Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 05 July 2021
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic From Childbearing to Childrearing: Parental Mental Health and Infant Development View all 16 articles

Anxious Attachment Mediates the Associations Between Early Recollections of Mother's Own Parental Bonding and Mother–Infant Bonding: A 2-Month Path Analysis Model

\nMaor Kalfon HakhmigariMaor Kalfon Hakhmigari1Yoav Peled,Yoav Peled2,3Haim Krissi,Haim Krissi2,3Sigal LevySigal Levy4Maayan Molmen-LichterMaayan Molmen-Lichter1Jonathan E. Handelzalts,
Jonathan E. Handelzalts1,5*
  • 1School of Behavioral Sciences, The Academic College of Tel Aviv-Yafo, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • 2The Helen Schneider Hospital for Women, Rabin Medical Center, Petah Tikva, Israel
  • 3Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • 4Statistics Education Unit, The Academic College of Tel Aviv-Yafo, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • 5Psychiatry Department, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States

Parental bonding (recollection of own parents' parenting), adult attachment, and mother–infant bonding are all closely related yet distinct concepts of the parent–child relationship, sometimes used interchangeably in the literature. This study aimed to examine the associations between these concepts in a longitudinal path analysis design. A total of 262 postpartum women who gave birth at the maternity ward of a large tertiary health center in Israel completed a demographic questionnaire, the Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (ECR), the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) at 1–4 days postpartum, and the Postpartum Bonding Questionnaire (PBQ) at 2 months postpartum. Parental care factor (PBI) was found to be associated with mother–infant bonding (PBQ), directly and indirectly through insecure anxious attachment (ECR). Denial of autonomy factor (PBI) was found to be associated with mother–infant bonding (PBQ) only through insecure anxious attachment (ECR). Encouragement of behavioral freedom factor (PBI) was found to be associated with mother–infant bonding (PBQ) in a simple correlation but not in the complete model. The results highlight the intergenerational aspects of parenting and suggest that early childhood interventions with parents may have a long-term impact on child-rearing though generations, and by that on children's development.

Introduction

Parental bonding, adult attachment, and mother–infant bonding are central concepts of the parent–child relationship (across generations) and are all related to children's development, growth, and wellbeing. These similar yet distinct concepts are sometimes used interchangeably in the literature while not clearly and consistently treated as different concepts (14).

In the present study, we aim to expand the understanding of the associations between these concepts by examining how parental bonding and adult attachment are associated with mother–infant bonding. Using a longitudinal path analysis design, we examine whether women's adult attachment orientations measured shortly after birth possibly mediate the associations between parental bonding (the new mother's recollection of the way she was mothered) and mother–infant bonding (her cognitions and feelings toward her infant) at 2 months postpartum. To our knowledge, this study is the first to describe the paths between these close yet different concepts using a longitudinal design.

Mother–infant bonding refers to the emotions and the feelings a mother has toward her infant and herself as a parent. Bonding is believed to emerge during pregnancy or immediately after birth (1). The quality of mother–infant bonding is considered central to infant wellbeing and the child's cognitive and emotional development (5, 6) and was found as an important factor in mothers' later relationships with their children (79). Postpartum bonding was found to be associated with a host of factors such as abuse in childhood, family psychiatric history, mother's psychopathology, as well as personality variables [e.g., (6, 10, 11)]. In this study, we focused on the possible associations of mother–infant bonding with the mother's caring model as evolving from her experiences with her own mother, experiences that may have shaped her internal working models of relationships—attachment orientations.

Although similar in name and conceptually closely related to mother–infant bonding, parental bonding refers to the way adults retrospectively report the quality of their parents' parenting, i.e., their childhood relationships and experiences with their parents (12). It was found that mothers' parenting history was associated with the quality of parenting they provide to their infants (13), as mothers who remembered being accepted by their mothers as children were more sensitive and less intrusive with their infants (14). On the other hand, lower recalled care in childhood by the own mother predicted higher dissatisfaction with overall own motherhood (15), while maternal experiences of emotional neglect in childhood were associated with more mother–infant bonding impairments (16).

The Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) is a scale designed for assessing parenting style retrospectively. Despite its widespread use, there is no consensus regarding its factor structure (17). It was initially conceptualized and assessed as compromised from two factors, care and overprotection (12). However, recent studies reported a different factor structure [e.g., (17)]. In this study, we intend to verify the often reported three-factor structure of PBI: care (reflecting perceptions of the mother as warm and understanding), denial of autonomy (reflecting perceptions of the mother as controlling and overprotective), and encouragement of behavioral freedom (reflecting perceptions of the mother as granting autonomy) (11, 1719). With regard to parental bonding and mother–infant bonding associations, mothers' perceptions of their child-rearing history (both care and overprotectiveness) were found to be associated with mother–infant bonding (20). However, a later study found that only higher overprotection measured during pregnancy was associated with bonding failure in the postpartum period (21). These findings underscore how the retrospective perception of parenting from own childhood may be relevant to the postpartum mother–infant relationship.

According to the Attachment Theory, early experiences with significant others are internalized and formulate working models that shape individuals' behaviors (22, 23). Those early relations create prototypes for later relationships with close others (24). Attachment with mother/primary caregiver, who can be sensitive and responsive to one's need or not be reliably available and supportive, contributes to the development of internal working models (25). These can mediate the relationship between mothers' early experiences to various aspects of current motherhood (3, 26).

Attachment and maternal–infant bonding refer to different aspects of the parent–child relationship, as bonding emphasizes the mother's tie to her infant, and attachment in general refers to the child's tie to his mother and other caregivers (1, 9). Several studies examined the association between those concepts with inconclusive results pattern. On the one hand, during the transition to motherhood, insecure attachment (anxious and avoidant) was associated directly with bonding difficulties among parents (27) and indirectly mediated by postpartum depression among mothers (28) or mediated by parenting stress (29). On the other hand, Van Bussel et al. (9) reported a weak correlation between mother–infant bonding and attachment style while maternal romantic attachment style predicted attachment with the fetus in the antenatal, but not with the baby in the postpartum period (30) and only women with a dual/disorganized attachment style reported lower bonding than with women with secure and insecure attachment styles (31). This inconclusive results pattern may be explained by the different measures used for attachment and bonding.

In this study, we refer to adult attachment orientations (32)—the internal working models of the mother herself. In order to examine these internal working models in adulthood, researchers focused on a person's attachment orientation, which is compromised from two dimensions: attachment-related avoidance, which reflects the extent to which a person mistrusts others' intention and therefore defensively strives to maintain behavioral and emotional independence, and attachment-related anxiety, which reflects the extent to which a person worries that others will not be available in times of need and anxiously seeks love and care (33).

Past research that examined the relationship between adult attachment and parental bonding during the transition to motherhood revealed that mothers' recollections of their own mothers as supportive and non-intrusive differentiated between securely and insecurely romantic attached participants (3). A recent study reported correlations between recalled care and overprotection in childhood (in opposite directions) and both anxious and avoidant attachment in a sample of pregnant women (15). These findings adhere to the results of several other studies that examined the relationship between parental bonding and adult attachment in diverse populations (2, 3436). It should be noted that we were interested in the association of recollections of parental bonding and adult attachment orientation, rather than the association of parental bonding and attachment to the fetus or attachment behaviors that are often studied (28, 37).

This study aimed to examine the associations between parental bonding, adult attachment orientations, and maternal–infant bonding of postpartum mothers. Despite the review presented here, to the best of our knowledge, although close by describing parent–child relationships (inter-generationally), these are distinct concepts. Parental bonding refers to the way the mother (in our sample) recalls the relationship with her mother in childhood (12). Her adult attachment orientation refers to the way these recollections, among other factors, have shaped the working models she has for relationships with others in adulthood (33). Finally, mother–infant bonding refers to the emotions and the feelings a mother has toward her infant and herself as a parent (1).

We propose a model in which the mother's childhood recollections of her mother parenting her influence her bonding with her infant, and this association is mediated by her adult attachment orientation. While we study parental bonding and adult attachment orientations shortly after birth (along with other demographic and obstetric control variables), we measure their influence on mother–infant bonding at 2 months postpartum. Hence, we hypothesize that insecure attachment orientations (both anxious and avoidant) will mediate the association between recalled parenting by the mother and mother–infant bonding, as higher levels of parental bonding (PBI) will be associated with lower levels of anxiety or avoidance attachment orientation, which will be associated with better mother–infant bonding at 2 months postpartum (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothesized mediation model; Insecure attachment orientations (ECR) will mediate the association between parental bonding (PBI) and mother-infant bonding (PBQ).

Materials and Methods

Sample

The final sample included 262 postpartum women who gave birth in the maternity wards of the Rabin Medical Center (RMC), a large tertiary health center in Israel. Eligibility criteria included delivering at least at 37 weeks' gestation, a singleton pregnancy, and Hebrew speaking. Information about recruitment, data collection, and dropout rates can be seen in Figure 2. Comparing dropouts (women who completed only the first time point) to women who completed the second time point with sufficient data showed that completers were less avoidant (ECR) [M = 2.5, SD = 0.9 vs. M = 2.8, SD = 1.0, F(1, 481) = 8.6, p = 0.003], lower in PBI denial factor [M = 0.7, SD = 0.1 vs. M = 0.9, SD = 0.9, F(1, 540) = 12.0, p = 0.001], and older [M = 32.0, SD = 4.7 vs. M = 31.0, SD = 5.4, F(1,587) = 4.5, p = 0.035]. In addition, primiparous women were less likely to complete the second time point (41 vs. 50%, p < 0.001), as were women with less than university educational level (38 vs. 53%, p < 0.001) and with below average income (43 vs. 56%, p = 0.002).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Recruitment and data collection.

The average age of the participants was 31.7 (±4.8), most (94%) were married, 84% were born in Israel, and 92% were Jewish. Most women (76%) had vaginal births, 9% had elective cesarean section, 10% had emergency cesarean sections, and 6% had an assisted vaginal birth. Just over half of all women (53% of the whole sample, 61% if excluding women who had elective CS) were administered an epidural, and 43% (49% if excluding women who had elective CS) had oxytocin for labor augmentation. For participants' demographic data, see Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographic characteristics and correlations with PBQ score.

Procedure

The study is part of a larger longitudinal study aimed at understanding associations between factors associated with birth and postpartum mental health during the first 6 months postpartum conducted between July 2018 and July 2019. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the RMC and the Academic College of Tel-Aviv Yaffo institutional review boards. Research assistants, graduate students with appropriate training in research ethics, approached all women at the maternity ward on a random day of the week, and after giving informed consent, the participants answered questionnaires at two time points:

T1 (1–4 days postpartum) in person at the maternity ward—obstetric data were taken from the medical files and women completed demographic questions, the Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (ECR) and the PBI.

T2 (2 months postpartum)—using online questionnaires, the participants completed the Postpartum Bonding Questionnaire (PBQ).

Participants who did not respond to the email invitation were reminded once with a phone call. Questionnaires and data output were generated using Qualtrics© 2015 (Qualtrics, Provo, UT, USA; http://www.qualtrics.com).

Measures

Sociodemographic Questionnaire

Sociodemographic questionnaire included questions about age, education level, marital/co-habiting relationship, income level (as compared to the national average per household at the time of the study), religious affiliation, country of origin, and the history or current existence of psychiatric disorders.

Obstetric Data

Obstetric data were extracted from medical records, recording number of previous births, infertility treatments, pregnancy risks, past abortions or miscarriages, and current birth data: type of birth as well as epidural and oxytocin administration. We treated “birth type” as a dichotomous variable for statistical reasons and according to relevant literature that claims that the importance of the birth type variable is whether it was expected or not (3840). Thus, vaginal birth and elective cesarean sections are considered “Expected birth,” while emergency cesarean section and vaginal assisted birth are considered “Unexpected birth.” Being primiparous, having higher education, having above average income, being married, having a psychiatric diagnosis, and having an unplanned birth type were dummy-coded as “1” while other values were coded as “0.”

Parental Bonding

Parental bonding was assessed with the PBI (12), a 25-item scale designed to measure retrospective recollections and perceptions of early parental attitudes and behaviors that has been widely used for assessing recollections of parent–child relations (41). We used the Hebrew version (42) and inquired only about recollections of mother's parenting. Participants were asked to rate the degree to which each item describes their mother's early behavior and attitudes, on a four-point scale, ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (very much). In order to verify the three-factor solution of the scale, we performed parallel analysis, a more accurate method for determining the number of factors in a set of items than the commonly used criterion of eigenvalue >1, which tends to extract too many factors (43). Parallel analysis indicated three underlying factors. Exploratory factor analysis using principal components analysis with varimax rotation provided a corresponding solution, almost identical to the factors found in a recent large population-based psychometric validation of the scale as well as other studies [see (17)] apart from item 3 that loaded in our study on the encouragement factor and in the mentioned research loaded on both encouragement and care and item 8 that loaded on the denial of autonomy scale in our study and on the encouragement factor in the mentioned study. These three factors were also found in our previous study of different sample (11). The three factors together explained 52% of the variance in the responses (see Table 2): (1) PBI-Care, reflecting perceptions of the mother as warm and understanding (12 items; α = 0.90); (2) PBI-Denial of autonomy, reflecting perceptions of the mother as controlling and overprotective (7 items; α = 0.79); and (3) PBI-Encouragement of behavioral freedom, reflecting perceptions of the mother as granting autonomy (6 items; α = 0.85). Scores were computed by calculating an average for each subscale with higher scores reflecting stronger perceptions. The intercorrelations between the three factors were significant yet moderate (0.23–0.38), supporting their use as three separate factors (see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Factor loadings for principal components analysis of PBI with Varimax rotation.

Adult Attachment

Adult attachment was assessed by the ECR (44), which assesses the dimensions of anxious and avoidant adult attachment. For the purpose of the study, we used an abbreviated, validated Hebrew version that consists of 24 items divided into two dimensions: anxious (12 items, e.g., “I worry about being abandoned”) and avoidant (12 items, e.g., “I feel discomfort when others get close to me”) (45). Participants rated the extent to which an item described themselves on a seven-point scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). A high score indicates higher anxious or avoidant attachment. This scale has been used in previous studies with postpartum women [e.g., (46, 47)]. In the current study, the internal reliability was good (α = 0.84 for anxiety and α = 0.83 for avoidance).

Mother–Infant Bonding

Mother–infant bonding was assessed by the Hebrew version of the PBQ (48, 49). This 25-item scale assessed the mother's feelings or attitudes toward her baby (e.g., “I feel close to my baby”). Statements are presented on a six-point scale, ranging from 0 (always) to 5 (never), with reverse coding of positive items. Responses are summed so that higher scores denote greater bonding difficulties (poorer bonding). Two items relating to the risk of abuse were not included due to ethical considerations (50). Internal reliability of the total scale was good (α = 0.89).

Statistical Analysis

Data were described as M(SD) and range or as counts and percentages. Correlations between the study variables were assessed using the Pearson correlation coefficient. Exploratory factor analysis for determining the factor structure of the PBI questionnaire used principal components analysis with varimax rotation. Items were assigned to factors on which their loading was 0.5 or higher. Path analysis with 1,000 bootstrap samples was used to test the mediation model. Data were analyzed using SPSS v.25 and AMOS v.25. Using the 15 observations per measured variable rule, we concluded that a sample of about 200 observations should be sufficient for testing our hypothesized model including possible covariates.

Results

Table 1 shows the sample demographics as well as correlations between the demographic characteristics and the outcome variable. The correlations between the study variables, as well as their means and standard deviations, are shown in Table 3. All study variables significantly correlated with each other.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations between the study variables.

Given the correlations shown in Table 1, the hypothesized model was tested with being primiparous, education, psychiatric diagnosis, and birth type as covariates, linked to the study variables with which they had significant correlations. The results showed that birth type and the PBI Encouragement factor had no significant effects on any of the other model variables, and so were excluded from the model. The resulting model is shown in Figure 3. Results show that the model had good fit [χ(3)2 = 1.95, p = 0.58, NFI = 0.99, TLI = 1.04, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.00] and accounted for 25% of the PBQ variance. PBI Care factor had both direct [β = −0.16, p = 0.02, 95% CI = (−0.28, −0.02)] and indirect effects on PBQ, going through anxious attachment [β = −0.03, p = 0.03, 95% CI = (−0.08, 0.00)]. We found no direct effect of the PBI Denial factor on PBQ, yet we found an indirect effect through anxious attachment [β = 0.04, p = 0.04, 95% CI = (0.00, 0.10)]. While PBI Encouragement factor was significantly correlated with PBQ as can be seen in Table 3, this correlation diminished in the presence of the other factors in the final model. Avoidant attachment did not serve as a mediator despite its correlations with the PBI factors and PBQ (see Table 3), as anxious attachment accounted for the mediation effect.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Final mediation model; ECR (anxious attachment orientation) mediating the association between PBI factors (parental bonding care and denial of autonomy) and PBQ. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. While not shown on the graph, this model was analyzed with being primiparous, education, and having a psychiatric diagnosis as covariates. Numbers above the lines are standardized regression weights, and numbers above the endogenous variables and multiple squared correlations.

In sum, we learn that anxious attachment mediates the relationships between PBI factors (Care Denial) and PBQ: lower PBI care factor was related to higher ECR attachment anxiety, which in turn was related to higher PBQ score, resulting in a negative indirect effect. In contrast, high PBI denial factor was related to higher ECR attachment anxiety, which was followed by higher PBQ score, hence the positive indirect effect. In addition, in the final mode, PBI Encouragement factor was not associated with PBQ and avoidant attachment was not found to be a mediator between PBI factor and PBQ.

Discussion

The current study aimed to examine the associations between parental bonding (PBI), adult attachment orientations, and mother–infant bonding (PBQ) among mothers, in a longitudinal design from childbirth to 2 months postpartum. Our findings indicate that parental bonding care factor was associated with mother–infant bonding both directly and indirectly through anxious attachment, while parental bonding denial of autonomy factor was associated with mother–infant bonding only indirectly through anxious attachment. The third factor, parental encouragement of behavioral freedom was associated with bonding in a simple correlation but was not associated with mother–infant bonding in the complete model. Additionally, parental care and parental denial of autonomy factors were associated with avoidant attachment, though avoidant attachment was not associated with mother–infant bonding. The study findings contribute to the existing literature in several aspects. First, our findings emphasize that parenting models that new mothers have absorbed from their mothers may have shaped their internal working models and, through those, but also directly, may be associated with the way they perceive their bond with their new infant. In particular, we found that childhood recollections of mothering that lack warmth and understanding or are characterized as controlling and overprotective may be associated with higher levels of anxious attachment orientation, which in turn may increase mother–infant bonding difficulties. Our results are in line with previous findings that emphasize that parents' recollections of the way they were parented are important to their postpartum parenthood mental health and psychological wellbeing (5153). This intergenerational perspective emphasizes that early childhood caretaking experiences of mothers may continue to compromise the mother's capacity to cope during her own parenthood (20) through her internal working models and the bond she perceives with her new infant (28). This finding is of importance as the quality of the maternal infant relationship postpartum, and in particular mother–infant bonding, is considered central to infant wellbeing, cognitive and emotional development, and adaptation throughout life (5456).

In addition, the findings contribute to the literature regarding the PBI factor structure by replicating the PBI's three factors structure as reported in recent studies [e.g., (17)]. Our study results put a spotlight on parental care, which was the only factor associated directly and indirectly through anxious attachment with mother–infant bonding. It emphasizes parental care as a possible distinct concept from the other two PBI factors (vis-à-vis bonding) and in line with previous studies that report parental care as a clear, cohesive, and stable factor (18, 41, 57). We suggest that anxious attachment orientation may explain the association between parental care and bonding, but our model also alludes to the possible direct association between parental care and mother–infant bonding. It underscores parental care as a significant variable in parenting (15) and as meaningful during the transition to parenthood (11).

In the complete model, the second PBI factor, parental denial of autonomy, was related to mother–infant bonding only through anxious attachment. This finding adds to the literature regarding this factor role, as a previous study found an association between parental denial of autonomy and maternal–fetal attachment (11). The third factor, parental encouragement of behavioral freedom, was not associated with mother–infant bonding in the model. Thus, the third factor, which is relatively new, is yet to be studied with relation to other variables in general and parenting in particular.

Although we hypothesized that both insecure attachment orientations (anxious and avoidant) would mediate the associations between parental bonding and mother–infant bonding, the results indicated differential mediation; only anxious attachment mediated this association in the overall model. It is important to note that we found a simple correlation between anxious as well as avoidant attachment orientations and mother–infant bonding while both parental bonding care and denial factors were associated with both anxious and avoidant attachment orientations. In general, our findings adhere to the literature linking insecure attachment orientations and parenting variables (58) and emphasized that insecure attachment has an important role during the transition to parenthood (59). Our findings demonstrate adult attachment and maternal–infant bonding as different aspects of the intergenerational perspective of parent–child relationship, as adult attachment orientations refer to the internal working models of relationships in adulthood and mother–infant bonding refers to the mother's tie to her infant (1, 9). However, various studies investigating the specific associations of avoidant and anxious attachment orientations and mother–infant bonding report inconsistent findings, similarly to our findings. For example, only avoidance was a significant predictor of mother–infant bonding when controlling for demographic variables and maternal mental health history (29). In another study, there was no direct relationship between attachment and mother–infant bonding; however, anxious attachment was associated with postpartum depression, and depressive symptoms predicted impaired bonding (30). Other studies investigated associations between other attachment orientations (secure attachment and dual/disorganized attachment) with bonding [e.g., (27, 31)]; therefore, more research is clearly needed to elucidate the role of the different insecure attachment orientations vis-à-vis parenting in general and the mediation of the association between parental bonding and mother–infant bonding in particular. Our model suggests that the way mothers report about the mothering they have received in childhood retrospectively may shape their anxious and avoidant orientations, but only anxious orientation was found to have a significant association with bonding to the infant in our model.

Although our study was in a longitudinal design using a relatively large sample, it is not without limitations. First, the study participants were mothers asked to report their childhood recollections of the way their mothers cared for them. Further research is needed both regarding fathers and memories of parenting experiences with fathers. Furthermore, the PBI is a self-report measure of parental bonding reported retrospectively that might be influenced by recall biases, though parental bonding recollections as measured by the PBI exhibited stability over a 20-year period, suggesting that recall biases of parental bonding may be modest (60). Second, in this study we used only self-report measures. Future research could use observational measures of attachment or mother–infant interactions as well as study the participants' mothers and infants to further learn about intergenerational perspectives in more ecological designs. Third, we measured our variables in the first 2 months' time frame, while future research could study parenting perceptions in longer periods after childbirth. Fourth, there was a difference between the first time point assessment that was done in-person and the second time point that was done online, causing a potential confounding. This difference in assessments is a result of following up on a fairly large sample. Recent research of offline vs. online assessment of the PBQ found no difference between assessment modalities in terms of associations with sociodemographic, reproductive, obstetric, and psychological outcomes (61). Lastly, our participants were mostly Jewish, sampled from one health center only, and this may impair our ability to generalize our findings, as parenting practices may be different across cultures.

In conclusion, the present study examined the associations between the close yet distinct concepts of the parent–child relationship: parental bonding, adult attachment orientations, and mother–infant bonding. Our findings demonstrate the associations between those three separate concepts in a longitudinal design, as our model emphasizes that parenting models mothers have received from their mothers may shape their internal working models and those in turn were associated with the way they perceive the bond with the new infant. These findings highlight the intergenerational conceptualization of parenting (62) and emphasize that early childhood interventions with parents might be significant for a long-term impact on the development of future generations.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Rabin Medical Center and the Academic College of Tel-Aviv Yaffo institutional review boards. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JH, MK, HK, and YP contributed to the conception and design of the study. MM-L and SL organized the database. SL performed the statistical analysis. MK and JH wrote the first draft of the manuscript. JH, MK, and YP wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Bicking Kinsey C, Hupcey JE. State of the science of maternal-infant bonding: a principle-based concept analysis. Midwifery. (2013) 29:1314–20. doi: 10.1016/j.midw.2012.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Matsuoka N, Uji M, Hiramura H, Chen Z, Shikai N, Kishida Y, et al. Adolescents' attachment style and early experiences: a gender difference. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2006) 9:23–9. doi: 10.1007/s00737-005-0105-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Priel B, Besser A. Adult attachment styles, early relationships, antenatal attachment, and perceptions of infant temperament: a study of first-time mothers. Pers Relatsh. (2000) 7:291–310. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00018.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Takács L, Smolík F, Kazmierczak M, Putnam SP. Early infant temperament shapes the nature of mother-infant bonding in the first postpartum year. Infant Behav Dev. (2020) 58:101428. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Fuchs A, Möhler E, Reck C, Resch F, Kaess M. The early mother-to-child bond and its unique prospective contribution to child behavior evaluated by mothers and teachers. Psychopathology. (2016) 49:211–6. doi: 10.1159/000445439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Tichelman E, Westerneng M, Witteveen AB, Van Baar AL, Van Der Horst HE, De Jonge A, et al. Correlates of prenatal and postnatal motherto-infant bonding quality: a systematic review. PLoS ONE. (2019). 14:101428. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0222998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Moehler E, Brunner R, Wiebel A, Reck C, Resch F. Maternal depressive symptoms in the postnatal period are associated with long-term impairment of mother-child bonding. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2006) 9:273–8. doi: 10.1007/s00737-006-0149-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. O'Higgins M, Roberts ISJ, Glover V, Taylor A. Mother-child bonding at 1 year; associations with symptoms of postnatal depression and bonding in the first few weeks. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2013) 16:381–9. doi: 10.1007/s00737-013-0354-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Van Bussel JCH, Spitz B, Demyttenaere K. Three self-report questionnaires of the early mother-to-infant bond: reliability and validity of the Dutch version of the MPAS, PBQ and MIBS. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2010) 13:373–84. doi: 10.1007/s00737-009-0140-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Farré-Sender B, Torres A, Gelabert E, Andrés S, Roca A, Lasheras G, et al. Mother–infant bonding in the postpartum period: assessment of the impact of pre-delivery factors in a clinical sample. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2018) 21:287–97. doi: 10.1007/s00737-017-0785-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Handelzalts JE, Preis H, Rosenbaum M, Gozlan M, Benyamini Y. Pregnant women's recollections of early maternal bonding: associations with maternal–fetal attachment and birth choices. Infant Ment Health J. (2018) 39:511–21. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Parker G, Tupling H, Brown LB. A Parental bonding instrument. Br J Med Psychol. (1979) 52:1–10. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1979.tb02487.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Klaus MH, Kennell JH. Maternal-Infant Bonding : The Impact of Early Separation or Loss On Family Development. St Louis: The CV Mosby Company. (1976) p. 257.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

14. Kretchmar MD, Jacobvitz DB. Observing mother-child relationships across generations: boundary patterns, attachment, and the transmission of caregiving. Fam Process. (2002) 41:351–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2002.41306.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Göbel A, Stuhrmann LY, Barkmann C, Schulte-Markwort M, Mudra S. Becoming a mother: predicting early dissatisfaction with motherhood at three weeks postpartum. Midwifery. (2020) 91:102824. doi: 10.1016/j.midw.2020.102824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lehnig F, Nagl M, Stepan H, Wagner B, Kersting A. Associations of postpartum mother-infant bonding with maternal childhood maltreatment and postpartum mental health: a cross-sectional study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. (2019). 19:278. doi: 10.1186/s12884-019-2426-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Xu MK, Morin AJS, Marsh HW, Richards M, Jones PB. Psychometric validation of the parental bonding instrument in a U.K. population–based sample: role of gender and association with mental health in mid-late life. Assessment. (2018) 25:716–28. doi: 10.1177/1073191116660813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mohr S, Preisig M, Fenton BT, Ferrero F. Validation of the French version of the parental bonding instrument in adults. Pers Individ Dif. (1999) 26:1065–74. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00210-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Narita T, Sato T, Hirano S, Gota M, Sakado K, Uehara T. Parental child-rearing behavior as measured by the parental bonding instrument in a japanese population: factor structure and relationship to a lifetime history of depression. J Affect Disord. (2000) 57:229–34. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00071-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hall RAS, Hoffenkamp HN, Tooten A, Braeken J, Vingerhoets AJJM, Van Bakel HJA. Child-rearing history and emotional bonding in parents of preterm and full-term infants. J Child Fam Stud. (2015) 24:1715–26. doi: 10.1007/s10826-014-9975-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ohara M, Nakatochi M, Okada T, Aleksic B, Nakamura Y, Shiino T, et al. Impact of perceived rearing and social support on bonding failure and depression among mothers: a longitudinal study of pregnant women. J Psychiatr Res. (2018) 105:71–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of Attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. (Hillsdale) (1978).

Google Scholar

23. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Basic Books (1982).

Google Scholar

24. Bretherton I. The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth. Dev Psychol. (1992) 28:759–75. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and Anger. New York, NY: Basic Books (1973).

Google Scholar

26. Jia Y, Cheng G, Ding F, Li B, Ta N, Zhang D. Mediation effect of adult attachment orientations between perceived parental warmth and the preference for infants. Curr Psychol. (2021) 40:113–25. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00360-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Little KK, Sockol LE. Romantic relationship satisfaction and parent-infant bonding during the transition to parenthood: an attachment-based perspective. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:2068. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hairston IS, Handelzalts JE, Assis C, Kovo M. Postpartum bonding difficulties and adult attachment styles:The mediating role of postpartum depression and childbirth-related PTSD. Infant Ment Health J. (2018) 39:198–208. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Nordahl D, Rognmo K, Bohne A, Landsem IP, Moe V, Wang CEA, et al. Adult attachment style and maternal-infant bonding: the indirect path of parenting stress. BMC Psychol. (2020) 8:58. doi: 10.1186/s40359-020-00424-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Chrzan-Detkoś M, Łockiewicz M. Maternal romantic attachment, and antenatal and postnatal mother–infant attachment in a sample of Polish women. Eur J Dev Psychol. (2015) 12:429–42. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2015.1036024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Nonnenmacher N, Noe D, Ehrenthal JC, Reck C. Postpartum bonding: the impact of maternal depression and adult attachment style. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2016) 19:927–35. doi: 10.1007/s00737-016-0648-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Applications of attachment theory and research. In: Mikulincer M, Shaver PR, editors. Applications of Social Psychology. New York, NY: Routledge (2020). p. 187–206. doi: 10.4324/9780367816407-10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics and Change. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2007).

Google Scholar

34. Berry K, Wearden A, Barrowclough C. Adult attachment styles and psychosis: an investigation of associations between general attachment styles and attachment relationships with specific others. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2007) 42:972–6. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0261-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Shadach E, Rappaport S, Dollberg D, Tolmacz R, Levy S. Relational entitlement, early recollections of parental care, and attachment orientation. Curr Psychol. (2018) 37:781–91. doi: 10.1007/s12144-017-9559-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Wiseman H, Mayseless O, Sharabany R. Why are they lonely? Perceived quality of early relationships with parents, attachment, personality predispositions and loneliness in first-year university students. Pers Indiv Diff. (2006) 40:237–48. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.05.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. da Rosa KM, Scholl CC, Ferreira LA, Trettim JP, da Cunha GK, Rubin BB, et al. Maternal-fetal attachment and perceived parental bonds of pregnant women. Early Hum Dev. (2021) 154:105310. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2021.105310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Handelzalts JE, Waldman Peyser A, Krissi H, Levy S, Wiznitzer A, Peled Y. Indications for emergency intervention, mode of delivery, and the childbirth experience. PLoS ONE. (2017) 12:e0169132. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kjerulff KH, Brubaker LH. New mothers' feelings of disappointment and failure after cesarean delivery. Birth. (2018) 45:19–27. doi: 10.1111/birt.12315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Zanardo V, Soldera G, Volpe F, Giliberti L, Parotto M, Giustardi A, et al. Influence of elective and emergency cesarean delivery on mother emotions and bonding. Early Hum Dev. (2016) 99:17–20. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Tsaousis I, Mascha K, Giovazolias T. Can parental bonding be assessed in children? Factor structure and factorial invariance of the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) between adults and children. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2012) 43:238–53. doi: 10.1007/s10578-011-0260-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Canetti L, Bachar E, Galili-Weisstub E, De-Nour AK, Shalev AY. Parental bonding and mental health in adolescence. Adolescence. (1997) 32:380–94.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

43. Hayton JC, Allen DG, Scarpello V. Factor retention decisions in exploratory factor analysis: a tutorial on parallel analysis. Organ Res Methods. (2004). 7:191–205. doi: 10.1177/1094428104263675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Brennan KA, Clark CL, Shaver PR. Self-report measurement of adult romantic attachment: an integrative overview. In Simpson JA, Rholes WS, editors, Attachment Theory and Close Relationships. New York: Guilford Press (1998). p. 46–76

Google Scholar

45. Mikulincer M, Florian V. Exploring individual differences in reactions to mortality salience: does attachment style regulate terror management mechanisms? J Pers Soc Psychol. (2000) 79:260–73. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.2.260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Iles J, Slade P, Spiby H. Posttraumatic stress symptoms and postpartum depression in couples after childbirth: the role of partner support and attachment. J Anxiety Disord. (2011) 25:520–30. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Marques R, Monteiro F, Canavarro MC, Fonseca A. The role of emotion regulation difficulties in the relationship between attachment representations and depressive and anxiety symptoms in the postpartum period. J Affect Disord. (2018) 238:39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Brockington I, Oates J, George S, et al. A Screening Questionnaire for mother-infant bonding disorders. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2001) 3:133–40. doi: 10.1007/s007370170010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Hairston IS, Solnik-Menilo T, Deviri D, Handelzalts JE. Maternal depressed mood moderates the impact of infant sleep on mother–infant bonding. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2016) 19:1029–39. doi: 10.1007/s00737-016-0652-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Muzik M, Bocknek EL, Broderick A, Richardson P, Rosenblum KL, Thelen K, et al. Mother-infant bonding impairment across the first 6 months postpartum: the primacy of psychopathology in women with childhood abuse and neglect histories. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2013) 16:29–38. doi: 10.1007/s00737-012-0312-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Duman B, Senturk Cankorur V, Taylor C, Stewart R. Prospective associations between recalled parental bonding and perinatal depression: a cohort study in urban and rural Turkey. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2018) 53:385–92. doi: 10.1007/s00127-018-1484-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Grant KA, Bautovich A, McMahon C, Reilly N, Leader L, Austin MP. Parental care and control during childhood: associations with maternal perinatal mood disturbance and parenting stress. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2012) 15:297–305. doi: 10.1007/s00737-012-0292-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Williams C, Patricia Taylor E, Schwannauer M. A web-based survey of mother–infant bond, attachment experiences, and metacognition in posttraumatic stress following childbirth. Infant Ment. Health J. (2016) 37:259–73. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Cirulli F, Berry A, Alleva E. Early disruption of the mother-infant relationship: effects on brain plasticity and implications for psychopathology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2003) 27:73–82. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(03)00010-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Johnson K. Maternal-infant bonding: a review of literature. Int J Childbirth Educ. (2013) 28:17–22. Retrieved from: https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/maternal-infant-bonding-review-literature/docview/1412226528/se-2?accountid=32511

Google Scholar

56. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Baumwell L. Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Dev. (2001) 72:748–67. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Terra L, Hauck S, Fillipon AP, Sanchez P, Hirakata V, Schestatsky S, et al. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Parental Bonding Instrument in a Brazilian female population. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2009) 43:348–54. doi: 10.1080/00048670902721053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Jones JD, Cassidy J, Shaver PR. Parents' Self-reported attachment styles: a review of links with parenting behaviors, emotions, and cognitions. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. (2015) 19:44–76. doi: 10.1177/1088868314541858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Simpson JA, Rholes WS. Adult attachment orientations and well-being during the transition to parenthood. Curr Opin Psychol. (2019) 25:47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Murphy E, Wickramaratne P, Weissman M. The stability of parental bonding reports: a 20-year follow-up. J Affect Disord. (2010) 27:307–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Lasheras G, Farré-Sender B, Osma J, Martínez-Borba V, Mestre-Bach G. Mother-infant bonding screening in a sample of postpartum women: comparison between online vs offline format. J Reprod Infant Psychol. (2021) 1–16. doi: 10.1080/02646838.2021.1921716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Belsky J, Sligo J, Jaffee SR, Woodward L, Silva PA. Intergenerational transmission of warm-sensitive-stimulating parenting: a prospective study of mothers and fathers of 3-year-olds. Child Dev. (2005) 76:384–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00852.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mother–infant bonding, parental bonding, adult attachment, postpartum, childbirth

Citation: Kalfon Hakhmigari M, Peled Y, Krissi H, Levy S, Molmen-Lichter M and Handelzalts JE (2021) Anxious Attachment Mediates the Associations Between Early Recollections of Mother's Own Parental Bonding and Mother–Infant Bonding: A 2-Month Path Analysis Model. Front. Psychiatry 12:682161. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.682161

Received: 17 March 2021; Accepted: 01 June 2021;
Published: 05 July 2021.

Edited by:

Jane Fisher, Monash University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Barbara Galland, University of Otago, New Zealand
Darpan Kaur, Mahatma Gandhi Missions Medical College and Hospital, India

Copyright © 2021 Kalfon Hakhmigari, Peled, Krissi, Levy, Molmen-Lichter and Handelzalts. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jonathan E. Handelzalts, jhandelzalts@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.