Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 04 February 2021
Sec. Aging Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic “We know what we are, but know not what we may be”: Metacognition and Self-awareness in the Aging Brain View all 7 articles

The Association Between Impaired Awareness and Depression, Anxiety, and Apathy in Mild to Moderate Alzheimer's Disease: A Systematic Review

  • Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London, United Kingdom

Objectives: Impaired awareness of cognitive and functional deficits is a common feature of Alzheimer's disease (AD). Although a lack of awareness has been suggested to be a protective factor against experiencing affective symptoms, such as depression, anxiety, and apathy which are common in AD, there is conflicting evidence about the links between them. This systematic review examines the evidence for an association between impaired awareness and depressive, anxiety, and apathy symptoms in mild to moderate AD.

Method: We searched four databases (OvidMedline, Embase, PsycInfo, and PsycArticles) using terms encompassing awareness, apathy, depression, anxiety, and mild-moderate AD. We included studies that assessed the relationship between awareness and depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, or apathy. We assessed included papers for quality and report results using a narrative approach, prioritizing high quality studies.

Results: We identified 1,544 articles, and twenty-seven studies fulfilled inclusion criteria (high-quality = 15; moderate-quality = 12). Most high-quality studies reported that impaired awareness in early-stage AD is cross-sectionally linked with fewer depressive symptoms and anxiety symptoms (correlation ranged from −0.3 to −0.7), but with more apathy.

Conclusions: High-quality studies suggested that in people with early AD, impaired awareness is related to fewer depressive and anxiety symptoms and to more apathy. Future research should focus on elucidating causality among impaired awareness and these symptoms in AD.

Introduction

Dementia is characterized by progressive cognitive decline, the presence of neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) and difficulties in activities of daily living (ADL) (1). The worldwide prevalence of dementia is currently around 50 million (2). Alzheimer's disease (AD) is the leading cause of dementia, corresponding to 62% of all dementia cases (3), and people with AD may lack insight or awareness of the illness or symptoms. Awareness is a construct related to accurate self-appraisal, and it has been defined as having a “realistic perception of one's personal situation, performance and functioning” (4). Awareness is not a unitary concept, and should be considered in relation to the specific objects or domains of awareness being assessed. In AD, people may have impaired awareness of different domains, such as of cognitive impairment, of deficits in ADLs, of affective symptoms or behavioral changes (5); and, a partial or complete absence of awareness of having an illness (6). Impaired awareness of deficits is a marked and common clinical feature of AD (7). Sixty percent of people with mild dementia present some degree of impaired awareness, and the prevalence and frequency increases with the progression of the disease (8). Impaired awareness of illness or deficits in AD, has also been referred to as lack of insight or anosognosia (9) and constitutes a challenge to treatment adherence, decreases quality of life, is associated with a worse prognosis, and increases carer and family burden (6, 10). For the purpose of this review, the term awareness is used to refer to awareness of illness, or of cognitive, functional or behavioral change related to AD.

Three common approaches are used to assess impaired awareness in AD: (1) clinician ratings of patient's awareness of illness; (2) the discrepancy between prediction of performance and actual performance; and (3) the discrepancy between patient and caregiver scores on cognitive, functional or behavioral outcomes (11). The first approach may be part of a routine clinical assessment, or involve a semi structured interview, where clinicians make a judgement about impaired awareness of illness or in a specific domain (12). A disadvantage of this approach is that it relies solely on the judgement of the clinician, and often considers impaired awareness as a unitary symptom that can be categorized by the clinician into, for example, “no,” “partial,” or “full” awareness of deficits (12). The second strategy scores the degree of awareness as the difference between an estimation of performance predicted by the patient on a neuropsychological task and the actual score achieved (7). This has the advantage of assesing symptomatology from the patient perspective, and providing a direct comparison of subjective and objective neuropsychological function, however is mainly limited to assessing awareness of cognitive performance (11). Moreover, there is the potential of bias, as a patient may over- or underestimate their performance for a variety of reasons which may be culturally orcontextually mediated, rather than related to dementia. The third strategy compares ratings given by patients regarding their performance on changes of cognition, mood, behavior, or ADLs against caregivers rating (13). The main limitation of this approach is that it assumes a caregiver is providing an objectively accurate rating, whereas, there is the potential for bias due to caregiver burden and distress (11). Nonetheless, significant correlations between caregiver and clinician reports have been found, with reports given by carers found to be accurate and reliable (5, 14). The extent of awareness varies across people at each stage of AD, and relationships regarding impaired awareness and NPS in mild to moderate AD remain uncertain (15, 16). In AD, depression and apathy are relatively common NPS, with a pooled prevalence of 42 and 49%, respectively (17). Anxiety is also a distressing affective symptom that affects 39% of the population living with AD (17). It remains unclear whether intact awareness of illness, health-loss and impairments in early AD is linked with these symptoms of depression, anxiety, and apathy; or if, on the other hand, these symptoms occurs independently of the level of awareness and insight, as a consequence of the neurodegenerative process or other environmental and individual variables. There is conflicting evidence particularly with depression, where some studies report that people with greater awareness of disease suffer from more depressive symptoms, potentially as an emotional and psychological reaction to having the illness (18, 19). Although the mechanisms underlying the relationship between awareness of disease and depression remain unclear (20), and other studies have reported no association between level of awareness and the presence of depressive symptoms (12, 13, 21). Less conflicting evidence has been found for apathy, where correlations between impaired awareness and more apathetic symptomatology have been previously reported (22, 23), although these studies included participants with severe AD. The association between anxiety and awareness remains unclear, as anxiety is often assessed only as a secondary outcome, however, one review has reported that unawareness is associated with lower anxiety scores in AD (11).

This systematic review aims to describe the associations between impaired awareness and the presence of depressive, anxiety, and apathy symptoms in early-AD; and in light of the results, consider possible causalities and underlying mechanisms of this relationship. Elucidating links between impaired awareness and these symptoms in early-AD, would provide a more in-depth understanding of the characteristics and progression of the disease, as impaired awareness, affective symptoms, and apathy are all related to prognosis of the disease, quality of life, and carer burden (15).

Understanding the role of awareness as a possible protective or risk factor for affective and apathetic symptoms, may aid in delivering more targeted psychological therapy in early AD, including awareness as a variable to consider when deciding therapeutic approaches. Thus, clarifying the relationship between awareness, and common symptoms in early AD, such as depression, anxiety, and apathy, could inform treatment and management plans, and may have a positive impact both for the person with AD and for their carers and family.

Methods

Search Strategy

The search strategy was developed according to PRISMA guidelines (24). We searched four databases (Embase, PsycInfo, OvidMedline, and PsycArticles) from inception to 27th of October 2020. We used the following search terms, with the Boolean operators “AND” and “OR” used to combine terms and concepts: “Alzheimer's disease” or “AD” or “Alz*” or “dementia” or DAT, AND “metacognition” or “aware*” or “metacognitive” or “insight” or “anosognosia” or “self-appraisal,” AND “depress*” or “low mood” or “major depressive episode” or “MDE” or “anxiety” or “apathy” or “neuropsychiatric symptoms” or “BPSD.”

Selection Criteria

Inclusion criteria were published, peer reviewed, original research studies of people with a diagnosis of mild-to-moderate AD. A diagnosis of AD was required to have been made using validated criteria, such as the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer Disease and Related Disorders (NINCDS–ADRDA) or the Diagnostical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM); and validated methods used to measure awareness and depression, anxiety, or apathy were required. We included studies in English or Spanish. As we only included data related to mild and moderate AD, we excluded studies containing only participants with other non-AD dementias, mild cognitive impairment, or severe dementia. Gray literature and unpublished studies were excluded.

Eligibility of the Studies and Quality Assessment

All potential studies were screened by two authors for eligibility and disagreements were discussed with a third independent reviewer.

We extracted relevant data from the included studies including study authors, year of publication, study design, number of participants, severity of AD, neuropsychiatric measure used, awareness domain and assessment measure, statistical method and association between awareness and neuropsychiatric symptom, and study conclusions.

For quality assessment we used the Newcastle-Ottawa instrument for case-control and cohort-studies (NOS) (25) and the NOS adapted-version for cross-sectional studies (26) for each study (Appendix 1 in Supplementary Material). There is no well-established threshold to define quality-level, therefore we used the definitions of level of quality implemented from a previous study (26). Total scores out of 10, from 1 to 4 were defined as low-quality, from 5 to 7 as moderate-quality, and equal or higher to 8 as high-quality.

Results

Search Results

We identified 1,544 references in the literature searches. A PRISMA flow chart of the search is shown in Figure 1. After removal of duplicates and irrelevant articles, 143 articles were selected for full-text eligibility assessment. Finally, 27 studies fulfilled inclusion and exclusion criteria.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart.

Study Characteristics

The main characteristics of each individual study are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies.

Assessment of Awareness

Among the included articles, sixteen different methods were used to assess awareness, with two studies using a battery of more than one instrument (36, 38). The Anosognosia Questionnaire for Dementia was the most commonly used instrument, reported in ten articles (5, 6, 23, 27, 28, 32, 33, 41, 46, 47). The “patient-caregiver discrepancy” approach was used by three studies (34, 35, 42) using different assessment scales. The following tools were used by two studies each: the Squire and Zouzuris Anosognosia scale (39, 43); the Anosognosia Rating Scale (ARS) (30, 48); the Assessment Scale of Psychosocial Impact of the Diagnosis of Dementia (ASPIDD) (16, 45); and the Memory Awareness Rating Scale (7, 36). The following tools were used in one study: the Guideline for the Rating of Awareness Deficits (29); and the Assessment of Impaired Insight (AII) (44). One study used sub-scores derived from insight items from a related tool (37); another study used a subtractive method (40); while Clare et al. (4) developed a multi-domain assessment for implicit and explicit awareness. The complete list and description of the instruments used by each of the included studies can be found in Appendix 2 (Supplementary Material).

Depression, Apathy, and Anxiety Assessment

In total, twenty-three studies addressed depressive symptoms using ten different assessment methods (4, 6, 16, 2834, 3648). Anxiety was assessed in four studies (4, 35, 37, 38), all of them using different measuring tools. Apathy and its relationship with awareness was assessed by eleven studies (5, 23, 27, 29, 32, 35, 37, 38, 41, 46, 47) using five different measuring tools. Appendix 3 (Supplementary Material) shows the NPS assessed, instrument used, and the number of studies that used each instrument.

Quality of the Studies

Within the twenty-seven studies, fifteen were ranked as high-quality (mean score = 8.33), and twelve as moderate-quality (mean score = 6.66). There were no low-quality studies (see Table 1). The overall mean score was 7.59 corresponding to moderate/high quality. Further details of quality assessment are found in Appendix 4 (Supplementary Material).

Association Between Impaired-Awareness and Depression, Anxiety, and Apathy

The associations between impaired awareness, depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and apathy are shown in Table 2. Table 3 summarizes the associations between impaired awareness and these symptoms according to quality of the studies.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results: association between impaired awareness and depression, anxiety, and apathy; data and summary of results.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Association between impaired-awareness and depression, anxiety, and apathy.

Impaired Awareness and Depression

Twenty-three studies assessed depression in relation to impaired awareness in mild-to-moderate AD. Among them, thirteen studies; ten of high-quality (16, 2833, 38, 39, 43) and three of moderate-quality (36, 37, 44), found significant evidence that impaired awareness correlates with fewer depressive symptoms, with correlations ranging from 0.3 to 0.7. In other words, according to these 13 studies, having better awareness of deficits in AD correlates with more depressive symptoms.

This negative association between impaired awareness and depression was assessed by different measures and tools. For example, a high-quality study (31) reported a negative correlation between impaired awareness and depressive symptoms on the HADS (r = −0.7). Similarly, the study by Jacus (38), found a strong association between impaired awareness predicting less depression measured by the BDI-II (OR = 4.8, 95% CI: 1.0–21.5). Likewise, the moderate-quality study of Horning et al. (37) found that better insight, was related to depressed mood, as measured by the Neurobehavioural Rating Scale (F = 6.0).

On the other hand, two studies, one of moderate quality (42) and the other of high quality (6) found that impaired awareness was related to greater depressive symptoms (P = 0.002; r: 0.53, respectively).

Finally, eight studies, two high-quality (4, 45) and six moderate-quality (34, 40, 41, 4648) concluded that there is no association, neither positive nor negative, between awareness and depressive symptoms.

Domains of Awareness and Depression

Most of the included studies analyzed the relationship between global awareness of deficits, illness or condition, and the presence of depression or depressive symptoms. Regarding specific domains of awareness, seven studies explored awareness of memory deficits; most of them supporting the trend of a negative correlation between impaired awareness and depressive symptoms (2931, 36, 43). While Nakaaki et al. (42) reported a positive association (P = 0.002) and Lehrner et al. (40) found no significant association (r = −0.28). A recent study by Bertrand et al. (28) reported a moderate positive association between having better awareness of executive deficits and more depressive symptoms (r = 0.43). Similarly, another high-quality study found that preserved awareness of emotional-state and preserved awareness of difficulties of social functioning were also correlated with greater depressive symptoms (r = 0.38 and r = 0.30, respectively) (16).

Impaired Awareness and Anxiety

Four studies, two high-quality (4, 38) and two moderate-quality (35, 37), assessed the relationship between impaired awareness and anxiety in mild-to-moderate AD. All of them found that there was a negative association, meaning that greater impairment of awareness is associated with fewer anxiety symptoms.

Clare et al. (4) carried out a multidimensional approach with cluster analysis to identify links between degree of awareness and different variables. Regarding anxiety, they found that the group of participants with lower scores on awareness reported significantly less anxiety than those of the moderate awareness group (F = 3.58) (4). Jacus (38) also found that level of awareness significantly predicts the level of anxiety (OR = 9.3, 95% CI: 2.18–39.96). Horning et al. (37) concluded that even after controlling for impaired cognitive function, level of awareness significantly predicts level of anxiety (F = 4.80; and Derousne et al. (35) found a negative correlation between impaired awareness and anxiety, which persisted over time (mean follow up time 21 months, r = −0.3).

Impaired Awareness and Apathy

Ten of eleven studies which assessed the relationship between impaired awareness and apathy in mild-to-moderate AD found a significant positive association, meaning that greater impairment of awareness is associated with higher levels of apathy. Of those ten studies, four were of high-quality (27, 32, 38, 46) and six of moderate-quality (5, 23, 35, 37, 41, 47). Of the high-quality studies, Amanzio et al. (27) found that apathy was a prominent feature in reduced awareness of behavioral changes (β = 0.3). One study focusing on quality of life found that impaired awareness and apathy were positively correlated among patients with mild dementia, but no significant association was seen in moderate AD (32). A study by Jacus (38) found a strong correlation between poorer awareness and greater apathy in mild-to-moderate AD, as measured by two different awareness instruments (OR = 4.8, 95% CI: 1.14–20.8; and OR = 9.3, 95% CI: 2.18–39.96). Similarly, Spalletta et al. (46) found that the severity of apathy symptoms was correlated with impaired behavioral awareness (t = −3.57). Likewise, six other moderate-quality studies also found this positive trend of association (5, 23, 35, 37, 41, 47). Three of these were longitudinal studies, which found that apathy had a positive correlation with impaired awareness which remained over time (r = 0.35) (35); that unawareness was a significant predictor of apathetic symptomatology (F = 19.1) (23); and that apathy was positively associated both with incidence and persistence of impaired awareness over a 12 month follow-up period (47). In contrast, one high-quality study found no significant association between apathy and any awareness domain once adjusting for dementia severity, as measured by MMSE score (OR = 1.00; 95% CI:0.94–1.06; p = 0.968) (29).

Discussion

The overall finding of this review is that most high-quality studies suggested a negative association between impaired awareness in early AD and depression and anxiety; but a positive association between impaired awareness and apathy (see Table 3).

Although the high-quality studies support the conclusion that impaired awareness is associated with fewer depressive symptoms there were conflicting results: 13 of the 23 studies assessing this association found this negative association, while eight found no association, and two studies found a positive association. One explanation for this lack of concordance between studies might be due to the different range of assessment approaches and instruments, both for impaired awareness and depressive symptomology in people with AD. Awareness is a complex concept and cannot be considered as a unitary entity (7). Some people with dementia may be more aware of some deficits and less aware of others, which might have an impact on the association with depressive features.

Additionally, this review highlights the lack of consistency in methods of assessing impaired awareness and revealed the many different methods that are used for the same purpose in clinical research. Due to the heterogeneity in assessment tools and statistical methods used among the included studies, we did not perform a meta-analysis. As has been noted in previous literature (47, 50), not having a gold-standard method to assess impaired awareness has resulted in the development of several instruments. We found sixteen different instruments, some of them specially created for awareness in dementia, others for awareness in any disease; some assessing global-awareness, while others assessing awareness of specific domains. The variability of how awareness is theoretically understood in dementia explains why there are several methods used to assess its impairment, as has been discussed in previous reviews (7). Until a consensus of the definition of awareness and consistent measurement tools are used, or unless studies specify clearly which domain of awareness they want to address, it will be difficult to draw clear conclusions regarding impairment of awareness and its clinical correlates in AD. Similarly, the results showed that depression, anxiety and apathy in AD research are also assessed by a wide range of instruments. Moreover, cultural and gender differences in depression are also relevant. Common instruments are potentially less sensitive to adequately addressing depressive symptoms in men (51), and one study that did not find an association used an Asian sample, with the authors hypothesizing that cultural influences in minimizing depressive feelings may have confounded the results (41). In addition, some studies measured depressive symptoms with instruments which are not designed for people with dementia (34). These issues in assessing depression may partly explain the greater discrepancy in the reviewed literature for the association between awareness and depression, compared to the consistency in the results for the association with apathy and anxiety. Despite this heterogeneity in assessment tools and statistical methods used, a strength of this review is the use of quality assessments of previous studies to clarify the relationship between impaired awareness and these common NPSin AD, as the majority of high quality studies demonstrated consistent results.

The mechanisms underlying the phenomena of impaired awareness remain an area of research. Cognitive theories suggest that impaired awareness is a consequence of impairments of executive, metacognitive systems, and also due to deficits in encoding autobiographical memory related to underlying neurodegeneration in relevant brain regions (11, 52). Accordingly, neuroimaging has shown that there is an association between dysfunction of temporomedial, temporoparietal and frontal regions and the presence of unawareness in dementia (53, 54). Biopsychosocial models propose that impaired awareness might reflect the impact of the AD diagnosis (bio-component), which may generate a tension in the person with dementia, who needs to integrate the new “AD-self” with the previous “healthy-self” (psycho-component). Thus, in this model prior beliefs, expectations, and motivational factors are interrelated with awareness of disease, while the social context also plays a role in how the disease is acknowledged (social-component) (7). Understanding the biological mechanisms and possible psychosocial components of impaired awareness in dementia is important in relating deficits in awareness to the occurrence of NPS. Elucidating these mechanisms may aid clinical practice, as clarifying the psychological components of the relationship between awareness and depression or anxiety may support the use of psychosocial interventions for these common symptoms (55).

Although the association between awareness and depressive symptoms may be due in part to underlying neurodegeneration, the etiology of depression in dementia remains unclear with multiple biological and psychological mechanisms likely to be involved (20). Related to the biopsychosocial model, depression may be a reaction to the awareness of memory deficits, daily difficulties, and to the possible “anticipatory grief” of losing a previous identity. The reduction of depressive symptoms with reduced awareness may be related to both underlying biological mechanisms including neurodegeneration and to coping strategies and integration of the “new-self.” These models are not mutually exclusive and may explain different aspects of the same phenomena. Alternatively, impaired awareness may be underestimated in depressed people living with AD, and the relationship between depressive symptoms and apparently preserved awareness may be partly due to the negative bias when reporting problems (22). Depressed patients with AD might underestimate their abilities when rating themselves, and in this case, the apparent discrepancy between self-rated and informant-rated abilities, may be distorted by the depressive symptoms (43). Therefore, patients will score as apparently having more preserved awareness, but this would be due to the effect of depressive symptoms rather than an accurate assessment of illness and deficits. Moreover, at the very early stages of AD, subjective concerns about cognitive function may lead to hyper-evaluation and underestimation of performance and abilities, which may also be considered part of the continuum of distorted awareness of function in AD (56).

Apathy is defined as a disorder related to diminished motivation, interest, and expression of emotions (57). Apathy has a strong correlation with frontal lobe dysfunction, sharing this neuropathological pathway with impaired awareness (49). One study found that more severe anosognosia predicted more apathy, with two possible theories: that impaired awareness emerged as an early response to frontal lobe dysfunction, while apathy appeared with more extensive frontal lobe degeneration; or apathy may be the consequence of a poorer adaptation response to the new limitations related to dementia (23). Temporality cannot assume causality, and both theories for apathy and awareness may be correct, assuming a biopsychosocial understanding of awareness where personal factors, such as coping strategies, are involved. Moreover, most of the included studies had a cross-sectional design, thus results only reflected associations without direction among variables, therefore more longitudinal studies are needed to explore possible causalities. Finally, apathy in dementia may occur in different domains -namely cognitive, behavioral, and emotional apathy-, and the accurate measurement of apathy and its clinical relationships will depend on whether the instrument used addressed apathy as a single domain entity (i.e., the NPI) or as a multidomain syndrome (i.e., the Apathy Inventory) (58).

The biopsychosocial model might be particularly relevant to explain the association of impaired awareness, affective symptoms and apathy in early-stage AD, as it includes the relational and experiential factors as well as recognizing the impact of neurodegeneration. Understanding the underlying mechanisms in the relationship between impaired awareness and affective symptoms and apathy is essential, as treatment targets may vary if we conceptualize the phenomenon from a biological or psychological perspective. In terms of dementia care, preserved awareness or greater insight may have positive implications, as it is associated with treatment acceptance and less carer burden (10). Nonetheless, the results from this review suggest that greater awareness of deficits may be associated with a higher risk of experiencing more depressive and anxiety symptomatology. There is evidence that some specific psychological interventions are effective in managing depression and anxiety in dementia (55), however, there is currently no effective intervention for apathy (59). Although apathy and depression in dementia share some common features which often lead to misinterpretation, there is evidence that they are two different syndromes (60), requiring different therapeutic approaches. The opposite trend of association between depression and apathy in relation with awareness reported from the high-quality studies in this review, supports the distinction of depression and apathy in AD as two independent syndromes that might be explained by different underlying mechanisms. Although any explanations are speculative and likely to be multifactorial, apathy may be more biologically driven and therefore, emerges as a consequence due to neurodegeneration, while depression and anxiety might result from additional psychological factors which are more likely to occur when the person is more aware of their deficits and impairments due to dementia.

Limitations

This review has several limitations. Firstly, we were unable to obtain raw or missing data, that may have enabled us to perform a meta-analysis and provide more conclusive evidence. Secondly, broader inclusion criteria for dementia severity and etiology could have resulted in more evidence to analyse and compare between sub-types of dementia, providing wider resources to understand different profiles of impaired awareness. Finally, we did not include gray literature and unpublished articles, which may have increased the risk of publication bias.

The most relevant limitation of the selected articles was the lack of a standard measure to assess awareness, which hindered the ability to compare results across studies. However, this is mainly due to the general limitation that emerges from the conceptualization of awareness itself, as has been previously discussed. Another limitation was the large variety of sample sizes within studies, and many studies had <100 participants; therefore, small sample sizes might have influenced the results and associations found in individual studies. Possible sources of bias and confounders were rarely addressed within studies, as for example the effect that distress or personality traits of a carer could have on rating their care-recipient performances (on the carer-patient discrepancy strategy) to assess impaired awareness. Similarly, the subjective nature of clinician judgements used in some studies could have been a source of bias that could have been avoided using more standardized standardized measuring instruments, both for impaired-awareness and for affective symptoms and apathy. It is important to note that most of the studies used the NPI to assess NPS, This is a widely used scale, however it assesses apathy and depression only as symptoms, rather than as distinct diagnoses or syndromes. In addition, as noted above, some tools used to assess depression, e.g., the HAM-D, are not specifically designed for an elderly population with dementia. This may lead to difficulties in classifying and interpreting symptoms of depression or apathy in AD, affecting the associations found with awareness in individual studies. Even though cognitive dysfunction was assessed in most studies as part of demographic characteristics or baseline measures, it was not commonly analyzed as a potential confounder in the included studies. Likewise, as was previously mentioned, gender as a moderator was not commonly assessed and no specific gender instruments were implemented. Methodology limitations were mainly related to the lack of justification of sample size and some studies had missing data. Finally, as has been highlighted in previous reviews (11), a general limitation of the methods to assess impaired awareness in AD is that the impact that awareness has on the patient's experiences, ADLs, and social relationships is usually not considered and this general measurement limitation might have affected the individual studies as well.

Conclusion

According to the majority of high-quality studies, impaired-awareness in mild-to-moderate AD is associated with fewer depressive and fewer anxiety symptoms, but more apathy. Understanding impaired awareness is clinically important due to its high prevalence in AD and because of the consequences it has on the person with dementia, their family, and on care. Additionally, the presence of affective symptoms and apathy are challenging, and more complicated when impaired awareness of disease obstructs their management. This review provides a useful summary of the relationship between these common NPSand impaired-awareness in mild-to-moderate AD, with the methodological strength of having conducted a quality assessment of the included studies. In accordance with previous reviews (7, 11), a more accurate and standardized method to assess impaired awareness in the context of AD is needed. Understanding how awareness relates to affective symptoms and apathy is important for people with dementia and their carers, and for healthcare professionals to provide effective interventions and better integrated management of dementia, and to improve the quality of life of people living with AD and their families.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

IA, GL, and JH conceived the idea for this study and interpreted the data. IA conducted the search, quality check on inclusion criteria, and drafted the manuscript and figures. JH and GL revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

JH was funded by a Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Career Development Fellowship (214547/Z/18/Z) and supported by UCLH NIHR BRC.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

IA acknowledges to CONICYT (Beca Chile, Magister en el extranjero 2017-2018) for providing the funding and support to be part of the MSc. Dementia: Treatment, Causes, and Research (Mental Health), Division of Psychiatry, UCL.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.633081/full#supplementary-material

References

1. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Patterson C. World Alzheimer Report 2018. London: Alzheimer's Disease International (2018).

Google Scholar

3. Kalaria RN, Maestre GE, Arizaga R, Friedland RP, Galasko D, Hall K, et al. Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia in developing countries: prevalence, management, and risk factors. Lancet Neurol. (2008) 7:812–26. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70169-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Clare L, Whitaker CJ, Nelis SM, Martyr A, Markova IS, Roth I, et al. Multidimensional assessment of awareness in early-stage dementia: a cluster analytic approach. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2011) 31:317–27. doi: 10.1159/000327356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Starkstein SE, Jorge R, Mizrahi R, Robinson RG. A diagnostic formulation for anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2006) 77:719–25. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.085373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Verhulsdonk S, Quack R, Hoft B, Lange-Asschenfeldt C, Supprian T. Anosognosia and depression in patients with Alzheimer's dementia. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2013) 57:282–7. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2013.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Clare L. The construction of awareness in early-stage Alzheimer's disease: a review of concepts and models. Br J Clin Psychol. (2004) 43(Pt 2):155–75. doi: 10.1348/014466504323088033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Mograbi DC, Ferri CP, Sosa AL, Stewart R, Laks J, Brown R, et al. Unawareness of memory impairment in dementia: a population-based study. Int Psychogeriatr. (2012) 24:931–9. doi: 10.1017/S1041610211002730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Markova IS, Berrios GE. Approaches to the assessment of awareness: conceptual issues. Neuropsychol Rehabil. (2006) 16:439–55. doi: 10.1080/09602010500373396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Castrillo Sanz A, Andrés Calvo M, Repiso Gento I, Izquierdo Delgado E, Gutierrez Ríos R, Rodríguez Herrero R, et al. Anosognosia in Alzheimer disease: prevalence, associated factors, and influence on disease progression. Neurología (English Edition). (2016) 31:296–304. doi: 10.1016/j.nrleng.2015.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Starkstein SE. Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease: diagnosis, frequency, mechanism and clinical correlates. Cortex. (2014) 61:64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Reed BR, Jagust WJ, Coulter L. Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease: relationships to depression, cognitive function, and cerebral perfusion. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (1993) 15:231–44. doi: 10.1080/01688639308402560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Migliorelli R, Teson A, Sabe L, Petracca G, Petracchi M, Leiguarda R, et al. Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease: a study of associated factors. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (1995) 7:338–44. doi: 10.1176/jnp.7.3.338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Snow AL, Norris MP, Doody R, Molinari VA, Orengo CA, Kunik ME. Dementia deficits scale: rating self-awareness of deficits. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disords. (2004) 18:22–31. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200401000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Cosentino S, Stern Y. Metacognitive theory and assessment in dementia: do we recognize our areas of weakness? J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2005)1:910–9. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lacerda IB, Santos RL, Neto JPS, Dourado MCN. Factors related to different objects of awareness in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2017) 31:335–42. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Zhao QF, Tan L, Wang HF, Jiang T, Tan MS, Tan L, et al. The prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer's disease: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2016) 190:264–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Harwood DG, Sultzer DL, Wheatley MV. Impaired insight in Alzheimer disease: association with cognitive deficits, psychiatric symptoms, and behavioral disturbances. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. (2000) 13:83–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

19. Lopez OL, Becker JT, Somsak D, Dew MA, DeKosky ST. Awareness of cognitive deficits and anosognosia in probable Alzheimer's disease. Eur Neurol. (1994) 34:277–82. doi: 10.1159/000117056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Enache D, Winblad B, Aarsland D. Depression in dementia: epidemiology, mechanisms, and treatment. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2011) 24:461–72. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834bb9d4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Michon A, Deweer B, Pillon B, Agid Y, Dubois B. Relation of anosognosia to frontal lobe dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1994) 57:805–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.57.7.805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Mograbi DC, Morris RG. On the relation among mood, apathy, and anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2014) 20:2–7. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Starkstein SE, Brockman S, Bruce D, Petracca G. Anosognosia is a significant predictor of apathy in Alzheimer's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2010) 22:378–83. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2010.22.4.378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. (2009) 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Wells G, Shea B, O'Connell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M, et al. The Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Non-Randomized Studies in Meta-Analysis. (2013). Available online at: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

26. Modesti PA, Reboldi G, Cappuccio FP, Agyemang C, Remuzzi G, Rapi S, et al. Panethnic differences in blood pressure in Europe: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0147601. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Amanzio M, Vase L, Leotta D, Miceli R, Palermo S, Geminiani G. Impaired awareness of deficits in Alzheimer's disease: the role of everyday executive dysfunction. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2013) 19:63–72. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Bertrand E, Mograbi DC, Brown RG, Landeira-Fernandez J, Morris RG. Heterogeneity of anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease according to the object of awareness. Psychology & Neuroscience, (2019) 12, 282–90. doi: 10.1037/pne0000164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Chen YL, Chen HM, Huang MF, Yeh YC, Yen CF, Chen CS. Clinical correlates of unawareness of deficits among patients with dementia due to Alzheimer's disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. (2014) 29:533–9. doi: 10.1177/1533317514523484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Cines S, Farrell M, Steffener J, Sullo L, Huey E, Karlawish J, et al. Examining the pathways between self-awareness and well-being in mild to moderate Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2015) 23:1297–306. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2015.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Clare L, Wilson BA, Carter G, Roth I, Hodges JR. Awareness in early-stage alzheimer's disease: relationship to outcome of cognitive rehabilitation. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2004) 26, 215–26. doi: 10.1076/jcen.26.2.215.28088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Conde-Sala JL, Rene-Ramirez R, Turro-Garriga O, Gascon-Bayarri J, Juncadella-Puig M, Moreno-Cordon L, et al. Clinical differences in patients with Alzheimer's disease according to the presence or absence of anosognosia: implications for perceived quality of life. J Alzheimers Dis. (2013) 33:1105–16. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-121360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Conde-Sala JL, Turro-Garriga O, Garre-Olmo J, Vilalta-Franch J, Lopez-Pousa S. Discrepancies regarding the quality of life of patients with Alzheimer's disease: a three-year longitudinal study. J Alzheimers Dis. (2014) 39:511–25. doi: 10.3233/JAD-131286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. DeBettignies BH, Mahurin RK, Pirozzolo FJ. Insight for impairment in independent living skills in Alzheimer's disease and multi-infarct dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (1990) 12:355–63. doi: 10.1080/01688639008400980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Derouesne C, Thibault S, Lagha-Pierucci S, Baudouin-Madec V, Ancri D, Lacomblez L. Decreased awareness of cognitive deficits in patients with mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (1999) 14:1019–30.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

36. Gilleen J, Greenwood K, Archer N, Lovestone S, David AS. The role of premorbid personality and cognitive factors in awareness of illness, memory, and behavioural functioning in Alzheimer's disease. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2012) 17:227–45. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2011.588007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Horning SM, Melrose R, Sultzer D. Insight in Alzheimer's disease and its relation to psychiatric and behavioral disturbances. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2014) 29:77–84. doi: 10.1002/gps.3972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Jacus JP. Awareness, apathy, and depression in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Brain Behav. (2017) 7:e00661. doi: 10.1002/brb3.661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kashiwa Y, Kitabayashi Y, Narumoto J, Nakamura K, Ueda H, Fukui K. Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease: association with patient characteristics, psychiatric symptoms and cognitive deficits. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2005) 59:697–704. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01439.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Lehrner J, Kogler S, Lamm C, Moser D, Klug S, Pusswald G, et al. Awareness of memory deficits in subjective cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. (2015) 27:357–66. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214002245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Mak E, Chin R, Ng LT, Yeo D, Hameed S. Clinical associations of anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2015) 30:1207–14. doi: 10.1002/gps.4275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Nakaaki S, Murata Y, Sato J, Shinagawa Y, Hongo J, Tatsumi H, et al. Impact of depression on insight into memory capacity in patients with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2008) 22:369–74. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181820f58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Oba H, Matsuoka T, Imai A, Fujimoto H, Kato Y, Shibata K, et al. Interaction between memory impairment and depressive symptoms can exacerbate anosognosia: a comparison of Alzheimer's disease with mild cognitive impairment. Aging Ment Health. (2019) 23:595–601. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2018.1442411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Smith CA, Henderson VW, McCleary CA, Murdock GA, Buckwalter JG. Anosognosia and Alzheimer's disease: the role of depressive symptoms in mediating impaired insight. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2000) 22:437–44. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4;1-0;FT437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Sousa MF, Santos RL, Nogueira ML, Belfort T, Rosa RD, Torres B, et al. Awareness of disease is different for cognitive and functional aspects in mild Alzheimer's disease: a one-year observation study. J Alzheimers Dis. (2015) 4:905–13. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Spalletta G, Girardi P, Caltagirone C, Orfei MD. Anosognosia and neuropsychiatric symptoms and disorders in mild Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. (2012) 29:761–72. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-111886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Turro-Garriga O, Garre-Olmo J, Calvo-Perxas L, Rene-Ramirez R, Gascon-Bayarri J, Conde-Sala JL. Course and determinants of anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease: a 12-month follow-up. J Alzheimers Dis. (2016) 51:357–66. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Vogel A, Waldorff FB, Waldemar G. Impaired awareness of deficits and neuropsychiatric symptoms in early Alzheimer's disease: the Danish Alzheimer Intervention Study (DAISY). J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2010)22:93–9. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2010.22.1.93

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Marshall GA, Fairbanks LA, Tekin S, Vinters HV, Cummings JL. Neuropathologic correlates of apathy in Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2006) 21:144–7. doi: 10.1159/000090674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Clare L, Wilson BA. Longitudinal assessment of awareness in early-stage Alzheimer's disease using comparable questionnaire-based and performance-based measures: a prospective one-year follow-up study. Aging Ment Health. (2006) 10:156–65. doi: 10.1080/13607860500311888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Martin LA, Neighbors HW, Griffith DM. The experience of symptoms of depression in men vs. women: analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. JAMA Psychiatry. (2013) 70:1100–6. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.1985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Lenzoni S, Morris RG, Mograbi DC. The petrified elf 10 years after: current evidence for mnemonic anosognosia. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:465. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00465

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Amanzio M, Torta DM, Sacco K, Cauda F, D'Agata F, Duca S, et al. Unawareness of deficits in Alzheimer's disease: role of the cingulate cortex. Brain. (2011) 134(Pt 4):1061–76. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hallam B, Chan J, Gonzalez Costafreda S, Bhome R, Huntley J. What are the neural correlates of meta-cognition and anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease? a systematic review. Neurobiol Aging. (2020) 94:250–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Orgeta V, Qazi A, Spector A, Orrell M. Psychological treatments for depression and anxiety in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. (2015) 207:293–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.148130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Bhome R, Berry AJ, Huntley JD, Howard RJ. Interventions for subjective cognitive decline: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. (2018) 8:e021610. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-021610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Robert P, Onyike CU, Leentjens AF, Dujardin K, Aalten P, Starkstein S, et al. Proposed diagnostic criteria for apathy in Alzheimer's disease and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Eur Psychiatry. (2009) 24:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Radakovic R, Abrahams S. Multidimensional apathy: evidence from neurodegenerative disease. Curr Opini Behav Sci. (2018) 22:42–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.12.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Theleritis C, Siarkos K, Katirtzoglou E, Politis A. pharmacological and nonpharmacological treatment for apathy in Alzheimer disease: a systematic review across modalities. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. (2017) 30:26–49. doi: 10.1177/0891988716678684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Zhu CW, Grossman HT, Sano M. Why do they just sit? apathy as a core symptom of Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2019) 27:395–405. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2018.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Alzheimer's disease, awareness, depression, anxiety, apathy

Citation: Azocar I, Livingston G and Huntley J (2021) The Association Between Impaired Awareness and Depression, Anxiety, and Apathy in Mild to Moderate Alzheimer's Disease: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychiatry 12:633081. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.633081

Received: 24 November 2020; Accepted: 04 January 2021;
Published: 04 February 2021.

Edited by:

Josep Garre-Olmo, Institute of Biomedical Research of Girona, Spain

Reviewed by:

Hikaru Oba, Osaka University, Japan
Maria Donata Orfei, IMT School for Advanced Studies Lucca, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Azocar, Livingston and Huntley. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ignacia Azocar, bS5hem9jYXIuMTcmI3gwMDA0MDt1Y2wuYWMudWs=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.