Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 10 June 2021
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Purple Haze: Issues on Cannabis Legalization View all 17 articles

Cannabis and Cognitive Functioning: From Acute to Residual Effects, From Randomized Controlled Trials to Prospective Designs

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Addiction, Faculty of Medicine, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 3Centre de Recherche de l'Institut Universitaire en Santé Mentale de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada

In recent years, several jurisdictions have revised their regulation policy toward both medical and recreational use of cannabis. These changes have elicited concerns regarding how legalization impacts academic achievement and work performance. This review evaluates the acute and long-term (residual) association between cannabis use and cognitive functioning that underlies poor academic and work performance. Relative to other reviews, this article focuses on cross-over randomized controlled trials and prospective designs given that they allow to test the impairing effects of cannabis exposure at the within-subject level. Acute cannabis cognitive effects are discussed separately for known confounding factors such as levels of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ9-THC), Δ9-THC:cannabidiol ratio, previous cannabis use and, comorbidity with psychosis-spectrum disorders. The cognitive residual effects of cannabis are detailed in relation to duration of abstinence, frequency of use, comorbidity with psychosis-spectrum disorders, types of cognitive domains assessed, and age of cannabis use initiation. Moreover, considering the fact that adequate longitudinal studies can make inferences about causality between cannabis use and impaired cognitive functioning when disentangling between-subject from within-subject variation, proofs for the three main non-mutually exclusive hypotheses about this relationship will be presented: i) the cognitive vulnerability hypothesis as part of the more general common antecedent hypothesis, ii) the concurrent cannabis impairing hypothesis, and iii) the neurotoxic hypothesis of cannabis. Current research provides evidence for mild to moderate acute cannabis effects on episodic and working memory, processing speed, and executive functions. Mild residual impairing effects were also observed in these exact same cognitive domains, suggesting that adverse effects following cannabis intoxication persist at least days or weeks following cannabis abstinence. Relative to adult-onset, adolescent-onset cannabis use seems to explain the dose-response relationship and is associated with longer lasting residual effects even in mild users (<weekly). The association between cannabis and cognition is likely explained by common antecedents, such that genetic and shared environment factors predispose individuals to both cannabis use and cognitive deficits, and to a lesser degree, neurotoxic effects.

Introduction

In recent years, several jurisdictions have revised their regulation policy toward both medical and recreational use of cannabis. These changes have elicited concerns regarding how state and federal legislations impact cannabis use prevalence. In addition to the Canadian legalization of recreational use in 2018, more than 30 US states have legalized medical cannabis use, and more than 10 states have legalized its recreational use. In adult populations (>26 years old), evidence points toward increases in frequency of use and in rates of cannabis use disorders (CUD) pre- to post-medical and recreational laws (1, 2). The literature evaluating adolescent cannabis users is more complex (1, 3, 4). Recreational, but not medical legalization, seems to positively affect cannabis use prevalence, and only the most severe form of cannabis misuse (i.e., CUD) is affected by legislation changes (1, 3, 5, 6).

Another concern is the marked increase in concentrations of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ9-THC), the principal psychoactive agent contained in cannabis, since the 1970s and most specifically since the last decade. Concentrations of Δ9-THC ranged between 0.5 and 4.0% in the 1970s, whereas contemporary strains from North America, Europe, and Australia attain concentrations of 15% and over (711).

A renewed interest in understanding the potential adverse effects of cannabis use from a public health perspective has emerged following these changes in regulatory policy and cannabis potency. One such potential adverse effect is its impact on cognitive functioning, which may translate into lower academic achievement (1215), decreased work performance (16, 17), and a rise in the number of motor vehicle accidents (1820). Increasingly, studies show that adolescence may be a particularly vulnerable period for the cognitive effects of cannabis use. The known psychoactive effects of cannabis are exerted through its two main components, Δ9-THC and cannabidiol (CBD), and their action on the endogenous cannabinoid system. The endocannabinoid system is also tightly involved in neurodevelopmental processes such as neuronal specification, migration and maturation, axonal elongation, and synaptogenesis; processes that continue to occur during adolescence (21). Consequently, it has been proposed that the effects cannabis exert on cognition would be more deleterious if age of onset occurred during adolescence.

It is therefore imperative to review the literature investigating the potential effects of cannabis use on cognitive functioning to inform the public, as well as stakeholders. The first part of this article offers a narrative review of studies examining the acute effects of cannabis. An emphasis is placed on understanding the contribution of specific confounding factors such as the content in Δ9-THC of cannabis products, the Δ9-THC:CBD ratio, previous cannabis use, and comorbidity with psychosis-spectrum disorders. Considering that acute effects are most robustly examined with double-blind cross-over randomized controlled trials (RCT) which mitigate potential sources of experimental bias by testing effects at the within-subject level, the section on acute effects primarily discusses findings from these cross-over experiments, unless specified otherwise. In a second section, we discuss the residual effects (or long-term effects following abstinence) of regular cannabis use with a focus on both meta-analyses of cross-sectional studies and longitudinal studies. This second section will review how (i) duration of abstinence, (ii) frequency of use, (iii) psychosis-spectrum comorbidity, (iv) types of cognitive domains assessed, and (v) age of cannabis use initiation interact with the residual cognitive effects of cannabis. Considering the fact that adequate longitudinal studies can make inferences about causality between cannabis use and impaired cognitive functioning when disentangling between-subject from within-subject variation, proofs for the three main non-mutually exclusive hypotheses about this relationship will be presented: (i) the cognitive vulnerability hypothesis as part of the more general common antecedent hypothesis, (ii) the concurrent cannabis impairing hypothesis, and (iii) the neurotoxic hypothesis of cannabis.

Acute Effects

Acute effects refer to those relative to exposure–that is, cannabis-induced intoxication. The vast majority of studies on acute effects report impaired cognitive performance following cannabis/Δ9-THC exposure. A recent meta-analysis including more than 52 studies and 1,580 healthy individuals shows that verbal learning and memory (e.g., encoding, consolidation, retrieval), and working memory are the cognitive domain most impaired by acute cannabis-induced intoxication (22). Indeed, exposure to Δ9-THC or cannabis extract exerts moderate cognitive deficits (effect sizes: g = 0.69; g = 0.51; g = 0.51, respectively), in these three domains (22). These results echo prior well-documented evidence of acute impairments in these domains, notably in humans (23) as well as in rodents and non-human primates (24). Administration of cannabis also seems to elicit mild to moderate adverse effects on processing speed (g = 0.38) and executive functioning (g = 0.37) (22). Lastly, the latter meta-analysis explored the effects of acute cannabis exposure on attention and inhibitory (i.e., response inhibition and decision making) performance and reported only mild detrimental effects (g = 0.24; g = 0.28, respectively) (22). Regarding the speed of processing domain, we found that the harmful effects of cannabis/Δ9-THC were smaller in the oral administration studies relative to studies using other routes of administration, including smoked administration (effects are reported in Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Acute effects of cannabis use on cognitive functions.

One sub-domain of cognitive functioning that has recently received much attention is social cognition, which refers to a set of processes involving social interactions. These processes include mainly emotion recognition and the interpretation of others' emotional states (e.g., theory of mind). Among the few studies that investigated the acute effects of cannabis use on performance during social cognition tasks, some have reported impairments in emotional recognition of ambiguous faces (25) or threatening emotions such as fear and anger (26, 27), while this was not the case for other studies (28, 29). It is probable, but not certain, that exposure to Δ9-THC induces deficits in emotional recognition. Additional studies are needed to assess the quality of the evidence. As such, research linking cannabis use to impairments in theory of mind is insufficient and does not allow for the interpretation of potential effects on this sub-domain of socio-cognitive functioning.

Δ9-THC Content

Cross-over designs have demonstrated that the effects of cannabis in infrequent users on several cognitive functions occur in a dose-response fashion (refer to Supplementary Table 1 for a summary of studies). For instance, it was demonstrated that for smoking, intravenous and oral administration of Δ9-THC, the higher dosage (or higher serum concentration) induced significantly more detrimental effects on verbal learning and memory, reaction times, and response inhibition relative to lower doses (3035). Hart et al. (36) also found a dose-response relationship when investigating reaction times on various cognitive tasks, but not on performance accuracy when task time limit was not a factor. In addition to the absence of a time limit, this negative finding on performance accuracy from Hart et al. (36) could be explained by the fact that participants were daily users. Indeed, daily cannabis users often exhibit tolerance to the acute effects of cannabis on cognition (see section Previous cannabis use) and this may hinder efforts to demonstrate a dose-response relationship of cannabis on cognition.

Two studies have specifically investigated the effect of increasing concentration of Δ9-THC on decision making tasks (33, 37). The first demonstrated that the proportion of trials showing impairment increased as a function of serum concentration of Δ9-THC (33). The second found that only the higher dose yielded impairments relative to placebo (37). The failure to observe an effect at both doses in the second study may be due to the participants being daily users with tolerance to the impairing effects of cannabis and to the use of a small dose lower than reported to have an effect in occasional users.

Specifically for attention and working memory domains, the literature reports mixed findings: while most studies observed that the severity of impairments are a function of Δ9-THC content or performance is solely affected by the higher dose (3032, 34, 35, 38, 39), some found that these domains were unaffected by Δ9-THC (32, 34, 36). Reconciliation of these contradictory findings is challenging considering the heterogeneity in the tasks used. A detailed analysis of 15 published studies assessing the dose effects of Δ9-THC on digit-span performance, demonstrated that negative results may be due to short task length (and low number of trials, e.g., 3-min Digit Span task), which imparts lower sensitivity to detect an effect compared to longer task durations (39). Altogether, there is converging evidence that the cannabis impairing effects on verbal learning and memory, response inhibition, and psychomotor speed occur in a dose-response fashion. The linear relationship between exposure to higher Δ9-THC content and worse performance on decision making, attention, and working memory were less robust, and are therefore probable at best.

Δ9-THC:CBD Ratio

While Δ9-THC is responsible for the widely known psychoactive effects of cannabis (e.g., euphoria, psychological well-being, sensory experiences and appetite) (40), the effects of CBD are less well-understood. CBD is believed to be responsible for the anxiolytic and anti-inflammatory effects associated with cannabis use (41). When administered alone, without other cannabinoids, CBD may also have antipsychotic effects (41). What complicates research and generalizability of findings is that concentrations of Δ9-THC and CBD vary as a function of cannabis strains. For example, low doses of CBD can potentiate intoxicating Δ9-THC effects, while higher doses of CBD may reduce the intoxicating properties of Δ9-THC (42). As such, because of their different and sometimes even antagonistic properties (40), it is highly probable that Δ9-THC and CBD also exert distinct effects on cognitive functioning. To disentangle the ramification of these chemical compounds, an increasing number of experimental studies have specifically investigated the effect of different Δ9-THC:CBD ratios on cognition [(43), refer to Supplementary Table 2 for a summary of studies].

When investigating memory function (the cognitive domain most consistently impaired by cannabis), Schoedel et al. (44) observed that working memory performance (i.e., reaction times) was impaired by a high dose of synthetic Δ9-THC (dronabinol) compared to a placebo. However, performance following three different dosages of nabiximol (a compound with a Δ9-THC:CBD ratio of 1) was not different from placebo. On the contrary, in another within-subject cross-over design, administration of both Δ9-THC alone and Δ9-THC in combination with CBD induced deficits on episodic and working memory tasks. Only in the condition of exclusive CBD administration did subjects perform as well as during the placebo condition (45). The discrepancy in findings between these two studies could be explained by different Δ9-THC:CBD ratios, such that only at specific ratios does CBD attenuates the impairing effects of Δ9-THC. Between-subject designs provide further evidence of CBD attenuating the acute memory effects of Δ9-THC (4648). For example, an experimental study exploring between-subjects contrasts found that healthy participants treated with placebo prior to receiving Δ9-THC presented poorer delayed but not immediate recall relative to baseline, while the group pre-treated with CBD showed no impairment (48). However, pre-treatment with CBD did not attenuate the deficits observed in other cognitive domains, such as working memory, psychomotor functioning and executive functions. Using a naturalistic design, studies have also reported that while individuals who used cannabis strains with lower CBD content had marked impairment on various memory tasks, those smoking cannabis high in CBD concentrations showed no performance deficits relative to the placebo condition, independent of Δ9-THC levels and baseline performance (46, 47).

Among other cognitive domains, Hindocha et al. (25) demonstrated that Δ9-THC exposure led to impaired emotional recognition when compared to both placebo and combined Δ9-THC and CBD conditions. For psychomotor function and driving performances, mixed evidence was found regarding the attenuating effect of CBD on Δ9-THC (45, 49, 50). Lastly, in an effort-related decision making task, CBD did not mitigate the impairing effect of Δ9-THC relative to placebo (51).

Altogether, CBD seems to dampen the deleterious cognitive effects of acute Δ9-THC exposure, for memory at the very least. While encouraging, these findings do not provide information on the potential long-term protective effects of higher CBD concentrations on chronic cannabis use. Unfortunately, this question remains difficult to address, even following legalization of cannabis use. Investigators would need to gather information on Δ9-THC and CBD concentrations in cannabis strains, in large cohorts of participants, followed longitudinally.

Previous Cannabis Use

Another confound observed in the literature relating to the acute effects of cannabis is the users' status (e.g., non-/occasional users or regular/heavy users) (refer to Supplementary Table 3 for a summary of studies). Tolerance to the undesirable physiological effects of cannabis use among regular users was evidenced by RCT. Indeed, following Δ9-THC exposure, frequent users presented blunted perceptual alterations, psychotomimetic effects, anxiety, and increases in cortisol relative to occasional cannabis users, findings that could not be explained by group differences in plasma Δ9-THC (52). Five studies using a between-subject approach (difference between groups) of a cross-over placebo-controlled design have further investigated the presence of tolerance effects for the impairing effects of cannabis on cognition. Individuals with a cannabis use disorder (CUD), relative to non-users (i.e., <once/month), showed smaller Δ9-THC-induced impairments in immediate and delayed verbal memory tasks, while performing worse during the placebo condition (52). Similarly, administration of Δ9-THC (following pre-treatment with haloperidol) produced significant performance deficits on verbal learning and spatial working memory (not on verbal memory) in non-users specifically (53). However, Colizzi et al. (54) demonstrated that occasional and non-users did not perform differently on verbal memory during the drug condition. Of note, in this latter study, the authors failed to observe general Δ9-THC induced memory deficits across the whole sample. This negative finding could be explained by a lower sample size (n = 24 vs. 28 and 52) and/or the use of an intermediate oral dosage of Δ9-THC (10 mg; a dosage typically lower than those used in studies quantifying impairments by Δ9-THC content, refer to doses in Supplementary Table 1).

Working memory performance was also shown to be associated with tolerance effects: non-users made more errors during the Δ9-THC condition relative to placebo when compared to occasional users (53). Similarly, reduced accuracy and increased reaction times on attention tasks were observed only among occasional users relative to placebo, and not among regular/heavy users (52, 55, 56). Studies investigating how previous cannabis use modulates performance on response inhibition tasks showed inconsistent evidence (5456). In summary, it appears that the most frequent users of cannabis develop a targeted tolerance to the most robust Δ9-THC effects on cognition (i.e., memory, working memory, and attention).

Comorbidity With Psychosis-Spectrum Disorders

Considering that acute Δ9-THC exposure can induce transient positive psychotic symptoms among healthy individuals (30), and that cannabis-related cognitive deficits resemble the constellation of cognitive impairments observed in psychosis (57), this section focused exclusively on the modulating effect of a psychosis diagnosis or psychosis vulnerability in the relationship between cannabis and cognition. Results from robust between-subject comparison (patients vs. healthy controls) of cross-over placebo-controlled designs (within-subject design) do suggest an enhanced sensitivity to the cognitive impairing effect of Δ9-THC in psychosis (refer to Supplementary Table 4 for a summary of studies). For instance, D'Souza et al. (58) demonstrated that schizophrenia patients, relative to non-psychiatric individuals, showed greater verbal learning and verbal memory deficits following Δ9-THC administration relative to placebo. Another study revealed that adults with a genetic vulnerability to the psychosis-inducing properties of cannabis (Val/Val carriers on the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene) were significantly more impaired on verbal and visual memory (not learning) following Δ9-THC exposure, relative to those with a low genetic vulnerability (Met/Met and Val/Met carriers) (59). However, these studies failed to observe other drug condition (Δ9-THC vs. placebo) by group (diagnosis or genetic vulnerability) interactions for attention performance and psychomotor speed (60, 61). Finally, in at least one study, negative results on the attention task seem to be driven by missing data and thus a low sample size (60). Convincing evidence from within-subject design revealed that a psychosis comorbidity may exacerbate the cognitive-impairing effects of cannabis, at the very least for memory.

Residual Effects

Cross-Sectional Studies

Residual effects refer to an array of measurable negative effects that persist after the state of intoxication. These residual effects have been assessed between ~12 h following cannabis exposure to more prolonged periods of abstinence (e.g., over 1 year). At least five meta-analyses including over 69 cross-sectional studies have collected data from more than 8,000 cannabis users and non-users who had undergone cognitive assessment (6064). Worsened performances were consistently reported for learning and memory domains, with effect sizes ranging from small to moderate (6064). Converging evidence from the meta-analyses also showed small deficits (Cohen's d ~0.2–0.3) in attention, executive functioning (i.e., inhibition and cognitive flexibility), and processing speed (refer to Table 2) (6062). Interestingly, most of these domains (i.e., learning and memory, processing speed, and executive functions) were also more negatively affected in acute phases of intoxication, which suggests that adverse effects following cannabis intoxication persist days following cannabis abstinence. However, these cognitive deficits are categorized as mild. In comparison, residual effects of other substances, namely alcohol, cocaine and methamphetamine, are generally categorized as moderate (refer to Table 2) (6567).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Residual effects of cannabis use on cognitive functions in comparison to other substances.

The aforementioned meta-analyses also investigated the potential moderating effect of covariates such as age of cannabis use onset, age of participants, duration of use, duration of abstinence, and frequency of use. There is converging evidence that neither age of cannabis initiation, age of participants (adolescents vs. adults), nor duration of use were significant moderators (6064). The other two covariates are discussed in the following sections. Finally, in section Comorbidity with psychosis-spectrum disorders we discussed results from other meta-analyses which have focused on how psychosis spectrum comorbidity impacts the residual cognitive effects of cannabis use.

Duration of Abstinence

When meta-analyses focused on more chronic residual effects relative to effects from short abstinence periods, users (generally adults) no longer showed cognitive deficits, or showed significantly milder deficits. This finding was demonstrated by Scott et al. (62) for abstinence periods that persisted for more than 3 days, by Schoeler et al. (64) following 10 days of abstinence, and by Schreiner et al. (60) after ~1 month of cannabis use abstinence. This suggests that these residual effects have a short-term duration, but more importantly, that they are reversible. In the case of other substances like alcohol, cocaine and methamphetamine, residual effects that persisted after a month of abstinence (e.g., attention, learning, memory, and executive functioning) were instead categorized as moderate to large effect sizes. Before prematurely concluding that cannabis use is safer than other substance use, it should be noted that the majority of studies focusing on alcohol, cocaine and methamphetamine only included individuals who correspond to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) criteria for substance abuse, which complicates comparisons between various substances.

Deficits Increase as a Function of Use

When the effects of the frequency of cannabis use or a diagnostic of CUD are assessed on the amplitude of associated cognitive deficits, research showed a dose-response effect. Schoeler et al. (64) ascertained that mild use (e.g., <10 joints per month) was not associated with decreases in cognitive functioning; regular use (multiple times per week) was associated with deficits that were characterized as mild; and finally, daily use was associated with deficits that ranged from mild to moderate. Moreover, the cognitive deficits from daily use resembled alcohol-induced impairments in terms of importance, more specifically with regards to episodic memory. Similarly, individuals who are seeking treatment for substance abuse show global cognitive deficits of moderate amplitude, whereas those who do not seek treatment for substance abuse show only mild deficits (62). These moderate effect sizes for heavy cannabis users (criteria for abuse) resemble the severity of cognitive impairments reported in studies investigating the residual effects of other substances. Of note, the comparison between the residual cognitive effects of cannabis relative to other substances is challenging considering that the meta-analyses investigating alcohol, cocaine and methamphetamine included individuals meeting criteria for abuse and/or dependence (6567), while the vast majority of studies on cannabis included a wide range of users (from light to heavy users) not meeting those criteria. With regards to the duration of cognitive deficits in regular and daily users, findings are difficult to interpret, given that they are controversial. That is, many authors report that cognitive deficits in intelligence quotient (IQ), attention and episodic memory (e.g., learning) that are associated with chronic (daily) cannabis use persist even 3 to 4 weeks following abstinence (6870). However, other studies have also shown that these residual effects are reversed with >1 month of abstinence, and this was also the case for chronic users (7174).

Altogether, residual effects of cannabis use can be observed on a myriad of cognitive abilities, such as learning and memory, executive functions, and processing speed. These deficits are generally less severe than those observed for alcohol, cocaine and methamphetamine and also seem to be reversed more quickly. However, effects of cannabis on memory (also possibly executive functioning and processing speed) are similar to those of alcohol and cocaine when frequency and severity of use are considered.

In the absence of experimental designs, studies evaluating the residual effects of cannabis are observational and usually utilize cross-sectional between-subject designs, in which users are compared to non-users matched on potential confounding variables. This type of research design does not allow for inferences on causality—that is, if the observed cognitive deficits were present or not before cannabis use and if they are not explained by other confounders. Consequently, the following section focused on longitudinal population-based and genetically-informed (co-twin designs) studies that better address these issues.

Comorbidity With Psychosis-Spectrum Disorders

Meta-analyses of cross-sectional studies do not provide support for hypothesis that individuals with psychosis are more sensitive to the residual effects of cannabis, in contrast to observations from acute challenge studies. To the contrary, two meta-analyses concluded that cannabis-using psychosis patients exhibited superior (small-to-moderate effects) cognitive functioning for attention, executive functions, working memory, delayed memory, verbal fluency, and visuo-spatial abilities relative to non-using patients (75, 76). A further meta-analysis of first-episode psychosis patients did not observe significant differences in neurocognitive performance between patients with and without cannabis use (77). It is important to interpret these results with caution. For example, studies that utilize a diagnosis of CUD as an inclusion criterion often include individuals with a current diagnosis alongside those with a history of CUD who are now in remission (75), therefore introducing noise to the data. Moreover, results that support higher cognitive function in cannabis-using patients do not extend to those with heavy use (daily) or CUD. In their large multi-country study, Ferraro et al. (78) confirmed that the higher IQ observed in cannabis-using patients relative to non-using patients was attributable to patients with occasional but not daily use. A recent exploratory analysis reported that among psychosis patients with CUD, greater cumulative cannabis exposure was associated with poorer performance across several cognitive domains (attention, working memory, delayed memory, decision making, and response inhibition) (79). The direct comparison of cognitive performance between cannabis users with and without co-morbid psychotic disorders provides further support for the hypothesis that individuals with psychosis are more sensitive to the cognition-impairing effects of heavy cannabis use. Following a 1-month abstinence period, significant improvements in verbal memory were observed for psychosis patients with CUD relative to non-psychiatric individuals with CUD while controlling for performance prior to abstinence (70). It was proposed that this greater recovery of memory function following abstinence reflects a greater vulnerability to its impairing effects in psychosis. Altogether, the available evidence suggests that individuals with psychotic disorders who are occasional (but not heavy) users of cannabis may represent a phenotypically distinct patient group with more intact (premorbid) cognitive functioning. Importantly, more severe patterns of cannabis use (e.g., CUD or daily use) eventually negatively interfere with cognitive performance; a finding that is in agreement with the literature on acute effects.

Longitudinal Observational Studies

Results from prospective designs may agree with three non-mutually exclusive hypotheses linking cannabis use and cognitive functioning. The cognitive vulnerability hypothesis postulates that cognitive deficits are already present before the onset of cannabis use for individuals who present higher risk of becoming regular users. This vulnerability hypothesis is often formulated within the more general common antecedent hypothesis. The latter proposes that common factors may predispose individuals to both cannabis use and mild cognitive decline in users, without cannabis use being the cause of these cognitive deficits, and without any specificity about the timing of such deficits. In contrast, the concurrent hypothesis posits that cannabis use is associated with cognitive deficits when controlling for premorbid cognitive performance, but only in short-term. It is proposed that abstinence or decreases in cannabis use should help alleviate these deficits. Lastly, the neurotoxicity hypothesis stipulates that past cannabis use induces a cognitive decline that persists even after individuals refrain from or decrease their cannabis use, when adjusting for cognitive functioning prior to cannabis use (see Figure 1 for a graphical representation of the three hypotheses within the context of mixed effects linear modeling).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Representation of the cognitive vulnerability, concurrent, and neurotoxicity hypotheses relative to the association between cannabis use and cognitive functioning. The cognitive vulnerability hypothesis (represented by the green square) posits that before onset of cannabis use, future cannabis users already exhibit cognitive deficits. The common antecedent hypothesis, which offers a more general framework than the cognitive vulnerability hypothesis, posits that unknown common factors could be responsible for cannabis use onset and mild cognitive deteriorations, without cannabis use being the causal factor of the aforementioned cognitive deficits. Black dotted arrows allow to investigate the neurotoxic hypothesis by testing if previous cannabis use (t−1) predicts subsequent cognitive functioning (t), while controlling for frequency of cannabis use at time t. Lastly, black bidirectional arrows between cognitive abilities and cannabis use at every time-point represent the concurrent hypothesis. Indeed, cognitive performance at time t is associated with cannabis use at time t, without necessarily persisting effects through time.

Cognitive Vulnerability and Common Antecedent Hypotheses

The premorbid cognitive vulnerability hypothesis (e.g., before the onset of cannabis use) has been confirmed by recent studies. Findings show that future cannabis users already show lower performance at IQ tasks (non-verbal and verbal), memory, and executive functions (e.g., inhibitory control) when compared to individuals who remain non-users (8084). As such, specific cognitive deficits seem to predispose individuals to earlier onset and more regular cannabis use. However, other studies did not provide evidence that cognitive impairment was apparent prior to cannabis use initiation (68, 8588). As evidenced by rigorous co-twin designs, this cognitive vulnerability disappears when investigating individuals nested in a family, such that monozygotic and dizygotic twins discordant for cannabis use or cannabis dependence do not show differences in cognitive abilities prior to cannabis initiation (83, 84). These later twin studies do not support the purely cognitive vulnerability hypothesis, but do support the idea that common antecedents such as family factors (i.e., genetic and shared environment factors) explain this cognitive vulnerability observed at the population level. Clinical and behavioral factors have been put forth as common factors that predispose individuals to both cannabis use and cognitive deficits (89). For example, externalizing disorders as well as behavioral disinhibition have been positively associated with substance use and negatively associated with IQ (90, 91), suggesting that youths exhibiting externalizing symptoms and delinquency are less likely to be motivated to perform well at school and thus disengage from learning, and are more likely to use substances as a consequence of these problems.

Concurrent Hypothesis

When accounting for premorbid cognitive performance, cannabis use was associated with cognitive decline, at least in the short-term (during the same assessment intervals), in executive functioning, general IQ, memory, processing speed, and visuospatial abilities in several studies (68, 71, 81, 85, 88, 92, 93). Declines in cognitive functioning were observed years after the onset of cannabis use and were obvious even when taking into account other substance use (68, 71, 81, 85, 93), academic achievement (68, 85, 92), externalizing problems or other mental health comorbidity (68, 71), and socioeconomic status (71, 81, 85, 88, 94). Without eliminating the possibility that these factors could have played a mitigating role, controlling for these covariates increases our confidence in the idea that cannabis could have deleterious effects on cognitive functioning. Only a few studies did not report concurrent impairing effects of cannabis use (82, 86). Of note, among the studies that investigated the concurrent hypothesis from a within-subject perspective, two out of three revealed that if an individual shows increases in cannabis use frequency at a given assessment, they will also show lower executive functions performance during that same assessment period (8082). The results were partially replicated within co-twin designs. Among several tests measuring non-verbal and verbal IQ, as well as executive functioning (i.e., working memory, response inhibition, and cognitive flexibility), poorer performance in twins who used cannabis more frequently than their co-twin was limited to two tasks (one measuring working memory, the other, non-verbal IQ) (83, 84, 95). Altogether, these findings are in line with impairments in cognitive domains that were underlined by meta-analyses of cross-sectional studies investigating residual effects of cannabis use, as well as studies focusing on the acute effects of Δ9-THC intoxication.

Neurotoxic Hypothesis

Longitudinal studies provide mixed evidence for the neurotoxic hypothesis. On the one hand, former regular users showed better cognitive development than current regular users (92) and even performed as well as non-users (71), suggesting that cannabis impairing effects tend to resolve following abstinence. Similarly, Jacobus et al. (93) demonstrated that cannabis users performed more poorly than non-users across various cognitive domains, yet this performance difference disappeared at the last follow-up when users had reduced their overall consumption. On the other hand, cannabis use frequency was shown to predict subsequent cognitive decline in executing functioning and verbal intelligence regardless of whether cannabis use continued (87, 88). Specifically, Castellanos-Ryan et al. (80) and Meier et al. (68) provided evidence that a significant reduction of cannabis use (from daily to light user) or abstinence in the 12 months prior to cognitive testing were still significantly associated with a decline in executive functioning and general IQ. Furthermore, in their population cohort, Morin et al. (81) observed that over and above the concurrent impairing effect of cannabis use at the individual level, if one increases their cannabis use frequency in a given year, one will also show lower performance on response inhibition a year later. This latter study provides robust evidence of a long-term (at least 12 months) or neurotoxic effect of cannabis use considering that individuals who changed their patterns of cannabis use through the follow-ups were compared to themselves. Despite these proofs of neurotoxic effects from cannabis use with extensive covariate control, we cannot rule out the possibility that part of the variance between cannabis and subsequent poorer cognitive performance comes from indirect causal effects, for example, through social milieu (96, 97).

Factors Modulating the Residual Cognitive Effects of Cannabis

Quantities Used

In line with cross-sectional studies, it is when we distinguish occasional, regular and heavy users that cognitive deficits in memory or processing speed become more apparent (71). Indeed, memory deficits associated with weekly use of cannabis are in the range of moderate effect sizes (98), which bears resemblance to the effects of alcohol abuse. Similarly, other findings show that for each 5-year period of cannabis use, performance on memory tasks progressively decrease (99). Beyond long-term memory, research has shown that frequency and dependence of cannabis use are positively related to worse executive function and IQ deficits (68, 80, 81, 84, 85, 87). A paucity of studies did not report dose-response effects on associated cognitive deficits (82, 83, 86, 100, 101) however, some of these studies assessed cognitive domains that are not considered to be affected by cannabis use (e.g., lexical knowledge) (83, 101).

Cognitive Domains

It is important to underline that not all longitudinal studies have assessed residual effects of cannabis use on cognitive functioning more broadly. For example, a few studies have focused solely on the association between cannabis use and verbal fluency (88) or orientation [Mini Mental State Examination: (101)], and have therefore not reported any associations between cannabis use and cognitive deficits. When considered alone, these studies may falsely lead us to believe that cannabis use does not alter cognitive performance, regardless of the studied cognitive domain. However, converging findings from all studies help better explain the relation between cannabis use and cognitive deficits. Indeed, among 10 prospective studies that assessed memory, eight reported specific deficits in this cognitive domain (71, 74, 8082, 92, 93, 98100). Likewise, 7 of 10 studies investigating associations between cannabis use and executive function (i.e., response inhibition) showed declines in performance linked to cannabis use (68, 8082, 84, 87, 93, 95, 99, 100). Findings of effect on processing speed, however, are less robust with three of seven studies reporting declines in performance linked to cannabis use (68, 71, 82, 92, 93, 98, 99). Finally, long-term effects of cannabis use on non-verbal IQ are mildly probable, as 6 of 10 studies have failed to show significant associations here (68, 71, 8186, 93, 95).

Age of Cannabis Initiation

An increasing number of studies have endeavored to test the hypothesis that adolescence consists in a vulnerable period to the impairing effects of cannabis use. Generally, results can be summarized as follows (i) for an equivalent consumption, cognitive deficits seem to be more important in those who initiated cannabis use younger (e.g., during adolescence) (68), (ii) deficits noted in adolescents are similar to those observed in adults, but appear following less intensive use of cannabis (80, 81, 87); (iii) a combination of both. For example, an interesting study showed potentially additive negative effects on global performance on IQ tasks between the number of years of cannabis use and age of onset that is earlier than 18 years old (68). Moreover, the dose-response relationship highlighted by Meier et al. (68) on IQ performance was explained by adolescent-onset cannabis use, not adult-onset use. Studies conducted on three independent samples of Canadian and US adolescents have shown that increases in cannabis use during high school predicted cognitive declines in performance on memory and executive functions tasks a few years after assessment (80, 81, 87). In addition to this, it should be noted that these cognitive effects were noted in young individuals who were for the most part not heavy users (<weekly use). Moreover, age of onset of cannabis use that was prior to 15 years old compared to age of onset that occurred after 14 years old was related to impaired development of inhibition capacities, independently of the frequency of cannabis use (80). Critically, these deficits seemed more permanent than the ones reported by adults (71, 98). That is, increases in cannabis use during adolescence were associated with declines in executive functioning and IQ scores at age 20, and even until age 38, and this was also the case for individuals who had considerably reduced their consumption 12 months prior to cognitive assessments (68, 80). Taken together, these findings suggest that adolescence represents a critical period for vulnerability to deleterious effects of cannabis use on cognitive functioning.

Discussion

The current comprehensive review highlights that the acute administration of cannabis/THC produces moderate impairments in episodic and working memory, as well as small to moderate deficits in processing speed and executive functions. Impairments in attention and impulsivity have also been documented but are smaller. In the case of speed of processing, there is evidence showing that the impairments are less severe in oral administration studies relative to studies using other routes of administration (e.g., smoked, inhaled, injected). Although some studies have shown that higher Δ9-THC concentrations are associated with more prominent cognitive impairments, further studies are required to establish what doses are problematic. Likewise, there is preliminary evidence showing the cannabidiol may attenuate Δ9-THC-induced cognitive impairments, but results are inconclusive thus far. While several studies on the acute effects of cannabis/Δ9-THC have paid attention to traditional cognitive domains such as attention, episodic memory, executive functions, speed of processing, and working memory, there is a relative lack of research on the effects of cannabis/Δ9-THC on social cognition (e.g., theory of mind and emotion recognition).

Cross-sectional studies on the residual cognitive effects have generally shown that cannabis is associated with cognitive deficits that are relatively small and seem to abate after a relatively short period of abstinence. Such studies seem to indicate that cannabis produces smaller cognitive deficits than those produced by alcohol, cocaine or methamphetamine, which typically produce moderate deficits in several cognitive domains. It is crucial to point out, however, that the meta-analyses on alcohol, cocaine and methamphetamines have been performed using studies involving individuals with a substance use disorder, whereas the great majority of studies on cannabis have been performed in occasional, regular or frequent users. Future studies in the field will need to pay attention to individuals meeting the criteria for a cannabis use disorder.

Due to the methodological limitations of cross-sectional studies, a growing number of high-quality longitudinal studies have been performed in recent years. In these studies, residual impairments were observed mostly in the same cognitive domains (e.g., verbal learning and memory, speed of processing) that have been shown to be impaired in the acute administration studies. Research results suggest that the cognitive effects following cannabis intoxication persist at least days or weeks following cannabis abstinence in regular users. Relative to adult-onset, adolescent-onset cannabis use seems to explain the dose-response relationship that has been observed and is associated with longer lasting residual effects even in not so heavy users (<weekly). The association between cannabis and cognition is likely explained by common antecedents, such as genetics and shared environment factors. To a lesser degree, cannabis may also produce neurotoxic effects. Further large-scale longitudinal studies on the cognitive effects of cannabis are required, paying careful attention to premorbid cognitive performance, dose-response, cannabis constituents, and potential common antecedents.

As for the cognitive effects of cannabis in individuals with a comorbid psychiatric disorder, such as schizophrenia, research results are unfortunately difficult to interpret as the vast majority of studies in the field have adopted cross-sectional designs. Clearly, longitudinal studies in these populations are warranted. Finally, it is worth mentioning that the literature on “synthetic cannabinoids” is scarce. Considering that “synthetic cannabinoids” are full agonists at CB1 receptors (in comparison, Δ9-THC is a partial agonist), they may theoretically produce cognitive impairments that are more prominent and longer lasting than those of cannabis (102). With a growing number of states and countries liberalizing their policies on cannabis, the study of the cognitive effects of cannabis has important implications, since cannabis smoking may be associated with lower academic achievement, decreased work performance, and increased rates of motor vehicle accidents. Careful attention will need to be paid to policies and program that could minimize these undesirable outcomes. Such measures include disseminating public health campaigns on the hazards of cannabis use, implementing evidence-based preventive interventions in schools, prohibiting the marketing of cannabis products in ways that are attractive to youth, taxing cannabis products based on their Δ9-THC content, and regulating maximal Δ9-THC concentrations.

Author Contributions

JB and SP reviewed the literature. JB wrote the manuscript. SP provided critical comments.

Funding

This study was supported by grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (FRN-148561 to SP and FRN-170130 to JB and SP).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

JB was supported by post-doctoral fellowships from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and the Fonds de Recherche du Québec en Santé. SP is holder of the Eli Lilly Canada Chair on schizophrenia research.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.596601/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Cerdá M, Mauro C, Hamilton A, Levy NS, Santaella-Tenorio J, Hasin D, et al. Association between recreational marijuana legalization in the United States and changes in marijuana use and cannabis use disorder from 2008 to 2016. JAMA Psychiatry. (2020) 77:165–71. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.3254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Williams AR, Santaella-Tenorio J, Mauro CM, Levin FR, Martins SS. Loose regulation of medical marijuana programs associated with higher rates of adult marijuana use but not cannabis use disorder. Addiction. (2017) 112:1985–91. doi: 10.1111/add.13904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Sarvet al, Wall MM, Fink DS, Greene E, Le A, Boustead AE, et al. Medical marijuana laws and adolescent marijuana use in the United States: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction. (2018) 113:1003–16. doi: 10.1111/add.14136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Dilley JA, Richardson SM, Kilmer B, Pacula RL, Segawa MB, Cerdá M. Prevalence of cannabis use in youths after legalization in Washington state. JAMA Pediatrics. (2019) 173:192–3. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.4458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Wall MM, Mauro C, Hasin DS, Keyes KM, Cerda M, Martins SS, et al. Prevalence of marijuana use does not differentially increase among youth after states pass medical marijuana laws: commentary on Stolzenberg et al. (2015) and reanalysis of US national survey on drug use in households data 2002-2011. Int J Drug Policy. (2016) 29:9–13. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2016.01.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Choo EK, Benz M, Zaller N, Warren O, Rising KL, McConnell KJ. The impact of state medical marijuana legislation on adolescent marijuana use. J Adolesc Heal. (2014) 55:160–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Cascini F, Aiello C, Di Tanna G. Increasing delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (δ−9-THC) content in herbal cannabis over time: systematic review and meta-analysis. Curr Drug Abuse Rev. (2012) 5:32–40. doi: 10.2174/1874473711205010032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. ElSohly MA, Mehmedic Z, Foster S, Gon C, Chandra S, Church JC. Changes in cannabis potency over the last 2 decades (1995-2014): analysis of current data in the United States. Biol Psychiatry. (2016) 79:613–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Potter DJ, Clark P, Brown MB. Potency of Δ9-THC and other cannabinoids in cannabis in England in 2005: implications for psychoactivity and pharmacology. J Forensic Sci. (2008) 53:90–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1556-4029.2007.00603.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Swift W, Wong A, Li KM, Arnold JC, McGregor IS. Analysis of cannabis seizures in NSW, Australia: cannabis potency and cannabinoid profile. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e70052. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Dujourdy L, Besacier F. A study of cannabis potency in France over a 25 years period (1992–2016). Forensic Sci Int. (2017) 272:72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.forsciint.2017.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Horwood LJ, Fergusson DM, Hayatbakhsh MR, Najman JM, Coffey C, Patton GC, et al. Cannabis use and educational achievement: findings from three Australasian cohort studies. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2010) 110:247–53. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Fergusson DM, Boden JM, Horwood LJ. Psychosocial sequelae of cannabis use and implications for policy: findings from the christchurch health and development study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatric Epidemiol. (2015) 50:1317–26. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1070-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Stiby AI, Hickman M, Munafò MR, Heron J, Yip VL, Macleod J. Adolescent cannabis and tobacco use and educational outcomes at age 16: birth cohort study. Addiction. (2015) 110:658–68. doi: 10.1111/add.12827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Lynskey MT, Coffey C, Degenhardt L, Carlin JB, Patton G. A longitudinal study of the effects of adolescent cannabis use on high school completion. Addiction. (2003) 98:685–92. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00356.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. MacDonald S, Hall W, Roman P, Stockwell T, Coghlan M, Nesvaag S. Testing for cannabis in the work-place: a review of the evidence. Addiction. (2010) 105:408–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02808.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bernerth JB, Walker HJ. Altered states or much to do about nothing? A study of when cannabis is used in relation to the impact it has on performance. Gr Organ Manag. (2020) 45:459–78. doi: 10.1177/1059601120917590

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Hartman RL, Huestis MA. Cannabis effects on driving skills. Clin Chem. (2013) 59:478–92. doi: 10.1373/clinchem.2012.194381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Ramaekers JG, Berghaus G, Van Laar M, Drummer OH. Dose related risk of motor vehicle crashes after cannabis use. Drug and Alcohol Depend. (2004) 73:109–19. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2003.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Asbridge M, Hayden JA, Cartwright JL. Acute cannabis consumption and motor vehicle collision risk: systematic review of observational studies and meta-analysis. BMJ. (2012) 344:e536. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Galve-Roperh I, Palazuelos J, Aguado T, Guzmán M. The endocannabinoid system and the regulation of neural development: potential implications in psychiatric disorders. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2009) 259:371–82. doi: 10.1007/s00406-009-0028-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Zhornitsky S, Pelletier J, Assaf R, Li C, Potvin S. Acute effects of partial CB1 receptor agonists on humans: a meta-analysis of human studies. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2021) 104:110063. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.110063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Broyd SJ, Van Hell HH, Beale C, Yücel M, Solowij N. Acute and chronic effects of cannabinoids on human cognition - a systematic review. Biol Psychiatry. (2016) 79:557–67. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Lichtman AH, Varvel SA, Martin BR. Endocannabinoids in cognition and dependence. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fat Acids. (2002) 66:269–85. doi: 10.1054/plef.2001.0351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hindocha C, Freeman TP, Schafer G, Gardener C, Das RK, Morgan CJA, et al. Acute effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol, cannabidiol and their combination on facial emotion recognition: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in cannabis users. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2015) 25:325–34. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ballard ME, Bedi G, De Wit H. Effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol on evaluation of emotional images. J Psychopharmacol. (2012) 26:1289–98. doi: 10.1177/0269881112446530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bossong MG, van Hell HH, Jager G, Kahn RS, Ramsey NF, Jansma JM. The endocannabinoid system and emotional processing: a pharmacological fMRI study with {increment}9-tetrahydrocannabinol. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2013) 23:1687–97. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Fusar-Poli P, Crippa J, Bhattacharyya S, Borgwardt SJ, Allen P, Martin-Santos R, et al. Distinct effects of A9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol on neural activation during emotional processing. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2009) 66:95–105. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Phan KL, Angstadt M, Golden J, Onyewuenyi I, Popovska A, De Wit H. Cannabinoid modulation of amygdala reactivity to social signals of threat in humans. J Neurosci. (2008) 28:2313–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5603-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. D'Souza DC, Perry E, MacDougall L, Ammerman Y, Cooper T, Wu Y, et al. The psychotomimetic effects of intravenous delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy individuals: implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacol. (2004) 29:1558–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hunault CC, Mensinga TT, Böcker KBE, Schipper CMA, Kruidenier M, Leenders MEC, et al. Cognitive and psychomotor effects in males after smoking a combination of tobacco and cannabis containing up to 69 mg delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC). Psychopharmacology. (2009) 204:85–94. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1440-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Curran VH, Brignell C, Fletcher S, Middleton P, Henry J. Cognitive and subjective dose-response effects of acute oral Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in infrequent cannabis users. Psychopharmacology. (2002) 164:61–70. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1169-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Ramaekers JG, Moeller MR, van Ruitenbeek P, Theunissen EL, Schneider E, Kauert G. Cognition and motor control as a function of Δ9-THC concentration in serum and oral fluid: limits of impairment. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2006) 85:114–22. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Heishman SJ, Arasteh K, Stitzer ML. Comparative effects of alcohol and marijuana on mood, memory, and performance. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (1997) 58:93–101. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(96)00456-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Böcker KBE, Gerritsen J, Hunault CC, Kruidenier M, Mensinga TT, Kenemans JL. Cannabis with high Δ9-THC contents affects perception and visual selective attention acutely: an event-related potential study. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2010) 96:67–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Hart CL, Van Gorp W, Haney M, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Effects of acute smoked marijuana on complex cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacol. (2001) 25:757–65. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00273-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Weinstein A, Brickner O, Lerman H, Greemland M, Bloch M, Lester H, et al. A study investigating the acute dose-response effects of 13 mg and 17 mg Δ 9- tetrahydrocannabinol on cognitive-motor skills, subjective and autonomic measures in regular users of marijuana. J Psychopharmacol. (2008) 22:441–51. doi: 10.1177/0269881108088194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Spindle TR, Cone EJ, Schlienz NJ, Mitchell JM, Bigelow GE, Flegel R, et al. Acute effects of smoked and vaporized cannabis in healthy adults who infrequently use cannabis: a crossover trial. JAMA Netw Open. (2018) 1:e184841. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.4841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Adam KCS, Doss MK, Pabon E, Vogel EK, de Wit H. Δ9-Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) impairs visual working memory performance: a randomized crossover trial. Neuropsychopharmacol. (2021) 45:1807–16. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0690-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Martin-Santos R, Crippa JA, Batalla A, Bhattacharyya S, Atakan Z, Borgwardt S, et al. Acute effects of a single, oral dose of d9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) and cannabidiol (CBD) administration in healthy volunteers. Curr Pharm Des. (2012) 18:4966–79. doi: 10.2174/138161212802884780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Mechoulam R, Parker LA, Gallily R. Cannabidiol: an overview of some pharmacological aspects. J Clin Pharmacol. (2002) 42:11S−9S. doi: 10.1002/j.1552-4604.2002.tb05998.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Solowij N, Broyd S, Marie GL, van Hell H, Martelozzo D, Rueb K, et al. A randomised controlled trial of vaporised Δ 9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol alone and in combination in frequent and infrequent cannabis users: acute intoxication effects. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2019) 269:17–35. doi: 10.1007/s00406-019-00978-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Colizzi M, Bhattacharyya S. Does cannabis composition matter? Differential effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol on human cognition. Curr Addiction Rep. (2017) 4:62–74. doi: 10.1007/s40429-017-0142-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Schoedel KA, Chen N, Hilliard A, White L, Stott C, Russo E, et al. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study to evaluate the subjective abuse potential and cognitive effects of nabiximols oromucosal spray in subjects with a history of recreational cannabis use. Hum Psychopharmacol. (2011) 26:224–36. doi: 10.1002/hup.1196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Morgan CJA, Freeman TP, Hindocha C, Schafer G, Gardner C, Curran HV. Individual and combined effects of acute delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol on psychotomimetic symptoms and memory function. Transl Psychiatry. (2018) 8:181. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0191-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Morgan CJA, Schafer G, Freeman TP, Curran HV. Impact of cannabidiol on the acute memory and psychotomimetic effects of smoked cannabis: naturalistic study. Br J Psychiatry. (2010) 197:285–90. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.077503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Morgan CJA, Gardener C, Schafer G, Swan S, Demarchi C, Freeman TP, et al. Sub-chronic impact of cannabinoids in street cannabis on cognition, psychotic-like symptoms and psychological well-being. Psychol Med. (2012) 42:391–400. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711001322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Englund A, Morrison PD, Nottage J, Hague D, Kane F, Bonaccorso S, et al. Cannabidiol inhibits THC-elicited paranoid symptoms and hippocampal-dependent memory impairment. J Psychopharmacol. (2013) 27:19–27. doi: 10.1177/0269881112460109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Arkell TR, Lintzeris N, Kevin RC, Ramaekers JG, Vandrey R, Irwin C, et al. Cannabidiol (CBD) content in vaporized cannabis does not prevent tetrahydrocannabinol (THC)-induced impairment of driving and cognition. Psychopharmacology. (2019) 236:2713–24. doi: 10.1007/s00213-019-05246-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Roser P, Gallinat J, Weinberg G, Juckel G, Gorynia I, Stadelmann AM. Psychomotor performance in relation to acute oral administration of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol and standardized cannabis extract in healthy human subjects. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2009) 259:284–92. doi: 10.1007/s00406-009-0868-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Lawn W, Freeman TP, Pope RA, Joye A, Harvey L, Hindocha C, et al. Acute and chronic effects of cannabinoids on effort-related decision-making and reward learning: an evaluation of the cannabis ‘amotivational' hypotheses. Psychopharmacology. (2016) 233:3537–52. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4383-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. D'Souza DC, Ranganathan M, Braley G, Gueorguieva R, Zimolo Z, Cooper T, et al. Blunted psychotomimetic and amnestic effects of Δ-9- tetrahydrocannabinol in frequent users of cannabis. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2008) 33:2505–16. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. D'Souza DC, Braley G, Blaise R, Vendetti M, Oliver S, Pittman B, et al. Effects of haloperidol on the behavioral, subjective, cognitive, motor, and neuroendocrine effects of Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in humans. Psychopharmacology. (2008) 198:587–603. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1042-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Colizzi M, McGuire P, Giampietro V, Williams S, Brammer M, Bhattacharyya S. Modulation of acute effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol on psychotomimetic effects, cognition and brain function by previous cannabis exposure. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2018) 28:850–62. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Theunissen EL, Kauert GF, Toennes SW, Moeller MR, Sambeth A, Blanchard MM, et al. Neurophysiological functioning of occasional and heavy cannabis users during THC intoxication. Psychopharmacology. (2012) 220:341–50. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2479-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Ramaekers JG, Kauert G, Theunissen EL, Toennes SW, Moeller MR. Neurocognitive performance during acute THC intoxication in heavy and occasional cannabis users. J Psychopharmacol. (2009) 23:266–77. doi: 10.1177/0269881108092393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Solowij N, Michie PT. Cannabis and cognitive dysfunction: Parallels with endophenotypes of schizophrenia? J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2007) 32:30–52.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

58. D'Souza DC, Abi-Saab WM, Madonick S, Forselius-Bielen K, Doersch A, Braley G, et al. Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol effects in schizophrenia: implications for cognition, psychosis, and addiction. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:594–608. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Henquet C, Rosa A, Krabbendam L, Papiol S, Fananás L, Drukker M, et al. An experimental study of catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met moderation of Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol-induced effects on psychosis and cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2006) 31:2748–57. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Schreiner AM, Dunn ME. Residual effects of cannabis use on neurocognitive performance after prolonged abstinence: a meta-analysis. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2012) 20:420–9. doi: 10.1037/a0029117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Lovell ME, Akhurst J, Padgett C, Garry MI, Matthews A. Cognitive outcomes associated with long-term, regular, recreational cannabis use in adults: a meta-analysis. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2020) 28:471–94. doi: 10.1037/pha0000326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Scott JC, Slomiak ST, Jones JD, Rosen AFG, Moore TM, Gur RC. Association of cannabis with cognitive functioning in adolescents and young adults a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. (2018) 75:585–95. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Grant I, Gonzalez R, Carey CL, Natarajan L, Wolfson T. Non-acute (residual) neurocognitive effects of cannabis use: a meta-analytic study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2003) 9:679–89. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703950016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Schoeler T, Kambeitz J, Behlke I, Murray R, Bhattacharyya S. The effects of cannabis on memory function in users with and without a psychotic disorder: findings from a combined meta-analysis. Psychol Med. (2016) 46:177–88. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715001646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Potvin S, Pelletier J, Grot S, Hébert C, Barr A, Lecomte T. Cognitive deficits in individuals with methamphetamine use disorder: a meta-analysis. Addict Behav. (2018) 80:154–60. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Potvin S, Stavro K, Rizkallah É, Pelletier J. Cocaine and cognition: a systematic quantitative review. J Addict Med. (2014) 8:368–76. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0000000000000066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Stavro K, Pelletier J, Potvin S. Widespread and sustained cognitive deficits in alcoholism: a meta-analysis. Addict Biol. (2013) 18:203–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00418.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Meier MH, Caspi A, Ambler A, Harrington HL, Houts R, Keefe RSE, et al. Persistent cannabis users show neuropsychological decline from childhood to midlife. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2012) 109: E2657–64. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206820109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Bolla KI, Brown K, Eldreth D, Tate K, Cadet JL. Dose-related neurocognitive effects of marijuana use. Neurology. (2002) 59:1337–43. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000031422.66442.49

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Rabin RA, Barr MS, Goodman MS, Herman Y, Zakzanis KK, Kish SJ, et al. Effects of extended cannabis abstinence on cognitive outcomes in cannabis dependent patients with schizophrenia vs non-psychiatric controls. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2017) 42:2259–71. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Fried PA, Watkinson B, Gray R. Neurocognitive consequences of marihuana - a comparison with pre-drug performance. Neurotoxicol Teratol. (2005) 27:231–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Pope HG, Gruber AJ, Hudson JI, Huestis MA, Yurgelun-Todd D. Cognitive measures in long-term cannabis users. J Clin Pharmcol. (2002) 42:41S-7S. doi: 10.1002/j.1552-4604.2002.tb06002.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Pope HG, Gruber AJ, Hudson JI, Huestis MA, Yurgelun-Todd D. Neuropsychological performance in long-term cannabis users. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2001) 58:909–15. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.10.909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hanson KL, Winward JL, Schweinsburg AD, Medina KL, Brown SA, Tapert SF. Longitudinal study of cognition among adolescent marijuana users over three weeks of abstinence. Addict Behav. (2010) 35:970–6. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Rabin RA, Zakzanis KK, George TP. The effects of cannabis use on neurocognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. (2011) 128:111–6. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Yücel M, Bora E, Lubman DI, Solowij N, Brewer WJ, Cotton SM, et al. The impact of cannabis use on cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of existing findings and new data in a first-episode sample. Schizophr Bull. (2012) 38:316–30. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Sánchez-Gutiérrez T, Fernandez-Castilla B, Barbeito S, González-Pinto A, Becerra-García JA, Calvo A. Cannabis use and nonuse in patients with first-episode psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis of studies comparing neurocognitive functioning. Eur Psychiatry. (2020) 63:e6. doi: 10.1192/j.eurpsy.2019.9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Ferraro L, La Cascia C, Quattrone D, Sideli L, Matranga D, Capuccio V, et al. Premorbid adjustment and iq in patients with first-episode psychosis: a multisite case-control study of their relationship with cannabis use. Schizophr Bull. (2020) 46:517–29. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbz077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Rabin RA, Zakzanis KK, Daskalakis ZJ, George TP. Effects of cannabis use status on cognitive function, in males with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. (2013) 206:158–65. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Castellanos-Ryan N, Pingault JB, Parent S, Vitaro F, Tremblay RE, Séguin JR. Adolescent cannabis use, change in neurocognitive function, and high-school graduation: a longitudinal study from early adolescence to young adulthood. Dev Psychopathol. (2017) 29:1253–66. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416001280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Morin JFG, Afzali MH, Bourque J, Stewart SH, Séguin JR, O'Leary-Barrett M, et al. A population-based analysis of the relationship between substance use and adolescent cognitive development. Am J Psychiatry. (2019) 176:98–106. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18020202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Infante MA, Nguyen-Louie TT, Worley M, Courtney KE, Coronado C, Jacobus J. Neuropsychological trajectories associated with adolescent alcohol and cannabis use: a prospective 14-year study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2020) 26:480–91. doi: 10.1017/S1355617719001395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Jackson NJ, Isen JD, Khoddam R, Irons D, Tuvblad C, Iacono WG, et al. Impact of adolescent marijuana use on intelligence: results from two longitudinal twin studies. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2016) 113:E500–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1516648113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Meier MH, Caspi A, Danese A, Fisher HL, Houts R, Arseneault L, et al. Associations between adolescent cannabis use and neuropsychological decline: a longitudinal co-twin control study. Addiction. (2018) 113:257–65. doi: 10.1111/add.13946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Fried P, Watkinson B, James D, Gray R. Current and former marijuana use: Preliminary findings of a longitudinal study of effects on IQ in young adults. Can Med Assoc J. (2002) 166:887–91.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

86. Mokrysz C, Landy R, Gage SH, Munafò MR, Roiser JP, Curran HV. Are IQ and educational outcomes in teenagers related to their cannabis use? A prospective cohort study. J Psychopharmacol. (2016) 30:159–68. doi: 10.1177/0269881115622241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Paige KJ, Colder CR. Long-term effects of early adolescent marijuana use on attentional and inhibitory controlle. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. (2020) 81:164–72. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2020.81.164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Boccio CM, Beaver KM. Examining the influence of adolescent marijuana use on adult intelligence: Further evidence in the causation versus spuriousness debate. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2017) 177:199–206. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Edalati H, Krank MD. Childhood maltreatment and development of substance use disorders: a review and a model of cognitive pathways. Trauma Violence Abus. (2016) 17:454–67. doi: 10.1177/1524838015584370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Isen J. A meta-analytic assessment of Wechsler's P>V sign in antisocial populations. Clin Psychol Rev. (2010) 423–35. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Tarter RE, Kirisci L, Mezzich A, Cornelius JR, Pajer K, Vanyukov M, et al. Neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2003) 160:1078–85. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Tait RJ, Mackinnon A, Christensen H. Cannabis use and cognitive function: 8-year trajectory in a young adult cohort. Addiction. (2011) 106:2195–203. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03574.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Jacobus J, Squeglia LM, Alejandra Infante M, Castro N, Brumback T, Meruelo AD, et al. Neuropsychological performance in adolescent marijuana users with co-occurring alcohol use: a three-year longitudinal study. Neuropsychology. (2015) 29:829–43. doi: 10.1037/neu0000203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Moffitt T, Meier M, Caspi A, Poulton R. Reply to rogeberg and daly: no evidence that socioeconomic status or personality differences confound the association between cannabis use and IQ decline. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2013) 110:E980–2. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1300618110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Ross JM, Ellingson JM, Rhee SH, Hewitt JK, Corley RP, Lessem JM, et al. Investigating the causal effect of cannabis use on cognitive function with a quasi-experimental co-twin design. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2020) 206: 107712. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.107712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Meier MH. Cannabis use and psychosocial functioning: evidence from prospective longitudinal studies. Curr Opin Psychol. (2021) 38:19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Cerdá M, Moffitt TE, Meier MH, Harrington HL, Houts R, Ramrakha S, et al. Persistent cannabis dependence and alcohol dependence represent risks for midlife economic and social problems: a longitudinal cohort study. Clin Psychol Sci. (2016) 4:1028–46. doi: 10.1177/2167702616630958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. McKetin R, Parasu P, Cherbuin N, Eramudugolla R, Anstey KJ. A longitudinal examination of the relationship between cannabis use and cognitive function in mid-life adults. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2016) 169:134–40. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Auer R, Vittinghoff E, Yaffe K, Künzi A, Kertesz SG, Levine DA, et al. Association between lifetime marijuana use and cognitive function in middle age the coronary artery risk development in young adults (CARDIA) study. JAMA Intern Med. (2016) 176:352–61. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2015.7841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Becker MP, Collins PF, Schultz A, Urošević S, Schmaling B, Luciana M. Longitudinal changes in cognition in young adult cannabis users. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2018) 40:529–43. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2017.1385729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Lyketsos CG, Garrett E, Liang KY, Anthony JC. Cannabis use and cognitive decline in persons under 65 years of age. Am J Epidemiol. (1999) 149:794–800. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Cengel HY, Bozhurt M, Evren C, Umut G, Keskinkilic C, Agachanli R. Evaluation of cognitive functions in individuals with synthetic cannabinoid use disorder and comparison to individuals with cannabis use disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 262:46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.01.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cannabis, delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol, cognition, longitudinal design, memory

Citation: Bourque J and Potvin S (2021) Cannabis and Cognitive Functioning: From Acute to Residual Effects, From Randomized Controlled Trials to Prospective Designs. Front. Psychiatry 12:596601. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.596601

Received: 19 August 2020; Accepted: 17 May 2021;
Published: 10 June 2021.

Edited by:

Nitya Jayaram-Lindstrom, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Robert M. Roth, Dartmouth College, United States
Wayne Denis Hall, The University of Queensland, Australia
Steven R. Laviolette, Western University, Canada

Copyright © 2021 Bourque and Potvin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stéphane Potvin, c3RlcGhhbmUucG90dmluJiN4MDAwNDA7dW1vbnRyZWFsLmNh

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.