Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 24 September 2020
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Neurobiological Biomarkers for Developing Novel Treatments of Substance and Non-Substance Addiction View all 45 articles

Dysfunction of the Prefrontal Cortex in Betel-Quid–Dependent Chewers

  • 1Department of Radiology, Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 2Health Management Center, Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 3Department of Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery, Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 4Department of Emergency, Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China

Betel quid is the fourth most popular psychoactive agent worldwide. Imaging studies have found altered brain structure in prefrontal cortex (PFC) in betel-quid dependent (BQD) chewers. However, the brain function in PFC associated with BQ use still remains unclear. The present study aimed to examine brain functional activity in PFC in individuals with BQD. This study recruited 48 participants with BQD and 22 normal controls (NCs). Both BQ-specific cue reactivity and Go/NoGo tasks were administered with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Behavioral results showed a deficit in the choice reaction time task in BQD group. The fMRI results of the cue reactivity task suggested that, individuals with BQD exhibited responses in right ventromedial PFC, left posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), left lateral parietal lobe (LPL), left middle temporal gyrus and left visual cortex, when seeing BQ images compared with control images. In the Go/NoGo task, relative to NCs group, individuals with BQD showed higher activity in right dorsolateral PFC, right PCC and bilateral LPL between NoGo and Go trials. Across these two tasks, we consistently found disrupted function in PFC in individuals with BQD, which might lead to impaired craving and response inhibition in BQ addiction. Results of current study might shed light on the neural mechanisms involved in BQ use, which could be used as potential guidelines for diagnosis and treatment of BQ dependence.

Introduction

Areca nut, the fruit of the areca palm, contains the principal active agent Arecoline (1), which is demonstrated to have a chemical structure analogous to that of nicotine (2). Thus, betel quid (BQ, the product of areca nut) is recognized as a group 1 carcinogen by International Agency for Cancer Research (1), and categorized as “addiction” when heavy use according to DSM-IV and ICD-10 (3, 4). BQ is now the fourth most commonly consumed psychoactive substance in the world, following only alcohol, nicotine, and caffeine (5, 6). A recent study with a large sample size in 6 Asian populations revealed a vast prevalence of BQ use in these counties (typically over 10%) (7). BQ use is reported to be highly associated with oral sub-mucous fibrosis and oral cancer (811). However, the limited understanding of BQ use makes it difficult for the development of effective treatment to those patients (12).

Several lines of researches have suggested that intensive BQ use shows similar symptoms of other addictive disorders. First, in the Asian Betel-quid Consortium study of 8922 participants from 6 countries, betel-quid use disorder was reported to meet DSM-V criteria for substance use disorder and had a high prevalence among users of betel-quid (7). Another study suggested that the 12-month prevalence of BQ use disorder was 18.0%, which exceeded that reported for DSM-V defined drug use disorder (3.9%) (13) and was comparable to the results of alcohol (13.9%) and nicotine (20.0%) use disorders (14, 15). Second, animal studies suggested that high dose of BQ use could induce cocaine-like physiological states, such as anxiety, dilated pupils, tachycardia, and elevated blood pressure (16). In addition, BQ use was reported to be associated with altered cognitive ability, including spatial short-term memory (17), attention and inhibition control (18), executive function (19) and attention bias to BQ cues (20).

Functional neuroimaging studies have implicated that structural and functional alterations related to BQ chewing and dependence. Recently, a review systematically assessed previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies on BQ addiction, and suggested that a key involvement of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in BQ dependency (21). For example, studies from our group suggested that BQ use was associated with decreased gray matter volumes in bilateral dorsolateral PFC and ventral medial PFC (22), altered white matter integrity in anterior thalamic radiation highly associated with PFC (23), and less cortical thickness in PFC (19). These neuroimaging studies proposed that the role of prefrontal cortical structures in BQ addiction, which plays an important role in cognitive behavioral and emotional changes in various kinds of addiction (2427). However, all the evidence in BQ addiction mainly came from studies of brain structure in BQ users. Little is known about functional role of PFC in individuals with betel-quid dependence (BQD).

Two common tasks employed in addiction neuroimaging studies are the cue reactivity and Go/NoGo tasks, in which neural activation is measured while the subject is viewing neutral versus addiction-related images and conducting the addiction-related Go and NoGo stimulus. Both of them can evoke the response from PFC. The present study aimed to investigate neural activity in PFC in individuals with BQD compared to normal controls (NCs). To do so, 48 men with BQD and 22 NCs were recruited to perform the BQ-related cue reactivity and Go/NoGo tasks under fMRI investigation. Consistent with previous findings that BQ use may be associated in part with structural deficits in PFC, we hypothesized that abnormal activation in PFC in individuals with BQD in both BQ-related cue reactivity and Go/NoGo tasks. We hoped that current study could improve our understanding of the roles of the PFC in BQ use.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Seventy male volunteers were recruited to participant in this study including 48 subjects with BQD and 22 NCs. Individuals with BQD met the following inclusion criteria: (1) 18 to 50 years of age; (2) Han nationality; (3) right-handed; (4) nine or more years of education; (5) meeting the diagnostic criteria for BQD according to DSM-V criteria for substance use disorders; (6) with no attempt to quit or experience of BQ abstinence in the past 6 months. The BQ use behaviors of BQD participants were recorded, including duration of BQ use (years), age of first BQ use, daily weight of BQ use (g), the number of BQ use days in a week and the brands of BQ products. Body mass index (BMI) was also recorded. The severity of BQ dependence was measured by Betel Quid Dependence Scale (BQDS) (28). The BQD group was additionally screened for alcohol and nicotine addiction with Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) and Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND).

Healthy subjects with matched age without BQ use were defined as the NCs group, which were recruited through a combination of targeted site sampling, advertisement and snowball sampling referrals. Subjects were excluded both as BQD chewers and healthy controls if they: (1) met the DSM-V criteria for other substance dependency, such as alcohol and nicotine; (2) had medical history of any neurological or psychiatric disorder; (3) had systemic illnesses (e.g. diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular disease, thyroid disorders, renal disorders, and epilepsy); (4) had contraindication to MRI examination; (5) had current use of psychotropic medications; (6) were not able to read and write Chinese or left–handed. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the research ethical committee of Xiangya Hospital of Central South University of Hunan Province, Changsha, China. All subjects were given written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Xiangya Hospital of Central South University of Hunan Province, Changsha, China.

Behavior Interviews and Cognitive Tests

Participants came to Xiangya Hospital to finish behavior interview and MRI scans in the same day. The behavior interview included several parts. The first part was the basic demographic information collection, including age, years of education, BMI, BQ use behaviors and so on. Then, they were asked to complete three simple cognitive tasks: choice reaction time task, working memory task, as well as the classic color-word Stroop task.

The choice reaction time task was adapted with default settings from Deary, Liewald and Nissan (29). The time required to finish this task was used as the index of general reaction time measure. The two-back working memory task with numbers was adapted from Xue and colleagues (30) and the number of correct response in each block was served as the index of working memory. The Chinese adaptation of Stroop task was used to assess the inhibition ability. The number of correct responses in 45s in the Color-Word condition was recorded as the index for this task.

MRI Protocol

In order to reduce the influence of addictive substances, all participants were required not to use caffeine, nicotine, alcohol and other addictive substance on the day of scanning. All MRI images were acquired using a Siemens 3.0T Prisma scanner at Xiangya Hospital. Standard settings were used to perform the scan. For example, foam pads were used to minimize head motion. Participants were instructed to have a rest, kept their head very still during the structural scan and responded to the instructions when doing functional scans. Stimulus presentation and timing of all stimuli and response events were achieved using Matlab (Mathworks) and Psychtoolbox (www.psychtoolbox.org) on an IBM-compatible PC. Participants’ responses were collected online using an MRI-compatible button box. The structural scan was performed using T1 MPRAGE sequence, covering whole brain with the following scanning parameters: TR/TE = 1900/3.18 ms, matrix = 320 × 320, number of slices = 256 and voxel size = 0.73 × 0.73 × 0.73 mm3, sagittal slice position. Functional scans were performed using EPI sequence with the following parameters: TR/TE = 2000 ms/30 ms, Matrix = 94 × 94, number of axial slices = 75, voxel size = 2.34 × 2.34 × 2.00 mm3.

fMRI Tasks

Participants performed one session of cue reactivity task and two sessions of Go/NoGo task inside the scanner. During the session of cue reactivity task, two types of cues were presented: the BQ image and control images. There were 20 images for each category, and each image were presented for three times. In order to keep the participants awake during this passive view task, 10 animal images were presented twice. These images were presented with a random order with each image was presented for 3 s. Participants were instructed to press a button whenever they saw an animal. There were two sessions of Go/NoGo tasks: (1) a control Go/BQ NoGo task (CGo task) in which they were asked to press a button when they saw a control image, and refrained from pressing the button when they saw a BQ-related image, and (2) a BQ Go/control NoGo task (BGo task) in which they were asked to press a button when they saw a BQ-related image, and refrained from pressing the button when they saw a control image. This Go/NoGo paradigm was adapted from previous studies (31), and allowed an examination of brain responses to BQ stimuli and the inhibition of pre-potent responses to BQ stimuli. Consistent with previous studies (31), each session consisted of 120 Go trials (75%) and 40 NoGo trials (25%). The NoGo trials were presented in pseudo-randomized order so that NoGo trial appeared with equal probability after one to five consecutive Go trials, and two NoGo trials appeared consecutively. Each stimulus was presented for 500 ms, followed by a fixation cross for 1.5 to 4 s with a mean of 2.5 s. The order of two versions of GO-NoGO tasks was counterbalanced across participants.

The correct ratio and reaction in the cue task were served as the behavior data of the task. The correct ratio for Go and NoGo trials as well as the reaction time for Go trials for each session were calculated as indexes for habitual-impulsive responding to the stimuli and inhibition of NoGo stimuli. Then, these behavior data were compared across groups.

fMRI Data Analysis

Image preprocessing and statistical analysis were carried out using FSL (www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Images were realigned to compensate for small residual head movements (32). Translational movement parameters never exceeded one voxel in any direction for any participant. Data was spatially smoothed using a 5-mm full-width-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel and filtered using a nonlinear high pass filter with a 100-s cutoff.

A two-step registration procedure was used where EPI images were first registered to the MPRAGE structural image, and then into standard MNI space, using affine transformations (32). Registration from MPRAGE structural image to standard space was further refined using FNIRT nonlinear registration (33, 34). Statistical analyses were performed in the native image space with statistical maps normalized to the standard space prior to higher-level analyses. The data was modeled at the first-level using a general linear model within FSL’s FILM module.

For the cue task, brain activations were modeled for BQ images, control images, and animal images separately. For the Go/NoGo task, brain activation in every trial was modeled for BQ Go, BQ Nogo, control Go, and control NoGo trials respectively. Error-related trials (misses and false alarms) were modeled together as a nuisance variable. The event onsets were convolved with canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF, double-gamma function) to generate regressors. Temporal derivatives were included as covariates of no interest in order to improve statistical sensitivity. Null events were not explicitly modeled, and therefore constituted an implicit baseline. The six movement parameters were also included as covariates in the model.

Higher-level analysis was created to model the activity difference across two groups. For cue reactivity task, the activation difference of BQ images vs. control images was compared between two groups to reveal the interaction between stimuli and group. For the Go/NoGo tasks in both sessions, the difference of NoGo trials vs. Go trials was compared to reveal the interaction between task and group. Higher-level random-effect models were tested for group analyses using FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effect stage 1 only (35, 36) with automatic outlier detection (37). The brain activation difference of BQ and control images, as well as NoGo and Go trials was also examined for BQD and NCs group independently using one-sample t-tests. Result images were thresholded with a height threshold of Z > 3.1 and a cluster probability of p < 0.05, corrected for whole-brain multiple comparisons based on Gaussian random field theory. The years of education was included as a covariate for all fMRI analysis.

Results

Behavioral Results

Table 1 showed the demographic and cognitive characteristics for all participants. These two groups were matched on age and BMI, but they showed differences in years of education. In BQD group, they showed an average of 15.23 years of using BQ, average age 17.13 to try their first BQ and 40.19 grams of BQ per day. In terms of cognitive tests, these two groups showed no difference regarding the Stroop task as well as the working memory task. Compared with NCs group, BQD group showed deficits in the choice reaction task (Table 1). Correlation analysis showed that BQDS score was significantly correlated with the following variables in BQD group: (1) years of education, r(48) = −0.338, p = 0.019; (2) estimated daily use in gram, r(48) = 0.461, p < 0.001. These results suggested that participants with higher BQDS scores were those with less education and higher daily consumption.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographic and clinical/cognitive characteristics of participants (M ± SD).

Cue Reactivity Task

The cue reactivity task was a very simple task and both groups performed very well in this task (over 97% of correct ratio of detecting animals) (Table 2). There was no difference in terms of either correct ratio or reaction time between groups. To investigate the activity difference between BQ and control images, contrasts of BQ-control images were tested with one sample t-test for each group and two sample t-test for group difference. Results suggested that in BQD group, BQ images activated a large set of brain areas than control images did, including right ventromedial PFC, left posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), left lateral parietal lobe (LPL), left middle temporal gyrus and left visual cortex (Table 3 and Figure 1). However, brain activity in response to these two categories of images showed no significant difference in NCs group as well as in comparison between BQD and NCs group.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 fMRI task behavioral data of different groups (M ± SD).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 fMRI activity of the cue reactivity task.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Brain activity difference between BQ images and control images in BQD group in the cue reactivity task. BQ images activated a large areas of brain than control images, including right ventromedial PFC, left posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), left lateral parietal lobe (LPL), left middle temporal gyrus and left visual cortex. However, in NCs group, these two categories of images showed no difference.

Go/Nogo Task

Table 2 also summarized major behavioral measures for both fMRI Go/NoGo tasks, including the correct ratio for Go and NoGo trials as well as the reaction time for Go trials. There was no difference if we compared these variables across groups. The 2 (Task: Go vs. NoGo) *2 (Stimuli: BQ vs. Control images) *2 (Groups: BQD vs. NC) ANOVA was performed on the correct ratio. The results suggested that there was a significant main effect of task (F(1,67) = 4.87, p = 0.03; NoGo trials showed lower correct ratio than Go trials) and a significant main effect of stimuli (F(1,67) = 22.20, p < 0.001; BQ images showed higher correct ratio than control images). But there was no significant main effect of group or other interaction effects (all p > 0.05).

We first compared brain activity difference between BQ and control images in each group. Results suggested there was significant difference in BQD group between NoGo and Go trials in the following regions: bilateral LPL, left lateral occipital cortex, and bilateral middle temporal gyrus (Table 4). However, there was no difference in NCs group when participants performed the Go/NoGo task. Group comparison showed some brain regions with higher activity between NoGo than Go trials in BQD than NCs group, including right dorsolateral PFC, right PCC and bilateral LPL (Table 4 and Figure 2). No regions showed higher activity in NCs group compared with BQD group.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 fMRI activity of the Go/NoGo task.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Brain activity difference between Nogo and Go trials in the Go/Nogo task. (A) Results suggested in BQD group, there was significant difference between NoGo and Go trials in the following regions: bilateral LPL, left lateral occipital cortex, and bilateral middle temporal gyrus. However, in NCs group, there was no difference when participants performed the Go/Nogo task. (B) Group comparison showed some brain regions showed higher activity in BQD than NCs group in right dorsolateral PFC, right PCC and bilateral LPL between NoGo and Go trials. However, there was no brain region with higher activity in the NCs than BQD group. The right hemisphere is displayed at the left side of the paper.

Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate brain functional activity in PFC among subjects with BQD involved in the BQ-related cue reactivity task and Go/NoGo task. Behavioral results showed the BQD group showed deficits in the choice reaction time task but not in either the Stroop task or the working memory task. fMRI results of the cue task suggested that BQ images activated a large set of brain areas than control images did, including right ventromedial PFC, left PCC, left LPL, left middle temporal gyrus and left visual cortex when seeing BQ images compared with control images. In the Go/NoGo task, relative to NCs group, individuals with BQD showed higher activity in right dorsolateral PFC, right PCC and bilateral LPL between NoGo and Go trials. As hypothesized, individuals with BQD showed abnormal activation in PFC in both of these two tasks, which implies that functional abnormality of prefrontal area in BQ addiction.

These results highlighted functional alternation of PFC in BQD participants. Structural alteration in PFC has been widely reported in addiction, including cocaine (3844), heroin (4548), opiates (49), cannabis (50), nicotine (51, 52), alcohol (53, 54), ketamine (55), MDMA (56), methamphetamine (57), internet (58), and online games (59, 60). In the field of BQ addiction, our group study investigated the relationship between cortical thickness and executive function in chronic chewers with BQD, suggesting that BQD group had thinner cortex in bilateral PFC, and cortical thickness of the bilateral dorsolateral PFC mediated the correlation of betel-quid chewing and executive function (19). Consistent with decreased gray matter volumes in bilateral dorsolateral PFC and ventral medial PFC (22), results suggested that structural alteration in PFC in BQ users with BQD. Recently, several studies using functional connectivity approach to investigate resting-state brain feature in BQD users consistently suggested that impaired resting-state functional connectivity in BQ users occurred in prefrontal regions (61, 62). Our current study extends these prior findings by providing new evidence with task fMRI for abnormal functional activity in prefrontal regions in individuals with BQD.

It is worth noting that we observed abnormal activity in ventromedial PFC in cue reactivity task and dorsolateral PFC in Go/NoGo task. Prefrontal cortex is one of the most frequently areas implicated in drug addiction, which was involved in addicted subjects during intoxication, craving, bingeing and withdrawal (24). Altered prefrontal cortex activity has been reported in nicotine and cocaine addiction (6365). Although activity among prefrontal cortex is highly integrated and flexible, a meta-analysis study suggests different region of prefrontal cortex is involved in different function (66). The dorsal PFC has been predominantly implicated in top-down control and meta-cognitive functions, the ventromedial PFC in emotion regulation (including conditioning and assigning incentive salience to drugs and drug-related cues), and the ventrolateral PFC and lateral OFC in automatic response tendencies and impulsivity (66). Consistent with previous studies in addition, our study provides novel evidence for the notion that abnormalities of ventromedial PFC in drugs and drug-related cues and dorsolateral PFC in top-down control in BQ dependence and further suggests that the dysfunction of functional activity in the ventromedial and dorsolateral PFC may contribute to impaired cue-induced craving and response inhibition in BQ dependence.

It should be noted that there were three limitations in current study. First, the study used an imbalanced sample. There were more individuals with BQD than normal participants. Second, this study only recruited male participants, so it should be cautious in generalizing the findings of this study to the females. Third, this study was done with outpatients, so there were inaccuracies regarding exact amount and histories of BQ use. Finally, although we performed a careful physical examination and obtained routine laboratory tests, all potential disease and confounding by other substances could not be totally excluded.

Conclusion

Our study used neuroimaging approaches to assess the characteristic of brain activation with BQ specific cue reactivity and Go/NoGo tasks. The findings showed that individuals with BQD exhibited elevated responses in right ventromedial PFC, left PCC, left LPL, left middle temporal gyrus and left visual cortex when seeing BQ images compared with control images in the cue reactivity task. In the Go/NoGo task, relative to NCs group, individuals with BQD showed higher activity in right dorsolateral PFC, right PCC and bilateral LPL between NoGo and Go trials. Disrupted prefrontal function was consistently revealed in BQ cue induced craving and inhibitory control in individuals with BQD, which implies that treatments that target this region may help alleviate some symptoms of addictive BQ use.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board at Xiangya Hospital of Central South University of Changsha, Hunan Province, China. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

XZ conceived and designed the experiments. FY, SL, DW, and CJ conducted the experiments and collected data. ZZ and ZQ analyzed the results. LK and CZ wrote and revised the main manuscript text. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by research grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 61972460 to XZ and 61802443 to FY), Foundation for the Author of National Excellent Doctoral Dissertation of PR China (FANEDD) (grant number 201411 to XZ), Natural Science Foundation of Hunan Province (grant number 2019JJ20037 to XZ, 2020JJ4883 to LK, 2020JJ4923 to FY), Philosophy and Social Science Foundation of Hunan Province (grant number 17YBA426 to FY and 16YBA036 to XZ) and Youth Research of Xiangya Hospital, Central South University (grant number 2017Q19 to FY).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. World Health Organization. International agency for research on cancer IARC monograph on the evaluation of carcinogenic risks to humans. Betel-Quid and Areca-nut Chewing and Some Areca-nut-Derived Nitrosamines. Lyon, France: IARC (2004).

Google Scholar

2. Lord GA, Lim CK, Warnakulasuriya S, Peters TJ. Chemical and analytical aspects of areca nut. Addict Biol (2002) 7(1):99–102. doi: 10.1080/13556210120091455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Mirza SS, Shafique K, Vart P, Arain MI. Areca nut chewing and dependency syndrome: is the dependence comparable to smoking? a cross sectional study. Subst Abuse Treatm Prevent Policy (2011) 6:23. doi: 10.1186/1747-597X-6-23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Papke RL, Horenstein NA, Stokes C. Nicotinic Activity of Arecoline, the Psychoactive Element of “Betel Nuts”, Suggests a Basis for Habitual Use and Anti-Inflammatory Activity. PloS One (2015) 10(10):e0140907. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Boucher BJ, Mannan N. Metabolic effects of the consumption of Areca catechu. Addict Biol (2002) 7(1):103–10. doi: 10.1080/13556210120091464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Yen HY, Chen PH, Ko YC, Chiang SK, Chang YY, Shiah YJ. Betel Quid Chewing, Personality and Mood: Betel Quid Chewing Associated with Low Extraversion and Negative Mood. Subst Use Misuse (2018) 53(11):1782–7. doi: 10.1080/10826084.2018.1432652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Lee CH, Ko AM, Yang FM, Hung CC, Warnakulasuriya S, Ibrahim SO, et al. Association of DSM-5 Betel-Quid Use Disorder With Oral Potentially Malignant Disorder in 6 Betel-Quid Endemic Asian Populations. JAMA Psychiatry (2018) 75(3):261–9. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Mehrtash H, Duncan K, Parascandola M, David A, Gritz ER, Gupta PC, et al. Defining a global research and policy agenda for betel quid and areca nut. Lancet Oncol (2017) 18(12):e767–e75. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(17)30460-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Jacob BJ, Straif K, Thomas G, Ramadas K, Mathew B, Zhang ZF, et al. Betel quid without tobacco as a risk factor for oral precancers. Oral Oncol (2004) 40(7):697–704. doi: 10.1016/j.oraloncology.2004.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Lee CH, Ko YC, Huang HL, Chao YY, Tsai CC, Shieh TY, et al. The precancer risk of betel quid chewing, tobacco use and alcohol consumption in oral leukoplakia and oral submucous fibrosis in southern Taiwan. Br J Cancer (2003) 88(3):366–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6600727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Tilakaratne WM, Klinikowski MF, Saku T, Peters TJ, Warnakulasuriya S. Oral submucous fibrosis: review on aetiology and pathogenesis. Oral Oncol (2006) 42(6):561–8. doi: 10.1016/j.oraloncology.2005.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Osborne PG, Ko YC, Wu MT, Lee CH. Intoxication and substance use disorder to Areca catechu nut containing betel quid: A review of epidemiological evidence, pharmacological basis and social factors influencing quitting strategies. Drug Alcohol Depend (2017) 179:187. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Grant BF, Saha TD, Ruan WJ, Goldstein RB, Chou SP, Jung J, et al. Epidemiology of DSM-5 Drug Use Disorder: Results From the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions-III. JAMA Psychiatry (2016) 73(1):39–47. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Chou SP, Goldstein RB, Smith SM, Huang B, Ruan WJ, Zhang H, et al. The epidemiology of DSM-5 nicotine use disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions-III. J Clin Psychiatry (2016) 84(8):310. doi: 10.4088/JCP.15m10114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Saha TD, Chou SP, Jung J, Zhang H, et al. Epidemiology of DSM-5 Alcohol Use Disorder: Results From the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions III. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 72(8):757–66. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Patidar KA, Parwani R, Wanjari SP, Patidar AP. Various terminologies associated with areca nut and tobacco chewing: A review. J Oral Maxillofacial Pathol Jomfp (2015) 19(1):69. doi: 10.4103/0973-029X.157205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Chiu MC, Shen B, Li SH, Ho MC. Spatial short-term memory is impaired in dependent betel quid chewers. Psychopharmacol (Berl) (2016) 233(15-16):2925–32. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4331-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ho MC, Li RH, Tang TC. Betel nut chewing effects on sustained attention and inhibitory control after sleep deprivation. Aust J Psychol (2015) 67(4):222–30. doi: 10.1111/ajpy.12081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Zhu X, Liu S, Liao W, Kong L, Jiang C, Yuan F. Executive function deficit in betel-quid-dependent chewers: Mediating role of prefrontal cortical thickness. J Psychopharmacol (2018) 32(12):1362–8. doi: 10.1177/0269881118806299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Shen B, Chiu MC, Li SH, Huang GJ, Liu LJ, Ho MC. Attentional bias to betel quid cues: An eye tracking study. Psychol Addictive Behav J Soc Psycholog Addictive Behav (2016) 30(6):705–11. doi: 10.1037/adb0000191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Sariah A, Liu Z, Pu W, Liu H, Xue Z, Huang X. Structural and Functional Alterations in Betel-Quid Chewers: A Systematic Review of Neuroimaging Findings. Front Psychiatry (2019) 10:16:16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Yuan F, Kong L, Zhu X, Jiang C, Fang C, Liao W. Altered gray-matter volumes associated with betel quid dependence. Front Psychiatry (2017) 8:139. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Yuan F, Zhu X, Kong L, Shen H, Liao W, Jiang C. White Matter Integrity Deficit Associated with Betel Quid Dependence. Front Psychiatry (2017) 8:201. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry (2002) 159(10):1642–52. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Volkow ND, Fowler JS. Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex (2000) 10(3):318–25. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Goldstein RZ. Role of dopamine, the frontal cortex and memory circuits in drug addiction: insight from imaging studies. Neurobiol Learn Memory (2002) 78(3):610–24. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2002.4099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Tomasi D. Addiction circuitry in the human brain. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol (2012) 52:321–36. doi: 10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-010611-134625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lee CY, Chang CS, Shieh TY, Chang YY. Development and validation of a self-rating scale for betel quid chewers based on a male-prisoner population in Taiwan: the Betel Quid Dependence Scale. Drug Alcohol Depen (2012) 121(1–2):18–22. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.07.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Deary IJ, Liewald D. Nissan J. A free, easy-to-use, computer-based simple and four-choice reaction time programme: the Deary-Liewald reaction time task. Behav Res Methods (2011) 43(1):258–68. doi: 10.3758/s13428-010-0024-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Xue G, Qi D, Zhen J, Chuansheng C. Mapping of verbal working memory in nonfluent Chinese–English bilinguals with functional MRI. NeuroImage (2004) 22(1):1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. He Q, Huang X, Zhang S, Turel O, Ma L, Bechara A. Dynamic Causal Modeling of Insular, Striatal, and Prefrontal Cortex Activities During a Food-Specific Go/NoGo Task. Biol Psychiatry: Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging (2019) 4(12):1080–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med Image Anal (2001) 5(2):143–56. doi: 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Non-linear registration, a.k.a. Spatial normalisation (FMRIB Technical Report TR07JA2). Oxford, United Kingdom: FMRIB Centre (2007).

Google Scholar

34. Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Non-linear optimization (FMRIB Technical Report TR07JA1). Oxford, United Kingdom: FMRIB Centre (2007).

Google Scholar

35. Woolrich MW, Behrens TEJ, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. Neuroimage (2004) 21(4):1732–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI. Neuroimage (2003) 20(2):1052–63. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00435-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Woolrich M. Robust group analysis using outlier inference. Neuroimage (2008) 41(2):286–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Franklin TR, Acton PD, Maldjian JA, Gray JD, Croft JR, Dackis CA, et al. Decreased gray matter concentration in the insular, orbitofrontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices of cocaine patients. Biol Psychiatry (2002) 51(2):134–42. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01269-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Moreno-López L, Catena A, Fernández-Serrano MJ, Delgado-Rico E, Stamatakis EA, Pérez-García M, et al. Trait impulsivity and prefrontal gray matter reductions in cocaine dependent individuals. Drug Alcohol Depen (2012) 125(3):208–14. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.02.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Sim ME, Lyoo IK, Streeter CC, Covell J, Sarid-Segal O, Ciraulo DA, et al. Cerebellar gray matter volume correlates with duration of cocaine use in cocaine-dependent subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology (2007) 32(10):2229–37. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Konova AB, Moeller SJ, Tomasi D, Parvaz MA, Alia-Klein N, Volkow ND, et al. Structural and behavioral correlates of abnormal encoding of money value in the sensorimotor striatum in cocaine addiction. Eur J Neurosci (2012) 36(7):2979–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08211.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Kaag AM, Crunelle CL, van Wingen G, Homberg J, van den Brink W, Reneman L. Relationship between trait impulsivity and cortical volume, thickness and surface area in male cocaine users and non-drug using controls. Drug Alcohol Depen (2014) 144:210–7. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Barrós-Loscertales A, Garavan H, Bustamante JC, Ventura-Campos N, Llopis JJ, Belloch V, et al. Reduced striatal volume in cocaine-dependent patients. Neuroimage (2011) 56(3):1021–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ansell EB, Rando K, Tuit K, Guarnaccia J, Sinha R. Cumulative Adversity and Smaller Gray Matter Volume in Medial Prefrontal, Anterior Cingulate, and Insula Regions. Biol Psychiatry (2012) 72(1):57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Yuan Y, Zhu Z, Shi J, Zou Z, Yuan F, Liu Y, et al. Gray matter density negatively correlates with duration of heroin use in young lifetime heroin-dependent individuals. Brain Cogn (2009) 71(3):223–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Wang X, Li B, Zhou X, Liao Y, Tang J, Liu T, et al. Changes in brain gray matter in abstinent heroin addicts. Drug Alcohol Depen (2012) 126(3):304–8. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.05.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Liu H, Hao Y, Kaneko Y, Ouyang X, Zhang Y, Xu L, et al. Frontal and cingulate gray matter volume reduction in heroin dependence: Optimized voxel-based morphometry. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2009) 63(4):563–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01989.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Yuan K, Qin W, Dong M, Liu J, Sun J, Liu P, et al. Gray matter deficits and resting-state abnormalities in abstinent heroin-dependent individuals. Neurosci Lett (2010) 482(2):101–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Lyoo IK, Pollack MH, Silveri MM, Ahn KH, Diaz CI, Hwang J, et al. Prefrontal and temporal gray matter density decreases in opiate dependence. Psychopharmacology (2006) 184(2):139–44. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0198-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Cousijn J, Wiers RW, Ridderinkhof KR, van den Brink W, Veltman DJ, Goudriaan AE. Grey matter alterations associated with cannabis use: results of a VBM study in heavy cannabis users and healthy controls. Neuroimage (2012) 59(4):3845–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Brody AL, Mandelkern MA, Jarvik ME, Lee GS, Smith EC, Huang JC, et al. Differences between smokers and nonsmokers in regional gray matter volumes and densities. Biol Psychiatry (2004) 55(1):77–84. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00610-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Liao Y, Tang J, Liu T, Chen X, Hao W. Differences between smokers and non-smokers in regional gray matter volumes: a voxel-based morphometry study. Addict Biol (2012) 17(6):977–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00250.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Benegal V, Antony G, Venkatasubramanian G, Jayakumar PN. IMAGING STUDY: Gray matter volume abnormalities and externalizing symptoms in subjects at high risk for alcohol dependence. Addict Biol (2007) 12(1):122–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2006.00043.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Mechtcheriakov S, Brenneis C, Egger K, Koppelstaetter F, Schocke M, Marksteiner J. A widespread distinct pattern of cerebral atrophy in patients with alcohol addiction revealed by voxel-based morphometry. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2007) 78(6):610–4. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.095869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Liao Y, Tang J, Corlett PR, Wang X, Yang M, Chen H, et al. Reduced dorsal prefrontal gray matter after chronic ketamine use. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 69(1):42–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Cowan RL, Lyoo IK, Sung SM, Ahn KH, Kim MJ, Hwang J, et al. Reduced cortical gray matter density in human MDMA (Ecstasy) users: a voxel-based morphometry study. Drug Alcohol Depen (2003) 72(3):225–35. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2003.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kim SJ, Lyoo IK, Hwang J, Chung A, Sung YH, Kim J, et al. Prefrontal grey-matter changes in short-term and long-term abstinent methamphetamine abusers. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol (2006) 9(02):221–8. doi: 10.1017/S1461145705005699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Zhou Y, Lin F-C, Du Y-S, Zhao Z-M, Xu J-R, Lei H. Gray matter abnormalities in Internet addiction: a voxel-based morphometry study. Eur J Radiol (2011) 79(1):92–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2009.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Weng C-B, Qian R-B, Fu X-M, Lin B, Han X-P, Niu C-S, et al. Gray matter and white matter abnormalities in online game addiction. Eur J Radiol (2013) 82(8):1308–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2013.01.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Han DH, Lyoo IK, Renshaw PF. Differential regional gray matter volumes in patients with on-line game addiction and professional gamers. J Psychiatr Res (2012) 46(4):507–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Zhu X, Zhu Q, Jiang C, Shen H, Wang F, Liao W, et al. Disrupted Resting-State Default Mode Network in Betel Quid-Dependent Individuals. Front Psychol (2017) 8:84. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Linli ZQ, Huang XJ, Liu ZN, Guo SX, Sariah A. A multivariate pattern analysis of resting-state functional MRI data in Naive and chronic betel quid chewers. Brain Imaging Behav (2020). doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00322-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Feil J, Sheppard D, Fitzgerald PB, Yücel M, Lubman DI, Bradshaw JL. Addiction, compulsive drug seeking, and the role of frontostriatal mechanisms in regulating inhibitory control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 35(2):248–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Tomasi D, Telang F. Addiction: beyond dopamine reward circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci (2011) 108(37):15037–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010654108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Zhu X, Zhu Q, Shen H, Liao W, Yuan F. Rumination and Default Mode Network Subsystems Connectivity in First-episode, Drug-Naive Young Patients with Major Depressive Disorder. Sci Rep (2017) 7:43105. doi: 10.1038/srep43105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci (2011) 12(11):652–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn3119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: betel quid dependence, brain function, prefrontal cortex, cue reactivity task, go/nogo task

Citation: Kong L, Zeng C, Yuan F, Liu S, Wang D, Jiang C, Zhan Z, Qian Z and Zhu X (2020) Dysfunction of the Prefrontal Cortex in Betel-Quid–Dependent Chewers. Front. Psychiatry 11:558367. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.558367

Received: 02 May 2020; Accepted: 27 August 2020;
Published: 24 September 2020.

Edited by:

Yanhui Liao, Sir Run Run Shaw Hospital, China

Reviewed by:

Yuzheng Hu, Zhejiang University, China
Xiaofeng Yang, Emory University, United States
Guangheng Dong, Zhejiang Normal University, China

Copyright © 2020 Kong, Zeng, Yuan, Liu, Wang, Jiang, Zhan, Qian and Zhu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xueling Zhu, xuercan@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.