Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 20 September 2021
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Cannabis and Emotion Processing View all 6 articles

Alcohol and Cannabis Use Disorder Symptom Severity, Conduct Disorder, and Callous-Unemotional Traits and Impairment in Expression Recognition

\nRobert James R. Blair
Robert James R. Blair1*Johannah Bashford-LargoJohannah Bashford-Largo1Ru ZhangRu Zhang1Avantika MathurAvantika Mathur1Amanda SchwartzAmanda Schwartz1Jaimie ElowskyJaimie Elowsky1Patrick TylerPatrick Tyler2Christopher J. HammondChristopher J. Hammond3Francesca M. FilbeyFrancesca M. Filbey4Matthew DobbertinMatthew Dobbertin1Sahil BajajSahil Bajaj1Karina S. BlairKarina S. Blair1
  • 1Center for Neurobehavioral Research, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, United States
  • 2Child and Family Translational Research Center, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, John Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 4Center for BrainHealth, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX, United States

Background: Alcohol and cannabis are commonly used by adolescents in the United States. Both alcohol use disorder (AUD) and cannabis use disorder (CUD) have been associated with reduced emotion expression recognition ability. However, this work has primarily occurred in adults and has not considered neuro-cognitive risk factors associated with conduct problems that commonly co-occur with, and precede, substance use. Yet, conduct problems are also associated with reduced emotion expression recognition ability. The current study investigated the extent of negative association between AUD and CUD symptom severity and expression recognition ability over and above any association of expression recognition ability with conduct problems [conduct disorder (CD) diagnostic status].

Methods: In this study, 152 youths aged 12.5–18 years (56 female; 60 diagnosed with CD) completed a rapid presentation morphed intensity facial expression task to investigate the association between relative severity of AUD/CUD and expression recognition ability.

Results: Cannabis use disorder identification test (CUDIT) scores were negatively associated with recognition accuracy for higher intensity (particularly sad and fearful) expressions while CD diagnostic status was independently negatively associated with recognition of sad expressions. Alcohol use disorder identification test (AUDIT) scores were not significantly associated with expression recognition ability.

Conclusions: These data indicate that relative severity of CUD and CD diagnostic status are statistically independently associated with reduced expression recognition ability. On the basis of these data, we speculate that increased cannabis use during adolescence may exacerbate a neuro-cognitive risk factor for the emergence of aggression and antisocial behavior.

Introduction

Substance use is common in the United States with lifetime prevalence rates of alcohol use disorder (AUD) and cannabis use disorder (CUD) of 29 and 6%, respectively (1, 2). Use of alcohol/cannabis during adolescence significantly increases the risk of developing AUD/CUD by adulthood (3). Alcohol use disorder and CUD are both significantly co-morbid with conduct disorder (CD) (4, 5) and associated with a significantly increased risk for aggression (69). While the associations between AUD, CUD, CD, and aggression likely at least partly reflects the adverse neurodevelopmental effects of alcohol/cannabis use on the adolescent brain (10, 11), an understanding of the neuro-cognitive underpinnings of these associations remains in its infancy.

A form of neuro-cognitive dysfunction associated with CD and aggression is impaired processing of emotional expressions [for a review of this literature, see (12)]. This manifests as both impaired expression recognition [that may be particularly marked for distress cues (13, 14)] and reduced neural responses, particularly within the amygdala, to distress cues [the sadness and fear of others; for a review of this literature, see (12)]. The suggestion is that the reduced responsiveness to the distress of others should be associated with reduced learning to avoid actions that harm other individuals because the individual finds the “punishment” of the other individual's distress less aversive (12). This results in reduced empathy for the victim, reduced guilt [i.e., the development of what have been termed callous-unemotional (CU) traits; e.g., (15)] and reduced avoidance of actions that harm other individuals [i.e., aggression (16)]. Indeed, the positive relationship between CU traits and aggression has been shown to be mediated by the reduced responsiveness of the amygdala to the distress of other individuals (17).

Impairments in expression processing have also been reported in individuals who have engaged in alcohol and cannabis use (18, 19). Behavioral studies of adults with AUD relatively consistently report impaired expression recognition relative to comparison individuals [for reviews of this literature, see (18, 20, 21)]—though there are conflicting results (22, 23). Neuro-imaging studies, again mostly conducted with adults, have shown that chronic alcohol use is associated with reduced BOLD responses to emotional expressions in prefrontal (e.g., dorsolateral and orbitofrontal cortex), limbic (amygdala, hippocampus, insula, and cingulate cortex), and other regions [e.g., temporal and parietal cortex and striatum (2427)]. Similarly, behavioral studies in adults with CUD, though less common, also report expression recognition impairment (2830). Moreover, neuro-imaging work indicates that adolescents and adults with significant cannabis use show reduced medial frontal cortical responses (25, 31) and atypical event-related potentials to emotional facial expressions (3235).

Notably, though, there are at least three gaps in the AUD/CUD literature. First, very little work has examined expression recognition in adolescents, rather than adults, with AUD/CUD. Yet, face/facial expression processing undergoes considerable neuro-development from adolescence to adulthood (36, 37). Adolescents engaging in AUD/CUD may be particularly compromised by exposure to these compounds. Second, almost no previous work has considered co-use rather than singly addressing the association of cannabis or alcohol use with expression recognition impairments [e.g., (28, 29, 38, 39)]. Co-use of alcohol and cannabis is particularly common in adolescents (40, 41). Previous association of expression recognition impairments in patients with AUD or CUD may represent high use levels of the other substance. Third, and most critically for this study, the majority of the previous work did not assess for comorbid CD (or level of CU traits) despite CD being the most common co-morbidity both in adolescents with CUD and AUD (4244) and, together with high CU traits, particularly associated with compromised expression recognition (16). As such it is possible that previous reports associating AUD/CUD with expression recognition impairment might reflect the biology underpinning antisocial behavior disorders.

In short, the goals of the current study were: (i) To determine, in a sample of adolescents, the extent to which severity of AUD/CUD was associated with the ability to accurately recognize the emotional expressions of other individuals over and above any association of CD diagnostic status or CU traits with expression recognition ability; and (ii) To determine, the extent of association between expression recognition ability, AUD/CUD severity, as indexed by scores on the Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test [AUDIT (45)] and the Cannabis Use Disorder Identification Test [CUDIT (46)], and level of CU traits, as indexed by the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits [ICU (47)], was associated with aggression. We predicted that: (i) If AUD and/or CUD is associated with compromised facial expression processing, as indicated by the previous literature [e.g., (20, 28)], then AUDIT and/or CUDIT scores would be inversely related to expression recognition accuracy even in models considering associations with co-morbid CD (and ICU scores); (ii) Expression recognition ability, AUD and CUD severity would be significantly associated with aggression.

Methods

Participants

Study participants included 158 youths aged 12.5–18 years from a residential treatment program or the surrounding community. They were recruited as part of a broader study determining neural correlates of youth with behavioral and emotional problems, specifically substance use disorders (at least 40% of the population) and mental health concerns [CD, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), major depressive disorder (MDD), and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD)]. None of these participants have been included in previous papers on this task. However, a number of the participants have been involved in previously published studies associating specific forms of neuro-cognitive dysfunction with severity of AUD/CUD (25, 4852). Six youths were excluded due to incomplete data. This resulted in a final sample of 152 youths (107 from the residential treatment program and 45 from the community); Mage = 16.54 (SD = 1.23), MIQ = 101.69 (SD = 11.65), 56 females. Participants recruited from the residential treatment program were enrolled in a highly supervised residential treatment program where they received random drug testing and did not have access to alcohol or drugs (they were abstinent from any substance for at least 4 weeks prior to testing). See Supplementary Methods for information on recruitment, consent/assent, and exclusion criteria.

Measures

Expression Recognition Task

The facial expression recognition task [Adapted From (53)] featured four basic emotions (angry, happy, sad, and fearful) from the well-validated Pictures of Facial Affect Set (54). Participants viewed static expressions of these four basic emotions. Each face had been morphed with a neutral expression from the same exemplar in 20% increments so that the expressions varied in emotional intensity from 100% (0% neutral) to 0% (100% neutral). Participants saw 240 expressions total (4 expressions 12 exemplars 5 intensity levels) presented in random order (i.e., rather than seeing each face morph from 0 to 100%). Each expression appeared for 200 ms and was followed by a response choice screen that required participants to make a forced choice among four possible responses: angry, happy, sad, and fearful. Participants' responses were self-paced. Following each response, a fixation cross (500 ms) appeared, followed by the next expression.

Substance Use Disorder Assessments

Participants completed both the AUDIT (55) and CUDIT (46) assessing symptom levels over the previous 12 months. These scales assess overall symptom severity of AUD and CUD, respectively, including overall quantity/frequency of use, abuse symptoms, and dependence symptoms. They show high validity, as higher scores on these scales are associated with a high likelihood of an AUD and/or CUD diagnosis, respectively (45, 46). Cigarette smoking status was determined via the Monitoring the Future Survey (56). Although participants were subject to random urine drug screening as part of the treatment program, they were not drug tested on the day of testing.

Aggression Assessments

Participants completed the Reactive–Proactive Aggression Questionnaire [RPQ (57)] and the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits [ICU (47)]. The RPQ indexes the participant's level of reactive and proactive aggression. The ICU indexes CU traits which are positively associated with an increased risk for reactive but particularly proactive aggression (15).

Statistical Analyses

Clinical Characteristics

Descriptive statistics were calculated for all demographic and clinical variables. Correlations were performed to examine potential associations between AUDIT and CUDIT scores and: (i) demographic variables (age, IQ); (ii) smoking [smoking scores ranged from 0 (“Never”) to 4 (“Regularly now”) based on the Monitoring the Future survey; (58)]; and (iii) aggression (RPQ scores). For sex, diagnostic status (CD, ADHD, MDD, and GAD) and medication prescriptions (stimulants, SSRIs, and anti-psychotics), significance of group differences (males vs. females, and cases vs. not cases) in AUDIT/CUDIT scores were determined by ANOVA.

Testing the Associations of AUD, CUD, CD Diagnostic Status, and CU Trait Severity With Expression Recognition Accuracy. Covariate Analysis

This was tested via a 2 (Group CD: CD diagnosis; no CD diagnosis)-by-2(Sex)-by-4 (Emotion: Angry, Happy, Sad, Fearful)-by-5 (Morph Intensity: 0, 20, 40, 60, 80, 100%) ANCOVA on participants' expression accuracy with AUDIT, CUDIT, ICU, and IQ scores and age used as continuous covariates. Given the number of clinical variables and thus constraints on power and the absence of a priori predictions with respect to interactions between the clinical variables, these were not included in the model. Given potential concerns regarding skewness/kurtosis of AUDIT/CUDIT scores, this analysis was repeated using Rankit transformed AUDIT and CUDIT scores.

Group-Based Analysis

Due to potential concerns regarding individual difference analyses given test-retest issues on many neuropsychological tasks (59, 60), the analyses were repeated using a group-based approach. Participants were grouped according to whether they met, or did not meet, clinical cutoffs on the AUDIT and/or CUDIT suggestive of adolescent AUD (AUDIT ≥ 4) or CUD [CUDIT ≥ 8 (45, 61)]. As such, the test involved a 2 (Group CD: CD diagnosis; no CD diagnosis)-by-2 (Group AUDIT: AUDIT ≥ 4; AUDIT < 4)-by-2(Group CUDIT: CUDIT ≥ 8; CUDIT < 8) ANOVA. Due to probable small cell sizes for some interactions, no inter-group interactions were included in this model.

Testing the Association of Expression Recognition, AUD, CUD, and CU Trait Severity With Aggression

This was tested via a univariate ANCOVA on aggression level as indexed by the RPQ total score with accuracy scores for sad, fearful, happy and angry expressions, AUDIT, CUDIT, and ICU scores as covariates.

Potential Confounds: Psychiatric Comorbidities

Given potential relationships between AUDIT/CUDIT scores and diagnostic status for other common psychiatric conditions within this sample (ADHD, MDD, GAD), our main ANCOVA was repeated three times with the addition of a group variable corresponding to diagnostic status for these conditions.

Medication Status

Given potential relationships between AUDIT/CUDIT scores and medication status within this sample (stimulants, SSRIs, or anti-psychotic medications), our main ANCOVA was repeated three times with the addition of a group variable corresponding to the prescribed use (or not) of these medications.

All analyses were conducted within SPSS 26.0.

Results

Clinical Characteristics

Of the 152 participants, 91 endorsed past-year use of either alcohol and/or cannabis. AUDIT scores ranged from 0 to 22 (M = 2.61, SD = 4.62) and CUDIT scores from 0 to 32 (M = 6.74, SD = 8.68). Sixty met the clinical cutoffs on the AUDIT and/or CUDIT suggestive of adolescent AUD (AUDIT ≥ 4) or CUD [CUDIT ≥ 8 (45, 61)]. Fifty-five had a CUDIT score ≥8 and 34 participants had an AUDIT score ≥ 4. Sixty-one participants had a CUDIT score = 0 and an AUDIT score = 0. AUDIT, but not CUDIT scores, showed significant skewness and kurtosis (respectively, 2.55 and 6.65 for AUDIT and 1.05 and −0.074 for CUDIT). Rankit transformation of AUDIT and CUDIT scores brought levels of skewness and kurtosis to acceptable levels (respectively, 0.86 and −0.25 for AUDIT and 0.80 and −0.39 for CUDIT).

Correlation analyses revealed a strong positive relationship between AUDIT and CUDIT scores (r = 0.597, p < 0.001—though variance inflation factors were <1.5; 1.07 and 1.10 for AUDIT and CUDIT, respectively). There were also significant correlations for AUDIT and CUDIT scores and tobacco smoking, IQ, and RPQ scores; see Table 1. Males and females significantly differed in AUDIT scores (females > males). All groups of cases corresponding to psychiatric diagnostic status differed in CUDIT scores (cases > non-cases; albeit p = 0.06 for GAD). All groups of cases corresponding to psychiatric diagnostic status differed in AUDIT scores (cases > non-cases; albeit p = 0.07 for ADHD). Groups differing according to SSRI medication prescription status significantly differed in CUDIT (p = 0.01) and AUDIT scores (p = 0.03). Groups differing according to antipsychotic medication prescription status significantly differed in CUDIT scores (p = 0.02) (for full details, see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical variables.

Testing the Associations of AUD, CUD, and CU Trait Severity and CD Diagnostic Status With Expression Recognition Accuracy

With respect to the primary aims of the current study, both the Morph Intensity-by-CUDIT [F(4, 544) = 2.72, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.020] and Morph Intensity-by-Emotion-by-CUDIT interactions were significant [F(12, 1, 632) = 2.60, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.019]. Increasing CUDIT scores were significantly negatively associated with recognition of higher intensity (particularly sad and fearful expression) morphs: rSad:20% = −0.19, rSad:40% = −0.27, rSad:80% = −0.21, rFearful:80% = −0.19, and rFearful:100% = −0.18. Increasing CUDIT scores were significantly positively associated with recognition of lower intensity angry morphs (rAngry:20% = 0.20). AUDIT scores showed no significant interactions with Morph Intensity [F(4, 580) = 1.54, p = 0.19, η2 = 0.011], Emotion [F(3, 435) = 1.26, p = 0.29, η2 = 0.009], or Morph Intensity-by-Emotion [F(12, 1, 740) = 0.70, p = 0.754, η2 = 0.005].

There was also a significant Expression-by-CD interaction [F(3, 408) = 3.18, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.023]; participants with CD were significantly less accurate than those without CD for sad expressions [t(149) = 2.29, p = 0.02] but not angry, happy, or fearful expressions [t(149) = −1.39 to 1.22, p = 0.167–0.227]. In addition, there was a significant Morph Intensity-by-ICU interaction [F(4, 544) = 2.39, p = 0.05, η2 = 0.017]; while there was no significant association between expression recognition and ICU score for the 20 and 40% morphs (r = −0.077 and −0.029, respectively), there was a significant association between expression recognition and ICU score for the 60, 80, and 100% morphs (r = −0.19, −0.21, and −0.25, respectively).

Additional significant findings included main effects of both IQ [F(1, 136) = 8.59, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.059] and sex [F(1, 136) = 5.63, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.040]. IQ was positively associated with expression accuracy (r = 0.26) while females were more accurate than males (Mfemale = 7.19); Mmale = 6.67). There was also a significant Morph Intensity-by-Sex interaction [F(4, 544) = 3.62, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.026]; males were less accurate than females for all morph intensities except 20%.

The results of the repetition of this analysis, involving rankit transformed CUDIT and AUDIT scores, mirrored the results of the analysis reported above (see Supplementary Material).

Group-Based Analysis

The results of the group-based ANOVA largely confirmed those of the ANCOVA outlined above (for details on the demographics of these groups see Supplementary Table 1). There was a significant CUDIT Group-by-Intensity interaction [F(4, 588) = 2.85, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.019] though the CUDIT Group-by-Morph Intensity-by-Emotion-interaction was not significant [F(12, 1764) = 1.54, p = 0.10, η2 = 0.010]. The Expression-by-CD interaction was also significant [F(3, 441) = 3.86, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.026]. There were no significant main effects or interactions with AUDIT Group.

Testing the Association of Expression Recognition, AUD, CUD, and CU Trait Severity With Aggression

Pearson correlation analyses revealed significant positive associations between aggression as indexed by the RPQ and both AUDIT and CUDIT scores (see Table 1) as well as ICU score (r = 0.53; p < 0.001) and sadness expression recognition (r = −0.17; p = 0.041) [though not with recognition of the other expressions; r = 0.001, 0.016, and −0.091; p = 0.99, 0.085, and 0.276 for angry, happy, and fearful expressions, respectively]. However, our univariate ANCOVA revealed only highly significant associations between ICU and RPQ scores [F(1, 128) = 38.67, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.232] and CUDIT and RPQ scores [F(1, 128) = 5.02, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.038]. There was also an ICU-by-CUDIT score interaction scores [F(1, 128) = 5.02, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.038]. AUDIT was not associated with RPQ scores in our ANCOVA [F(1, 128) = 0.69, p = 0.41, η2 = 0.005] nor was recognition for any of the expressions [F(1, 128) = 0.005–0.787, p = 0.38–0.94, η2 = 0.0–0.006). ICU-by-CUDIT interaction effects were observed, whereby CUDIT scores were positively associated with aggression in individuals whose ICU scores were <29 (r = 0.484, p < 0.001) but unrelated to aggression in individuals whose ICU scores were 28 or greater (r = 0.074, p = 0.686). Similarly, ICU scores were positively associated with aggression in individuals whose CUDIT scores were <8 (r = 0.592, p < 0.001) but significantly less (Steiger's z = 3.47, p = 0.0005) associated with aggression in individuals whose CUDIT scores were eight or greater (r = 0.315, p = 0.023).

Potential Confounds: Psychiatric Comorbidities

Our additional ANCOVAs including the addition of a group variable corresponding to diagnostic status ADHD, MDD, GAD, SAD, and PTSD largely replicated our main analysis. In almost all cases, the Morph Intensity-by-CUDIT, Morph Intensity-by-Emotion-by-CUDIT, Expression-by-CD interaction, and Morph Intensity-by-ICU interactions were significant (the exception was the ANCOVA) including ADHD diagnostic status where the Morph Intensity-by-CUDIT and Morph Intensity-by-ICU interactions only reached trend level significance (p = 0.057 and 0.068, respectively; for full results, see Supplementary Table 2).

Medication Status

Our additional ANCOVAs including the addition of group variables corresponding to the prescribed use (or not) of anti-psychotic, SSRI, or stimulant medications largely replicated our main analysis. In almost all cases, the Morph Intensity-by-CUDIT, Morph Intensity-by-Emotion-by-CUDIT, Expression-by-CD interaction, and Morph Intensity-by-ICU interactions were significant (the exception was the ANCOVA) including SSRI medication status where the Morph Intensity-by-ICU interactions only reached trend level significance (p = 0.067; for full results, see Supplementary Table 2).

Additional Substance Use

Our additional ANCOVAs including the addition of group variables corresponding to the use (or not) of cocaine/crack, methamphetamine, or inhalants largely replicated our main analysis. In almost all cases, the Morph Intensity-by-CUDIT, Morph Intensity-by-Emotion-by-CUDIT, Expression-by-CD interaction, and Morph Intensity-by-ICU interactions were significant (the exception was the ANCOVA) including cocaine/crack use where the Expression-by-CD interaction only reached trend level significance (p = 0.075; for full results, see Supplementary Table 2).

Discussion

The current study aimed to determine the extent to which severity of AUD/CUD was associated with emotion expressions recognition ability and aggression. This study revealed that: (i) CUDIT (but not AUDIT) scores were associated with reduced expression recognition ability, particularly for sad and fearful facial expressions, over and above reduced expression recognition ability associated with both CD diagnostic status and CU traits and (ii) CUDIT and ICU scores were particularly associated with aggression in this sample.

Reduced expression recognition ability, particularly the ability to recognize distress cues, has long been associated with increased aggression [e.g., (62)] and the presence of CU traits [for a review of this literature, see (12)]. The suggestion is that the reduced response to the distress of others is associated with reduced avoidance of actions that harm other individuals, reduced guilt, and reduced empathy for potential victims (12). The current study, consistent with considerable previous work [for a review, see (12)], revealed that both CD diagnostic status and higher levels of CU traits were associated with reduced expression recognition ability. Within this study, CD diagnostic status was particularly associated with reduced sadness expression ability. This is consistent with fMRI work indicating particularly compromised neural responsiveness to sad facial expressions (63). Level of CU traits was associated with generally reduced expression recognition ability across facial expressions, particularly for “easier” (higher intensity) morphs. While some data have associated CU traits with reduced responsiveness particularly to distress cues [see for a review, (12)], other data has indicated that closely related psychopathic traits are associated with more general reduced expression recognition ability [for a meta-analysis of the literature, see (13)].

The main goal of this study was to determine the extent to which severity of AUD/CUD was associated with emotion expression recognition ability. In particular, our primary aim was to address three gaps in the existing literature on AUD/CUD and expression recognition ability, specifically (i) the relative absence of work examining expression recognition in adolescents, rather than adults, with AUD/CUD; (ii) the relative specificity vs. generality of expression recognition alterations in adolescents in relation to AUD compared to CUD severity; and (iii) the extent to which any associations between AUD/CUD and expression recognition ability existed when potential confounds of CD diagnostic status and level of CU traits were accounted for. As such, the current study revealed that the association between AUD/CUD and reduced expression recognition ability reported in adults [e.g., (18, 19)] is also observed in adolescents. Moreover, it suggested that the association was particularly strong for CUD, relative to AUD, severity, and that this association is present even when potential confounds associated with comorbid CD and co-existing levels of CU traits are taken into account.

We did not specifically predict that there would be a stronger association between CUDIT, relative to AUDIT, scores, and expression recognition ability. Previous work with adults with alcohol use difficulties has relatively consistently associated AUD with reduced expression recognition ability [for reviews of this literature, see (18, 20, 21)]—though there are conflicting results (22, 23). Moreover previous neuro-imaging work, mostly conducted with adults, has shown chronic alcohol use is associated with reduced BOLD responses to emotional expressions in cortical and subcortical regions previously shown to be responsive to emotional expressions (2427). It is possible that any association between AUD severity and reduced expression recognition ability is notably weaker in adolescents and that only the prolonged, severe exposure seen in adults with AUD-related concerns is associated with reduced expression recognition ability. However, it is also possible that the effects for CUDIT scores but not AUDIT scores reflected a statistical artifact driven by the greater variance in CUDIT scores in this sample (sdCUDIT = 8.68; sdAUDIT = 4.62)—or the restricted variance in the AUDIT scores may have limited the capacity to identify AUDIT-expression recognition associations [though note that this has not been the case in other work investigating other functions in an overlapping sample (48, 51, 64)]. While the results were similar in the group-based ANOVA, there were rather more participants meeting the CUDIT cut-off (N = 55) than the AUDIT cut-offs (N = 34) and very few participants only meeting the AUDIT but not CUDIT cut-off (N = 5). Moreover, and in line with previous work with adolecents (65, 66), co-use of alcohol and cannabis was very common in this sample and the correlation of AUDIT and CUDIT scores was highly significant (even if the variance inflation factors were not high enough to indicate collinearity was a significant concern). As such, it is probably important to be cautious about any definitive conclusions regarding AUD severity and expression recognition ability in adolescents. However, irrespective of conclusions with respect to AUD severity in adolescents, the current results strongly indicate that CUD severity, as indexed by CUDIT score, is associated with reduced expression recognition ability, particularly for distress cues (sad and fearful expressions), and that this reduced ability cannot be accounted for by co-morbid CD diagnostic status or severity of CU traits.

Developmentally, early conduct problems are one of the main predictors of later emergence of substance use disorders (6769). Reduced response control and atypical reinforcement-based decision-making, seen in individuals with conduct problems [e.g., (70, 71)], have been identified as risk factors for the emergence of substance use disorders (7274). CU traits, which are particularly associated with reduced expression recognition ability [see for a review (12)], have also been identified as a risk factor for the emergence of substance use disorders (75). The current data indicate that reduced expression recognition may also result from substance (cannabis) use. This is consistent with fMRI data indicating disrupted face processing and reduced emotional responding as a function of CUDIT score in adolescents (25, 52, 76). As such, premorbid/pre-drug exposure neurobehavioral phenotypes associated with CD that convey risk for the development of substance use disorders may potentially be exacerbated by cannabis or alcohol exposure during adolescence [given other data indicating that substance negatively impacts the function of regions implicated in response control and reinforcement-based decision-making in adolescents (49, 51)]. In short, substance use may exacerbate adolescent CD. This suggests that particular care may be necessary when treating adolescents presenting with both CD and substance use.

Notably, our ANCOVA analysis on the RPQ data revealed that history of aggression (RPQ score) was associated with ICU and CUDIT scores (though not AUDIT scores). CU traits have long been associated with an increased risk for aggression (77, 78) as has substance use (43, 44). Again, it is necessary to be cautious regarding the contributions of CUD severity relative to AUD severity (given the greater variance in CUDIT scores in this sample; see above). Indeed, in other work we have seen a relationship between AUD severity and disruption in neural systems involved in reactive aggression/retaliation (79). Interestingly, our current findings showed that CU traits and CUD severity have statisically seperable associations with aggression. Furthermore, they identified an interaction in the association of ICU and CUDIT scores with aggression, with ICU scores being robustly correlated with aggression among youth below the clinical cut-off CUDIT score and CUDIT scores being robustly correlated with aggression among youth with levels of self-reported ICU below the cut-off (Frick, personal communication). These data suggest relatively independent pathways to aggressive responding occurring through CU traits and separately through youth cannabis use that become difficult to untangle in individuals who are clinically more severe in either their CU traits or CUD symptom severity. Unfortunately, the absence of a longitudinal design means that we cannot disentangle the chronology of these associations [see above (6)].

The results of this study should be viewed in light of several limitations. First, we did not conduct urine or breathalyzer testing for alcohol or cannabis use on the day of testing. However, this concern is mitigated by the fact that all the participants with significant substance use history were residents of a highly supervised residential treatment facility and subject to random drug testing as part of treatment for at least 4 weeks prior to testing. Second, this study was cross-sectional. As such, the associations reported in the present study might reflect neurotoxic/neuroplastic effects of cannabis use on the developing brain and/or pre-existing risk factors for CUD. Third, there was a high degree of psychiatric co-morbidity in the residential treatment sample and thus the current findings might reflect the psychiatric co-morbidities. Some previous work has excluded participants with psychiatric conditions [e.g., (8082)]. The problem with this approach is that approximately 80% of adolescents with a SUD present with one or more co-morbid psychiatric conditions (83, 84). Indeed, AUD and CUD are associated with a number of co-morbid psychiatric conditions (85, 86). As such, studies that exclude youth with psychiatric comorbidities are clinically atypical and may not generalize. Moreover, controlling for ADHD, MDD, and/or GAD diagnostic status in our analyses did not significantly alter our main study findings (see Supplementary Table 2). As such, it is unlikely current findings might reflect the psychiatric co-morbidities. Fourth, a number of the study participants were medicated and as such the current findings might reflect this medication status rather than CUD. However, controlling for anti-psychotic, SSRI, or stimulant medication status in our analyses did not significantly alter our main study findings (see Supplementary Table 2). As such, it is unlikely current findings reflect medication usage. Fifth, the AUDIT scores within this sample showed concerning levels of skewness and kurtosis potentially affecting the interpretation of the main ANCOVA analysis. Importantly though, the results of both the dimensional analysis using transformed AUDIT and CUDIT scores and the group-based analysis mirrored the results of our main ANCOVA analysis indicating that the current results are not an artifact of skewness within the AUDIT scores.

In summary, in an adolescent sample with variable levels of psychopathology and substance use, CUDIT scores, CD diagnostic status, and ICU scores were all associated with expression recognition accuracy. Higher CUDIT scores were associated with relatively weaker recognition accuracy for higher intensity (particularly sad and fearful) morphs. Higher ICU scores were also associated with relatively weaker recognition accuracy for higher intensity morphs. CD diagnostic status was associated with poorer recognition of sad expressions. CUDIT scores and ICU scores were also particularly associated with aggression (though the statistical model examining simultaneously associations with aggression, indicated only CUDIT and ICU scores and not expression recognition ability independently of these). The current data are consistent with previous data indicating that CD diagnostic status and ICU scores are associated with reduced expression recognition ability. This reduced ability is thought to underpin the empathy impairments associated with these conditions and increase the risk for aggression. The current data indicate that CUD severity, as indexed by the CUDIT, is also associated with reduced expression recognition ability. As such increased cannabis use during adolescence may exacerbate a neuro-cognitive risk factor for the emergence of aggression and antisocial behavior.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Boys Town Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to writing the manuscript. JB-L and MD were particularly involved in clinical care of the participants. AS and JE were particularly involved in data collection.

Funding

This work was supported by Boys Town National Research Hospital. RB was supported by the National Institutes of Health (P20GM130461) and the Rural Drug Addiction Research Center at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln. CH was supported by K12-DA000357 and R34-DA050292. The funders had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, decision to publish, or manuscript preparation.

Conflict of Interest

CH receives grant support from the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry [AACAP Physician Scientist Career Development Award, K12DA000357], the National Network of Depression Centers, and the Armstrong Institute at Johns Hopkins Bayview and serves as a scientific advisor for the National Courts and Science Institute and as a subject matter expert for the Substance Abuse Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA).

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all participants and their families for their participation.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.714189/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Saha TD, Chou SP, Jung J, Zhang H, et al. Epidemiology of DSM-5 alcohol use disorder: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions III. JAMA Psychiatry. (2015) 72:757–66. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Hasin DS, Kerridge BT, Saha TD, Huang B, Pickering R, Smith SM, et al. Prevalence and correlates of DSM-5 cannabis use disorder, 2012-2013: findings from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions-III. Am J Psychiatry. (2016) 173:588–99. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15070907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Winters KC, Lee CYS. Likelihood of developing an alcohol and cannabis use disorder during youth: association with recent use and age. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2008) 92:239–47. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Mueser KT, Rosenberg SD, Drake RE, Miles KM, Wolford G, Vidaver R, et al. Conduct disorder, antisocial personality disorder and substance use disorders in schizophrenia and major affective disorders. J Stud Alcohol. (1999) 60:278–84. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Schubiner H, Tzelepis A, Milberger S, Lockhart N, Kruger M, Kelley BJ, et al. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder among substance abusers. J Clin Psychiatry. (2000) 61:244–51. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v61n0402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Blair RJR. Modeling the comorbidity of cannabis abuse and conduct disorder/conduct problems from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. J Dual Diagn. (2020) 16:3–21. doi: 10.1080/15504263.2019.1668099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Hammond CJ, Chaney A, Hendrickson B, Sharma P. Cannabis use among U.S. adolescents in the era of marijuana legalization: a review of changing use patterns, comorbidity, and health correlates. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2020) 32:221–34. doi: 10.1080/09540261.2020.1713056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Kolla NJ, Mishra A. The endocannabinoid system, aggression, and the violence of synthetic cannabinoid use, borderline personality disorder, antisocial personality disorder, and other psychiatric disorders. Front Behav Neurosci. (2018) 12:41. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Puhalla AA, Berman ME, Coccaro EF, Fahlgren MK, McCloskey MS. History of childhood abuse and alcohol use disorder: relationship with intermittent explosive disorder and intoxicated aggression frequency. J Psychiatr Res. (2020) 125:38–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Filbey FM, McQueeny T, DeWitt SJ, Mishra V. Preliminary findings demonstrating latent effects of early adolescent marijuana use onset on cortical architecture. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2015) 16:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Squeglia LM, Tapert SF, Sullivan EV, Jacobus J, Meloy MJ, Rohlfing T, et al. Brain development in heavy-drinking adolescents. Am J Psychiatry. (2015) 172:531–42. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14101249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Blair RJR. Traits of empathy and anger: implications for psychopathy and other disorders associated with aggression. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2018) 373:20170155. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Dawel A, O'Kearney R, McKone E, Palermo R. Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2012) 36:2288–304. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Marsh AA, Blair RJ. Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2008) 32:454–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Annual research review: a developmental psychopathology approach to understanding callous-unemotional traits in children and adolescents with serious conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2014) 55:532–48. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Blair RJR. Facial expressions, their communicatory functions and neuro-cognitive substrates. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2003) 358:561–72. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lozier LM, Cardinale EM, VanMeter JW, Marsh AA. Mediation of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression by amygdala response to fear among children with conduct problems. JAMA Psychiatry. (2014) 71:627–36. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Castellano F, Bartoli F, Crocamo C, Gamba G, Tremolada M, Santambrogio J, et al. Facial emotion recognition in alcohol and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 59:147–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Miller MA, Bershad AK, de Wit H. Drug effects on responses to emotional facial expressions: recent findings. Behav Pharmacol. (2015) 26:571–9. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bora E, Zorlu N. Social cognition in alcohol use disorder: a meta-analysis. Addiction. (2017) 112:40–8. doi: 10.1111/add.13486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Donadon MF, Osorio Fde L. Recognition of facial expressions by alcoholic patients: a systematic literature review. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2014) 10:1655–63. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S65376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Dethier M, El Hawa M, Duchateau R, Blairy S. Emotional facial expression recognition and expressivity in type I and type II alcohol dependent patients. J Nonverbal Behav. (2014) 38:89–105. doi: 10.1007/s10919-013-0161-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Kornreich C, Petit G, Rolin H, Ermer E, Campanella S, Verbanck P, et al. Decoding of nonverbal language in alcoholism: a perception or a labeling problem? Psychol Addict Behav. (2016) 30:175–83. doi: 10.1037/adb0000147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Alba-Ferrara L, Muller-Oehring EM, Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A, Schulte T. Brain responses to emotional salience and reward in alcohol use disorder. Brain Imaging Behav. (2016) 10:136–46. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9374-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Leiker EK, Meffert H, Thornton LC, Taylor BK, Aloi J, Abdel-Rahim H, et al. Alcohol use disorder and cannabis use disorder symptomatology in adolescents are differentially related to dysfunction in brain regions supporting face processing. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2019) 292:62–71. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2019.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Marinkovic K, Oscar-Berman M, Urban T, O'Reilly CE, Howard JA, Sawyer K. Alcoholism and dampened temporal limbic activation to emotional faces. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2009) 33:1880–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01026.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. O'Daly OG, Trick L, Scaife J, Marshall J, Ball D, Phillips ML, et al. Withdrawal-associated increases and decreases in functional neural connectivity associated with altered emotional regulation in alcoholism. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2012) 37:2267–76. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Bayrakci A, Sert E, Zorlu N, Erol A, Saricicek A, Mete L. Facial emotion recognition deficits in abstinent cannabis dependent patients. Compr Psychiatry. (2015) 58:160–4. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Hindocha C, Wollenberg O, Carter Leno V, Alvarez BO, Curran HV, Freeman TP. Emotional processing deficits in chronic cannabis use: a replication and extension. J Psychopharmacol. (2014) 28:466–71. doi: 10.1177/0269881114527359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Platt B, Kamboj S, Morgan CJ, Curran HV. Processing dynamic facial affect in frequent cannabis-users: evidence of deficits in the speed of identifying emotional expressions. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2010) 112:27–32. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gruber SA, Rogowska J, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Altered affective response in marijuana smokers: an fMRI study. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2009) 105:139–53. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Torrence RD, Rojas DC, Troup LJ. Residual effects of cannabis use on attentional bias towards fearful faces. Neuropsychologia. (2018) 119:482–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Torrence RD, Rojas DC, Troup LJ. Awareness of emotional expressions in cannabis users: an event-related potential study. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:69. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Troup LJ, Andrzejewski JA, Torrence RD. The effects of sex and residual cannabis use on emotion processing: an event-related potential study. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2019) 27:318–25. doi: 10.1037/pha0000265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Troup LJ, Torrence RD, Andrzejewski JA, Braunwalder JT. Effects of cannabis use and subclinical depression on the P3 event-related potential in an emotion processing task. Medicine (Baltimore). (2017) 96:e6385. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000006385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Blair KS, Geraci M, Korelitz K, Otero M, Towbin K, Ernst M, et al. The pathology of social phobia is independent of developmental changes in face processing. Am J Psychiatry. (2011) 168:1202–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Wu M, Kujawa A, Lu LH, Fitzgerald DA, Klumpp H, Fitzgerald KD, et al. Age-related changes in amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotional face processing from childhood into young adulthood. Hum Brain Mapp. (2016) 37:1684–95. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Foisy ML, Kornreich C, Fobe A, D'Hondt L, Pelc I, Hanak C, et al. Impaired emotional facial expression recognition in alcohol dependence: do these deficits persist with midterm abstinence? Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2007) 31:404–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00321.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Rupp CI, Derntl B, Osthaus F, Kemmler G, Fleischhacker WW. Impact of social cognition on alcohol dependence treatment outcome: poorer facial emotion recognition predicts relapse/dropout. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2017) 41:2197–206. doi: 10.1111/acer.13522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Metrik J, Gunn RL, Jackson KM, Sokolovsky AW, Borsari B. Daily patterns of marijuana and alcohol co-use among individuals with alcohol and cannabis use disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2018) 42:1096–104. doi: 10.1111/acer.13639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Schlienz NJ, Lee DC. Co-use of cannabis, tobacco, and alcohol during adolescence: policy and regulatory implications. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2018) 30:226–37. doi: 10.1080/09540261.2018.1465399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Armstrong TD, Costello EJ. Community studies on adolescent substance use, abuse, or dependence and psychiatric comorbidity. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2002) 70:1224–39. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.70.6.1224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM. Conduct and attentional problems in childhood and adolescence and later substance use, abuse and dependence: results of a 25-year longitudinal study. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2007) 88 Suppl 1:S14–26. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Oshri A, Rogosch FA, Burnette ML, Cicchetti D. Developmental pathways to adolescent cannabis abuse and dependence: child maltreatment, emerging personality, and internalizing versus externalizing psychopathology. Psychol Addict Behav. (2011) 25:634–44. doi: 10.1037/a0023151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fairlie AM, Sindelar HA, Eaton CA, Spirito A. Utility of the AUDIT for screening adolescents for problematic alcohol use in the emergency department. Int J Adolesc Med Health. (2006) 18:115–22. doi: 10.1515/IJAMH.2006.18.1.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Adamson SJ, Kay-Lambkin FJ, Baker AL, Lewin TJ, Thornton L, Kelly BJ, et al. An improved brief measure of cannabis misuse: the Cannabis Use Disorders Identification Test-Revised (CUDIT-R). Drug Alcohol Depend. (2010) 110:137–43. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Frick PJ. Inventory of Callous–Unemotional Traits. University of New Orleans (2004). doi: 10.1037/t62639-000

CrossRef Full Text

48. Aloi J, Blair KS, Crum KI, Bashford-Largo J, Zhang R, Lukoff J, et al. Alcohol use disorder, but not cannabis use disorder, symptomatology in adolescents is associated with reduced differential responsiveness to reward versus punishment feedback during instrumental learning. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2020) 5:610–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Aloi J, Blair KS, Crum KI, Meffert H, White SF, Tyler PM, et al. Adolescents show differential dysfunctions related to alcohol and cannabis use disorder severity in emotion and executive attention neuro-circuitries. NeuroImage Clin. (2018) 19:782–92. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Aloi J, Blair KS, Meffert H, White SF, Hwang S, Tyler PM, et al. Alcohol use disorder and cannabis use disorder symptomatology in adolescents is associated with dysfunction in neural processing of future events. Addict Biol. (2021) 26:e12885. doi: 10.1111/adb.12885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Aloi J, Meffert H, White SF, Blair KS, Hwang S, Tyler PM, et al. Differential dysfunctions related to alcohol and cannabis use disorder symptoms in reward and error-processing neuro-circuitries in adolescents. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2019) 36:100618. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Blair RJR, White SF, Tyler PM, Johnson K, Lukoff J, Thornton LC, et al. Threat responsiveness as a function of cannabis and alcohol use disorder severity. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. (2019) 29:526–34. doi: 10.1089/cap.2019.0004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Marsh AA, Yu H, Pine DS, Blair RJR. Oxytocin improves specific recognition of positive facial expressions. Psychopharmacology. (2010) 209:225–32. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1780-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Pictures of Facial Affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1976).

Google Scholar

55. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction. (1993) 88:791–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Miech RA, Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975-2014: Vol. I, Secondary School Students. Ann Arbor: Institute of Social Research (2015).

Google Scholar

57. Raine A, Dodge K, Loeber R, Gatzke-Kopp L, Lynam D, Reynolds C, et al. The reactive-proactive aggression questionnaire: differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggress Behav. (2006) 32:159–71. doi: 10.1002/ab.20115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Miech RA, Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975-2014: Vol. I, Secondary School Students. Ann Arbor: Institute of Social Research (2015).

Google Scholar

59. Chen G, Pine DS, Brotman MA, Smith AR, Cox RW, Haller SP. Beyond the intraclass correlation: a hierarchical modeling approach to test-retest assessment. bioRxiv.2021.01.04.425305 (2021). doi: 10.1101/2021.01.04.425305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Hedge C, Powell G, Sumner P. The reliability paradox: why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behav Res Methods. (2017) 50:1166–86. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Schultz NR, Bassett DT, Messina BG, Correia CJ. Evaluation of the psychometric properties of the cannabis use disorders identification test - revised among college students. Addict Behav. (2019) 95:11–5. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Blair RJR, Coles M. Expression recognition and behavioural problems in early adolescence. Cogn Dev. (2000) 15:421–34. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00039-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Passamonti L, Fairchild G, Goodyer IM, Hurford G, Hagan CC, Rowe JB, et al. Neural abnormalities in early-onset and adolescence-onset conduct disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2010) 67:729–38. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Aloi J, Crum KI, Blair KS, Zhang R, Bashford-Largo J, Bajaj S, et al. Individual associations of adolescent alcohol use disorder versus cannabis use disorder symptoms in neural prediction error signaling and the response to novelty. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2021) 48:100944. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Merrin GJ, Thompson K, Leadbeater BJ. Transitions in the use of multiple substances from adolescence to young adulthood. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2018) 189:147–53. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Patrick ME, Terry-McElrath YM, Lee CM, Schulenberg JE. Simultaneous alcohol and marijuana use among underage young adults in the United States. Addict Behav. (2019) 88:77–81. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Defoe IN, Khurana A, Betancourt LM, Hurt H, Romer D. Disentangling longitudinal relations between youth cannabis use, peer cannabis use, and conduct problems: developmental cascading links to cannabis use disorder. Addiction. (2018) 114:458–93. doi: 10.1111/add.14456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Hawes SW, Trucco EM, Duperrouzel JC, Coxe S, Gonzalez R. Developmental pathways of adolescent cannabis use: risk factors, outcomes and sex-specific differences. Subst Use Misuse. (2018) 54:271–81. doi: 10.1080/10826084.2018.1517177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Merikangas KR, He JP, Burstein M, Swanson SA, Avenevoli S, Cui L, et al. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2010) 49:980–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Hwang S, Nolan ZT, White SF, Williams WC, Sinclair S, Blair RJ. Dual neurocircuitry dysfunctions in disruptive behavior disorders: emotional responding and response inhibition. Psychol Med. (2016) 46:1485–96. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716000118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. White SF, Pope K, Sinclair S, Fowler KA, Brislin SJ, Williams WC, et al. Disrupted expected value and prediction error signaling in youths with disruptive behavior disorders during a passive avoidance task. Am J Psychiatry. (2013) 170:315–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12060840

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Buchel C, Peters J, Banaschewski T, Bokde AL, Bromberg U, Conrod PJ, et al. Blunted ventral striatal responses to anticipated rewards foreshadow problematic drug use in novelty-seeking adolescents. Nat Commun. (2017) 8:14140. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Norman AL, Pulido C, Squeglia LM, Spadoni AD, Paulus MP, Tapert SF. Neural activation during inhibition predicts initiation of substance use in adolescence. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2011) 119:216–23. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Stice E, Yokum S, Burger KS. Elevated reward region responsivity predicts future substance use onset but not overweight/obesity onset. Biol Psychiatry. (2013) 73:869–76. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Massey SH, Newmark RL, Wakschlag LS. Explicating the role of empathic processes in substance use disorders: a conceptual framework and research agenda. Drug Alcohol Rev. (2018) 37:316–32. doi: 10.1111/dar.12548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Ketcherside A, Filbey FM. Mediating processes between stress and problematic marijuana use. Addict Behav. (2015) 45:113–8. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Kimonis ER, Fanti K, Goldweber A, Marsee MA, Frick PJ, Cauffman E. Callous-unemotional traits in incarcerated adolescents. Psychol Assess. (2014) 26:227–37. doi: 10.1037/a0034585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Marsee MA, Frick PJ, Barry CT, Kimonis ER, Munoz LC, Aucoin Centifanti KJ. Profiles of the forms and functions of self-reported aggression in three adolescent samples. Dev Psychopathol. (2014) 26:705–20. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Blair RJR, Bajaj S, Sherer N, Bashford-Largo J, Zhang R, Aloi J, et al. Alcohol use disorder and cannabis use disorder symptomatology in adolescents and aggression: associations with recruitment of neural regions implicated in retaliation. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2020) 6:536–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Beck A, Schlagenhauf F, Wustenberg T, Hein J, Kienast T, Kahnt T, et al. Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 66:734–42. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Enzi B, Lissek S, Edel MA, Tegenthoff M, Nicolas V, Scherbaum N, et al. Alterations of monetary reward and punishment processing in chronic cannabis users: an FMRI study. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0119150. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Nees F, Witt SH, Dinu-Biringer R, Lourdusamy A, Tzschoppe J, Vollstadt-Klein S, et al. BDNF Val66Met and reward-related brain function in adolescents: role for early alcohol consumption. Alcohol. (2015) 49:103–10. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2014.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Chan YF, Dennis ML, Funk RR. Prevalence and comorbidity of major internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents and adults presenting to substance abuse treatment. J Subst Abuse Treat. (2008) 34:14–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2006.12.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. SAMHSA. Key Substance Use and Mental Health Indicators in the US: Results From the 2016 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (2017). p. 1–101.

85. Moss HB, Lynch KG. Comorbid disruptive behavior disorder symptoms and their relationship to adolescent alcohol use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2001) 64:75–83. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(00)00233-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Volkow ND, Koob GF, McLellan AT. Neurobiologic advances from the brain disease model of addiction. N Engl J Med. (2016) 374:363–71. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1511480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: expression recognition, conduct disorder, callous-unemotional traits, cannabis use disorder, alcohol use disorder

Citation: Blair RJR, Bashford-Largo J, Zhang R, Mathur A, Schwartz A, Elowsky J, Tyler P, Hammond CJ, Filbey FM, Dobbertin M, Bajaj S and Blair KS (2021) Alcohol and Cannabis Use Disorder Symptom Severity, Conduct Disorder, and Callous-Unemotional Traits and Impairment in Expression Recognition. Front. Psychiatry 12:714189. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.714189

Received: 25 May 2021; Accepted: 18 August 2021;
Published: 20 September 2021.

Edited by:

Anita Cservenka, Oregon State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Liangsuo Ma, Virginia Commonwealth University, United States
Lucia M. Alba Ferrara, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Argentina

Copyright © 2021 Blair, Bashford-Largo, Zhang, Mathur, Schwartz, Elowsky, Tyler, Hammond, Filbey, Dobbertin, Bajaj and Blair. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Robert James R. Blair, james.blair@boystown.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.