Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 11 March 2021
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Biotyping in Psychiatry View all 10 articles

The Auditory Steady-State Response: Electrophysiological Index for Sensory Processing Dysfunction in Psychiatric Disorders

\nShunsuke Sugiyama
Shunsuke Sugiyama1*Kazutaka Ohi
Kazutaka Ohi1*Ayumi KuramitsuAyumi Kuramitsu1Kentaro TakaiKentaro Takai1Yukimasa MutoYukimasa Muto1Tomoya TaniguchiTomoya Taniguchi2Tomoaki KinukawaTomoaki Kinukawa2Nobuyuki TakeuchiNobuyuki Takeuchi3Eishi MotomuraEishi Motomura4Makoto NishiharaMakoto Nishihara5Toshiki ShioiriToshiki Shioiri1Koji InuiKoji Inui6
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Gifu University Graduate School of Medicine, Gifu, Japan
  • 2Department of Anesthesiology, Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Nagoya, Japan
  • 3Depatment of Psychiatry, Aichi Medical University, Nagakute, Japan
  • 4Department of Neuropsychiatry, Mie University Graduate School of Medicine, Tsu, Japan
  • 5Multidisciplinary Pain Center, Aichi Medical University, Nagakute, Japan
  • 6Departmernt of Functioning and Disability, Institute for Developmental Research, Kasugai, Japan

Sensory processing is disrupted in several psychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and autism spectrum disorder. In this review, we focus on the electrophysiological auditory steady-state response (ASSR) driven by high-frequency stimulus trains as an index for disease-associated sensory processing deficits. The ASSR amplitude is suppressed within the gamma band (≥30 Hz) among these patients, suggesting an imbalance between GABAergic and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-mediated neurotransmission. The reduced power and synchronization of the 40-Hz ASSR are robust in patients with schizophrenia. In recent years, similar ASSR deficits at gamma frequencies have also been reported in patients with bipolar disorder and autism spectrum disorder. We summarize ASSR abnormalities in each of these psychiatric disorders and suggest that the observed commonalities reflect shared pathophysiological mechanisms. We reviewed studies on phase resetting in which a salient sensory stimulus affects ASSR. Phase resetting induces the reduction of both the amplitude and phase of ASSR. Moreover, phase resetting is also affected by rare auditory stimulus patterns or superimposed stimuli of other modalities. Thus, sensory memory and multisensory integration can be investigated using phase resetting of ASSR. Here, we propose that ASSR amplitude, phase, and resetting responses are sensitive indices for investigating sensory processing dysfunction in psychiatric disorders.

Introduction

Recent studies have identified multiple shared genetic associations and other commonalities among psychiatric disorders. For example, genome-wide association studies suggest shared molecular pathomechanisms between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder (1, 2), whereas large-scale imaging analyses have revealed similar white matter abnormalities (3) in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorders. Recent genetic (2, 4) and neuroimaging studies (5, 6) have also demonstrated shared molecular and neurostructural abnormalities between schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder. Currently, psychiatric disorders continue to be classified based on observed symptoms rather than underlying pathogenic mechanisms. Classifications such as the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) (7) and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) (8) have contributed to the standardization of diagnoses and treatment in clinical practice; however, they provide little information regarding neurobiological mechanisms and treatment targets. Indeed, overemphasis on differential diagnosis according to symptom clusters and clinical history has revealed little about the pathological mechanisms underlying these psychiatric disorders. Therefore, it is important to investigate common biological abnormalities across multiple psychiatric disorders. To address this issue, the National Institute of Mental Health is currently attempting to construct a biological framework for understanding the etiology and symptomology of psychiatric disorders (9).

A common symptom of multiple psychiatric disorders is sensory processing dysfunction (10, 11). Neurophysiological approaches such as magnetoencephalography (MEG) and electroencephalography (EEG) can reveal the electrical activity of neuronal ensembles at high temporal resolution, thereby providing quantitative indices of illness that also reflect disease-associated abnormalities at the cellular level. In this review, we focus on the auditory steady-state response (ASSR), an electrophysiological response driven by a train of stimuli delivered at a sufficiently high rate. ASSR recorded using MEG or EEG has been reported to reach maximum amplitude at approximately 40 Hz (12, 13). Previous MEG (14) and positron-emission tomography (15) studies have reported that ASSR originates in the primary auditory cortex and associated subcortical areas (16). The ASSR has been interpreted as a reflection of oscillatory gamma-band activity representing auditory objects (1719). Moreover, neural oscillations in the gamma frequency band are believed critical for information processing across cortical networks (20, 21). For example, gamma-band activity increases in the visual (22, 23), auditory (24, 25), and somatosensory cortices (26) in response to modality-specific sensory stimuli. Gamma-band activity is also related to working memory and increases in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex during memory processing (2729). Therefore, gamma-band activity is involved in a wide range of brain activities, from low-level sensory processing to higher cognitive functions. Further, ASSR amplitude and phase are believed to reflect the balance between inhibitory GABAergic activity and excitatory glutamatergic activity mediated by the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor (3032). Thus, ASSR abnormalities as measured by MEG and EEG can reveal aspects of aberrant neurotransmission and neuronal excitation within specific brain circuits.

In 1999, Kwon et al. first demonstrated that patients with schizophrenia showed reduced power and synchronization of the 40-Hz ASSR (33), and subsequent studies by other groups replicated this finding (3437). A meta-analysis also concluded that 40-Hz ASSR deficits are robust in schizophrenia (38). These ASSR deficits are consistent with anatomic abnormalities of the auditory cortex observed by magnetic resonance imaging (39, 40). Such ASSR deficits at gamma frequencies have also been discovered in bipolar disorder (4143) and autism spectrum disorder (44). In this review, we first summarize ASSR abnormalities in each of these psychiatric disorders and discuss the potential commonalities in pathophysiology suggested by these observations. Second, we review studies suggesting that modulation of ASSR amplitude and phase by rare auditory patterns or addition of multimodal stimuli, termed phase resetting, also yield useful index for psychiatric disorders. We propose that ASSR is a sensitive index for investigating sensory memory and multisensory integration deficits in psychiatric disorders.

ASSR Deficits in Psychiatric Disorders

Schizophrenia

Most studies documenting ASSR deficits in schizophrenia have been conducted in the chronic disease phase, suggesting a relationship with symptom expression. Intriguingly, however, reduced evoked power and phase locking of the 40-Hz ASSR have also been documented in first-episode patients (35), high-risk individuals before the onset of psychosis (37), and in first-degree relatives (45, 46). In contrast, individuals with schizotypal personality disorder did not exhibit ASSR deficits (46, 47). These findings suggest that these ASSR deficits reflect pathological development independent of disease course or the side effects of long-term antipsychotic medication.

These ASSR deficits are most consistently observed at 40 Hz, whereas responses are usually intact at 20 and 30 Hz [although reduced ASSR at 30 Hz (35) and enhanced ASSR at 20 Hz (48) have been reported]. Recent studies have also reported impaired evoked ASSR power and phase locking at 80 Hz in schizophrenia (36, 49), and these abnormalities were associated with the severity of hallucinations (36) and negative symptoms, such as flat affect, anhedonia, and poverty of speech (49). Tada et al. reported that deficits in the 40-Hz ASSR during a 300–500 ms train were associated with more severe clinical symptoms and cognitive deficits (37). Moreover, patients with schizophrenia taking new generation antipsychotics exhibited significantly increased 40-Hz ASSR synchronization (45). Collectively, these findings indicate that ASSR may also be a useful quantitative index for current clinical symptoms and treatment response.

Bipolar Disorder

Patients with bipolar disorder show a pattern of ASSR deficits similar to that of patients with schizophrenia. To our knowledge, O'Donnell et al. first reported reduced evoked ASSR power at 20, 30, 40, and 50 Hz as well as reduced phase synchronization at 20, 40, and 50 Hz among patients with unmedicated bipolar disorder during manic or mixed episodes using EEG (41). Such ASSR deficits have also been documented in depressive (42), euthymic (43), and manic (41) states in the first episode (35) and the chronic state (4143) and in both medicated (42, 43) and unmedicated patients (41).

In contrast to a comparative group of patients with bipolar disorder, no ASSR deficits were observed in a parallel group with major depressive disorder (50). In fact, to our knowledge, only one study has reported ASSR deficits in major depressive disorder (51), and reduced ASSR power was found at 30 Hz but not at 40 Hz as observed in patients with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia (51). These findings suggest that major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder have distinct neurophysiological bases and further that 40-Hz ASSR can be used to distinguish bipolar disorder from major depressive disorder (50).

Autism Spectrum Disorder

Wilson et al. first reported reduced 40-Hz ASSR power in 7–17-year-old children and adolescents with autism using MEG (52), with a greater reduction in the left hemisphere. Thereafter, Rojas et al. found reduced evoked power and phase locking of left and right 40-Hz ASSRs among both adults with autism and parents of children with autism (53), suggesting that ASSR is a useful index for diagnosis and risk evaluation. However, utility may be limited to adults as ASSR amplitude increases from childhood through adolescence and plateaus in early adulthood (54). Further, no significant deficits in 20- and 40-Hz ASSRs were found among 5–7-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder (55).

Shared Pathophysiology Among Psychiatric Disorders

Patients with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and autism spectrum disorder demonstrate similar patterns of ASSR deficits, suggesting shared neural circuit dysfunction. One emerging hypothesis is that ASSR deficits reflect dysfunction of the GABAergic and/or NMDAergic systems. Blockers of NMDA receptors, such as phencyclidine and ketamine, evoke psychotic symptoms in healthy individuals, exacerbate positive symptoms in patients with schizophrenia, and induce various schizotypic electrophysiological and behavioral abnormalities in experimental animal models (56). For instance, Sohal et al. demonstrated that optogenetic downregulation of parvalbumin-positive GABAergic interneuron activity in mice reduced gamma-band oscillations (57), whereas Sivarao et al. reported that the 40-Hz ASSR in awake rats depended on the degree of NMDA receptor channel blockade (30). Collectively, these findings are consistent with evidence implicating GABA (58) and/or NMDA (59) transmission impairment in schizophrenia.

Post-mortem brain studies of patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder have also reported reduced interneuron density in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus (60). Similar to the GABAergic dysfunction in bipolar disorder is the therapeutic efficacy of the mood stabilizer valproate, which has been shown to increase GABA turnover in rat brain (61). Moreover, valproate has been reported to increase GABA levels in human plasma, suggesting that it enhances GABA activity in the central nervous system (62). However, poor understanding of the mechanism of action of valproate in bipolar disorder is a limitation (63), and valproate is not effective in treating schizophrenia or autism spectrum disorder, despite sharing the GABAergic dysfunction hypothesis. A recent study of induced pluripotent stem cell-derived organoids from patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder found enhanced GABAergic specification (64), suggesting that the reduction in GABAergic neurons observed after disease onset is a compensatory response to maintain the excitatory/inhibitory balance within neural circuits during cortical development.

Conversely, 40-Hz ASSR deficits have not been observed in patients with major depressive disorder. Hirano et al. showed that spontaneous gamma band activity is high in patients with schizophrenia and that the degree of 40-Hz ASSR deficits was associated with increased spontaneous gamma-band activity (65). Moreover, ketamine, an NMDA receptor antagonist, was effective in treating depression (66) and increases resting-state gamma-band activity (67). Therefore, patients with major depressive disorder, in contrast to those with schizophrenia, may have reduced spontaneous gamma-band activity, and consequently, ASSR deficits may not have been observed. However, spontaneous gamma-band activity has not yet been investigated in patients with major depressive disorder. The number of reports on ASSR in major depressive disorder is small, and similarities and differences with other diseases that have ASSR deficits need to be discussed in the future.

Dysfunction of the GABAergic system has also been implicated in autism spectrum disorder. For example, multiple mouse models of autism established via toxins or manipulation of associated genes exhibit reduced number of neocortical parvalbumin-positive inhibitory neurons (68). A post-mortem study also reported reduced GABA-synthesizing enzymes in parietal and cerebellar cortices of patients with autism spectrum disorder (69), whereas a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study reported reduced GABA concentration in the auditory and frontal cortices of living patients (70). These GABAergic deficits may result in a relative excess of glutamatergic activity. Indeed, Fatemi's hyper-glutamatergic hypothesis of autism spectrum disorder posits that deficits in GABA-synthesizing enzymes and increased GABA uptake by astrocytes led to excess cortical glutamate (71).

Autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia also share behavioral symptoms such as difficulties with social cognition, social interaction, and executive functions (72). In fact, autism spectrum disorder was initially believed to be an early stage of schizophrenia (73). Furthermore, an altered ratio of excitatory to inhibitory cortical activity has been reported in both autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia (74). Yizhar et al. demonstrated that psychosocial dysfunction, a trait common to both disorders, was associated with increased excitation/inhibition ratio in mouse prefrontal cortex (75). Therefore, understanding the causes of excitation/inhibition imbalance could provide clues to the pathophysiology of these disorders as well as to novel treatment strategies. Further, ASSR could be a sensitive electrophysiological indicator reflecting the excitation/inhibition imbalance common among schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and autism spectrum disorder.

Perspectives on Neurophysiological Research Using Phase Resetting of ASSR

Phase resetting is a phenomenon that occurs when a stimulus perturbs the phase within a neural oscillation. Resetting the phase of ongoing neural oscillation induces the synchronization of different neurons or brain regions (76). Phase resetting is the fundamental mechanism underlying synchronization, and neural synchronization is believed to play a role in information processing (77), neuronal communication (78), motor coordination (79), and memory (80). For example, in clinical research, epilepsy is considered a disease that results from neuronal hyper-synchronization (81). The generation of resting tremor in Parkinson's disease has been suggested to be owing to abnormal synchronization of neuronal activity (82). In schizophrenia, the disruption of neural synchronization is believed to be related to fragmented cognitive experience (83).

A salient sensory stimulus on ASSR causes phase resetting that modulates the amplitude and phase (Figure 1A) Rohrbaugh et al. first reported that a foreground auditory stimulus reduced both the amplitude and latency of a 40-Hz ASSR evoked by a background rhythmic probe stimulus (8486). In addition, phase resetting of the 40-Hz ASSR has been reported following a sudden change in stimulus frequency or intensity (87). In a study using an oddball paradigm, button pressing in response to a rare stimulus also caused phase resetting of the 40-Hz ASSR (88). Furthermore, Ross et al. reported that the ASSR was modulated by changing stimulus onset (19), violating the periodicity of a sound stimulus (89), and introducing an interfering stimulus (90). These findings suggest that perturbing stimuli reset the oscillations and shift the ASSR phase back to that of the driving source (90).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Phase resetting of the auditory steady-state response (ASSR). (A) Modulation of the ASSR by stimulus intensity (sound pressure). Repeated presentation of a 25-ms pure tone (upper middle) elicits the 40-Hz ASSR. An abrupt increase in sound pressure at 700 ms causes a reduction in amplitude and phase (phase resetting). The location of estimated dipoles (left panel), source-strength waveforms (middle), and enlarged waveforms on an expanded time axis (right) are also shown. (B) Increasing sound pressure reduces ASSR latency. The Y-axis shows changes in the peak latency interval over time relative to the control condition. The control stimulus is a 1,000-ms train of clicks at 40 Hz. The test stimuli are a 500-ms train of clicks identical to the control stimulus and a subsequent 500-ms click-train of the same frequency but altered sound pressure compared with the control stimulus (−5, −10, −15, 5, 10, or 15 dB). The degree of phase resetting depends on the magnitude of the sound pressure change. (C) Modulation of the ASSR by deviant stimuli (odd ball condition). The Y-axis shows changes in the peak latency interval over time compared with the control-only (left) and test-only (right) conditions. The control stimulus is a 1,200-ms train of 25-ms pure tones. The test stimulus is a similar train of pure tones in which the tone sound pressure at 700 ms is increased by 15 dB. Under an oddball paradigm, phase resetting is observed when either the control or test stimulus is rare (deviant). (D) Modulation of the ASSR by multimodal stimulation. The Y-axis shows changes in the peak latency interval over time compared with the control condition. As the test stimulus, an electrical pulse is presented to the dorsum of the left or right hand at 700 ms during the train of 25-ms pure tones. Tactile stimulation causes phase resetting of the ASSR, and this cross-modal effect is observed from approximately 50–125 ms after the onset of tactile stimulation.

Our recent study indicated that increasing the sound pressure can induce a proportionate reduction in ASSR latency (Figure 1B) (91). We also demonstrated that ASSR latency can be shortened without changing the physical characteristics of the peripheral input (92). Using an oddball paradigm, we found that a control stimulus with unchanging sequence shortened the ASSR latency when presented with a low probability among other stimulus patterns (Figure 1C). These findings indicate that ASSR phase resetting can be induced by an intrinsic comparison process based on sensory memory. Sensory memory impairment has been reported in several neurological and psychiatric disorders, primarily using mismatch negativity (MMN) (93), a negative component of the event-related potential elicited by a deviant stimulus embedded in repetitive stimuli (an oddball paradigm), with maximum negativity at Fz and positivity at the mastoid (94). Mismatch negativity reflects the automatic change detection process based on short-term sensory memory and thus serves as an index of sensory memory disruption (95). For example, patients with schizophrenia (96, 97), autism spectrum disorder (98), and Alzheimer's disease (99) have all demonstrated smaller auditory MMN waveforms than healthy controls. Although previous studies have reported that ASSR is modulated by selective attention (100, 101), our paradigms (91, 92), such as oddball paradigms which are typically used to detect MMN, do not require conditions of attention. Changes in ASSR during such odd ball paradigms (91, 92) may facilitate efficient assessment of sensory memory impairments in psychiatric disorders because such measurements do not require multiple stimulus repetitions, thereby reducing experimental time and patient burden.

We also recently demonstrated reduced ASSR latency by simultaneous tactile stimulation (Figure 1D) (102), strongly suggesting that cross-modal input increases the speed of ongoing auditory processing. This cross-modal ASSR paradigm may thus permit the assessment of multimodal sensory integration with high test–retest reliability (103). Moreover, the 40-Hz ASSR is considered superior for providing information on processing speed compared with other sensory paradigms because peak latency can be measured reliably every 12.5 ms. Indeed, our findings of reduced ASSR latency during multimodal stimulation are consistent with previous studies demonstrating faster object recognition using both auditory and visual features compared with either modality alone and with the appearance of unique early-onset multimodal ERP waveforms originating from both sensory and frontal cortex (104, 105). Although previous studies have shown impaired multisensory integration in patients with schizophrenia (106) and autism spectrum disorder (107), psychophysical rather than neurophysiological indicators were assessed. We suggest that the ASSR serves as a robust electrophysiological index of multisensory integration deficits in psychiatric disorders.

Conclusion

Patients with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and autism spectrum disorder all exhibit deficits in the ASSR at gamma-band frequencies, suggesting shared pathomechanisms including dysregulation of cortical excitatory/inhibitory balance. Moreover, ASSR magnitude and phase reflect auditory memory, multimodal sensory integration, and the comparison of incoming sensory stimuli with previous memory traces. Thus, ASSR could be a sensitive electrophysiological index for sensory processing deficits in psychiatric disorders.

Author Contributions

SS conducted the literature review, SS and KI drafted the manuscript, SS and EM created the figure, AK, KT, YM, TT, TK, NT, MN, and TS provided valuable critical input on the manuscript, and KO edited the final draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP20K16624 to SS.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Lee SH, Ripke SH, Neale BM, Faraone SV, Purcell SM, Perlis RH, et al. Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat Genet. (2013) 45:984–94. doi: 10.1038/ng.2711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet. (2013) 381:1371–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62129-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Koshiyama D, Fukunaga M, Okada N, Morita K, Nemoto K, Usui K, et al. White matter microstructural alterations across four major psychiatric disorders: mega-analysis study in 2937 individuals. Mol Psychiatry. (2020) 25:883–95. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0553-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Geschwind DH, Flint J. Genetics and genomics of psychiatric disease. Science. (2015) 349:1489–94. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa8954

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Pinkham AE, Hopfinger JB, Pelphrey KA, Piven J, Penn DL. Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res. (2008) 99:164–75. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Sugranyes G, Kyriakopoulos M, Corrigall R, Taylor E, Frangou S. Autism spectrum disorders and schizophrenia: meta-analysis of the neural correlates of social cognition. PLOS ONE. (2011) 6:e25322. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases ICD-10. 10th ed. Geneva: World Health Organization (1992).

Google Scholar

8. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

9. Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, et al. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry. (2010) 167:748–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kas MJ, Fernandes C, Schalkwyk LC, Collier DA. Genetics of behavioural domains across the neuropsychiatric spectrum; of mice and men. Mol Psychiatry. (2007) 12:324–30. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Harrison LA, Kats A, Williams ME, Aziz-Zadeh L. The importance of sensory processing in mental health: a proposed addition to the research domain criteria (RDoC) and suggestions for RDoC 2.0. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:103. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Galambos R, Makeig S, Talmachoff PJ. A 40-Hz auditory potential recorded from the human scalp. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (1981) 78:2643–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.78.4.2643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ross B, Borgmann C, Draganova R, Roberts LE, Pantev C. A high-precision magnetoencephalographic study of human auditory steady-state responses to amplitude-modulated tones. J Acoust Soc Am. (2000) 108:679–91. doi: 10.1121/1.429600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Ross B. A novel type of auditory responses: temporal dynamics of 40-Hz steady-state responses induced by changes in sound localization. J Neurophysiol. (2008) 100:1265–77. doi: 10.1152/jn.00048.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Pastor MA, Artieda J, Arbizu J, Marti-Climent JM, Peñuelas I, Masdeu JC. Activation of human cerebral and cerebellar cortex by auditory stimulation at 40 Hz. J Neurosci. (2002) 22:10501–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-23-10501.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Herdman AT, Lins O, Van Roon P, Stapells DR, Scherg M, Picton TW. Intracerebral sources of human auditory steady-state responses. Brain Topogr. (2002) 15:69–86. doi: 10.1023/A:1021470822922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Santarelli R, Maurizi M, Conti G, Ottaviani F, Paludetti G, Pettorossi VE. Generation of human auditory steady-state responses (SSRs). II: addition of responses to individual stimuli. Hear Res. (1995) 83:9–18. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(94)00185-S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Santarelli R, Conti G. Generation of auditory steady-state responses: linearity assessment. Scand Audiol Suppl. (1999) 51:23–32.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

19. Ross B, Picton TW, Pantev C. Temporal integration in the human auditory cortex as represented by the development of the steady-state magnetic field. Hear Res. (2002) 165:68–84. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00285-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Uhlhaas PJ, Haenschel C, Nikolić D, Singer W. The role of oscillations and synchrony in cortical networks and their putative relevance for the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2008) 34:927–43. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Bosman CA, Lansink CS, Pennartz CM. Functions of gamma-band synchronization in cognition: from single circuits to functional diversity across cortical and subcortical systems. Eur J Neurosci. (2014) 39:1982–99. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gray CM, König P, Engel AK, Singer W. Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature. (1989) 338:334–7. doi: 10.1038/338334a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O. Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends Cogn Sci. (1999) 3:151–62. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01299-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Fukushima M, Saunders RC, Leopold DA, Mishkin M, Averbeck BB. Spontaneous high-gamma band activity reflects functional organization of auditory cortex in the awake macaque. Neuron. (2012) 74:899–910. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Polomac N, Leicht G, Nolte G, Andreou C, Schneider TR, Steinmann S, et al. Generators and connectivity of the early auditory evoked gamma band response. Brain Topogr. (2015) 28:865–78. doi: 10.1007/s10548-015-0434-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Faivre N, Dönz J, Scandola M, Dhanis H, Bello Ruiz J, Bernasconi F, et al. Self-grounded vision: hand ownership modulates visual location through cortical β and γ oscillations. J Neurosci. (2017) 37:11–22. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0563-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Carr MF, Karlsson MP, Frank LM. Transient slow gamma synchrony underlies hippocampal memory replay. Neuron. (2012) 75:700–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lundqvist M, Rose J, Herman P, Brincat SL, Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Gamma and beta bursts underlie working memory. Neuron. (2016) 90:152–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Yamamoto J, Suh J, Takeuchi D, Tonegawa S. Successful execution of working memory linked to synchronized high-frequency gamma oscillations. Cell. (2014) 157:845–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Sivarao DV, Chen P, Senapati A, Yang Y, Fernandes A, Benitex Y, et al. 40 Hz Auditory steady-state response is a pharmacodynamic biomarker for cortical NMDA receptors. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2016) 41:2232–40. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Light GA, Zhang W, Joshi YB, Bhakta S, Talledo JA, Swerdlow NR. Single-dose memantine improves cortical oscillatory response dynamics in patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2017) 42:2633–9. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Tada M, Kirihara K, Koshiyama D, Fujioka M, Usui K, Uka T, et al. Gamma-band auditory steady-state response as a neurophysiological marker for excitation and inhibition balance: a review for understanding schizophrenia and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Clin EEG Neurosci. (2020) 51:234–43. doi: 10.1177/1550059419868872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Kwon JS, O'Donnell BF, Wallenstein GV, Greene RW, Hirayasu Y, Nestor PG, et al. Gamma frequency range abnormalities to auditory stimulation in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1999) 56:1001–5. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.11.1001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Light GA, Hsu JL, Hsieh MH, Meyer-Gomes K, Sprock J, Swerdlow NR, et al. Gamma band oscillations reveal neural network cortical coherence dysfunction in schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry. (2006) 60:1231–40. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Spencer KM, Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. Gamma-band auditory steady-state responses are impaired in first episode psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 64:369–75. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Tsuchimoto R, Kanba S, Hirano S, Oribe N, Ueno T, Hirano Y, et al. Reduced high and low frequency gamma synchronization in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2011) 133:99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Tada M, Nagai T, Kirihara K, Koike S, Suga M, Araki T, et al. Differential alterations of auditory gamma oscillatory responses between pre-onset high-risk individuals and first-episode schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. (2016) 26:1027–35. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Thuné H, Recasens M, Uhlhaas PJ. The 40-Hz auditory steady-state response in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. (2016) 73:1145–53. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Wright IC, Rabe-Hesketh S, Woodruff PW, David AS, Murray RM, Bullmore ET. Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (2000) 157:16–25. doi: 10.1176/ajp.157.1.16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Honea R, Crow TJ, Passingham D, Mackay CE. Regional deficits in brain volume in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Am J Psychiatry. (2005) 162:2233–45. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. O'Donnell BF, Hetrick WP, Vohs JL, Krishnan GP, Carroll CA, Shekhar A. Neural synchronization deficits to auditory stimulation in bipolar disorder. NeuroReport. (2004) 15:1369–72. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000127348.64681.b2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Oda Y, Onitsuka T, Tsuchimoto R, Hirano S, Oribe N, Ueno T, et al. Gamma band neural synchronization deficits for auditory steady state responses in bipolar disorder patients. PLOS ONE. (2012) 7:e39955. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Rass O, Krishnan G, Brenner CA, Hetrick WP, Merrill CC, Shekhar A, et al. Auditory steady state response in bipolar disorder: relation to clinical state, cognitive performance, medication status, and substance disorders. Bipolar Disord. (2010) 12:793–803. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00871.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Rojas DC, Wilson LB. γ-band abnormalities as markers of autism spectrum disorders. Biomark Med. (2014) 8:353–68. doi: 10.2217/bmm.14.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hong LE, Summerfelt A, McMahon R, Adami H, Francis G, Elliott A, et al. Evoked gamma band synchronization and the liability for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2004) 70:293–302. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Rass O, Forsyth JK, Krishnan GP, Hetrick WP, Klaunig MJ, Breier A, et al. Auditory steady state response in the schizophrenia, first-degree relatives, and schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res. (2012) 136:143–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Brenner CA, Sporns O, Lysaker PH, O'Donnell BF. EEG synchronization to modulated auditory tones in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and schizotypal personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2003) 160:2238–40. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Vierling-Claassen D, Siekmeier P, Stufflebeam S, Kopell N. Modeling GABA alterations in schizophrenia: a link between impaired inhibition and altered gamma and beta range auditory entrainment. J Neurophysiol. (2008) 99:2656–71. doi: 10.1152/jn.00870.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Hamm JP, Gilmore CS, Picchetti NA, Sponheim SR, Clementz BA. Abnormalities of neuronal oscillations and temporal integration to low- and high-frequency auditory stimulation in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 69:989–96. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Isomura S, Onitsuka T, Tsuchimoto R, Nakamura I, Hirano S, Oda Y, et al. Differentiation between major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder by auditory steady-state responses. J Affect Disord. (2016) 190:800–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Chen J, Gong Q, Wu F. Deficits in the 30-Hz auditory steady-state response in patients with major depressive disorder. NeuroReport. (2016) 27:1147–52. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Wilson TW, Rojas DC, Reite ML, Teale PD, Rogers SJ. Children and adolescents with autism exhibit reduced MEG steady-state gamma responses. Biol Psychiatry. (2007) 62:192–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Rojas DC, Maharajh K, Teale P, Rogers SJ. Reduced neural synchronization of gamma-band MEG oscillations in first-degree relatives of children with autism. BMC Psychiatry. (2008) 8:66. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-66

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Rojas DC, Maharajh K, Teale PD, Kleman MR, Benkers TL, Carlson JP, et al. Development of the 40Hz steady state auditory evoked magnetic field from ages 5 to 52. Clin Neurophysiol. (2006) 117:110–7. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.08.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ono Y, Kudoh K, Ikeda T, Takahashi T, Yoshimura Y, Minabe Y, et al. Auditory steady-state response at 20 Hz and 40 Hz in young typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2020) 74:354–61. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Jentsch JD, Roth RH. The neuropsychopharmacology of phencyclidine: from NMDA receptor hypofunction to the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. (1999) 20:201–25. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00060-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Sohal VS, Zhang F, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K. Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature. (2009) 459:698–702. doi: 10.1038/nature07991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Lewis DA, Curley AA, Glausier JR, Volk DW. Cortical parvalbumin interneurons and cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. (2012) 35:57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kantrowitz JT, Javitt DC. N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor dysfunction or dysregulation: the final common pathway on the road to schizophrenia? Brain Res Bull. (2010) 83:108–21. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Benes FM, Berretta S. GABAergic interneurons: implications for understanding schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2001) 25:1–27. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00225-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Löscher W. Valproate enhances GABA turnover in the substantia nigra. Brain Res. (1989) 501:198–203. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91044-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Shiah IS, Yatham LN, Baker GB. Divalproex sodium increases plasma GABA levels in healthy volunteers. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. (2000) 15:221–5. doi: 10.1097/00004850-200015040-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Rosenberg G. The mechanisms of action of valproate in neuropsychiatric disorders: can we see the forest for the trees? Cell Mol Life Sci. (2007) 64:2090–103. doi: 10.1007/s00018-007-7079-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Sawada T, Chater TE, Sasagawa Y, Yoshimura M, Fujimori-Tonou N, Tanaka K, et al. Developmental excitation-inhibition imbalance underlying psychoses revealed by single-cell analyses of discordant twins-derived cerebral organoids. Mol Psychiatry. (2020) 25:2695–711. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0844-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Hirano Y, Oribe N, Kanba S, Onitsuka T, Nestor PG, Spencer KM. Spontaneous gamma activity in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. (2015) 72:813–21. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Zarate CA Jr, Singh JB, Carlson PJ, Brutsche NE, Ameli R, Luckenbaugh DA, et al. A randomized trial of an N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist in treatment-resistant major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2006) 63:856–64. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Rivolta D, Heidegger T, Scheller B, Sauer A, Schaum M, Birkner K, et al. Ketamine dysregulates the amplitude and connectivity of high-frequency oscillations in cortical-subcortical networks in humans: evidence from resting-state magnetoencephalography-recordings. Schizophr Bull. (2015) 41:1105–14. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Gogolla N, Leblanc JJ, Quast KB, Südhof TC, Fagiolini M, Hensch TK, et al. Common circuit defect of excitatory-inhibitory balance in mouse models of autism. J Neurodev Disord. (2009) 1:172–81. doi: 10.1007/s11689-009-9023-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Fatemi SH, Halt AR, Stary JM, Kanodia R, Schulz SC, Realmuto GR. Glutamic acid decarboxylase 65 and 67 kDa proteins are reduced in autistic parietal and cerebellar cortices. Biol Psychiatry. (2002) 52:805–10. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01430-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Rojas DC, Singel D, Steinmetz S, Hepburn S, Brown MS. Decreased left perisylvian GABA concentration in children with autism and unaffected siblings. Neuroimage. (2014) 86:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Fatemi SH. The hyperglutamatergic hypothesis of autism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 32:911, author reply 912–3. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Cheung C, Yu K, Fung G, Leung M, Wong C, Li Q, et al. Autistic disorders and schizophrenia: related or remote? An anatomical likelihood estimation. PLOS ONE. (2010) 5:e12233. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kolvin I. Studies in the childhood psychoses. I. Diagnostic criteria and classification. Br J Psychiatry. (1971) 118:381–4. doi: 10.1192/bjp.118.545.381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Gao R, Penzes P. Common mechanisms of excitatory and inhibitory imbalance in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Curr Mol Med. (2015) 15:146–67. doi: 10.2174/1566524015666150303003028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Prigge M, Schneider F, Davidson TJ, O'Shea DJ, et al. Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature. (2011) 477:171–8. doi: 10.1038/nature10360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Varela F, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2001) 2:229–39. doi: 10.1038/35067550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Singer W. Synchronization of cortical activity and its putative role in information processing and learning. Annu Rev Physiol. (1993) 55:349–74. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.55.030193.002025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci. (2005) 9:474–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Schnitzler A, Gross J. Normal and pathological oscillatory communication in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2005) 6:285–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Fell J, Axmacher N. The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:105–18. doi: 10.1038/nrn2979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Netoff TI, Clewley R, Arno S, Keck T, White JA. Epilepsy in small-world networks. J Neurosci. (2004) 24:8075–83. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1509-04.2004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Hurtado JM, Lachaux JP, Beckley DJ, Gray CM, Sigvardt KA. Inter- and intralimb oscillator coupling in parkinsonian tremor. Mov Disord. (2000) 15:683–91. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(200007)15:4<683::aid-mds1013>3.0.co;2-#

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Tononi G, Edelman GM. Schizophrenia and the mechanisms of conscious integration. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. (2000) 31:391–400. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00056-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Rohrbaugh JW, Varner JL, Paige SR, Eckardt MJ, Ellingson RJ. Event-related perturbations in an electrophysiological measure of auditory function: a measure of sensitivity during orienting? Biol Psychol. (1989) 29:247–71. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(89)90022-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Rohrbaugh JW, Varner JL, Paige SR, Eckardt MJ, Ellingson RJ. Auditory and visual event-related perturbations in the 40 Hz auditory steady-state response. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1990) 76:148–64. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90213-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Rohrbaugh JW, Varner JL, Paige SR, Eckardt MJ, Ellingson RJ. Event-related perturbations in an electrophysiological measure of auditory sensitivity: effects of probability, intensity and repeated sessions. Int J Psychophysiol. (1990) 10:17–32. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(90)90041-B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Makeig S, Galambos R. The CERP: event related perturbation in steady-state responses. In: Basar E, Bullock T, editors. Brain Dynamics: Progress and Perspectives. Berlin: Springer. (1989). p. 375–400. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-74557-7_30

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Rockstroh B, Müller M, Heinz A, Wagner M, Berg P, Elbert T. Modulation of auditory responses during oddball tasks. Biol Psychol. (1996) 43:41–55. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05175-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Ross B, Pantev C. Auditory steady-state responses reveal amplitude modulation gap detection thresholds. J Acoust Soc Am. (2004) 115(5 Pt 1):2193–206. doi: 10.1121/1.1694996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Ross B, Herdman AT, Pantev C. Stimulus induced desynchronization of human auditory 40-Hz steady-state responses. J Neurophysiol. (2005) 94:4082–93. doi: 10.1152/jn.00469.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Motomura E, Inui K, Kawano Y, Nishihara M, Okada M. Effects of sound-pressure change on the 40 Hz auditory steady-state response and change-related cerebral response. Brain Sci. (2019) 9:203. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9080203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Sugiyama S, Kinukawa T, Takeuchi N, Nishihara M, Shioiri T, Inui K. Change-related acceleration effects on auditory steady-state response. Front Syst Neurosci. (2019) 13:53. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Bartha-Doering L, Deuster D, Giordano V, am Zehnhoff-Dinnesen A, Dobel C. A systematic review of the mismatch negativity as an index for auditory sensory memory: from basic research to clinical and developmental perspectives. Psychophysiology. (2015) 52:1115–30. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Näätänen R, Gaillard AW, Mäntysalo S. Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol (Amst). (1978) 42:313–29. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Näätänen R, Jacobsen T, Winkler I. Memory-based or afferent processes in mismatch negativity (MMN): a review of the evidence. Psychophysiology. (2005) 42:25–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00256.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Shelley AM, Ward PB, Catts SV, Michie PT, Andrews S, McConaghy N. Mismatch negativity: an index of a preattentive processing deficit in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. (1991) 30:1059–62. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(91)90126-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Catts SV, Shelley AM, Ward PB, Liebert B, McConaghy N, Andrews S, et al. Brain potential evidence for an auditory sensory memory deficit in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (1995) 152:213–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.2.213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Chen TC, Hsieh MH, Lin YT, Chan PS, Cheng CH. Mismatch negativity to different deviant changes in autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin Neurophysiol. (2020) 131:766–77. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.10.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Pekkonen E, Jousmäki V, Könönen M, Reinikainen K, Partanen J. Auditory sensory memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: an event-related potential study. NeuroReport. (1994) 5:2537–40. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199412000-00033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Skosnik PD, Krishnan GP, O'Donnell BF. The effect of selective attention on the gamma-band auditory steady-state response. Neurosci Lett. (2007) 420:223–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.04.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Bidet-Caulet A, Fischer C, Besle J, Aguera PE, Giard MH, Bertrand O. Effects of selective attention on the electrophysiological representation of concurrent sounds in the human auditory cortex. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:9252–61. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1402-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Sugiyama S, Kinukawa T, Takeuchi N, Nishihara M, Shioiri T, Inui K. Tactile cross-modal acceleration effects on auditory steady-state response. Front Integr Neurosci. (2019) 13:72. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Tan HR, Gross J, Uhlhaas PJ. MEG-measured auditory steady-state oscillations show high test–retest reliability: a sensor and source-space analysis. NeuroImage. (2015) 122:417–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Giard MH, Peronnet F. Auditory-visual integration during multimodal object recognition in humans: a behavioral and electrophysiological study. J Cogn Neurosci. (1999) 11:473–90. doi: 10.1162/089892999563544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Senkowski D, Molholm S, Gomez-Ramirez M, Foxe JJ. Oscillatory beta activity predicts response speed during a multisensory audiovisual reaction time task: a high-density electrical mapping study. Cereb Cortex. (2006) 16:1556–65. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Tseng HH, Bossong MG, Modinos G, Chen KM, McGuire P, Allen P. A systematic review of multisensory cognitive-affective integration in schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 55:444–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Baum SH, Stevenson RA, Wallace MT. Behavioral, perceptual, and neural alterations in sensory and multisensory function in autism spectrum disorder. Prog Neurobiol. (2015) 134:140–60. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ASSR, gamma-band oscillation, phase resetting, electroencephalography, magnetoencephalography, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, autism spectral disorder

Citation: Sugiyama S, Ohi K, Kuramitsu A, Takai K, Muto Y, Taniguchi T, Kinukawa T, Takeuchi N, Motomura E, Nishihara M, Shioiri T and Inui K (2021) The Auditory Steady-State Response: Electrophysiological Index for Sensory Processing Dysfunction in Psychiatric Disorders. Front. Psychiatry 12:644541. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.644541

Received: 21 December 2020; Accepted: 22 February 2021;
Published: 11 March 2021.

Edited by:

Shinsuke Koike, The University of Tokyo, Japan

Reviewed by:

Minah Kim, Seoul National University Hospital, South Korea
Mariko Tada, The University of Tokyo, Japan

Copyright © 2021 Sugiyama, Ohi, Kuramitsu, Takai, Muto, Taniguchi, Kinukawa, Takeuchi, Motomura, Nishihara, Shioiri and Inui. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shunsuke Sugiyama, s0450032@yahoo.co.jp; Kazutaka Ohi, k_ohi@gifu-u.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.