Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 19 June 2020
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

Family Environment, Neurodevelopmental Risk, and the Environmental Influences on Child Health Outcomes (ECHO) Initiative: Looking Back and Moving Forward

Nicole R. Bush,Nicole R. Bush1,2Lauren S. WakschlagLauren S. Wakschlag3Kaja Z. LeWinnKaja Z. LeWinn1Irva Hertz-PicciottoIrva Hertz-Picciotto4Sara S. NozadiSara S. Nozadi5Sarah PieperSarah Pieper6Johnnye LewisJohnnye Lewis5Dominik BiezonskiDominik Biezonski6Clancy BlairClancy Blair7Julianna DeardorffJulianna Deardorff8Jenae M. NeiderhiserJenae M. Neiderhiser9Leslie D. LeveLeslie D. Leve10Amy J. ElliottAmy J. Elliott11Cristiane S. DuarteCristiane S. Duarte6Claudia Lugo-CandelasClaudia Lugo-Candelas6T. Michael O&#x;SheaT. Michael O’Shea12Lyndsay A. AvalosLyndsay A. Avalos13Grier P. PageGrier P. Page14and and Jonathan Posner*Jonathan Posner6* on behalf of Program Collaborators for Environmental influences on Child Health Outcomes
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Weill Institute for Neurosciences, University of California, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 2Department of Pediatrics, University of California, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Medical Social Sciences and Institute for Innovations in Developmental Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, Il, United States
  • 4Department of Public Health Sciences, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, United States
  • 5Community Environmental Health Program, College of Pharmacy, University of New Mexico Health Sciences Center, Albuquerque, NM, United States
  • 6Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, United States
  • 7Department of Population Health, New York University, New York, NY, United States
  • 8Community Health Sciences, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States
  • 9Department of Psychology, Penn State University, University Park, PA, United States
  • 10Prevention Science Institute, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, United States
  • 11Center for Pediatric and Community Research, Avera Research Institute, Sioux Falls, SD, United States
  • 12Department of Pediatrics, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, United States
  • 13Division of Research, Kaiser Permanente Northern California, Oakland, CA, United States
  • 14Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, RTI, Atlanta, GA, United States

The family environment, with all its complexity and diverse components, plays a critical role in shaping neurodevelopmental outcomes in children. Herein we review several domains of the family environment (family socioeconomic status, family composition and home environment, parenting behaviors and interaction styles, parental mental health and functioning, and parental substance use) and discuss how these domains influence neurodevelopment, with particular emphasis on mental health outcomes. We also highlight a new initiative launched by the National Institutes of Health, the Environmental influences on Child Health Outcomes (ECHO) program. We discuss the role that ECHO will play in advancing our understanding of the impact of the family environment on children’s risk for psychiatric outcomes. Lastly, we conclude with important unanswered questions and controversies in this area of research, highlighting how ECHO will contribute to resolving these gaps in our understanding, clarifying relationships between the family environment and children’s mental health.

Introduction

A child’s family environment, with all its complexity and diverse components, plays a critical role in shaping neurodevelopmental and psychiatric outcomes. The term “neurodevelopment” connotes the developmental unfolding of behavior, cognition, and emotion underpinned by brain maturation (1). Neurodevelopmental health reflects integrated brain–behavior patterns that promote flexible adaptation and regulation in response to shifting environmental demands. In contrast, neurodevelopmental disorders reflect delays or deviations in behavioral and psychological function due to atypicalities in brain development with associated impairment (2). Family environments with the varied opportunities, challenges, and experiences they provide, influence neurodevelopment with attendant effects on children’s motor and sensory development, temperament, cognitive abilities, and behavioral and emotional responses. Although the importance of the family environment in shaping children’s neurodevelopment is widely accepted, rarely has this been investigated systematically across the broad spectrum of family environmental domains or inclusive of a broad range of neurodevelopmental outcomes assessed simultaneously and prospectively. Richer examination of the environmental exposome is required to advance psychiatry (3). A newly launched multidisciplinary National Institutes of Health (NIH) initiative, the Environmental influences on Child Health Outcomes (ECHO) program, is well positioned to address these gaps and advance the knowledge base of child and adolescent psychiatry.

The NIH ECHO program is structured to study the influence of physical, chemical, biological, social, and behavioral exposures on child development within a large population of children in the United States (http://echochildren.org). It is a national, multisite study that brings together approximately 70 extant pregnancy and child cohorts and will comprise more than 50,000 children and their families (http://echochildren.org/pediatric-cohorts/). Building upon these successful extant cohorts with rich longitudinal databases and banked biospecimen repositories, ECHO promises to vastly expand our understanding of the determinants of neurodevelopment by creating large collective sample sizes, harmonizing data collection across studies, providing a shared measurement framework, and sampling across geographically, racially, ethnically, and socioeconomically diverse populations. Further, ECHO is leveraging the cohorts’ affiliated multidisciplinary research teams, with specific expertise in the study of children and families, to meaningfully advance knowledge of factors that promote optimal neurodevelopment and physical health of U.S. children. As such, the ECHO national consortium is designed to play a leading role in systematically examining how environmental, chemical, and social exposures from preconception to early childhood shape unfolding neurodevelopmental pathways and in understanding the etiology of comorbidities. Prior studies of the influence of the family environment have often focused on a single feature or have not robustly considered co-occurring exposures, such as toxicants (4). Thus, the robust nature of exposure measurement in ECHO provides an unparalleled opportunity to shed light on those features of the family environment that shape mental health outcomes, independently and in interaction with other environmental adversities. Further, ECHO has the capacity to incorporate genomics within its examination of family environmental risk factors for psychiatric disorders (5), in a manner that could advance population health through its focus on early neurodevelopment and opportunities to inform prevention efforts (6).

In this paper, we provide a brief review of the role that the family environment plays in shaping children’s neurodevelopment, with emphasis on factors contributing to risk for psychiatric disorders, acknowledging that the outcomes reviewed do not encompass all of neurodevelopment. Given the size and scope of this literature, we limit the scope of our review to focus on domains of the family environment with a strong evidence base supporting impact on offspring neurodevelopment, directing our review to the following primary domains of the family environment: family socioeconomic factors, family composition and home environment, parenting behaviors and interaction styles, parental mental health and functioning, and parental substance use. In considering associations between the family environment and neurodevelopment, we emphasize elements that the ECHO study is poised to address. We also highlight themes that cut across these family environment domains, considering, for example, bidirectionality of influences between the child and the family environment, as well as interactions across family environment domains (Figure 1). We begin with a review of the literature, covering established associations between the aforementioned domains of the family environment and child neurodevelopment and highlighting mechanisms underlying those associations, when known. We then present an overview of extant data from the ECHO cohorts with available data across those family environment domains. Lastly, we outline important unanswered questions and controversies in this research, highlighting how ECHO can contribute to resolving these critical gaps in our understanding of the role of the family environment and revealing opportunities for prevention of, and intervention for, neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Family environment and neurodevelopment. Our review focuses on four primary domains of the family environment with a strong evidence base supporting their impact on the neurodevelopment of children (green ovals): Family composition, Home environment, Parenting behaviors and interaction styles, and Parental mental health and functioning (including parental substance use). Family socioeconomic status (large blue oval) intersects with each of these domains. Four cross-cutting themes impacting each of the four family environment domains are also highlighted (brown rectangles): Developmental timing (the neurodevelopmental influence of the family environment varies depending on the developmental stage of the child), Individual differences (the neurodevelopmental influence of the family environment varies depending on traits of the child), Bidirectional (the family environment influences the child, but the child also influences the family environment), and Interactions (the neurodevelopmental influence of one family environment domain may be contingent on the others).

Review of the Influence of Family Environment Domains on Children’s Neurodevelopment

Family Socioeconomic Factors

Roughly one in five children live in poverty in the U.S. (7), and in 2015, 51% of children in U.S. public schools were from low-income families (8). For decades, investigations focused on socioeconomic status (SES) in childhood indicate that children in lower SES families have poorer neurodevelopmental outcomes across multiple domains (9, 10), including global measures of cognitive performance like IQ and academic achievement (11), as well as specific cognitive domains such as language processing, working memory, and cognitive control (12, 13). Low SES is associated with externalizing disorders (10), including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and disruptive behavior disorders (14), as well as internalizing behaviors such as anxiety and depression (15), though these latter associations have been less consistent (16). As with other mental and physical health outcomes, associations between SES and neurodevelopment are graded, with better functioning observed with increasing levels of SES (17), and the largest negative impact found in impoverished children (18).

Children born into low-SES families often encounter a broad range of adverse exposures that frequently coexist within a family or neighborhood of low SES. Early life adversities more common among low-SES children include material and nutritional deprivation (7), a less complex language environment, (8, 9) psychosocial stress (10), and more frequent exposure to toxic chemicals such as tobacco smoke and lead (17). Material deprivation refers to the impact of low SES on a parent or caregiver’s ability to provide a cognitively enriching environment in the form of toys and books, and also encompasses the quality and quantity of early educational opportunities (7, 9). Children from low-SES households are also more likely to experience psychosocial stressors, which include interpersonal violence, crowding, neighborhood violence and disorder, and disruptions in their relationships with caregivers (15). Additionally, low SES has been associated with reduced breastfeeding (19). Children from low-SES homes are thus less likely to receive breastfeeding’s beneficial effects on offspring IQ (20) and maternal well-being (21). Whether adversities that co-exist with low SES partially or fully mediate associations between a family’s SES and neurodevelopmental outcomes in the children is still unanswered. Studies such as ECHO with large and diverse sampling across developmental stages and exposures can examine mediation effects and determine the degree to which coexisting factors, including potential genetic liabilities, account for the influence of low SES on neurodevelopment.

Recently, investigators have identified neural correlates of lower SES in childhood, resulting in a nascent, brain-based understanding of how SES may “get under the skin.” While studies demonstrate the influence of lower SES on widely distributed brain regions (22), the most consistent effects are reported in brain areas implicated in executive function, language, and emotion processing (22). For example, several studies indicate that low SES is associated with alterations in the morphology of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), which broadly supports self-regulation, reasoning, and decision-making (23). Studies have also identified SES-related functional and volumetric alterations in the hippocampus and amygdala, regions critical to learning, memory, and threat/emotion processing. Structural brain differences in the frontal and temporal lobes have been shown to explain as much as 20% of the deficits in academic achievement among low-SES children (18). Though still a field in its infancy, neuroimaging studies of SES correlates may ultimately help inform interventions aimed at mitigating the adverse impact of low SES on neurodevelopment.

Family Composition

Decades of research confirm that variations in family composition are associated with differential neurodevelopment. Children reared in two-parent families generally fare better than those raised in single-parent families in terms of cognitive, educational, and behavioral outcomes, and these associations are consistent across age groups (24). Much of this research has focused on children of divorce, who demonstrate poorer academic and emotional functioning compared to children in two-parent families (25, 26), though effects of family disruption may confound this observation. Moreover, transitions in and out of two-parent families are complex and appear to have unique and varying effects on child outcomes depending on the type of transition, the outcome of interest, and the population studied (see Co-Occurrence and Context of Exposures for further discussion) (27, 28). Increasingly, studies have considered the effects of parental separation due to incarceration on children’s emotional and behavioral health. A 2012 meta-analysis (29) concluded that parental incarceration was consistently associated with children’s antisocial behavior, but not with educational or mental health outcomes. This association persisted even when controlling for parental criminality and/or children’s antisocial behavior prior to parental incarceration. Additionally, birth order may be a significant factor in neurodevelopment. Firstborn children are often reported to have higher intelligence compared to their later born siblings (30, 31), but may also be at higher risk for certain negative outcomes, such as ADHD (32). Recent longitudinal research on birth order and cognitive outcomes, however, has yielded inconsistent or null findings (33).

A number of pathways have been proposed to explain how family composition affects children’s neurodevelopment. Associations appear to be largely mediated through other aspects of the family, many of which are covered in this review, including SES, parent–child interactions, and parental stress (25, 34, 35). For example, the importance of SES and parent–child interactions could be related to the availability of resources that may be less accessible to children in single parent and/or larger families. The same logic applies to birth order, such that parenting resources diminish with each birth, negatively affecting children later in the birth order (36) [although recent large studies have questioned this, due to finding no effect of birth order on IQ or personality (37)]. Whereas some research suggests that bivariate associations between family composition and child neurobehavioral outcomes are largely attenuated once individual, family, and neighborhood characteristics are accounted for (38), enduring, negative effects have nonetheless been shown for children of divorce across a range of outcomes (25, 39). Taken together, this body of research suggests that family composition plays a role in children’s neurodevelopment, though the effects appear to be connected to other factors such as culture, child sex, parental psychopathology, parenting behavior, and interparental conflict (40).

Home Environment

Creating and maintaining an optimal, safe, supportive, and stimulating early home environment is considered an essential factor in promoting children’s well-being and long-term adaptive functioning, particularly in the presence of other environmental and contextual adversities (e.g., poverty) (41). Characteristics of a child’s home environment and its quality can be classified into two broad categories: functional and structural. Functional characteristics specify the emotional climate and the quality of interactions and relationships among members of the household (e.g., parent–child interactions and parental stress) and are covered elsewhere in this article. Here, we focus on structural characteristics, which reflect the physical, and often observable, aspects of the home environment (e.g., residential crowding, quality of construction materials used in the house, proper facilities for food storage and hygiene, access to learning resources).

Low-quality structural characteristics of the home environment are often associated with adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes (42). As an example, many materials that have been or currently are used in building houses include heavy metals and other toxicants that can potentially jeopardize children’s safety by increasing their exposures, which negatively impact children’s cognitive and behavioral outcomes (43, 44). Some of these toxic chemicals include (1) polyvinyl chloride or other chlorinated plastics used in pipes, flooring and ceiling coatings, (2) volatile-organic and semi-volatile compounds in fabrics including carpets and furniture that can volatilize at room temperature and be inhaled and/or dermally absorbed, and (3) heavy metals (e.g., arsenic, antimony, cadmium, cobalt, lead, mercury) found in a variety of forms such as paints, fabrics, thermostats, window blinds, and water pipes (4547). Exposure to these toxic chemicals has been associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes such as ADHD and autism (46, 4850).

Lack of infrastructure also remains a potential source of increased exposure for many populations in the US. For example, 14% of Native Americans lack access to regulated drinking water, while only 0.6% of the US population as a whole lacks that access (51). This increases risk for ingestion of pathogens or contaminants including heavy metals and industrial chemicals (similar to those linked above to diminished cognitive abilities, behavioral problems, ADHD, and autism) (51, 52). Lack of infrastructure can also result in homes with no or substandard heat and cooking sources associated with poor indoor air quality from particulate and other combustion product emissions. Combustion of dirty heating materials such as coal in substandard burners also increases the release of metals into the home environment (53, 54). Not only can exposures to toxicants described here contribute to adverse developmental outcomes, but the associated infrastructure deficits in the home environment may impact educational achievement if, for example, lack of electricity and heat inhibits the child’s ability to complete school assignments at home (55). Reasons for lack of infrastructure are varied and range from an inability to pay for service to a lack of services provided in low-density rural communities.

At even greater risk for exposure through substandard living environment and poor infrastructure is the staggering proportion of children in the United States who are growing up in families experiencing homelessness or housing insecurity. This number according to a 2014 report was estimated to be 2.5 million children, with 42% being under the age of 6, reflecting a large increase since 2006 (one in 50 children in 2006 compared to one in 30 in 2014) (56, 57). Homeless children with families fare better than those without other family members, yet both groups have poorer school performance than housed children; nevertheless, trajectories of school performance in homeless children can be heterogeneous (58). Children experiencing homelessness are also more likely to come from impoverished families with prolonged exposure to many factors associated with children’s behavioral and emotional problems such as domestic violence and abuse, parents’ drug abuse and/or poor mental health and extreme poverty (59). Children experiencing homelessness compared to housed children—even those living in poverty—are more likely to experience health problems (e.g., asthma), developmental delays (e.g., language and motor skill delays), hyperactivity and inattention, externalizing and internalizing behavioral problems, academic difficulties, and psychiatric disorders (e.g., conduct disorder) (6062). These problems may be further exacerbated by limited access to diagnostic and treatment facilities and the increasing stress on those systems as homelessness increases.

Parenting Behaviors and Interaction Style

Parents’ interactions with their children are among the strongest proximal predictors of children’s neurodevelopment. One of the primary approaches to conceptualizing parenting has been a dimensional approach focused on continuous dimensions of specific parental behaviors, such as sensitivity, warmth, or control. A second approach has been to categorize parents into discrete parenting styles based on a combination of parenting dimensions, yielding parenting classifications such as authoritative, authoritarian, or permissive parenting (63, 64). In both approaches, a core parenting behavior in infancy and early toddlerhood is parental responsiveness, which has been defined as being accepting, nurturing, supportive, sensitive, and warm; conversely, low levels indicate insensitive, unresponsive, and rejecting parental behavior (64, 65). A recent meta-analysis of over 1,400 studies comprising more than one million children concluded that parental responsiveness has a significant positive association with the child’s later academic performance, as well as a protective effect against externalizing problems (including conditions like ADHD and conduct disorder), an association that was present for both maternal and paternal responsiveness (66). The important role of parental responsiveness has also been well-studied in the attachment literature with meta-analyses indicating a significant association between parental responsiveness and children’s attachment security (67).

In the decades following the seminal work of Bell (68, 69) that challenged the notion that children are merely passive recipients of parental socialization effects, there has been a growing body of research on the bidirectional nature of parenting behavior and children’s neurodevelopment. For example, parental responsiveness has been shown to lower risk for externalizing behaviors in the child, but this association was mediated, at least in one study, by the child’s performance on a battery of executive function tasks (70). In addition, children’s behavioral problems have been shown to have a reciprocal influence on negative parenting (71, 72). Such research on bidirectional associations has been strengthened by genetically informed studies that can measure, or control for, the role of shared genes between parent and child. For example, a longitudinal adoption study revealed a pathway from the biological mother’s ADHD symptoms to the adoptive mother’s low sensitivity that was mediated by the adopted child’s impulsive behavior (73). In other words, a child’s ADHD symptoms may give rise to ineffective parenting, rather than parenting giving rise to the child’s symptoms. A related but distinct construct is parent–child dyadic synchrony, which represents reciprocal adaptation of behaviors and affect between parent and child to form a single relational unit (74). A systematic review of children age 2 months to 5 years found that dyadic synchrony was associated with neurodevelopmental health, including higher IQ, attachment security, and fewer internalizing symptoms (75). Taken together, this work highlights the complexity of parent–child interactions and how parental behaviors and responsiveness may both influence, and be influenced by, the offspring’s behavior and neurodevelopment.

Studies that examine parenting after early toddlerhood and into adolescence often focus on the quality of parenting behavior, with a predominant focus on parental warmth, hostility or conflict, and control (76). Of these dimensions, parental hostility has been most commonly associated with adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes in children, including internalizing and externalizing behaviors and poorer executive functions (77, 78). Higher levels of parental warmth, on the other hand, have been linked with more positive neurodevelopmental outcomes like academic achievement, social competence, prosocial behaviors, emotional and behavioral regulation, and lower levels of externalizing behaviors (79). Interestingly, high levels of parental warmth and support have also been associated with higher levels of child inhibition across childhood (80, 81), suggesting that parental warmth may have different effects depending upon a child’s temperamental profile. The influence of parenting quality may also differ as a function of parent gender, with one study of Chinese children finding that father’s warmth, but not mother’s, was associated with child academic performance (82).

The parental behavior of “scaffolding,” defined as verbal or physical guidance from parents including support of autonomy and encouragement of problem solving and children’s choices (83, 84), has also been associated with higher levels of executive functioning during early childhood, including skills like working memory and cognitive flexibility (83), as well as better arithmetic skills (85) and improved early reading skills (86). The impact of scaffolding on neurodevelopment, however, may vary as a function of a child’s genetic background; one study found that maternal scaffolding was associated with higher levels of toddler behavioral problems for children at low familial risk, as indicated by the birth parents’ externalizing and internalizing problems, but with lower levels of behavioral problems for at high familial risk (87). Overall, parenting behaviors and interactional styles have robust associations with child neurodevelopment, but the complex interplay between parenting and children’s genetics require data from large-scale longitudinal studies such as ECHO.

Parental Mental Health and Functioning

In recent decades, there has been a sharp increase in research examining the role of parental psychopathology, stress, and trauma during pregnancy and the effects on offspring neurodevelopment. This work, which has focused largely on maternal effects [with some exceptions noted (88)], is often termed “fetal programming” and draws on concepts from evolutionary biology (89, 90) to posit that maternal experience and resultant biochemistry provide crucial information to the developing fetus. Fetal programming may improve the fetus’ likelihood of survival but may also detrimentally affect other aspects of development and health. For example, a number of studies have suggested that prenatal maternal trauma (e.g., war, divorce, physical and sexual abuse, natural disasters), particularly during the first trimester (91), contributes to neurobehavioral abnormalities in the child including motor impairments, emotional reactivity, hyperactivity, cognitive deficits, language delays, difficult temperament, social withdrawal, and aggression (91, 92), and may increase intrauterine inflammation with subsequent effects on offspring learning and memory (93). These neurobehavioral effects may persist into adulthood, with some studies linking prenatal trauma to an increased risk of post-traumatic stress disorder, schizophrenia, depression, and/or substance abuse disorder in the offspring (94, 95). Prenatal maternal stress need not be “traumatic” to impact the offspring. Accumulating research points to effects of a variety of indices of maternal stress (e.g., self-report of stressful life events or perceived daily stress) on offspring neurodevelopmental outcomes (96, 97), including increased infant reactivity, poorer emotional and behavioral regulation, and differences in brain structure and function in infancy and childhood (96, 98). It is important to note, however, that mild to moderate exposure to stress during pregnancy may not always be detrimental to the fetus. Indeed, some studies suggest it is associated with better offspring mental and psychomotor development (99), particularly when exposure occurs later in pregnancy (99) or when there are congruent levels of adversity during pregnancy and postpartum (96).

In contrast to fetal programming, genetic transmissions are also critical to consider. Indeed, associations between prenatal maternal stress and offspring neurodevelopment may in part, or even fully, be accounted for by shared genetic inheritance (100). Testing these competing hypotheses requires longitudinal assessments, coupled with detailed phenotypic, genetic, and epigenetic assays—a breadth and depth of data that, prior to ECHO, has rarely been available, particularly in U.S. cohorts.

In addition to prenatal maternal stress, maternal psychopathology, including depression, has been repeatedly associated with negative effects on offspring’s physical, cognitive, and affective development. Perinatal maternal depression (PMD) affects 11.9% of women worldwide, with prevalence rates ranging from 9 to 22% for prenatal depression and 7 to 19% for postnatal depression (101, 102). PMD increases risk for pregnancy complications, preterm birth, intrauterine growth restriction, and low birth weight (103, 104), all of which may impact the child’s subsequent neurodevelopment. Indeed, infants of prenatally depressed mothers show increased negative affect, irritability, growth retardation, and delays in cognitive, motor, and emotional development (105), as well as altered psychophysiological measures (e.g., vagal tone) and early brain development (e.g., increased right frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) activity and altered amygdala microstructure and functional connectivity) (106108). Though only a handful of longitudinal studies have been conducted, they suggest that PMD exposure increases risk for externalizing problems in childhood and for depression in adolescents, potentiating risk for long-term psychiatric consequences (109111).

During the postnatal period, parental stress and psychopathology also adversely affect offspring neurodevelopment, increasing risk for anxiety, emotional reactivity, cognitive deficits, language delays, aggression, substance abuse, and a host of other mental health problems throughout life (105, 107). Adverse neurodevelopmental effects to the offspring have been implicated from a broad range of parental psychopathologies (e.g., depression, bipolar disorder, and psychotic disorders), as well as non-psychiatric illnesses (e.g. cancer, chronic inflammatory disorders) (112), as demonstrated in large-scale, national registries (113, 114). Although pre- and post-natal exposure to maternal stress and psychopathology both impact offspring neurodevelopment, mechanistically they differ. Prenatal maternal stress is thought to exert its intergenerational influence primarily through its effects on the placenta, hypothalamicpituitaryadrenal (HPA) axis, and inflammation (115, 116). For example, when the HPA axis is activated in distressed mothers, peptides such as corticotropin-releasing hormone are released by the placenta, thereby exacerbating the effects of stress on both the mother and fetus (117, 118). Similarly, maternal cytokines increase with maternal stress, and some may pass through the placenta, thereby creating an inflammatory environment for the fetus (119). Conversely, the mechanisms by which postnatal maternal stress and psychopathology influence offspring neurodevelopment are more behavioral in nature, with adverse effects on caretaking (e.g., maltreatment, malnourishment, physical and sexual abuse) and mother-infant attachment. However, intriguing experimental research also suggests that maternal stress physiology is transmittable to her child’s physiology through touch, even when the baby cannot see the mother’s face, suggesting that maternal-to-infant transmission of stress may involve mechanisms beyond overt parent–child interactions (120, 121). These mechanistic pathways, however, are largely theorized and do not yet have a strong evidence base. Parallel to evidence from the parenting literature, there is also evidence for transactional or bidirectional relations between parental and child mood and behavior, beginning in early childhood, adding complexity to the process of understanding postnatal parental mental health impacts on children (122, 123). Large-scale longitudinal studies such as ECHO that measure maternal mental health during pregnancy and parental mental health postnatally will help disentangle the effects of parental mental health on child neurodevelopment.

Parental Substance Use

There is a vast literature on prenatal exposure to both licit and illicit drugs and adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes, including alcohol, smoking, cocaine, methamphetamine, opioids, and marijuana. The common and most robustly demonstrated effect across substance exposure type is an association with offspring behavioral disinhibition and associated clinical patterns (124, 125). Behavioral disinhibition encompasses trait-like deficits in cognitive and behavioral control that are evident from the first years of life (126, 127). Behavioral disinhibition includes impairments in top-down cognitive control processes subserved by the lateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices, impaired development of bottom-up arousal and reward centers in the nucleus accumbens and orbitofrontal cortex, as well as memory-related hippocampal structures (128). It presages a range of clinical problems including externalizing behaviors and related disorders and poor health and social functioning, more generally via its risk-taking and impulsive features (129, 130). Prenatal alcohol and cigarette exposure have the most strongly demonstrated and the most coherent patterns in relation to the development of disinhibition beginning in early life. Those links and their association with offspring substance use and severe antisocial behavior are particularly powerful as they highlight an intergenerational mechanism of transmission (131, 132). We highlight some of these links illustratively below.

Prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) is known to impact a range of neurodevelopmental domains such as attention, behavior, academic achievement, cognition, language development, memory, and motor and visuospatial development in children. The effects of PAE on neurodevelopment appear to vary by the dose, pattern, and timing of exposure during gestation, and the magnitude of the effect may vary by neurodevelopmental domain. On one end of the spectrum, heavy PAE is the cause of Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (133), which is characterized by neurological abnormalities (e.g. seizures), developmental delays, intellectual deficits, growth deficits, and distinct facial features. Heavy PAE has been linked to deficits in children’s performance, verbal IQ, and interpersonal skills (134, 135) and there is robust evidence of its links to behavioral disinhibition (e.g., hyperactivity, attentional deficits, and delinquency) (135). The findings for lower levels of PAE have been much more inconsistent—both deficits (136, 137) and benefits (138) in general cognitive functioning have been noted, and several studies have found no relationship with child behavioral problems (139). These inconsistencies may be due to a lack of adequate control for potential confounders such as home environment or SES.

Alcohol’s toxic effects on fetal neurodevelopment can occur through direct or indirect pathways. Directly, alcohol crosses the placenta and can impair neuronal maturation. Alcohol may also inhibit the transfer across the placenta of essential cofactors and/or antioxidants necessary for neuronal maturation (140). Alcohol induces oxidative stress through the generation of free radicals, hypoxia, and altered metabolism—all of which can lead to significant oxidative damage in fetal tissue and reductions in oxidant defense mechanisms in the fetus (141). Alcohol-induced oxidative stress and oxidative tissue damage have been linked to developmental delays (142). As alcohol is metabolized, free radicals are produced, which target polyunsaturated fatty acid chains in brain tissue and membranes, damaging developing neurons and resulting in neurodevelopmental impairment (143) or, in more severe cases, inducing uncontrolled apoptosis and fetal brain damage (144146).

Prenatal exposure to opioids, in the majority of children, gives rise to neonatal abstinence syndrome (147), characterized by hyperactivity of the central and autonomic nervous systems. While these abstinence effects are short-lived, epidemiologic research on the longer-term effects of in utero opioid exposure suggests a number of cognitive, motor, and behavioral deficits such as inattention and an increase in ADHD (148). Animal models further suggest a significant impact on fetal development including disruptions of neuronal migration and cell survival (149), decreases in dendrite length and branch number in pyramidal neurons in the somatosensory cortex (150), and increased neuronal apoptosis resulting in memory deficits (149).

In multiple independent studies using a variety of study designs, prenatal tobacco exposure (PTE) has been linked to behavioral disinhibition patterns beginning in early childhood (e.g., atypical externalizing trajectories, heightened negative affectivity), across childhood and adolescence (e.g., oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder) and into adulthood (e.g. criminal behavior, substance abuse) (125, 151, 152). Associations of PTE with disruptive behavior and impaired executive function under motivational conditions are consistently found as well as comorbid disruptive and ADHD syndromes, with weaker or inconsistent links to ADHD alone and “cool” executive functions (153155). The biologic plausibility of these patterns is undergirded by evidence from basic neuroscience (156), and from mechanistic studies demonstrating exposure-related alterations in neural organization (i.e., atypical white matter developmental trajectories in infants) and structural and functional exposure-related patterns in adolescence that have been implicated in antisocial behavior and substance abuse (e.g., orbitofrontal cortical thinning and functional decrements in response in the ventral striatum during reward anticipation).

As with PAE and PTE, studies examining prenatal cocaine, methamphetamine, opioid, marijuana, and others (e.g., MDMA), demonstrate widespread effects on neurodevelopment from infancy through later childhood. Effects have been observed on measures of attention, working memory and executive function, speed of processing, verbal ability, fine motor ability, and semantic and episodic memory (157). Longer-term effects have been observed in a number of studies, with a recent systematic review of prenatal cocaine exposure indicating small-to-moderate effects on language and memory measures into adolescence and young adulthood (158). Additionally, effects on children’s brain structure and function have been observed with some specificity. For example, methamphetamine exposure is associated with reduced volume and/or surface area of the basal ganglia and hippocampus (159), and cocaine exposure with reduced volume in the cerebellum, corpus callosum, and occipital and parietal lobes (160). Systematic review of the small neuroimaging literature of prenatal drug exposure tends to indicate vulnerability in dopaminergic projections within the mesocortical system as a potential common pathway of effects (160).

Links between prenatal substance exposure and behavioral disinhibition patterns and their clinical/functional correlates (e.g., antisocial personality disorder, criminality) are robust and independent of obvious confounders, including SES, parental mental health and substance abuse history, and quality of the family environment (161). However, prenatal substance use may have underlying genetic substrates predisposing to behavioral disinhibition (i.e., impulsive, nonconforming, sensation-seeking, and risky behavior), which would make these patterns attributable to genetic transmission rather than teratologic effects (162164). As a result, statistical control alone is inadequate for separating direct casual effects from family variation (165, 166). This issue has been addressed most rigorously in relation to prenatal smoking and behavioral disinhibition patterns, where the Surgeon General has assessed evidence as “suggestive but not conclusive” (167). Studies have creatively used quasi-experimental, genetically-sensitive designs to tease apart familial and teratologic mechanisms. Most influential have been large population-based studies using discordant sibling designs leveraging within family differences in sibling exposure to test whether individual level exposure has an independent effect (168). The majority of these studies have failed to show independent exposure effects, leading to conclusions that exposure is merely a marker for genetic risk (168170). The conclusion that these associations are spurious, however, is called into question by evidence that independent exposure effects are present when behavioral genetic designs are coupled with high quality exposure measurement (165).

In addition to maternal prenatal substance use, contributions of postnatal substance use to offspring neurodevelopment have been examined, particularly as a means of comparing putative teratologic vs. family relationship correlates (171). Although early studies took an either/or approach, it is increasingly evident that the neurodevelopmental impact of exposure timing must be understood in a more nuanced manner. Both additive and interactive effects have been demonstrated, likely reflecting a combination of teratologic effects, disruptions in home environment and parental responsiveness, and heritability (125, 172). ECHO’s large sample size, robust pre- and postnatal exposure measurement and varied participant cohort designs provide a rare opportunity to disentangle these pathways.

Co-Occurrence and Context of Exposures

Adverse exposures within the family environment are often situated within a landscape intermixed with additional exposures (3). For example, hazardous waste sites, chemical plants, smelters, and other polluting industries are disproportionately situated near low-SES communities (173). It is thus possible that the influence of low SES on neurodevelopment may, in some cases, be mediated or exacerbated by chemical toxicants more common in a low SES environment. This example of “double jeopardy” arises not only from the intersection of exposures across domains (SES and chemical toxicants), but also in how exposures can synergistically increase health risks. For example, maternal periconceptional nutrition is influenced by family SES and may protect high SES children from the adverse impact of pesticides on risk for autistic spectrum disorders (ASD) and intellectual development (174). Therefore, low-SES children may not only be more likely to be exposed to some chemical toxicants, but their neuropsychiatric sequelae may be exacerbated by other risk factors disproportionately represented in a low SES family environment. Rich exposure measurement in ECHO will advance our ability to conduct well-powered examinations that control for confounding by other exposures and/or allow for the determination of cumulative and interactive effects, although careful attention will need to be paid to modeling and interpretation given the complexities of multi-exposure models (175). In addition, the public health significance of a given exposure may vary substantially by neurodevelopmental outcome. Therefore, in addition to considering multiple exposures, ECHO presents an opportunity to also examine multiple-outcomes, and pinpoint the specificity of cumulative and interactive effects of family context exposures (175).

It is also important to note that associations between neurodevelopment and the family environment may be contingent upon the culture in which the family is situated. For example, overcrowding (see Home Environment) has been shown to predict negative neurodevelopmental outcomes for children in Western cultures (176), which are characterized by individualism and a high demand for personal space and privacy; however, a high number of people living in a household may benefit children’s socio-emotional development in cultures that emphasize collective child-rearing and have a higher tolerance for crowding. Similarly, associations between neurodevelopment and the home environment may vary in urban vs. rural settings (177). Children raised in more remote areas, where they have broad access to natural settings for play and socialization, may be less affected by limited access to “standard” learning materials—access to natural settings that increase free play and peer interactions may even promote a child’s socio-emotional and cognitive competence (178).

Another example of the importance of culture and context in shaping neurodevelopment is evident in the influence of family composition and parental transitions. As noted above, children reared in two- vs. single-parent families generally fare better on neurodevelopmental outcomes (see Family Composition). However, recent research using birth cohort data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study revealed that among Black children and girls (compared to other racial/ethnic groups and boys) transitions from two- to one-parent families negatively influenced cognitive achievement, but among White children (regardless of gender) the negative impact was more evident with behavioral outcomes (28). Conversely, transitions into two-parent families had adverse behavioral effects on Latino children. Findings from the Boricua Youth Study have similarly reinforced the importance of context in moderating the relationship between family composition and neurodevelopment, demonstrating, for example, that transitions from two- to single-parent families have a greater impact on children’s internalizing symptoms among Puerto Rican children living in Puerto Rico relative to those living in the New York (179). Rich representation in ECHO, and sufficient power should advance understanding in these realms.

In addition, disparities by sex and gender in rates of various neurodevelopmental disorders, as well as discoveries of sex-specific biological mechanisms of exposure transmission and gender as a social determinant of health and moderator of sex effects, point to the important role of sex and gender in the context of the associations described above (180). However, sex-specific findings across studies are remarkably mixed and contractictory (181), and fathers’ roles are sorely understudied, limiting progress in this domain. A sex- and gender-informed perspective is required for advancements in discovery, enhanced relevance of scientific findings, and improved patient care (181). Large samples of boys and girls, assessing both maternal and paternal contributions to children’s environment and child sex- and gender-specific vulnerabilities or resiliencies to exposures, are needed.

Developmental Timing and the Family Environment

Although the family environment plays an important role in neurodevelopment across the life course, there is longstanding acknowledgement of the importance of critical and/or sensitive periods of development (182, 183), during which brain and physiologic systems are particularly plastic. This plasticity to environment is largely adaptive, although in contexts with significant adverse exposures it can contribute to aberrant neurodevelopment (184). The embryonic and fetal period may be particularly important sensitive windows during which the effects of adversity can be transmitted across generations, and prenatal programming may begin to set the stage for subsequent psychopathology. Postnatally, early childhood (185) and the pubertal period (186, 187), when substantial neurobiological and hormonal changes occur rapidly, may also be key periods of sensitivity when the effects of SES, parenting, and the home and external social environment have substantial impacts across a range of neurodevelopmental outcomes. Outside of animal models, developmental research to date has rarely had the capacity to systematically test for sensitive periods because studies typically focus on a particular developmental period, pre–postnatal exposures are highly correlated (e.g. harsh parenting or parental depression may not be limited to a specific developmental period), and/or sample sizes are inadequate for examining within group differences in exposure timing. In light of the interactions between family/social and chemical stressors described earlier, and the accumulating evidence for sensitive periods for toxin effects on neurodevelopment (188, 189), attention to developmental period is imperative. ECHO will be uniquely positioned to examine these questions with its large sample size, common protocol and extensive high-quality data on a range of exposures from preconception through pregnancy, early-to-mid childhood, and through adolescence. In doing so, ECHO will deliver more mechanistic and developmentally-informed insights into contextual factors—including the family environment—that influence child neuromaturation and identify modifiable elements of the environment that can promote optimal health and development with an eye towards targeted prevention as early as possible in the clinical sequence.

Echo and the Family Environment

The ECHO study is poised to address some of the major gaps in the examination of the complex influences of the family environment on psychiatric outcomes, beginning in utero. In developing the ECHO study, the National Institutes of Health (NIH) solicited applications from existing cohorts to address the influence of pre, peri, and postnatal environmental exposures on childhood development. The ECHO study can thus leverage existing data (i.e., data collected by ECHO cohorts prior to the onset of the ECHO study, or pre-ECHO-wide data), as well as prospective data (i.e., data collected by ECHO cohorts after the onset of the ECHO study, or the ECHO-wide protocol) to lead inquiry into pediatric health and disease. The consortium prioritizes five primary outcome domains: neurodevelopment (highlighted here), as well as pre-/perinatal outcomes, airway, obesity, and positive health. The consortium sample of children has roughly equal numbers of male and female children, and will collect data on gender identity across development, which will advance consideration of sex and gender in the etiology and prevalence of those outcomes.

Existing data that ECHO cohorts collected prior to the onset of ECHO (pre-ECHO-wide data) followed protocols specific to each cohort’s scientific aims. Conversely, prospective ECHO data (ECHO-wide protocol) will include a systematic protocol with uniform data elements across all ECHO sites, harmonizing data collection and facilitating shared, or pooled, data analysis. Prospective ECHO data collection is ongoing and will continue through 2023. A description of the ECHO-wide protocol is detailed here: https://echochildren.org/echo-program-protocol/

ECHO intends to make both the existing and prospective data (i.e. pre-ECHO-wide data and ECHO-wide protocol) available to the consortium of investigators within ECHO, as well as to the broader scientific community in order to maximize impact, intellectual engagement, and data utilization. Herein, we present an overview of existing ECHO data (pre-ECHO-wide data) within the family environment domains that we have reviewed (Tables 1A–C), additional family environment variables not directly discussed in this review (Supplementary Table S1), as well as the ECHO recruitment sites and geographic diversity of its cohorts (Figure 2).

TABLE 1A
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1A (A–C) ECHO cohorts and sample size with family environment survey data.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 ECHO recruitment sites. Red dots represent the locations of the recruitment sites for the ECHO cohorts. States and regions are color-coded by poverty rates.

We compiled pre-ECHO-wide data data from ECHO cohorts from the ECHO Data Analysis Center (ECHO-DAC), which conducted a survey via secure, web-based Voxco (http://www.voxco.com/) of the specific survey instruments used with mothers, fathers, and children at each life stage by each ECHO cohort. The survey was sent to all ECHO cohort principal investigators, all of whom completed it. Information from this survey was tabulated by the ECHO-Data Analysis Center (ECHO-DAC). Based on these data sources, Tables 1A–C provide the number of ECHO cohorts with data available in each of the family environment domains. These estimates assume that there is sufficient similarity across instruments to allow harmonization of data across ECHO cohorts. The ECHO surveys were conducted between January and March 2018, and the surveys can be assessed through the ECHO metadata catalog (http://www.echoportal.org/); registration is required.

Out of 70 ECHO cohorts, the vast majority (between 55 and 69 cohorts) included primary measures of SES (income, education, occupation and employment status) and family composition (marital status, 61 cohorts) (Table 1A). A large number of cohorts also include measures of maternal psychosocial stress (52 cohorts) as well as alcohol and tobacco use (58 and 61 cohorts, respectively). While a smaller number of cohorts included measures of maternal social networks, maternal adverse childhood events, and exposure to interpersonal violence, these exposures may nonetheless be sufficiently well powered in an ECHO-wide analysis. Many ECHO cohorts also include information on father’s SES and marital status, with a smaller number including measures of paternal psychosocial stress, family psychiatric history, and substance use (Table 1B). Few cohorts included measures of father’s social networks, exposure to interpersonal violence, adverse childhood experiences, or racial discrimination. Finally, there is broad representation across the ECHO cohorts of additional measures that further contextualize the child’s environment including: detailed information on family composition such as household size and birth order, as well as parent–child relationship measures and childcare arrangements (Table 1C).

Figure 2 displays the location of the recruitment sites for the ECHO cohorts and was generated in ARCGIS version 10.3 (https://www.arcgis.com/features/index.html). Information on the location of the ECHO cohorts and their recruitment sites was assessed by a Voxco survey sent by the ECHO-DAC to all cohort principal investigators; all cohorts completed the survey. States are color coded by childhood poverty rates, which were obtained on 2/14/2018 from the US Census Bureau (https://www.census.gov/topics/income-poverty/poverty.html).

To illustrate the utility and strength that ECHO will provide for the field, we highlight an ongoing ECHO analysis examining the complexities inherent to the search for causes of autism. Meta-analysis indicates a null association between maternal cigarette smoking and increased risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (190). These studies, however, have been limited, not adequately controlling for confounders or inappropriately controlling for intermedia. At the same time, over a dozen studies have reported significant associations between prenatal exposures to air pollutants and increased risk for ASD (191). Because the components of air pollution and cigarette smoke share thousands of chemicals in common, this discrepancy raises a conundrum—is it credible that air pollution, which represents a generally lower level of exposure, has a causal effect on ASD while cigarette smoking, which generally includes much higher concentrations, has no impact?

Far better than any single or small set of studies, ECHO can effectively address this issue. Indeed, an ECHO effort is currently underway to conduct a multi-site examination of the association between prenatal cigarette smoke and the risk for ASD that will systematically account for a wide range of potential confounders within the family environment, including sociodemographic factors, health care access, maternal health conditions and her use of medications and other substances, family history of developmental and psychiatric conditions, and other environmental pollutants such as pesticides and particulate matter. This coordinated, distributed analysis will culminate in a statistically powerful meta-analysis of harmonized data from across the country, which has previously been difficult to achieve. Future ECHO analyses in this arena will integrate genomics to further advance this inquiry.

Conclusions

Despite the significant advances made in understanding the relationship between family environment and neurodevelopment, important gaps persist that ECHO is well positioned to address. First, prior research has relied heavily upon cross-sectional approaches and/or retrospective reporting on the family environment, many of which were not designed a priori to examine the impact of the family environment on neurodevelopment. Prospective, longitudinal methodologies, and high-quality measurements, such as have been and will be used in ECHO, will allow for in-depth examination of specific factors influencing offspring neurodevelopmental trajectories with more precise estimates of how these factors mediate changes in ongoing developmental processes and across multiple potential sensitive periods of development. Second, many studies of the family environment have relied on convenient or highly-selected samples, calling into question their generalizability to the broader US population. ECHO, with its large, diverse cohorts from across the US, will address generalizability and will also foster more rigorous methods to strengthen causal inferences, such as propensity score analysis and/or repeated measures analyses (e.g. using within-child, fixed effects modeling to partially account for endogeneity).

With increasing evidence that the origins of mental health, disorder, and corollary neurodevelopmental disruptions begin even before birth (192), the large-scale, prospective consortium approach of ECHO is vital to explicating the contribution of a broad range of perinatal and early childhood environmental exposures to these pathways (79). Exposures are multifaceted and complex and their effects may vary depending on when they occur during development. Within the ECHO framework, we have a roadmap for deepening and broadening characterization of the role of multiple features of the family environment in the developmental unfolding of mental disorders. Systematic specification of malleable family processes that underlie emergent and sustained mental health risk is the crucial next step towards early life prevention of developmental psychopathology and its comorbidities.

Author Contributions

Each of the authors contributed to conceptualizing, drafting, and editing the manuscript.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the Environmental influences on Child Health Outcomes (ECHO) program, Office of The Director, National Institutes of Health, under Award Number U2COD023375 (Coordinating Center), U24OD023382 (Data Analysis Center), and UG3OD023271, UG3OD023365, UG3OD023344, UG3OD023328, UG3OD023332, UG3OD023389, UG3OD023279, UG3OD023348, NIEHS (National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences R01-ES015359; P30-ES023513, UG3OD023305 (NYU), U24OD023319.

Disclaimer

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Conflict of Interest

JP has received research support from Takeda (formerly Shire) and Aevi Genomics and consultancy fees from Innovative Science.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank our ECHO colleagues, the clinical, program, and research staff; and the children and their parents who agreed to participate in the ECHO protocols. We also acknowledge the contributions of the following ECHO program collaborators:

 ECHO Components—Coordinating Center: Duke Clinical Research Institute, Durham, North Carolina: Benjamin DK, Smith PB, Newby KL; Data Analysis Center: Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland: Jacobson LP; Research Triangle Institute, Durham, North Carolina: Parker CB; Person-Reported Outcomes Core: Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois: Gershon R, Cella D; Children’s Health and Exposure Analysis Resource: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York City, New York: Teitelbaum S; Wright RO; Wadsworth Center, Albany, New York: Aldous, KM, RTI International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina: Fennell T; University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota: Hecht SS, Peterson L; Westat, Inc., Rockville, Maryland: O’Brien B; Idea States Pediatric Trials Network: University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock: Lee JY, Snowden J

 ECHO Awardees and Cohorts: University of Washington, Seattle, WA, Karr C, Sathyanarayana S, Tylavsky FA; University of California at Davis, Davis, CA, Bennett DH, Schweitzer JB; University of New Mexico Health Sciences Center, Albuquerque, NM Lewis JL; New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, Canino G, Monk C; New York University, New York, NY; University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, Ganiban JM; Avera McKennan Hospital and University Health Center, Sioux Falls, SD; University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, Fry R.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00547/full#supplementary-material.

References

1. Bale TL, Baram TZ, Brown AS, Goldstein JM, Insel TR, McCarthy MM, et al. Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 68:314–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Wakschlag LS, Perlman SB, Blair RJ, Leibenluft E, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Pine DS, et al. The neurodevelopmental basis of early childhood disruptive behavior: irritable and callous phenotypes as exemplars. Am J Psychiatry (2017) 175:114–30. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17010045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Guloksuz S, van Os J, Rutten B. The exposome paradigm and the complexities of environmental research in psychiatry. JAMA Psychiatry 75 (2018) 75:985–6. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.1211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Niedzwiecki MM, Walker DI, Vermeulen R, Chadeau-Hyman M, Jones DP, Miller GW. The Exposome: Molecules to Populations. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol (2019) 59:107–27. doi: 10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-010818-021315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Martin J, Taylor MJ, Lichtenstein P. Assessing the evidence for shared genetic risks across psychiatric disorders and traits. Psychol Med (2018) 48:1759–74. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717003440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Merikangas KR, Merikangas AK. Harnessing Progress in Psychiatric Genetics to Advance Population Mental Health. Am J Public Health (2019) 109:S171–s175. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2019.304948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. DeNavas-Walt C, Proctor BD. Income and poverty in the United States: 2014. Washington, DC (2015).

Google Scholar

8. Suitts S, Barba P, Dunn K. A new majority: low income students now a majority in the nation"s public schools. Atlanta, GA (2015).

Google Scholar

9. Ackerman BP, Brown ED, Izard CE. The relations between contextual risk, earned income, and the school adjustment of children from economically disadvantaged families. Dev Psychol (2004) 40:204. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Economic deprivation and early childhood development. Child Dev (1994) 65:296–318. doi: 10.2307/1131385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Sirin SR. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Rev Educ Res (2005) 75:417–53. doi: 10.3102/00346543075003417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Blair C, Granger DA, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R, Cox M, Greenberg MT, et al. Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Dev (2011) 82:1970–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Noble KG, Norman MF, Farah MJ. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev Sci (2005) 8:74–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Shaw DS, Shelleby EC. Early-starting conduct problems: Intersection of conduct problems and poverty. Annu Rev Clin Psychol (2014) 10:503–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Merz EC, Tottenham N, Noble KG. Socioeconomic status, amygdala volume, and internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 47 (2017) 47:1–12. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2017.1326122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Merikangas KR, He JP, Burstein M, Swanson SA, Avenevoli S, Cui L, et al. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2010) 49:980–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Dev Sci (2007) 10:464–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatr (2015) 169:822–9. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.1475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Li R, Darling N, Maurice E, Barker L, Grummer-Strawn LM. Breastfeeding rates in the United States by characteristics of the child, mother, or family: the 2002 National Immunization Survey. Pediatrics (2005) 115:e31–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Kramer MS, Aboud F, Mironova E, Vanilovich I, Platt RW, Matush L, et al. Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: new evidence from a large randomized trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2008) 65:578–84. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Stuebe A. The risks of not breastfeeding for mothers and infants. Rev Obstetrics Gynecol (2009) 2:222. doi: 10.3909/riog0093]

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Brito N, Noble K. Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Front Neurosci (2014) 8:276. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Johnson SB, Riis JL, Noble KG. State of the art review: poverty and the developing brain. Pediatrics Peds. 137 (2016) 137:2015–3075. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-3075

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Carlson MJ, Corcoran ME. Family structure and children’s behavioral and cognitive outcomes. J Marriage Fam (2001) 63:779–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2001.00779.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Amato PR. The impact of family formation change on the cognitive, social, and emotional well-being of the next generation. Future Child (2005) 15:75–96. doi: 10.1353/foc.2005.0012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Amato PR, Keith B. Parental divorce and the well-being of children: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bul (1991) 110:26–46. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.26

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Cooksey EC. Consequences of young mothers’ marital histories for children’s cognitive development. J Marriage Fam (1997) 59:245–61. doi: 10.2307/353468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lee D, McLanahan S. Family structure transitions and child development; instability, selection, and population heterogeneity. Am Sociol Rev (2015) 80:738–63. doi: 10.1177/0003122415592129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Murray J, Farrington DP, Sekol I. Children’s antisocial behavior, mental health, drug use, and educational performance after parental incarceration: a systematic review and meta-analysis. psychol Bull (2012) 138:175. doi: 10.1037/a0026407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Bjerkedal T, Kristensen P, Skjeret GA, Brevik JI. Intelligence test scores and birth order among young Norwegian men (conscripts) analyzed within and between families. Intelligence (2007) 35:503–14. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Boomsma DI, van Beijsterveld TCEM, Beem AL, Hoekstra RA, Polderman TJC, Bartels M. Intelligence and birth order in boys and girls. Intelligence (2008) 36:630–4. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kampouri M, Kyriklaki A, Roumeliotaki T, Koutra K, Anousaki D, Sarri K, et al. Patterns of early-life social and environmental exposures and child cognitive development, Rhea Birth Cohort, Crete, Greece. Child Dev (2018) 89:1063–73. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wichman AL, Rodgers JL, MacCallum RC. A multilevel approach to the relationship between birth order and intelligence. Pers Soc Psychol Bull (2006) 32:117–27. doi: 10.1177/0146167205279581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Magnuson K, Berger LM. Family structure states and transitions: associations with children’s wellbeing during middle childhood. J Marriage Fam (2009) 71:575–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2009.00620.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Sandberg J, Rafail P. Family size, cognitive outcomes, and familial interaction in stable, two-parent families: United States, 1997-2002. Demography (2014) 51:1895–931. doi: 10.1007/s13524-014-0331-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Zajonc RB. The family dynamics of intellectual development. Am Psychol (2001) 56:490. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.6-7.490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Damian RI, Roberts BW. The associations of birth order with personality and intelligence in a representative sample of US high school students. J Res Pers (2015) 58:96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Ford T, Goodman R, Meltzer H. The relative importance of child, family, school and neighbourhood correlates of childhood psychiatric disorder. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol (2004) 39:487–96. doi: 10.1007/s00127-004-0782-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Amato PR. Children of divorce in the 1990s: an update of the Amato and Keith (1991) meta-analysis. J Fam Psychol (2001) 15:355–70. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.15.3.355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Weaver JM, Schofield TJ. Mediation and moderation of divorce effects on children’s behavior problems. J Family Psychol (2015) 29:39–48. doi: 10.1037/fam0000043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol (2002) 53:371–99. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Bradley RH, Putnick DL. Housing quality and access to material and learning resources within the home environment in developing countries. Child Dev (2012) 83:76–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01674.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Meeker JD, Sathyanarayana S, Swan SH. Phthalates and other additives in plastics: human exposure and associated health outcomes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2009) 364:2097–113. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Jurewicz J, Hanke W. Exposure to phthalates: reproductive outcome and children health. A review of epidemiological studies. Int J Occup Med Environ Health (2011) 24:115–41. doi: 10.2478/s13382-011-0022-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Thompson RC, Moore CJ, vom Saal FS, Swan SH. Plastics, the environment and human health: current consensus and future trends. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2009) 364:2153–66. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Nicolescu R, Petcu C, Cordeanu A, Fabritius K, Schlumpf M, Krebs R, et al. Environmental exposure to lead, but not other neurotoxic metals, relates to core elements of ADHD in Romanian children: performance and questionnaire data. Environ Res (2010) 110:476–83. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2010.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Larsson M, Weiss B, Janson S, Sundell J, Bornehag CG. Associations between indoor environmental factors and parental-reported autistic spectrum disorders in children 6-8 years of age. Neurotoxicology (2009) 30:822–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2009.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Bellinger DC. Prenatal Exposures to Environmental Chemicals and Children’s Neurodevelopment: An Update. Saf Health Work (2013) 4:1–11. doi: 10.5491/SHAW.2013.4.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Posner J, Polanczyk GV, Sonuga-Barke E. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet (2020) 395:450–62. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)33004-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Gallo EF, Posner J. Moving towards causality in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: overview of neural and genetic mechanisms. Lancet Psychiatry (2016) 3:555–67. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00096-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Lewis J, Hoover J, MacKenzie D. Mining and Environmental Health Disparities in Native American Communities. Curr Environ Health Rep (2017) 4:130–41. doi: 10.1007/s40572-017-0140-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Dodson RE, Udesky JO, Colton MD, McCauley M, Camann DE, Yau AY, et al. Chemical exposures in recently renovated low-income housing: Influence of building materials and occupant activities. Environ Int (2017) 109:114–27. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2017.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Krieger J, Higgins DL. Housing and health: time again for public health action. Am J Public Health (2002) 92:758–68. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.92.5.758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Burgos S, Ruiz P, Koifman R. Changes to indoor air quality as a result of relocating families from slums to public housing. Atmos Environ (1994) (2013) 70:179–85. doi: 10.1016/j.atmosenv.2012.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Melhuish EC, Phan MB, Sylva K, Sammons P, Siraj-Blatchford I, Taggart B. Effects of the home learning environment and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. J Soc Issues (2008) 64:95–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Bassuk EL, DeCandia CJ, Tsertsvadze A, Richard MK. The effectiveness of housing interventions and housing and service interventions on ending family homelessness: a systematic review. Am J Orthopsychiat (2014) 84:457–74. doi: 10.1037/ort0000020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Bassuk EL, DeCandia CJ, Beach CA, Berman F. America"s Youngest Outcasts: A Report Card on Child Homelessness Washington, DC: The National Center on Family Homelessness, American Institutes for Research (2014).

Google Scholar

58. Obradović J, Long JD, Cutuli JJ, Chan CK, Hinz E, Heistad D, et al. Academic achievement of homeless and highly mobile children in an urban school district: Longitudinal evidence on risk, growth, and resilience. Dev Psychopathol (2009) 21:493–518. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Grant R, Gracy D, Goldsmith G, Shapiro A, Redlener IE. Twenty-five years of child and family homelessness: where are we now? Am J Public Health (2013) 103(Suppl 2):e1–10. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Obradovic J. Effortful Control and Adaptive Functioning of Homeless Children: Variable- and Person-focused Analyses. J Appl Dev Psychol (2010) 31:109–17. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2009.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Grant R, Shapiro A, Joseph S, Goldsmith S, Rigual-Lynch L, Redlener I. The health of homeless children revisited. Adv Pediatr (2007) 54:173–87. doi: 10.1016/j.yapd.2007.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Cutuli JJ, Herbers JE, Rinaldi M, Masten AS, Oberg CN. Asthma and behavior in homeless 4- to 7-year-olds. Pediatrics (2010) 125:145–51. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Baumrind D. Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child Dev 37 (1966) 37:887–907. doi: 10.2307/1126611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Maccoby EE, Martin JA. Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In: Mussen PH, editor. Handbook of child psychology: formerly Carmichael"s Manual of child psychology. Wiley, New York, NY (1983).

Google Scholar

65. Wakschlag LS, Hans SL. Relation of maternal responsiveness during infancy to the development of behavior problems in high-risk youths. Dev Psychol (1999) 35:569. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Pinquart M, Kauser R. Do the Associations of Parenting Styles With Behavior Problems and Academic Achievement Vary by Culture? Results From a Meta-Analysis. Cult Divers Ethn Minor Psychol (2017) 24:75–100. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Wolff MS, Ijzendoorn MH. Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Dev (1997) 68:571–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04218.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Bell RQ. A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. psychol Rev (1968) 75:81. doi: 10.1037/h0025583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Bell RQ. Stimulus control of parent or caretaker behavior by offspring. Dev Psychol (1971) 4:63. doi: 10.1037/h0030374

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Sulik MJ, Blair C, Mills-Koonce R, Berry D, Greenberg M. Early parenting and the development of externalizing behavior problems: longitudinal mediation through children’s executive function. Child Dev (2015) 86:1588–603. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Combs-Ronto LA, Olson SL, Lunkenheimer ES, Sameroff AJ. Interactions between maternal parenting and children’s early disruptive behavior: Bidirectional associations across the transition from preschool to school entry. J Abnormal Child Psychol (2009) 37:1151. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9332-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Hipwell A, Kennan K, Kasza K, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Bean T. Reciprocal influences between girls’ conduct problems and depression, and parental punishment and warmth: A six year prospective analysis. J Abnormal Child Psychol (2008) 36:663–77. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9206-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Harold GT, Leve LD, Barrett D, Elam K, Neiderhiser JM, Natsuaki MN, et al. Biological and rearing mother influences on child ADHD symptoms: revisiting the developmental interface between nature and nurture. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2013) 54:1038–46. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony: Biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Curr Dir psychol Sci (2007) 16:340–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Leclère C, Viaux S, Avril M, Achard C, Chetouani M, Missonnier S, et al. Why synchrony matters during mother-child interactions: a systematic review. PloS One (2014) 9:e113571. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Bornstein M, Leventhal T, Lerner R. Ecological settings and processes in developmental systems. In: Volume 4 of the Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Wiley, New York, NY (2015).

Google Scholar

77. Bayer JK, Ukoumunne OC, Mathers M, Wake M, Abdi N, Hiscock H. Development of children’s internalising and externalising problems from infancy to five years of age. Aust New Z J Psychiatry (2012) 46:659–68. doi: 10.1177/0004867412450076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Grolnick WS, Pomerantz EM. Issues and challenges in studying parental control: Toward a new conceptualization. Child Dev Perspect (2009) 3:165–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00099.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Hane AA, Cheah C, Rubin KH, Fox NA. The role of maternal behavior in the relation between shyness and social reticence in early childhood and social withdrawal in middle childhood. Soc Dev (2008) 17:795–811. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00481.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Park S-Y, Belsky J, Putnam S, Crnic K. Infant emotionality, parenting, and 3-year inhibition: Exploring stability and lawful discontinuity in a male sample. Dev Psychol (1997) 33:218. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.2.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Rubin KH, Hastings PD, Stewart SL, Henderson HA, Chen X. The consistency and concomitants of inhibition: Some of the children, all of the time. Child Dev (1997) 68:467–83. doi: 10.2307/1131672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Chen X, Liu M, Li D. Parental warmth, control, and indulgence and their relations to adjustment in Chinese children: a longitudinal study. J Family Psychol (2000) 14:401. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.14.3.401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Fay-Stammbach T, Hawes DJ, Meredith P. Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Dev Perspect (2014) 8:258–64. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Mermelshtine R. Parent–child learning interactions: A review of the literature on scaffolding. Br J Educ Psychol (2017) 87:241–54. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Casey BM, Dearing E, Dulaney A, Heyman M, Springer R. Young girls’ spatial and arithmetic performance: The mediating role of maternal supportive interactions during joint spatial problem solving. Early Childhood Res Q (2014) 29:636–48. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Dieterich SE, Assel MA, Swank P, Smith KE, Landry SH. The impact of early maternal verbal scaffolding and child language abilities on later decoding and reading comprehension skills. J School Psychol (2006) 43:481–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2005.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Leve LD, Harold GT, Ge X, Neiderhiser JM, Shaw D, Scaramella LV. Structured parenting of toddlers at high versus low genetic risk: Two pathways to child problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2009) 48:1102–9. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b8bfc0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Yehuda R, Daskalakis NP, Lehrner A, Desarnaud F, Bader HN, Makotkine I, et al. Influences of maternal and paternal PTSD on epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor gene in Holocaust survivor offspring. Am J Psychiatry (2014) 171:872–80. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13121571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Pluess M, Belsky J. Prenatal programming of postnatal plasticity? Dev Psychopathol (2011) 23:29–38. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Ellis BJ, Del Giudice M. Beyond allostatic load: Rethinking the role of stress in regulating human development. Dev Psychopathol (2014) 26:1–20. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Coussons-Read ME. Effects of prenatal stress on pregnancy and human development: mechanisms and pathways. Obstetric Med (2013) 6:52–7. doi: 10.1177/1753495x12473751

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Wadhwa PD, Sandman CA, Garite TJ. The neurobiology of stress in human pregnancy: implications for prematurity and development of the fetal central nervous system. Prog Brain Res (2001) 133:131–42. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(01)33010-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Golan H, Lev V, Hallak M, Sorokin Y, Huleihel M. Specific neurodevelopmental damage in mice offspring following maternal inflammation during pregnancy. Neuropharmacology (2005) 48:903–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Watson JB, Mednick SA, Huttunen M, Wang X. Prenatal teratogens and the development of adult mental illness. Dev Psychopathol (1999) 11:457–66. doi: 10.1017/S0954579499002151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Khashan AS, Abel KM, McNamee R, Pedersen MG, Webb RT, Baker PN, et al. Higher risk of offspring schizophrenia following antenatal maternal exposure to severe adverse life events. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2008) 65:146–52. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, Glynn LM. Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology (2012) 95:7–21. doi: 10.1159/000327017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Dunkel Schetter C. Psychological Science on Pregnancy: Stress Processes, Biopsychosocial Models, and Emerging Research Issues. Annu Rev Psychol (2011) 62:531–58. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Demir-Lira OE, Voss JL, O'Neil JT, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Wakschlag LS, Booth JR. Early-life stress exposure associated with altered prefrontal resting-state fMRI connectivity in young children. Dev Cognit Neurosci (2016) 19:107–14. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Davis EP, Sandman CA. The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Dev (2010) 81:131–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01385.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Lugo-Candelas C, Monk C, Duarte CS, Lee S, Posner J. Shared genetic factors, fetal programming, and the transmission of depression. Lancet Psychiatry (2018) 5:771–3. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30356-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Woody CA, Ferrari AJ, Siskind DJ, Whiteford HA, Harris MG. A systematic review and meta-regression of the prevalence and incidence of perinatal depression. J Affect Disord (2017) 219:86–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Underwood L, Waldie K, D’Souza S, Peterson ER, Morton S. A review of longitudinal studies on antenatal and postnatal depression. Arch Womens Ment Health (2016) 19:711–20. doi: 10.1007/s00737-016-0629-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Bonari L, Pinto N, Ahn E, Einarson A, Steiner M, Koren G. Perinatal risks of untreated depression during pregnancy. Can J Psychiatry (2004) 49:726–35. doi: 10.1177/070674370404901103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Grote NK, Bridge JA, Gavin AR, Melville JL, Iyengar S, Katon WJ. A meta-analysis of depression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2010) 67:1012–24. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Field T, Diego M, Hernandez-Reif M. Prenatal depression effects on the fetus and newborn: a review. Infant Behav Dev (2006) 29:445–55. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Field T, Fox NA, Pickens J, Nawrocki T. Relative right frontal EEG activation in 3-to 6-month-old infants of” depressed” mothers. Dev Psychol (1995) 31:358. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.3.358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Posner J, Cha J, Roy AK, Peterson BS, Bansal R, Gustafsson HC, et al. Alterations in amygdala-prefrontal circuits in infants exposed to prenatal maternal depression. Transl Psychiatry (2016) 6:e935. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Rifkin-Graboi A, Bai J, Chen H, Hameed WB, Sim LW, Tint MT, et al. Prenatal maternal depression associates with microstructure of right amygdala in neonates at birth. Biol Psychiatry (2013) 74:837–44. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Barker ED, Jaffee SR, Uher R, Maughan B. The contribution of prenatal and postnatal maternal anxiety and depression to child maladjustment. Depress Anxiety (2011) 28:696–702. doi: 10.1002/da.20856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Pawlby S, Hay DF, Sharp D, Waters CS, O’Keane V. Antenatal depression predicts depression in adolescent offspring: prospective longitudinal community-based study. J Affect Disord (2009) 113:236–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Wang Y, Bernanke J, Peterson BS, McGrath P, Stewart J, Chen Y, et al. The association between antidepressant treatment and brain connectivity in two double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trials: a treatment mechanism study. Lancet Psychiatry (2019) 6:667–74. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30179-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Visser A, Huizinga GA, van der Graaf WT, Hoekstra HJ, Hoekstra-Weebers JE. The impact of parental cancer on children and the family: a review of the literature. Cancer Treat Rev (2004) 30:683–94. doi: 10.1016/j.ctrv.2004.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Lindblad F, Weitoft GR, Hjern A. Maternal and paternal psychopathology increases risk of offspring ADHD equally. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci (2011) 20:367–72. doi: 10.1017/S2045796011000564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Joelsson P, Chudal R, Uotila J, Suominen A, Sucksdorff D, Gyllenberg D, et al. Parental psychopathology and offspring attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a nationwide sample. J Psychiatr Res (2017) 94:124–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. O’donnell K, O’connor T, Glover V. Prenatal stress and neurodevelopment of the child: focus on the HPA axis and role of the placenta. Dev Neurosci (2009) 31:285–92. doi: 10.1159/000216539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Meyer U, Yee BK, Feldon J. The neurodevelopmental impact of prenatal infections at different times of pregnancy: the earlier the worse? Neurosci (2007) 13:241–56. doi: 10.1177/1073858406296401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Weinstock M. Alterations induced by gestational stress in brain morphology and behaviour of the offspring. Prog Neurobiol (2001) 65:427–51. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(01)00018-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. King BR, Smith R, Nicholson RC. The regulation of human corticotrophin-releasing hormone gene expression in the placenta. Peptides (2001) 22:795–801. doi: 10.1016/S0196-9781(01)00393-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Girard S, Sebire G. Transplacental Transfer of Interleukin-1 Receptor Agonist and Antagonist Following Maternal Immune Activation. Am J Reprod Immunol (2016) 75:8–12. doi: 10.1111/aji.12444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Waters SF, West TV, Mendes WB. Stress contagion: Physiological covariation between mothers and infants. psychol Sci (2014) 25:934–42. doi: 10.1177/0956797613518352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Waters SF, West TV, Karnilowicz HR, Mendes WB. Affect Contagion Between Mothers and Infants: Examining Valence and Touch. J Exp Psychol Gen (2017) 146:1043–51. doi: 10.1037/xge0000322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Roubinov DS, Epel ES, Adler NE, Laraia BA, Bush NR. Transactions between maternal and child depressive symptoms emerge early in life. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol (2019), 1–11. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2019.1644649

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Gross HE, Shaw DS, Burwell RA, Nagin DS. Transactional processes in child disruptive behavior and maternal depression: A longitudinal study from early childhood to adolescence. Dev Psychopathol (2009) 21:139–56. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Fisher PA, Lester BM, De Garmo DS, Lagasse LL, Lin H, Shankaran S, et al. The combined effects of prenatal drug exposure and early adversity on neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood and adolescence. Dev Psychopathol (2011) 23:777–88. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Clark CAC, Espy KA, Wakschlag L. Developmental pathways from prenatal tobacco and stress exposure to behavioral disinhibition. Neurotoxicol Teratol (2016) 53:64–74. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2015.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Iacono WG, Malone SM, McGue M. Behavioral Disinhibition and the Development of Early-Onset Addiction: Common and Specific Influences. Annu Rev Clin Psychol (2008) 4:325–48. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Zucker RA, Donovan JE, Masten AS, Mattson ME, Moss HB. Early Developmental Processes and the Continuity of Risk for Underage Drinking and Problem Drinking. Pediatrics (2008) 121:S252–72. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2243B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Nigg JT. Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2017) 58:361–83. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci (2011) 108:2693–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Krueger RF, Piasecki TM. Toward a dimensional and psychometrically-informed approach to conceptualizing psychopathology. Behav Res Ther (2002) 40:485–99. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00016-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Chapman K, Tarter RE, Kirisci L, Cornelius MD. Childhood Neurobehavior Disinhibition Amplifies the Risk of Substance Use Disorder: Interaction of Parental History and Prenatal Alcohol Exposure. J Dev Behav Pediatr (2007) 28:219–24. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3180327907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Masten AS, Faden VB, Zucker RA, Spear LP. Underage Drinking: A Developmental Framework. Pediatrics (2008) 121:S235–51. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2243A

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Kleinman KP, Guthrie LB, Bellinger DC, Taveras EM, et al. Infant feeding and childhood cognition at ages 3 and 7 years: Effects of breastfeeding duration and exclusivity. JAMA Pediatr (2013) 167:836–44. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Steinhausen HC, Willms J, Spohr HL. Correlates of psychopathology and intelligence in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1994) 35:323–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01165.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Mattson SN, Riley EP. A review of the neurobehavioral deficits in children with fetal alcohol syndrome or prenatal exposure to alcohol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (1998) 22:279–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03651.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL, Sokol RJ, Chiodo LM, Corobana R. Maternal age, alcohol abuse history, and quality of parenting as moderators of the effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on 7.5-year intellectual function. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2004) 28:1732–45. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000145691.81233.FA

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Willford J, Leech S, Day N. Moderate prenatal alcohol exposure and cognitive status of children at age 10. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2006) 30:1051–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00119.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Flak AL, Su S, Bertrand J, Denny CH, Kesmodel US, Cogswell ME, et al. The association of mild, moderate, and binge prenatal alcohol exposure and child neuropsychological outcomes: a meta-analysis. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2014) 38:214–26. doi: 10.1111/acer.12214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Sayal K, Draper ES, Fraser R, Barrow M, Davey Smith G, Gray R. Light drinking in pregnancy and mid-childhood mental health and learning outcomes. Arch Dis Child (2013) 98:107–11. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2012-302436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Gupta KK, Gupta VK, Shirasaka T. An Update on Fetal Alcohol Syndrome-Pathogenesis, Risks, and Treatment. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2016) 40:1594–602. doi: 10.1111/acer.13135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Henderson GI, Chen JJ, Schenker S. Ethanol, oxidative stress, reactive aldehydes, and the fetus. Front Biosci (1999) 4:D541–50. doi: 10.2741/A450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Wells PS, Anderson DR, Rodger MA, Forgie MA, Florack P, Touchie D, et al. A randomized trial comparing 2 low-molecular-weight heparins for the outpatient treatment of deep vein thrombosis and pulmonary embolism. Arch Intern Med (2005) 165:733–8. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.7.733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Gemma S, Vichi S, Testai E. Metabolic and genetic factors contributing to alcohol induced effects and fetal alcohol syndrome. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2007) 31:221–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Cohen-Kerem R, Koren G. Antioxidants and fetal protection against ethanol teratogenicity. I. Review of the experimental data and implications to humans. Neurotoxicol Teratol (2003) 25:1–9. doi: 10.1016/S0892-0362(02)00324-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Guerri C, Montoliu C, Renau-Piqueras J. Involvement of free radical mechanism in the toxic effects of alcohol: implications for fetal alcohol syndrome. Adv Exp Med Biol (1994) 366:291–305. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-1833-4_20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Ramachandran V, Perez A, Chen J, Senthil D, Schneker S, Henderson GI. In utero ethanol exposure causes mitochondrial dysfunction, which can result in apoptotic cell death in fetal brain: a potential role for 4-hydroxynonenal. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2001) 25:862–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2001.tb02292.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Patrick SW, Schumacher RE, Benneyworth BD, Krans EE, McAllister JM, Davis MM. Neonatal abstinence syndrome and associated health care expenditures: United States, 2000-2009. Jama (2012) 307:1934–40. doi: 10.1016/j.ynpm.2012.06.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Bunikowski R, Grimmer I, Heiser A, Metze B, Schafer A, Obladen M. Neurodevelopmental outcome after prenatal exposure to opiates. Eur J Pediatr (1998) 157:724–30. doi: 10.1007/s004310050923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Wang Y, Han T-Z. Prenatal exposure to heroin in mice elicits memory deficits that can be attributed to neuronal apoptosis. Neuroscience (2009) 160:330–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.02.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Lu R, Liu X, Long H, Ma L. Effects of prenatal cocaine and heroin exposure on neuronal dendrite morphogenesis and spatial recognition memory in mice. Neurosci Lett (2012) 522:128–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.06.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Wakschlag LS, Leventhal BL, Pine DS, Pickett KE, Carter AS. Elucidating Early Mechanisms of Developmental Psychopathology: The Case of Prenatal Smoking and Disruptive Behavior. Child Dev (2006) 77:893–906. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00909.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Brennan PA, Grekin ER, Mortensen EL, Mednick SA. Relationship of Maternal Smoking During Pregnancy With Criminal Arrest and Hospitalization for Substance Abuse in Male and Female Adult Offspring. Am J Psychiatry (2002) 159:48–54. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.1.48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Wiebe SA, Clark CA, De Jong DM, Chevalier N, Espy KA, Wakschlag L, et al. Prenatal tobacco exposure and self-regulation in early childhood: Implications for developmental psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol (2015) 27:397–409. doi: 10.1017/S095457941500005X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Wakschlag LS, Pickett KE, Kasza KE, Loeber R. Is Prenatal Smoking Associated With a Developmental Pattern of Conduct Problems in Young Boys? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2006) 45:461–7. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000198597.53572.3e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Ball SW, Gilman SE, Mick E, Fitzmaurice G, Ganz ML, Seidman LJ, et al. Revisiting the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD. J Psychiatr Res (2010) 44:1058–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Slotkin T. Prenatal exposure to nicotine: What can we learn from animal models? In: Zagon I, Slotkin T, editors. Maternal Substance Abuse and the Developing Nervous System. New York: Academic Press (1992). p. 97–124.

Google Scholar

157. Ross EJ, Graham DL, Money KM, Stanwood GD. Developmental consequences of fetal exposure to drugs: what we know and what we still must learn. Neuropsychopharmacology (2015) 40:61–87. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Buckingham-Howes S, Berger SS, Scaletti LA, Black MM. Systematic review of prenatal cocaine exposure and adolescent development. Pediatrics (2013). peds. 2012-0945 131:e1917–36. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-0945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Chang L, Smith LM, LoPresti C, Yonekura ML, Kuo J, Walot I, et al. Smaller subcortical volumes and cognitive deficits in children with prenatal methamphetamine exposure. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging (2004) 132:95–106. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Derauf C, Kekatpure M, Neyzi N, Lester B, Kosofsky B. Neuroimaging of children following prenatal drug exposure. Seminars in cell & developmental biology (2009) 20:441–54. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2009.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Wakschlag LS, Pickett KE, Cook E, Benowitz NL, Leventhal BL. Maternal Smoking During Pregnancy and Severe Antisocial Behavior in Offspring: A Review. Am J Public Health (2002) 92:966–74. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.92.6.966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. D’Onofrio BM, Lahey BB, Turkheimer E, Lichtenstein P. Critical Need for Family-Based, Quasi-Experimental Designs in Integrating Genetic and Social Science Research. Am J Public Health (2013) 103:S46–55. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Jaffee SR, Price TS. Gene–environment correlations: a review of the evidence and implications for prevention of mental illness. Mol Psychiatry (2007) 12:432–42. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Danielle MD, Jennifer KJ, Richard JV, Richard JR. Testing Between-Family Associations in Within-Family Comparisons. psychol Sci (2000) 11:409–13. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Estabrook R, Massey SH, Clark CA, Burns JL, Mustanski BS, Cook EH, et al. Separating Family-Level and Direct Exposure Effects of Smoking During Pregnancy on Offspring Externalizing Symptoms: Bridging the Behavior Genetic and Behavior Teratologic Divide. Behav Genet (2016) 46:389–402. doi: 10.1007/s10519-015-9762-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. England LJ, Aagaard K, Bloch M, Conway K, Cosgrove K, Grana R, et al. Developmental toxicity of nicotine: A transdisciplinary synthesis and implications for emerging tobacco products. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2017) 72:176–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Health, U.D.o. & Services, H. The health consequences of smoking—50 years of progress: a report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health (2014). p. 17.

Google Scholar

168. D’onofrio BM, Singh AL, Iliadou A, Lambe M, Hultman CM, Grann M, et al. Familial confounding of the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring criminality: a population-based study in Sweden. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2010) 67:529–38. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Skoglund C, Chen Q, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H. Familial confounding of the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD in offspring. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2014) 55:61–8. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Maughan B, Taylor A, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Prenatal smoking and early childhood conduct problems: testing genetic and environmental explanations of the association. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2004) 61:836–43. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Robinson M, Oddy WH, Li J, Kendall GE, de Klerk NH, Silburn SR, et al. Pre- and postnatal influences on preschool mental health: a large-scale cohort study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2008) 49:1118–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01955.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

172. Hussong AM, Cai L, Curran PJ, Flora DB, Chassin LA, Zucker RA. Disaggregating the distal, proximal, and time-varying effects of parent alcoholism on children’s internalizing symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2008) 36:335–46. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9181-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Bullard RD, Wright BH. Environmental justice for all: community perspectives on health and research needs. Toxicol Ind Health (1993) 9:821–41. doi: 10.1177/074823379300900508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Schmidt RJ, Kogan V, Shelton JF, Delwiche L, Hansen RL, Ozonoff S, et al. Combined Prenatal Pesticide Exposure and Folic Acid Intake in Relation to Autism Spectrum Disorder. Environ Health Perspect (2017) 125:097007. doi: 10.1289/EHP604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. VanderWeele TJ. Outcome-wide epidemiology. Epidemiol (Cambridge Mass.) (2017) 28:399. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000641

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Solari CD, Mare RD. Housing crowding effects on children’s wellbeing. Soc Sci Res (2012) 41:464–76. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2011.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

177. De Marco A, Vernon-Feagans L. Rural Neighborhood Context, Child Care Quality, and Relationship to Early Language Development. Early Educ Dev (2013) 24:792–812. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2013.736036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. Dyment JE, Bell AC. Grounds for movement: green school grounds as sites for promoting physical activity. Health Educ Res (2008) 23:952–62. doi: 10.1093/her/cym059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Santesteban-Echarri O, Eisenberg RE, Bird HR, Canino GJ, Duarte CS. Family structure, transitions and psychiatric disorders among Puerto Rican children. J Child Family Stud (2016) 25:3417–29. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0498-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Riecher-Rössler A. Sex and gender differences in mental disorders. Lancet Psychiatry (2017) 4:8–9. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30348-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Rich-Edwards JW, Kaiser UB, Chen GL, Manson JE, Goldstein J. Sex and Gender Differences Research Design for Basic, Clinical and Population Studies: Essentials for Investigators. Endocrine Rev (2018) 39:424–39. doi: 10.1210/er.2017-00246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Bornstein MH. Sensitive periods in development: structural characteristics and causal interpretations. psychol Bull (1989) 105:179. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.105.2.179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

183. Hamilton HL. Sensitive periods during development. Ann New York Acad Sci (1952) 55:177–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1952.tb26533.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

184. Meredith R. Sensitive and critical periods during neurotypical and aberrant neurodevelopment: a framework for neurodevelopmental disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2015) 50:180–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. Bush NR, Boyce WT. The contributions of early experience to biological development and sensitivity to context. In: Handbook of developmental psychopathology Boston, MA (2014). p. 287–309.

Google Scholar

186. Byrne ML, Whittle S, Vijayakumar N, Dennison M, Simmons JG, Allen NB. A systematic review of adrenarche as a sensitive period in neurobiological development and mental health. Dev Cogn Neurosci (2017) 25:12–28. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Holder MK, Blaustein JD. Puberty and adolescence as a time of vulnerability to stressors that alter neurobehavioral processes. Front Neuroendocrinol (2014) 35:89–110. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

188. Heyer DB, Meredith RM. Environmental toxicology: Sensitive periods of development and neurodevelopmental disorders. NeuroToxicology (2017) 58:23–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2016.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

189. Braun JM. Early-life exposure to EDCs: role in childhood obesity and neurodevelopment. Nat Rev Endocrinol (2017) 13:161–73. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2016.186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

190. Wang C, Geng H, Liu W, Zhang G. Prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal factors associated with autism: a meta-analysis. Medicine (2017) 96:e6696. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000006696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

191. Lam J, Sutton P, Kalkbrenner A, Windham G, Halladay A, Koustas E, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of multiple airborne pollutants and autism spectrum disorder. PloS One (2016) 11:e0161851. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

192. O’Donnell KJ, Meaney MJ. Fetal origins of mental health: The developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry (2016) 174:319–28. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neurodevelopment, family, home, parenting, fetal programming

Citation: Bush NR, Wakschlag LS, LeWinn KZ, Hertz-Picciotto I, Nozadi SS, Pieper S, Lewis J, Biezonski D, Blair C, Deardorff J, Neiderhiser JM, Leve LD, Elliott AJ, Duarte CS, Lugo-Candelas C, O’Shea TM, Avalos LA, Page GP and Posner J (2020) Family Environment, Neurodevelopmental Risk, and the Environmental Influences on Child Health Outcomes (ECHO) Initiative: Looking Back and Moving Forward. Front. Psychiatry 11:547. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00547

Received: 11 October 2019; Accepted: 27 May 2020;
Published: 19 June 2020.

Edited by:

Kerstin Jessica von Plessen, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

James Blair, National Institute of Mental Health (NIH), United States
Anne Amalie Elgaard Thorup, Copenhagen University Hospital, Denmark

Copyright © 2020 Bush, Wakschlag, LeWinn, Hertz-Picciotto, Nozadi, Pieper, Lewis, Biezonski, Blair, Deardorff, Neiderhiser, Leve, Elliott, Duarte, Lugo-Candelas, O’Shea, Avalos, Page and Posner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jonathan Posner, Jonathan.posner@nyspi.columbia.edu

†See Acknowledgments for full listing of collaborators

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.