entity different from pure OCD and schizophrenia, but also a relatively distinct neurophysiologic condition (60).

Tourette syndrome is a neurodevelopmental disorder mainly characterized by tics, although most patients also experience sensory disturbances, especially in terms of premonitory urges and sensory hypersensitivity, which may account for comorbid OCD, ADHD, and autism spectrum disorder, with a possible partially common pathophysiology underlying them (93). One study described that patients with Tourette syndrome and OCD had shorter N200 and P300 latencies (43), thus confirming the above-mentioned common cortical hyperarousal state hypothesized for both conditions (93). Another article seems to be also in line with neuroimaging findings (18, 94) when describing a P300 amplitude reduction in the anterior scalp regions in both OCD and Tourette syndrome with OCD patients (59).

A study in patients with functional constipation found that they were also obsessive, anxious, and depressed, with reduced P300 amplitude at F7 compared to controls (65). The authors speculate that, in patients with functional constipation, asymmetric forebrain abnormal activities in the two hemispheres might initiate some implicit automatic processing, such as somatization and OCD, in order to cope with painful experiences caused by anxiety and depression.

Cognitive dysfunction of implicit processing might also be involved in impaired visual communication and information processing. One of the above-listed studies (73) did not consider comorbidities separately due to the low number of overlapping psychiatric conditions. The most active brain regions in the few OCD patients included were the frontal areas.

P300 in OCD Compared to Other Conditions

Shorter N200 and P300 latencies in OCD compared to social phobia and normal controls were believed to be an OCD-associated phenomenon (speeding of cognitive processing) (48). In two studies, P300 amplitudes were significantly smaller in schizophrenia and OCD patients than in healthy subjects (54, 60), a finding which is in line with the hypothesis of brain volume changes in both psychiatric illnesses (18, 76, 92).

One group of researchers assessed working memory and attentional capacities in OCD and opioid addicted: the abstinent group showed a notable delay of the P300 latency compared to controls and OCD only over the right occipital region, while OCD patients exhibited a significant prolongation of the P300 recorded over the central prefrontal area compared to addicts and healthy controls (56). Although this was a rather complex study, the results of which were not fully in line with previous data on the P300 latency in OCD, it focused on the peculiar phenomenological aspect that addicts are quite similar to obsessionals when craving for drugs becomes irresistible, such as an obsession (95, 96).

P300 was reduced in participants with both OCD and Tourette syndrome and OCD over the anterior scalp regions, whereas the P300 oddball effect was significantly larger in participants with Tourette syndrome compared to all other groups (59). Therefore, the authors speculated that Tourette syndrome may be characterized by an enhanced working memory that updates processes and the superimposition of OCD might lead to a reduction of these processes (59). In one study only, P300 amplitude was found to be smaller in patients than in their siblings and also smaller in siblings than in controls; a logistic regression analysis showed that higher P300 amplitude and better performance at neuropsychological tests of the frontal cortex function were predictors for control subjects, whereas higher P200 amplitude predicted both patients and their siblings (66). The authors concluded that this pattern might be an endophenotype of OCD.

Pediatric Studies

We found five articles on P300 in pediatric OCD patients (43, 64, 68, 70, 73). Some investigators emphasized that pediatric patients have reduced P300 amplitude (64, 68, 70), altered response time (70), and partial increase of P300 amplitude after SSRI treatment (68). The replicated finding of decreased P300 amplitude in OCD children and adolescents and the correlation between illness severity and P300 amplitude led the authors to suggest that this psychophysiological feature might be considered as a sensitive tool for measuring the biological aspects of OCD severity (64, 68).

Another study investigated the auditory information processing in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome overlapping with ADHD or OCD (43). Tourette syndrome patients with OCD had shorter N200 and P300 latencies, indicating cortical hyperarousal, similarly to pure OCD patients (20, 42, 44, 4749, 52, 55). This pivotal concept, i.e., the fact that OCD patients may have a distinctive tendency to be aroused and to exhibit strong defensive reactions to minimal stimulation, as already highlighted by some authors (19, 20) for adults with OCD, seems to be present also in young patients. Pediatric OCD patients showed higher P300 amplitudes during “Go” vs. “No/Go” trials and during incongruent vs. congruent trials, thus confirming abnormal frontal hyperactivity also in young OCD patients (73).

Discussion

This review included 35 studies with different paradigms (e.g., classical visual or auditory oddball, novelty, Go/No-Go), which have different recording modes (from only a few to 256 electrodes) and several objectives (e.g., localization of dysfunctional brain areas and treatment response) in adults, adolescents, and children. This heterogeneity did not allow us to perform any meta-analytic calculation, although the most relevant findings about P300 in OCD have been addressed in this review and summarized in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of the relevant data found in obsessive-compulsive disorder regarding the event-related potential components considered in this review.

Neuroimaging-based multimodal approaches have led to the conclusion that subtle brain structural changes and functional abnormality are present in OCD (15, 17, 18), with PET studies showing hypermetabolism in the orbital frontal cortex of these patients (85, 97). In the context of structural changes in OCD, the value of a decreased amplitude of P300 becomes evident, as reported by several studies (28, 45, 51, 54, 55, 6468, 70, 72), similarly to those performed in schizophrenia (77). The finding of attenuated P300 amplitude has also been considered to be a pattern of genetic endophenotype in OCD (75, 76), which has also been confirmed in pediatric studies (64, 68, 70). Moreover, it was found to correlate with illness severity, suggesting that the reduction in P300 amplitude may be a sensitive tool for measuring some biological aspects of OCD severity (64, 68), also based on the evidence that P300 is generated along a widely distributed network that includes several brain areas implicated in the pathophysiology of OCD (1218, 26). Of note, one study in schizo-OCD patients showed a pattern of alteration, i.e., a reduced P300 amplitude as in schizophrenia (60), which suggests the occurrence of brain structural abnormalities also in this peculiar clinical psychosis with relevant obsessional symptoms (8890).

Nevertheless, there are also studies, depending on the type of paradigm used but suggesting the possible existence of different expressions of OCD within its clinical spectrum, reporting enhanced P300 amplitude in patients with OCD, compared to healthy controls (50, 52, 58, 6163, 66, 69). Most of these studies considered this result as the electrophysiological correlate of an OCD trait consisting in a special tendency to get aroused (50, 77). Notably, only one of these studies (69) revealed that there was a weak relationship between ERP abnormalities and symptoms severity in OCD; more specifically, the authors reported only a weak correlation between compulsion estimation and the P300 valence effect (69). Based on the enhanced P300 amplitude, a pattern of cortical hyperarousal and over-focused attention in OCD has been hypothesized (19, 20, 4244, 4650), as also supported by the findings on processing negativity, which were interpreted as an indication of the existence of a hyperactivation of the frontal cortical mechanisms (12, 16, 19, 20, 85).

The decreased N200 latency in OCD (19, 4244, 47, 48) and P300 (20, 4244, 4749, 52, 55) further emphasizes the concept of dysfunctional speed of information processing, possibly leading to some clinical features, such as intrusive thoughts that increase anxiety. The loss of normal inhibitory processes (28, 53, 57, 65, 73) would then serve as a basic source for the initiation of compulsion, with the to decrease anxiety.

The pattern of different amplitude seems to suggest the presence of different expressions (structural abnormalities vs. brain dysfunction) within the clinical spectrum of OCD. For instance, patients with a severe symptomatology can be seen (64, 68), including those with schizo-OCD (60), as belonging to the first group (structural abnormalities), whose abnormalities mainly concern the frontal brain areas (85, 97). The dysfunction of the frontal lobes has been implicated in OCD (12, 85). Flor-Henry suggested that OCD may be secondary to a prominent frontal lobe dysfunction, along with a loss of the physiological inhibitory processes (98). Other studies confirmed the involvement of the cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical loop, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the prefrontal cortex in OCD (918, 85, 97). However, frontal damage or dysfunction has been found in a number of psychiatric disorders in addition to OCD, and particularly schizophrenia. Some neuroimaging studies indicate that OCD symptoms are associated with altered activity in the orbito-frontal cortex (12, 99, 100), being this finding likely due to the dominant role of the frontal lobe in executive functioning and self-regulatory behaviors (12, 100, 101), which are both altered in several psychiatric illnesses. Therefore, the frontal abnormality may reflect a final common pathway for abnormal behavior (12, 28). Alternatively, different disorders may result from the dysfunction of different frontal subsystems (12, 28). For instance, the dorsolateral frontal region has been found to be impaired in schizophrenia, whereas the orbito-frontal areas have been implicated in OCD (12, 102).

Nevertheless, the above-mentioned features may not fully explain the inconsistency between the enhanced and reduced P300 in OCD, thus making the interpretation of these results challenging. Furthrmore, the fact that several data have been published by the same research group needs be taken into account (64, 67, 68, 70). However, it should be noted that most OCD individuals show a peculiar propensity to get aroused and typically exhibit strong defensive reactions even to minimal stimulation (19, 20, 4246, 48, 50, 55, 71, 72). Translationally, these are the patients who can benefit from a targeted cognitive-behavioral rehabilitation, associated with drug therapy. On the contrary, pediatric subjects, schizo-OCD patients, and severe OCD cases frequently show low-amplitude P300 (28, 45, 51, 54, 55, 64, 65, 67, 68, 70, 72), a finding in line with the brain imaging data reported by different authors (1518). Based on the P300 parameters, follow-up studies after drug and rehabilitation therapy are warranted in order to gain further insights on the disease severity and the possibility of clinical and cognitive improvement (64, 103). In this scenario, it is worth to highlight that Morault et al. (49) have already suggested that some pre-treatment features of ERPs might be associated with a more favorable outcome after treatment.

The laterality pattern, i.e., left<right, proposed by Flor-Henry (85), was partially confirmed by some N200/P300 studies and topographic mapping in OCD (20, 42, 44, 47, 49, 62, 65), whereas the partial recovery of P300 changes, in parallel with the clinical improvement (47, 49, 51, 62, 67, 68), seems to be mainly attributable to the effect of serotonin on ERP amplitude (68).

Regarding the effect of treatment, only one study (67) used psychotherapy combined with drugs. In this context, two studies have provided useful psychophysiological profiles in individuals with OCD with clinical and pharmacological implications, thus suggesting that the outcome of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) combined to SSRI should be studied along with ERPs (47, 49). In addition, there were also a few reports of P300 amplitude with partial recovery after adequate pharmacological treatment (51, 67, 68, 70).

Conclusions and Future Directions

Notwithstanding the limitations of the studies and their heterogeneous methodology, the findings reviewed here seem to support that the different P300 patterns observed might suggest the presence of different expressions (structural vs. functional brain abnormalities) within the clinical spectrum of OCD. Event-related potentials may also be used as a treatment monitoring marker at the individual level, especially for both pharmacological treatment and/or CBT (104). In particular, the development of novel P3a paradigms in combination with P3b tasks seems to be promising for a more extensive application and reliability of ERPs (103). Moreover, similarly to patients with migraine, who can be re-tested during treatment and follow-up in order to detect an improvement of P300 habituation (35), also OCD patients might be serially evaluated over time to identify any possible change in their neurophysiological correlates of cortical hyperarousal, over-focused attention, and response inhibition. The same holds true for children and adolescents with decreased P300 amplitude at baseline, who might be re-evaluated during or after treatment in order to assess whether P300 changes reflect pharmacological and/or psychotherapeutic effects (67, 68).

Nevertheless, the majority of the articles reviewed here included very small cohorts of patients, thus emphasizing again the importance of objective evaluations in neuropsychiatric and psychological disorders, as well as the need for a systematic examination and the application of standardized procedures to obtain more realiable guidelines in the near future. For instance, it would be intriguing to apply the three-oddball paradigm (31) to record N100, MMN, and P3a with the same conscruct. Their abnormality would suggest a frontal dysfunction and might help the differentiation between OCD individuals, severe cases, and schizo-OCD. Other studies may deal with the lack of P300 habituation by using two or three blocks of stimuli, as already done in patients with migraine (35, 105). Finally, further data using multidimensional measurement techniques (e.g., behavioral, electrophysiological, structural, metabolic) will be also necessary before the relationship between brain (mainly frontal) dysfunction and OCD psychopathology can be conclusively clarified.

Author Contributions

AR, GL, and RF designed research, performed literature review, analyzed data, wrote the paper, and revised the text. All authors contributed to this manuscript and agree with its content.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Goodman WK, Grice DE, Lapidus KA, Coffey BJ. Obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am. (2014) 37:257–67. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2014.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013).

Google Scholar

3. Pitres A, Régis E. Les Obsessions et les Impulsions. Paris: Octave Doin. (1902).

Google Scholar

4. Sandler J, Hazari A. The 'obsessional': on the psychological classification of obsessional character traits and symptoms. Br J Med Psychol. (1960) 33:113–22. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1960.tb01232.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Ingram IM. The obsessional personality and obsessional illness. Am J Psychiatry. (1961) 117:1016–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.117.11.1016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Pancheri P. (1992). Ossessioni, Compulsioni E Continuum Ossessivo. Rome: Il Pensiero Scientifico Editore.

7. Black JL. Obsessive compulsive disorder: a clinical update. Mayo Clin Proc. (1992) 67:266–75. doi: 10.1016/s0025-6196(12)60104-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Baup N. Reconnaitre un trouble obsessionnel compulsif. Rev Prat. (2007) 57:27–36.

Google Scholar

9. Boldrini M, Del Pace L, Placidi GP, Keilp J, Ellis SP, Signori S, et al. Selective cognitive deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder compared to panic disorder with agoraphobia. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2005) 111:150–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00247.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Cetinay Aydin P, Gulec Oyekcin D. Obsesif kompulsif bozuklukta bilissel islevler. Turk Psikiyatri Derg. (2013) 24:266–74.

Google Scholar

11. Shin NY, Lee TY, Kim E, Kwon JS. Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:1121–30. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Pirau L, Lui F. Frontal lobe syndrome. In: StatPearls. Treasure Island, FL: StatPearls Publishing (2020).

Google Scholar

13. Berthier ML, Kulisevsky J, Gironell A, Heras JA. Obsessive-compulsive disorder associated with brain lesions: clinical phenomenology, cognitive function, and anatomic correlates. Neurology. (1996) 47:353–61. doi: 10.1212/wnl.47.2.353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Mindus P, Rasmussen SA, Lindquist C. Neurosurgical treatment for refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder: implications for understanding frontal lobe function. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (1994) 6:467–77. doi: 10.1176/jnp.6.4.467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Saxena S, Brody AL, Schwartz JM, Baxter LR. Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. (1998) 35:26–37.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

16. Brem S, Grunblatt E, Drechsler R, Riederer P, Walitza S. The neurobiological link between OCD and ADHD. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. (2014) 6:175–202. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0146-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hazari N, Narayanaswamy JC, Venkatasubramanian G. Neuroimaging findings in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a narrative review to elucidate neurobiological underpinnings. Indian J Psychiatry. 61(Suppl 1) (2019) S9–29. doi: 10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_525_18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Park SE, Kim BC, Yang JC, Jeong GW. MRI-based multimodal approach to the assessment of clinical symptom severity of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Investig. (2020) 17:777–85. doi: 10.30773/pi.2020.0124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Ciesielski KT, Beech HR, Gordon PK. Some electrophysiological observations in obsessional states. Br J Psychiatry. (1981) 138:479–84. doi: 10.1192/bjp.138.6.479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Beech HR, Ciesielski KT, Gordon PK. Further observations of evoked potentials in obsessional patients. Br J Psychiatry. (1983) 142:605–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.142.6.605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Sutton S, Braren M, Zubin J, John ER. Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science. (1965) 150:1187–8. doi: 10.1126/science.150.3700.1187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Pritchard WS. Psychophysiology of P300. Psychol Bull. (1981) 89:506–40.

Google Scholar

23. Picton TW. The P300 wave of the human event-related potential. J Clin Neurophysiol. (1992) 9:456–79. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199210000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Polich J, Kok A. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review. Biol Psychol. (1995) 41:103–46. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05130-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. de Tommaso M, Betti V, Bocci T, Bolognini N, Di Russo F, Fattapposta F, et al. Pearls and pitfalls in brain functional analysis by event-related potentials: a narrative review by the Italian Psychophysiology and Cognitive Neuroscience Society on methodological limits and clinical reliability-part I. Neurol Sci. (2020) 41:2711–35. doi: 10.1007/s10072-020-04420-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Linden DE, Prvulovic D, Formisano E, Vollinger M, Zanella FE, Goebel R, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of target detection: an fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. Cereb Cortex. (1999) 9:815–23. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.8.815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2008) 137:649–72. doi: 10.1037/a0013170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Malloy P, Rasmussen S, Braden W, Haier RJ. Topographic evoked potential mapping in obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence of frontal lobe dysfunction. Psychiatry Res. (1989) 28:63–71. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90198-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Polich J, Criado JR. Neuropsychology and neuropharmacology of P3a and P3b. Int J Psychophysiol. (2006) 60:172–85. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol. (2007) 118:2128–48. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Raggi A, Tasca D, Rundo F, Ferri R. Stability of auditory discrimination and novelty processing in physiological aging. Behav Neurol. (2013) 27:193–200. doi: 10.3233/BEN-120261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Raggi A, Iannaccone S, Cappa SF. Event-related brain potentials in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a review of the international literature. Amyotroph Lateral Scler. (2010) 11:16–26. doi: 10.3109/17482960902912399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Raggi A, Plazzi G, Pennisi G, Tasca D, Ferri R. Cognitive evoked potentials in narcolepsy: a review of the literature. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:1144–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Raggi A, Ferri R. Cognitive evoked potentials in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: a review of the literature. Rev Neurosci. (2012) 23:311–23. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Raggi A, Ferri R. Information processing in migraine: a review of studies on P300. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. (2020) 45:131–44. doi: 10.1007/s10484-020-09469-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Raggi A, Lanza G, Ferri R. Auditory mismatch negativity in bipolar disorder: a focused review. Rev Neurosci. (2021). doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2021-0010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Ritter W, Simson R, Vaughan HG Jr. Event-related potential correlates of two stages of information processing in physical and semantic discrimination tasks. Psychophysiology. (1983) 20:168–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1983.tb03283.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Perrault N, Picton TW. Event-related potentials recorded from the scalp and nasopharynx I N1 and P2. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1984) 59:177–94. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(84)90058-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Cantone M, Lanza G, Ranieri F, Opie GM, Terranova C. Editorial: non-invasive brain stimulation in the study and modulation of metaplasticity in neurological disorders. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:721906. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.721906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Perrault N, Picton TW. Event-related potentials recorded from the scalp and nasopharynx. II. N2, P3 and slow wave. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1984) 59:261–78. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(84)90044-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Dawson GD. A summation technique for the detection of small evoked potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1954) 6:65–84. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(54)90007-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Towey J, Bruder G, Hollander E, Friedman D, Erhan H, Liebowitz M, et al. Endogenous event-related potentials in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (1990) 28:92–8. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(90)90626-d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Drake ME Jr, Hietter SA, Padamadan H, Bogner JE, Andrews JM, Weate S. Auditory evoked potentials in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Clin Electroencephalogr. (1992) 23:19–23. doi: 10.1177/155005949202300106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Towey J, Bruder G, Tenke C, Leite P, DeCaria C, Friedman D, et al. Event-related potential and clinical correlates of neurodysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. (1993) 49:167–81. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(93)90103-n

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Towey JP, Tenke CE, Bruder GE, Leite P, Friedman D, Liebowitz M, et al. Brain event-related potential correlates of overfocused attention in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychophysiology. (1994) 31:535–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02346.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. de Groot CM, Torello MW, Boutros NN, Allen R. Auditory event-related potentials and statistical probability mapping in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin Electroencephalogr. (1997) 28:148–54. doi: 10.1177/155005949702800306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Morault PM, Bourgeois M, Laville J, Bensch C, Paty J. Psychophysiological and clinical value of event-related potentials in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (1997) 42:46–56. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00228-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Miyata A, Matsunaga H, Kiriike N, Iwasaki Y, Takei Y, Yamagami S. Event-related potentials in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (1998) 52:513–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.1998.00427.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Morault P, Guillem F, Bourgeois M, Paty J. Improvement predictors in obsessive-compulsive disorder. An event-related potential study. Psychiatry Res. (1998) 81:87–96. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(98)00091-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Di Russo F, Zaccara G, Ragazzoni A, Pallanti S. Abnormal visual event-related potentials in obsessive-compulsive disorder without panic disorder or depression comorbidity. J Psychiatr Res. (2000) 34:75–82. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(99)00030-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Sanz M, Molina V, Martin-Loeches M, Calcedo A, Rubia FJ. Auditory P300 event related potential and serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients. Psychiatry Res. (2001) 101:75–81. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00250-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Mavrogiorgou P, Juckel G, Frodl T, Gallinat J, Hauke W, Zaudig M, et al. P300 subcomponents in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Psychiatr Res. (2002) 36:399–406. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(02)00055-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Herrmann MJ, Jacob C, Unterecker S, Fallgatter AJ. Reduced response-inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder measured with topographic evoked potential mapping. Psychiatry Res. (2003) 120:265–71. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(03)00188-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Kim MS, Kang SS, Youn T, Kang DH, Kim JJ, Kwon JS. Neuropsychological correlates of P300 abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2003) 123:109–23. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(03)00045-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Kivircik BB, Yener GG, Alptekin K, Aydin H. Event-related potentials and neuropsychological tests in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2003) 27:601–6. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(03)00047-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Papageorgiou C, Rabavilas A, Liappas I, Stefanis C. Do obsessive-compulsive patients and abstinent heroin addicts share a common psychophysiological mechanism? Neuropsychobiology. (2003) 47:1–11. doi: 10.1159/000068868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kim MS, Kim YY, Yoo SY, Kwon JS. Electrophysiological correlates of behavioral response inhibition in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety. (2007) 24:22–31. doi: 10.1002/da.20195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Gohle D, Juckel G, Mavrogiorgou P, Pogarell O, Mulert C, Rujescu D, et al. Electrophysiological evidence for cortical abnormalities in obsessive-compulsive disorder - a replication study using auditory event-related P300 subcomponents. J Psychiatr Res. (2008) 42:297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Thibault G, Felezeu M, O'Connor KP, Todorov C, Stip E, Lavoie ME. Influence of comorbid obsessive-compulsive symptoms on brain event-related potentials in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 32:803–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Pallanti S, Castellini G, Chamberlain SR, Quercioli L, Zaccara G, Fineberg NA. Cognitive event-related potentials differentiate schizophrenia with obsessive-compulsive disorder (schizo-OCD) from OCD and schizophrenia without OC symptoms. Psychiatry Res. (2009) 170:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Ischebeck M, Endrass T, Simon D, Kathmann N. Auditory novelty processing is enhanced in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety. (2011) 28:915–23. doi: 10.1002/da.20886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Andreou C, Leicht G, Popescu V, Pogarell O, Mavrogiorgou P, Rujescu D, et al. P300 in obsessive-compulsive disorder: source localization and the effects of treatment. J Psychiatr Res. (2013) 47:1975–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Endrass T, Koehne S, Riesel A, Kathmann N. Neural correlates of feedback processing in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. (2013) 122:387–96. doi: 10.1037/a0031496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Yamamuro K, Ota T, Nakanishi Y, Matsuura H, Okazaki K, Kishimoto N, et al. Event-related potentials in drug-naive pediatric patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 230:394–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.09.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Li X, Feng R, Wu H, Zhang L, Zhao L, Dai N, et al. Psychological characteristics and GoNogo research of patients with functional constipation. Medicine (Baltimore). (2016) 95:e5685. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000005685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Ozcan H, Ozer S, Yagcioglu S. Neuropsychological, electrophysiological and neurological impairments in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder, their healthy siblings and healthy controls: Identifying potential endophenotype(s). Psychiatry Res. (2016) 240:110–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Yamamuro K, Okada K, Kishimoto N, Ota T, Iida J, Kishimoto T. A longitudinal, event-related potential pilot study of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder with 1-year follow-up. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2016) 12:2463–71. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S117100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Yamamuro K, Ota T, Iida J, Kishimoto N, Nakanishi Y, Matsuura H, et al. A longitudinal event-related potential study of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor therapy in treatment-naive pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder patients. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 245:217–23. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Dayan-Riva A, Berger A, Anholt GE. Early cognitive processes in OCD: An ERP study. J Affect Disord. (2019) 246:429–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.12.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Okazaki K, Yamamuro K, Iida J, Ota T, Nakanishi Y, Matsuura H, et al. Intra-individual variability across cognitive task in drug-naive pediatric patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 264:421–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Wojcik GM, Masiak J, Kawiak A, Schneider P, Kwasniewicz L, Polak N, et al. New protocol for quantitative analysis of brain cortex electroencephalographic activity in patients with psychiatric disorders. Front Neuroinform. (2018) 12:27. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2018.00027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Kloft L, Riesel A, Kathmann N. Inhibition-related differences between tic-free and tic-related obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from the N2 and P3. Exp Brain Res. (2019) 237:3449–59. doi: 10.1007/s00221-019-05688-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Wolff N, Chmielewski W, Buse J, Roessner V, Beste C. Paradoxical response inhibition advantages in adolescent obsessive compulsive disorder result from the interplay of automatic and controlled processes. Neuroimage Clin. (2019) 23:101893. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Polich J, Ladish C, Bloom FE. P300 assessment of early Alzheimer's disease. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1990) 77:179–89. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(90)90036-d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Hunerli D, Emek-Savas DD, Cavusoglu B, Donmez Colakoglu B, Ada E, Yener GG. Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease is associated with decreased P300 amplitude and reduced putamen volume. Clin Neurophysiol. (2019) 130:1208–17. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.04.314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Turetsky BI, Dress EM, Braff DL, Calkins ME, Green MF, Greenwood TA, et al. The utility of P300 as a schizophrenia endophenotype and predictive biomarker: clinical and socio-demographic modulators in COGS-2. Schizophr Res. (2015) 163:53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.09.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Beech HR. Ritualistic activity in obsessional patients. J Psychosom Res. (1971) 15:417–22. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(71)90022-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Drewe EA. Go - no go learning after frontal lobe lesions in humans. Cortex. (1975) 11:8–16. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(75)80015-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Linden DE. The p300: where in the brain is it produced and what does it tell us? Neuroscientist. (2005) 11:563–76. doi: 10.1177/1073858405280524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Nunez PL, Srinivasan R. Electric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of EEG. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press (2006).

Google Scholar

81. Kavanagh RN, Darcey TM, Lehmann D, Fender DH. Evaluation of methods for three-dimensional localization of electrical sources in the human brain. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. (1978) 25:421–9. doi: 10.1109/TBME.1978.326339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Michel CM, Brunet D. EEG source imaging: a practical review of the analysis steps. Front Neurol. (2019) 10:325. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Rauch SL, Jenike MA, Alpert NM, Baer L, Breiter HC, Savage CR, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow measured during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder using oxygen 15-labeled carbon dioxide and positron emission tomography. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1994) 51:62–70. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010062008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Pujol J, Blanco-Hinojo L, Macia D, Alonso P, Harrison BJ, Martinez-Vilavella G, et al. Mapping alterations of the functional structure of the cerebral cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Cereb Cortex. (2019) 29:4753–62. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Flor-Henry P. (1990). Le syndrome obsessionnel-compulsif: reflet d'un defaut de regulation fronto-caudee de l'hemisphere gauche? Encephale 16 Spec No 325–329.

86. Ohta M, Kokai M, Morita Y. Features of obsessive-compulsive disorder in patients primarily diagnosed with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2003) 57:67–74. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2003.01081.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Gruzelier JH. Hemispheric imbalances in schizophrenia. Int J Psychophysiol. (1984) 1:227–40. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(84)90043-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Poyurovsky M, Hramenkov S, Isakov V, Rauchverger B, Modai I, Schneidman M, et al. Obsessive-compulsive disorder in hospitalized patients with chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. (2001) 102:49–57. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(01)00238-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Kayahan B, Ozturk O, Veznedaroglu B, Eraslan D. Obsessive-compulsive symptoms in schizophrenia: prevalance and clinical correlates. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2005) 59:291–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01373.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Owashi T, Ota A, Otsubo T, Susa Y, Kamijima K. Obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in Japanese inpatients with chronic schizophrenia - a possible schizophrenic subtype. Psychiatry Res. (2010) 179:241–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Baytunca B, Kalyoncu T, Ozel I, Erermis S, Kayahan B, Ongur D. Early onset schizophrenia associated with obsessive-compulsive disorder: clinical features and correlates. Clin Neuropharmacol. (2017) 40:243–5. doi: 10.1097/WNF.0000000000000248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Ford JM. Schizophrenia: the broken P300 and beyond. Psychophysiology. (1999) 36:667–82.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

93. Isaacs D, Riordan H. Sensory hypersensitivity in Tourette syndrome: a review. Brain Dev. (2020) 42:627–38. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2020.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Hsu CJ, Wong LC, Wang HP, Lee WT. The multimodality neuroimage findings in individuals with Tourette syndrome. Pediatr Neonatol. (2020) 61:467–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pedneo.2020.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Goldstein WN. Obsessive-compulsive behavior, DSM-III, and a psychodynamic classification of psychopathology. Am J Psychother. (1985) 39:346–59. doi: 10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.1985.39.3.346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Pitman RK. A cybernetic model of obsessive-compulsive psychopathology. Compr Psychiatry. (1987) 28:334–43. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(87)90070-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Baxter LR Jr. Positron emission tomography studies of cerebral glucose metabolism in obsessive compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 55 Suppl. (1994) 54–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

98. Flor-Henry P. Cerebral Basis of Psychopathology. Boston, MA: John Wright. (1983).

99. Levin S. Frontal lobe dysfunctions in schizophrenia–II. Impairments of psychological and brain functions. J Psychiatr Res. (1984) 18:57–72. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(84)90047-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Stuss DT, Benson DF. Neuropsychological studies of the frontal lobes. Psychol Bull. (1984) 95:3–28.

Google Scholar

101. Faglioni P. Il lobo frontale. Denes G, Pizzamiglio L, editors. In: Manuale di Neuropsicologia. 2nd ed. (Bologna: Zanichelli) (1996) 701–750.

102. Weinberger DR. Schizophrenia and the frontal lobe. Trends Neurosci. (1988) 11:367–70. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(88)90060-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Polich J. Clinical application of the P300 event-related brain potential. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am. (2004) 15:133–61. doi: 10.1016/s1047-9651(03)00109-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. McKay D, Sookman D, Neziroglu F, Wilhelm S, Stein DJ, Kyrios M, et al. Efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 225:236–46. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.11.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Evers S, Bauer B, Suhr B, Husstedt IW, Grotemeyer KH. Cognitive processing in primary headache: a study on event-related potentials. Neurology. (1997) 48:108–13. doi: 10.1212/wnl.48.1.108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cortical hyperarousal, information processing, obsessive-compulsive disorder, over-focused attention, p300, translational neuroscience

Citation: Raggi A, Lanza G and Ferri R (2021) A Review on P300 in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 12:751215. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.751215

Received: 31 July 2021; Accepted: 01 November 2021;
Published: 23 November 2021.

Edited by:

Yanghua Tian, First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, China

Reviewed by:

Yasuo Terao, Kyorin University, Japan
Martin Desseilles, University of Namur, Belgium

Copyright © 2021 Raggi, Lanza and Ferri. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giuseppe Lanza, glanza@oasi.en.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.