Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 08 July 2021
Sec. Mood Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic The Heterogeneity of Neuropsychiatric Disorders View all 12 articles

Childhood Maltreatment Was Correlated With the Decreased Cortical Function in Depressed Patients Under Social Stress in a Working Memory Task: A Pilot Study

\nMengying Ma,Mengying Ma1,2Xiao Zhang,
Xiao Zhang1,2*Yuyanan Zhang,Yuyanan Zhang1,2Yi Su,Yi Su1,2Hao Yan,Hao Yan1,2Haoyang Tan,Haoyang Tan3,4Dai Zhang,,Dai Zhang1,2,3Weihua Yue,,
Weihua Yue1,2,5*
  • 1Institute of Mental Health, The Sixth Hospital, Peking University, Beijing, China
  • 2Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Ministry of Health & National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, Peking University, Beijing, China
  • 3Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
  • 5PKU-IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University, Beijing, China

Background: Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a common psychiatric disorder associated with working memory (WM) impairment. Neuroimaging studies showed divergent results of the WM process in MDD patients. Stress could affect the occurrence and development of depression, in which childhood maltreatment played an important role.

Methods: Thirty-seven MDD patients and 54 healthy control subjects were enrolled and completed a WM functional magnetic resonance imaging task with maintenance and manipulation conditions under stress and non-stress settings. We collected demographical and clinical data, using 17-item Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD-17) and Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) in MDD patients. In the WM task, we analyzed the main diagnosis effect and explored the correlation of impaired brain regions in MDD patients with CTQ and HAMD-17.

Results: No group differences were found in the accuracy rate and reaction time between the two groups. MDD patients had lower brain activation in following regions (PFWE < 0.05). The left fusiform gyrus showed less activation in all conditions. The right supplementary motor area (SMA) exhibited decreased activation under non-stress. The anterior prefrontal cortex showed reduced activation during manipulation under stress, with the β estimations of the peak voxel showing significant group difference negatively correlated with childhood sex abuse (PBonferroni < 0.05).

Conclusions: In our pilot study, MDD patients had reduced brain activation, affecting emotional stimuli processing function, executive function, and cognitive control function. Childhood maltreatment might affect brain function in MDD. This work might provide some information for future studies on MDD.

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a common psychiatric disorder (1), which usually leads people to suffering from emotional disturbances and cognitive impairments (2, 3). Working memory (WM) involves the capability to memorize, retrieve, and utilize the information for a limited period (4) and is incredibly easily impaired in MDD patients (5, 6). Numbers of studies found widespread increased brain activations during the cognitive process in MDD patients (7), including the anterior prefrontal cortex (APFC) (8), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (9, 10), and cingulate cortex (11). However, some studies showed hypoactive brain regions, including the frontal cortex, temporal cortex, insula, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and parietal cortex in depressed patients (1214). These divergent results suggested the complexity of this issue, and potential factors might be the types of WM (15) and the levels of stress. Besides, childhood maltreatment has been considered to accelerate the development of depression (1618). Moreover, a decreased volume of prefrontal cortex might play a mediated role in the relationship between childhood maltreatment and declined cognitive functioning (1921). However, how can childhood maltreatment affect the WM process is complicated as stress has both direct neuroendocrine (22) and indirect methylation (23) effects on the development of depression. We hypothesized that the neural basis of the childhood maltreatment effects at different WM task conditions were different. To explore this hypothesis, we designed a WM task (24, 25) with varying subtasks (maintenance vs. manipulation) and varying stress levels (non-competition vs. competition) to compare the childhood maltreatment effects in different conditions.

Materials and Methods

Participants

In this study, 53 MDD patients and 64 healthy controls (HCs) were recruited. The patients were outpatients recruited from Peking University Sixth Hospital. We used the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) diagnostic criteria of depression disorder, without other comorbidities of the DSM-IV-TR Axis I disorders. Two psychiatrists assessed the patients by using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (version 5.0) (26). All HCs were enrolled by advertising in the community and social media and evaluated by using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition to exclude any mental disorder (27). The current study was approved by the ethical committee of the Peking University Sixth Hospital. All the participants were given detailed information about the purpose and procedures of the study and signed the written consents.

We used the 17-item Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD-17) to evaluate the symptom severity (28). In addition, we used Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) to examine how could childhood maltreatment affect brain function in adult MDD patients (29). Subjects were included using the following criteria: (1) between 18 and 55 years of age, (2) right-handed, (3) Chinese Han lineage, and (4) MDD patients needed to get a HAMD-17 score ≥17. Subjects were excluded with the following criteria: (1) any current or history of neurological disease, (2) a history of more than 5-min loss of consciousness, (3) contraindications for magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanning, (4) electroconvulsive therapy within 6 months or history of severe medical illness, (5) other genetic disease, (6) serious impulsive behavior or suicide attempts, and (7) pregnancy and lactation.

We excluded subjects with low image quality or who did not complete the task (six MDD patients and four HCs) or with an accuracy rate of the maintenance <50% under competitive/non-competitive setting (two MDD patients, no HCs) or with head motion of more than 3° rotation/3-mm translation (eight MDD patients and six HCs). Finally, 37 MDD subjects and 54 HCs were included in the analysis (Table 1). Among the 37 patients, 10 patients were drug-naive. In addition, 17 MDD patients were taking selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (fluoxetine, escitalopram, sertraline), six were taking serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (venlafaxine, duloxetine), two were taking noradrenergic and specific serotonergic antidepressants (mirtazapine), one was taking dopamine norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, DNRIs (bupropion), and one was taking flupentixol and melitracen.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and Behavioral Characteristics of MDD patients and HCs.

WM Paradigm and Image Acquisition

We developed an event-related “number calculation WM” task from previous works (30, 31) and newly comprised alternating competitive and non-competitive blocks (Figure 1). We validated that both the different subtasks and the different stress levels were successfully introduced in this task from our previous study (28), in which the detailed description of this task could be found. A 3.0-T GE Discovery MR750 scanner was used for scanning all participants at the Center for MRI Research, Peking University Institute of Mental Health. The parameters of the functional MRI are as follows: each echoplanar image included 33 (thickness/gap = 4.2/0 mm) axial slices, which covered the whole cerebrum and cerebellum (repetition time/echo time = 2,000/30 ms, flip angle = 90°, field of view = 22.4 × 22.4 cm2, matrix = 64 × 64). The protocol parameters were selected for optimizing the quality and stability of the blood oxygenation level-dependent signal with the exclusion of the first four images as dummy scans.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Working memory paradigm incorporating social competition stress. In the stressed component, subjects were led to believe that they were playing against a “competitor” of similar age and gender and were judged as winning or losing based on their speed and accuracy, which subsequently resulted in ~70% loss feedback. In the less stressed blocks, there was no competitor, and subjects received neutral feedback. In all the working memory manipulation and maintenance tasks, an array of two-number digits was encoded and held in working memory over 3 to 4 s. In working memory maintenance condition, subjects responded to which of the two maintained digits was larger or smaller as indicated. In working memory manipulation condition, subjects performed subtraction on one of the numbers held in working memory, followed by a response as to which result was larger or smaller as indicated. Subjects performed two runs counterbalanced for trial and stimuli presentation order over ~20 min. All instructions were translated to Chinese.

Processing and Statistical Analyses of the MRI

We used MATLAB 2016b and SPM12 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) for analyzing the functional MRI data. The preprocessing of the data was performed as following steps: (1) slice timing correction, (2) realigning to the first volume and correcting the head motion, (3) spatially normalizing into standard stereotaxic space (Montreal Neurological Institute template) using a fourth-degree B-spline interpolation, and (4) using an 8-mm Gaussian kernel to spatial smoothing. After preprocessing, the voxel size of the image data was 3 × 3 × 3 mm3. We modeled every task-evoked stimulus as an independent delta function, and it convolved with the typical hemodynamic response function, controlling the systematic differences of global activity by normalizing ratio to the whole-brain global mean. And we used a 128-s high-pass filter for temporal filter. We modeled each event of task-evoked stimulus for performing trials correctly. In addition, we modeled the residual movement and incorrect response parameters as regressors of no interest. In this study, we planned to contrast the brain activation at the maintenance subtask or manipulation subtask under stress, non-stress setting and stress vs. non-stress, and between the two groups of MDD patients and HCs. Second-level analyses were subsequently taken, and the variability of intersubject was regarded as a random effect.

After controlling age, we used a flexible 2 × 2 analysis of variance in SPM12 to analyze the main effect of diagnosis, the main effect of stress, and the diagnosis × stress interaction effect. The significant level was set as p < 0.05 with whole-brain family-wise error (FWE) correction at both the maintenance subtask and the manipulation WM subtask. Then, we compared the main effect of diagnosis at stress maintenance, non-stress maintenance, stress manipulation, and non-stress manipulation separately in SPM12 to understand the group differences more specifically under each WM condition. The second-level analyses were carried on without any brain mask.

Statistical Analyses of the Clinical and Behavioral Data

We used a standard statistical package (IBM SPSS 26.0, Chicago, IL) to analyze demographic and clinical data, including t-test and χ2 test. The behavioral data [accuracy rate and reaction time (RT)] of two groups at the maintenance or manipulation phase under stress or non-stress setting were analyzed by SPSS to explore the diagnosis × stress × task–difference interaction effect.

We first extracted the β estimations in the corresponding contrast images of each condition and setting in each group for the peak coordinates found in the aforementioned second-level brain image analyses. Then, we analyzed correlation between the β estimations values of the MDD group and the clinical variables (HAMD-17, CTQ). The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Results

Demographic and Behavioral Results

We studied 37 MDD patients and 54 HCs who were currently living in Beijing. Both groups had similar gender distribution and had achieved similar educational levels. While HCs were slightly younger (Table 1). We included age as a covariate in subsequent analyses. MDD patients had an average illness duration of 18.28 months, with a mean HAMD-17 score of 24.35. We also obtained the CTQ for MDD patients, and the results are listed in Table 1.

In terms of the accuracy rate, we observed a significant main effect of stress with a higher accuracy rate under the stress task (F = 30.586, p < 0.001), whereas, for task difference, we found a higher accuracy rate under maintenance task (F = 11.598, p < 0.001). The interaction effects of task difference × stress was significant (F = 10.126, p = 0.002; Figure 2). However, no group differences were found between MDD patients and HCs (Table 2). As for RT, we observed a significant main effect of task difference, with longer RT in the manipulation condition (F = 11.473, p = 0.001). Meanwhile, we observed a significant main effect of task-difference, with longer RT in the manipulation condition (F = 362.629, p < 0.001). There were no main effects of group, or interaction effect among the three factors.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Stress and task difference in the whole sample (n = 91). During the WM maintenance condition, trials with stress were associated with relatively increased accuracy (p < 0.001). This effect was not so evident during the WM manipulation condition, resulting in a significant task by stress interaction (p = 0.002).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Description of behavioral performances of MDD patients and HCs.

WM-Related Brain Activation

During each of the WM maintenance and manipulation conditions under stress or non-stress setting in both MDD and HC groups, regions in the prefrontal, parietal, temporal, occipital cortices, and striatum were robustly activated, along with well-established deactivation in areas of default mode network during cognitive task, including the medial PFC (MPFC) and posterior cingulate cortex (p < 0.05, whole-brain FWE correction; Figure 3, Supplementary Tables 1, 2).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Working memory–related brain activity and stress effect in MDD patients and healthy control subjects (data were shown for p < 0.001, uncorrected). Upper left: The brain activity of WM manipulation and maintenance subtasks in MDD group under non-stress setting. Upper middle: The brain activity of WM manipulation and maintenance subtasks in MDD group under stress setting. Upper right: The brain activity of comparison of stress vs. non-stress setting within the MDD group under different subtask patterns. Bottom left: The brain activity of WM manipulation and maintenance subtasks in HC group under non-stress setting. Bottom middle: The brain activity of WM manipulation and maintenance subtasks in HC group under stress setting. Bottom right: The brain activity of comparison of stress vs. non-stress setting within the HC group under different subtask patterns.

Group Differences Under WM Maintenance Condition

Under the WM maintenance condition, the main effect of stress and interaction effect of diagnosis × stress were not significant. However, the main effect of diagnosis was significant in the left fusiform, left postcentral gyrus, middle cingulum, left superior temporal gyrus, and left precuneus (p < 0.05 whole-brain FWE correction, cluster >50; Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Main effect of group difference between MDD patients and HCs (controlling for age, p < 0.05, voxel-wise whole-brain FWE corrected, cluster size >50).

Then, we focused on the group differences under stress setting (p < 0.05, whole-brain FWE correction, cluster >2; Figure 4). MDD patients had reduced activation in the left fusiform (x = −50, y = −60, z = −14, T = 5.60, cluster size = 49). While under non-stress setting (p < 0.05 whole-brain FWE correction, cluster > 2; Figure 4), MDD patients had decreased activation in the right supplementary motor area (SMA) (x = 4, y = 14, z = 62, T = 5.60, cluster size = 13) and left fusiform gyrus (x = −50, y = −60, z = −16, T = 4.92, cluster size = 6).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Group differences under working memory maintenance and manipulation subtasks (data were controlling for age, p < 0.05, voxel-wise whole-brain FWE corrected). Main group differences under the working memory maintenance subtask. (A) The left fusiform gyrus showed decreased activation in MDD patients under the maintenance and manipulation conditions in stress and non-stress settings. (B) The right SMA exhibited reduced activation in MDD patients under the maintenance and manipulation conditions only in non-stress setting. (C) Under stress setting, MDD patients showed less activation in the right APFC in the manipulation condition. (D) In MDD group, and the β estimations of the right APFC peak voxel under stress manipulation subtask were negatively correlated with sex abuse (r = −0.43, p = 0.008) and physical neglect (r = −0.37, p = 0.026) score in the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire.

Group Differences Under WM Manipulation Condition

Under the WM manipulation condition, the main effect of stress and interaction effect of diagnosis × stress were not significant. However, the main effect of diagnosis was significant in many brain regions, including the fusiform gyrus, precuneus, cingulate gyrus, inferior occipital gyrus, culmen in the left hemisphere, and superior frontal gyrus, middle occipital gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, and pyramis in the right hemisphere (p < 0.05 FWE correction, cluster >50; Table 3).

Then, we focused on the group differences under stress setting (p < 0.05, whole-brain FWE correction, cluster > 2; Figure 4), MDD patients showed less activation in the left fusiform (x = −52, y = −60, z = −16, T = 5.12, cluster size = 14), and right APFC (x = 32 y = 60, z = 2, T = 4.93, cluster size = 3). While under non-stress setting, the main effect of diagnosis (p < 0.05 whole-brain FWE correction) lay in the right SMA (x = 4, y = 14, z = 62, T = 5.75, cluster size = 23) and left fusiform gyrus (x = −50, y = −60, z = −16, T = 4.97, cluster size = 7).

Correlation Analysis

In the MDD patients, we did not find any significant correlation between the HAMD-17 score and the β estimations of the peak voxel, which showed significant difference between HCs and MDD patients. While we found the β estimations of the peak voxel in APFC under stress manipulation task (x = 32, y = 60, z = 2) were negatively correlated with CTQ sex abuse (r = −0.43, p = 0.008; Figure 4D). These β estimations were also negatively correlated with CTQ physical neglect (r = −0.37, p = 0.026; Figure 4D), but could not withstand Bonferroni correction.

Discussion

Main Findings

In the current pilot study, we aimed to explore the neural changes in MDD patients by investigating brain function associated with a stress-related WM task. We found that compared to HCs, MDD patients showed comprehensive less brain activation during both the WM maintenance and manipulation conditions. Particularly, we found decreased brain activation in the left fusiform under both stress and non-stress settings in both WM maintenance and manipulation conditions. Notably, the activation in the right SMA showed group differences in both WM maintenance and manipulation conditions under non-stress but not stress setting. We also found a reduced APFC activation in MDD under WM manipulation task under stress setting, which was negatively correlated with the CTQ sex abuse.

Left Fusiform Gyrus

Our study found that the activation of the left fusiform gyrus was decreased in MDD patients in both maintenance and manipulation conditions under non-stress or stress setting. The fusiform gyrus is the most crucial part of the high-level visual cortex, which is associated with the recognition of facial expressions (32, 33), integration of cognitive information, and emotional modulation (34). Previous studies had observed reduced gray matter volume, thinner cortical thickness, and less surface area of the fusiform gyrus in patients with depression (3537). The fusiform gyrus may integrate the emotional and cognitive processes by modulating the visual stimulation processes. Moreover, compared to HCs, the connectivity between the fusiform gyrus and medial orbitofrontal cortex was decreased in MDD patients (38), which suggested that the fusiform gyrus had an indirect effect on the WM function and emotional modulation. In addition, the reduced activation of fusiform gyrus may also be related to the impaired function of the attention biases of negatively emotional stimuli (32, 39). We speculated that in the WM task, the role of the emotional stimuli process was impaired under both stress and non-stress settings or whether patients with MDD saw the face of a competitor or not.

Right Supplementary Motor Area

Meta-analyses showed consistent activation of SMA in the WM task, which indicated that the SMA included in the widespread frontoparietal network was part of the core WM network (4, 40, 41). Besides, the visual attention function of executive function is regulated by the SMA, precentral gyrus and ACC cortical network (42), which benefits the linking of sensory information to the learning and execution of movement sequences (43). Reduced SMA volumes and impairment in implicit motor sequence learning have been observed in MDD patients (44). Meanwhile, Sarkheil et al. (45) found that the SMA was associated with the psychomotor features (such as motor behavior) of depression, and they speculated that the increased functional connectivity between the SMA and other regions might suggest that recruiting more brain resources was needed for completing the more complicated task in MDD patients. Moreover, MDD patients were sensitive to stress (46). In our study, the decreased activation of SMA under non-stress but not stress setting might also support this view. As a negative stimulation, the face of a competitor might affect the visual attention of executive function, which resulted in “they potentially required additional serial mental processing steps” (47). Hence, the function of the SMA in MDD patients might be increased for making compensation in the WM task under stress, which led to increasing the activation of the SMA and reducing the difference compared to HCs.

Anterior Prefrontal Cortex

Studies showed that the APFC (Brodmann area 10, BA10) was involved in WM, episodic memory, prospective memory, and the consideration of multiple relations in the meantime (48, 49). Compared to children with non-depressed mothers, children with depressed mothers showed decreased activation in the APFC during the N-back task (50), which suggested that the activation of APFC might be an endophenotype of depression. Besides, the activation of the APFC has been observed in relatively simple tasks and would increase with the difficulty load of WM task in healthy subjects (51). Moreover, a prior work reported that higher activation in the APFC was related to WM and choice-difficulty effects associated with self-control (52). Besides, APFC is a brain region that is sensitive to stress (53), and subjects with posttrauma stress disorder showed decreased gray matter volume in APFC compared with control subjects (54). Hence, MDD patients might keep the same cognitive control function with HCs under the non-stress setting, or in the maintenance condition in the stress setting. However, facing both stress and manipulation subtask, which means more choice difficulties, the cognitive control function in MDD patients was decreased compared to HCs.

Childhood Sex Abuse and APFC Activation

Childhood maltreatment, symptoms of negative emotionality, poor friend support, and externalizing problems in childhood and adolescence are risk factors for early-onset MDD patients (55, 56). Besides, childhood maltreatment can cause a series of physiological and neurohumoral reactions, including reduced volumes in the prefrontal cortex (57), and may lead individuals to being susceptible to depression (5860). Previous studies exhibited that the volumes of ventromedial PFC and rostral prefrontal cortex were reduced in children and adults who suffered from physical and sexual abuse (61, 62). Furthermore, compared to individuals without childhood maltreatment, the activity in the MPFC was decreased in individuals who suffered from childhood maltreatment during emotional and neutral memory encoding and recognition (22). Meanwhile, substance abuse and stress can bring about long-lasting changes by modulating of gene expression or epigenetic mechanisms in the brain, and indeed an abnormal pattern of genome-wide DNA methylation in APFC of subjects with alcohol use disorder (63). In our study, childhood sex abuse might disrupt the function of APFC in direct and indirect ways, such as brain activation and DNA methylation, and then contribute to the development of depression.

Limitations

There are several potential limitations in our study. First, the sample size was not large enough. Therefore, it was only a pilot study. In the future, we need to enlarge the sample to test and verify the current findings. Second, we did not distinguish the depression subtypes, which might be the reason why there was no correlation between the β estimations of peak voxel and the HAMD-17 score. Therefore, we need to investigate the differences among different subtypes of depression and study the relationship between clinical symptoms and underline mechanism in a larger sample.

Conclusion

In our pilot study, the decreased brain activation of the left fusiform gyrus, SMA, and APFC helps us to understand the abnormalities of the emotional stimuli processing function, executive function, and cognitive control function in MDD. Childhood maltreatment might play a crucial role in the development of MDD. Although, the findings of this study might not be conclusive, they could provide some information for other researchers. In the future, we need to explore the impaired brain circuits under stress, including the function and connection between the brain regions, which were found in our article in a larger sample.

Data Availability Statement

The data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committee of the Peking University Sixth Hospital. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

WY and DZ designed and supervised the study. HT designed the task. MM, YZ, XZ, and YS recruited subjects and performed the study. MM, YZ, and XZ organized data. MM and XZ analyzed the data and wrote the paper. HY, HT, and WY gave instruction for the analysis and modified the paper. All collaborators reviewed and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by National Key R&D Program of China (2016YFC1307000, 2017YFC1311100), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (81825009, 81901358, 81221002, and 82001416), Academy of Medical Sciences Research Unit (2019-I2M-5-006), and Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing (2020-NKX-XM-12).11

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank National Center for Protein Sciences at Peking University in Beijing, China, for assistance with MRI data acquisition.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.671574/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Smith K. Mental health: a world of depression. Nature. (2014) 515:181. doi: 10.1038/515180a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Hammar A, Ardal G. Cognitive functioning in major depression–a summary. Front Hum Neurosci. (2009) 3:26. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.026.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Hasselbalch BJ, Knorr U, Kessing LV. Cognitive impairment in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. (2011) 134:20–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, Reetz K, Laird AR, Schulz JB, et al. Modelling neural correlates of working memory: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. Neuroimage. (2012) 60:830–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Marazziti D, Consoli G, Picchetti M, Carlini M, Faravelli L. Cognitive impairment in major depression. Eur J Pharmacol. (2010) 626:83–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.08.046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Christopher G, MacDonald J. The impact of clinical depression on working memory. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2005) 10:379–99. doi: 10.1080/13546800444000128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fitzgerald PB, Srithiran A, Benitez J, Daskalakis ZZ, Oxley TJ, Kulkarni J, et al. An fMRI study of prefrontal brain activation during multiple tasks in patients with major depressive disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. (2008) 29:490–501. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Zweerings J, Zvyagintsev M, Turetsky BI, Klasen M, Konig AA, Roecher E, et al. Fronto-parietal and temporal brain dysfunction in depression: a fMRI investigation of auditory mismatch processing. Hum Brain Mapp. (2019) 40:3657–68. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Walter H, Wolf RC, Spitzer M, Vasic N. Increased left prefrontal activation in patients with unipolar depression: an event-related, parametric, performance-controlled fMRI study. J Affect Disord. (2007) 101:175–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Siegle GJ, Thompson W, Carter CS, Steinhauer SR, Thase ME. Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: related and independent features. Biol Psychiatry. (2007) 61:198–209. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Rose EJ, Simonotto E, Ebmeier KP. Limbic over-activity in depression during preserved performance on the n-back task. Neuroimage. (2006) 29:203–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Fitzgerald PB, Laird AR, Maller J, Daskalakis ZJ. A meta-analytic study of changes in brain activation in depression. Hum Brain Mapp. (2008) 29:683–95. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Pu S, Yamada T, Yokoyama K, Matsumura H, Kobayashi H, Sasaki N, et al. A multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy study of prefrontal cortex activation during working memory task in major depressive disorder. Neurosci Res. (2011) 70:91–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2011.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Hugdahl K, Rund BR, Lund A, Asbjornsen A, Egeland J, Ersland L, et al. Brain activation measured with fMRI during a mental arithmetic task in schizophrenia and major depression. Am J Psychiatry. (2004) 161:286–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Brissenden JA, Somers DC. Cortico-cerebellar networks for visual attention and working memory. Curr Opin Psychol. (2019) 29:239–47. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Park C, Rosenblat JD, Brietzke E, Pan Z, Lee Y, Cao B, et al. Stress, epigenetics, and depression: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 102:139–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Teicher MH, Samson JA, Anderson CM, Ohashi K. The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2016) 17:652–66. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Short AK, Baram TZ. Early-life adversity and neurological disease: age-old questions and novel answers. Nat Rev Neurol. (2019) 15:657–69. doi: 10.1038/s41582-019-0246-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Saleh A, Potter GG, McQuoid DR, Boyd B, Turner R, MacFall JR, et al. Effects of early life stress on depression, cognitive performance and brain morphology. Psychol Med. (2017) 47:171–81. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716002403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Treadway MT, Waskom ML, Dillon DG, Holmes AJ, Park MTM, Chakravarty MM, et al. Illness progression, recent stress, and morphometry of hippocampal subfields and medial prefrontal cortex in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. (2015) 77:285–94. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hanson JL, Chung MK, Avants BB, Rudolph KD, Shirtcliff EA, Gee JC, et al. Structural variations in prefrontal cortex mediate the relationship between early childhood stress and spatial working memory. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:7917–25. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0307-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. van Harmelen AL, van Tol MJ, Dalgleish T, van der Wee NJ, Veltman DJ, Aleman A, et al. Hypoactive medial prefrontal cortex functioning in adults reporting childhood emotional maltreatment. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2014) 9:2026–33. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Montalvo-Ortiz JL, Bordner KA, Carlyle BC, Gelernter J, Simen AA, Kaufman J. The role of genes involved in stress, neural plasticity, and brain circuitry in depressive phenotypes: convergent findings in a mouse model of neglect. Behav Brain Res. (2016) 315:71–4. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Zhang X, Yan H, Yu H, Zhao X, Shah S, Dong Z, et al. Childhood urbanization affects prefrontal cortical responses to trait anxiety and interacts with polygenic risk for depression. bioRxiv [Preprint]. (2019):246876. doi: 10.1101/246876

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Li Q, Yan J, Liao J, Zhang X, Liu L, Fu X, et al. Distinct effects of social stress on working memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neurosci Bull. (2021) 37:81–93. doi: 10.1007/s12264-020-00579-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. (1998) 59(Suppl. 20):22–33; quiz 4–57.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

27. Maffei C, Fossati A, Agostoni I, Barraco A, Bagnato M, Deborah D, et al. Interrater reliability and internal consistency of the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis II personality disorders (SCID-II), version 2.0. J Pers Disord. (1997) 11:279–84. doi: 10.1521/pedi.1997.11.3.279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hamilton M. Rating depressive patients. J Clin Psychiatry. (1980) 41(12 Pt. 2):21–4.

Google Scholar

29. Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, Foote J, Lovejoy M, Wenzel K, et al. Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. Am J Psychiatry. (1994) 151:1132–6. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.8.1132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Tan HY, Chen Q, Goldberg TE, Mattay VS, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Weinberger DR, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met modulation of prefrontal-parietal-striatal brain systems during arithmetic and temporal transformations in working memory. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:13393–401. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4041-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Tan HY, Chen AG, Kolachana B, Apud JA, Mattay VS, Callicott JH, et al. Effective connectivity of AKT1-mediated dopaminergic working memory networks and pharmacogenetics of anti-dopaminergic treatment. Brain. (2012) 135(Pt. 5):1436–45. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Surguladze S, Brammer MJ, Keedwell P, Giampietro V, Young AW, Travis MJ, et al. A differential pattern of neural response toward sad versus happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:201–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Weiner KS, Zilles K. The anatomical and functional specialization of the fusiform gyrus. Neuropsychologia. (2016) 83:48–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Arnone D, Job D, Selvaraj S, Abe O, Amico F, Cheng Y, et al. Computational meta-analysis of statistical parametric maps in major depression. Hum Brain Mapp. (2016) 37:1393–404. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Yrondi A, Nemmi F, Billoux S, Giron A, Sporer M, Taib S, et al. Grey matter changes in treatment-resistant depression during electroconvulsive therapy. J Affect Disord. (2019) 258:42–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.07.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Brunye TT, Moran JM, Holmes A, Mahoney CR, Taylor HA. Non-invasive brain stimulation targeting the right fusiform gyrus selectively increases working memory for faces. Brain Cogn. (2017) 113:32–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Grill-Spector K, Weiner KS. The functional architecture of the ventral temporal cortex and its role in categorization. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2014) 15:536–48. doi: 10.1038/nrn3747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Rolls ET, Cheng W, Du J, Wei D, Qiu J, Dai D, et al. Functional connectivity of the right inferior frontal gyrus and orbitofrontal cortex in depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2020) 15:75–86. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Maggioni E, Delvecchio G, Grottaroli M, Garzitto M, Piccin S, Bonivento C, et al. Common and different neural markers in major depression and anxiety disorders: a pilot structural magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2019) 290:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2019.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cona G, Semenza C. Supplementary motor area as key structure for domain-general sequence processing: a unified account. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 72:28–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. (2005) 25:46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Ottowitz WE, Dougherty DD, Savage CR. The neural network basis for abnormalities of attention and executive function in major depressive disorder: implications for application of the medical disease model to psychiatric disorders. Harv Rev Psychiatry. (2002) 10:86–99. doi: 10.1080/10673220216210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Gilbert SJ, Burgess PW. Executive function. Curr Biol. (2008) 18:R110–4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.12.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Johansen-Berg H, Behrens TE, Robson MD, Drobnjak I, Rushworth MF, Brady JM, et al. Changes in connectivity profiles define functionally distinct regions in human medial frontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2004) 101:13335–40. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403743101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Sarkheil P, Odysseos P, Bee I, Zvyagintsev M, Neuner I, Mathiak K. Functional connectivity of supplementary motor area during finger-tapping in major depression. Compr Psychiatry. (2020) 99:152166. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. van Winkel M, Nicolson NA, Wichers M, Viechtbauer W, Myin-Germeys I, Peeters F. Daily life stress reactivity in remitted versus non-remitted depressed individuals. Eur Psychiatry. (2015) 30:441–7. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Fehr T, Code C, Herrmann M. Common brain regions underlying different arithmetic operations as revealed by conjunct fMRI-BOLD activation. Brain Res. (2007) 1172:93–102. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.07.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Gilbert SJ, Spengler S, Simons JS, Steele JD, Lawrie SM, Frith CD, et al. Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): a meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. (2006) 18:932–48. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Velanova K, Jacoby LL, Wheeler ME, McAvoy MP, Petersen SE, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic correlates of sustained and transient processing components engaged during controlled retrieval. J Neurosci. (2003) 23:8460–70. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-24-08460.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Reising MM, Bettis AH, Dunbar JP, Watson KH, Gruhn M, Hoskinson KR, et al. Stress, coping, executive function, and brain activation in adolescent offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Child Neuropsychol. (2018) 24:638–56. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2017.1307950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Chahine G, Diekhof EK, Tinnermann A, Gruber O. On the role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in cognitive 'branching': an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. (2015) 77:421–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Jimura K, Chushak MS, Braver TS. Impulsivity and self-control during intertemporal decision making linked to the neural dynamics of reward value representation. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:344–57. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0919-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Koolschijn PC, van Haren NE, Lensvelt-Mulders GJ, Hulshoff Pol HE, Kahn RS. Brain volume abnormalities in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. (2009) 30:3719–35. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Bromis K, Calem M, Reinders A, Williams SCR, Kempton MJ. Meta-analysis of 89 structural MRI studies in posttraumatic stress disorder and comparison with major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2018) 175:989–98. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17111199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Wilson S, Vaidyanathan U, Miller MB, McGue M, Iacono WG. Premorbid risk factors for major depressive disorder: are they associated with early onset and recurrent course? Dev Psychopathol. (2014) 26(4 Pt. 2):1477–93. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414001151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Hill RM, Pettit JW, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR, Klein DN. Escalation to major depressive disorder among adolescents with subthreshold depressive symptoms: evidence of distinct subgroups at risk. J Affect Disord. (2014) 158:133–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Shifflett H, Iyengar S, Beers SR, Hall J, et al. Brain structures in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: a sociodemographically matched study. Biol Psychiatry. (2002) 52:1066–78. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01459-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Opel N, Redlich R, Dohm K, Zaremba D, Goltermann J, Repple J, et al. Mediation of the influence of childhood maltreatment on depression relapse by cortical structure: a 2-year longitudinal observational study. Lancet Psychiatry. (2019) 6:318–26. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30044-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Opel N, Redlich R, Zwanzger P, Grotegerd D, Arolt V, Heindel W, et al. Hippocampal atrophy in major depression: a function of childhood maltreatment rather than diagnosis? Neuropsychopharmacology. (2014) 39:2723–31. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Kelly PA, Viding E, Wallace GL, Schaer M, De Brito SA, Robustelli B, et al. Cortical thickness, surface area, and gyrification abnormalities in children exposed to maltreatment: neural markers of vulnerability? Biol Psychiatry. (2013) 74:845–52. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Busso DS, McLaughlin KA, Brueck S, Peverill M, Gold AL, Sheridan MA. Child abuse, neural structure, and adolescent psychopathology: a longitudinal study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2017) 56:321–8.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Edmiston EE, Wang F, Mazure CM, Guiney J, Sinha R, Mayes LC, et al. Corticostriatal-limbic gray matter morphology in adolescents with self-reported exposure to childhood maltreatment. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2011) 165:1069–77. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Gatta E, Grayson DR, Auta J, Saudagar V, Dong E, Chen Y, et al. Genome-wide methylation in alcohol use disorder subjects: implications for an epigenetic regulation of the cortico-limbic glucocorticoid receptors (NR3C1). Mol Psychiatry. (2019) 26:1029–41. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0449-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stress, major depression disorder, working memory, anterior prefrontal cortex, childhood maltreatment

Citation: Ma M, Zhang X, Zhang Y, Su Y, Yan H, Tan H, Zhang D and Yue W (2021) Childhood Maltreatment Was Correlated With the Decreased Cortical Function in Depressed Patients Under Social Stress in a Working Memory Task: A Pilot Study. Front. Psychiatry 12:671574. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.671574

Received: 25 February 2021; Accepted: 25 May 2021;
Published: 08 July 2021.

Edited by:

Gianluca Serafini, San Martino Hospital (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Alberto Forte, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Xueqin Song, Zhengzhou University, China

Copyright © 2021 Ma, Zhang, Zhang, Su, Yan, Tan, Zhang and Yue. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Weihua Yue, dryue@bjmu.edu.cn; Xiao Zhang, zhangx@bjmu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.