- 1Harbin Institute of Technology, Shenzhen, Shenzhen, China
- 2Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
- 3Faculty of Education, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
Outcome expectancies have been found to play important roles in addictive behaviors. Research has shown that implicit outcome expectancies (OE) were significantly correlated with Internet gaming behaviors among players with Internet gaming disorder (IGD). However, few empirical studies have further examined the relationship between implicit OE and players with IGD. This study first strengthened the implicit association between Internet games and negative outcomes using an evaluative conditioning paradigm (EC) and then examined the effects of increasing the negativity of implicit OE on Internet gaming impulsivity. Thirty-nine college students who were diagnosed as players with IGD participated in the study. Manipulation checks showed that after the EC was introduced, participants associated Internet gaming stimuli more closely with negative outcomes than with positive outcomes. After the implicit OE were effectively altered to be negative, players with IGD performed better in the delay discounting paradigm, showing a lower impulsivity with respect to Internet gaming.
Introduction
Internet gaming has become a dominant leisure activity of young people's life (1). In a recent report from the China Internet Network Information Center, more than half (58.4%) of the overall Internet users in China are active gamers (2). However, the excessive use of Internet games can lead to an Internet gaming disorder (IGD) (3) that can cause severe damage to young people's mental health and social functioning (4–7). Correspondingly, a diagnosis of IGD has been formally introduced into the 11th revision of International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) and the fifth edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) as a disorder (8, 9).
Like people who suffer from substance dependence or pathological gambling, the most important trait of gamers with IGD is believed to be impulsivity. Researchers believe that IGD is related to the disposition to engage in rash and spontaneous behavior without concern for the risks or the negative future consequences associated with such behavior (10–12). Gamers with IGD not only demonstrated higher self-reported impulsivity in questionnaires (13, 14) but were also less able to control their impulses in behavioral experiments. Using the delay discounting paradigm, adolescents with IGD were found to be more likely to overvalue immediate outcomes and neglect future outcomes, suggesting that impulsivity may be a behavioral marker of IGD (15).
Some researchers believe that the high impulsivity of young adults who engage in addictive behaviors, despite warnings concerning the dangers of such behaviors, might be related to the general tendency for people to have anticipated positive consequences for such actions that outweigh the possible negative outcomes of those actions (16). Thus, an individual's decision to initiate and maintain addictive behaviors likely reflects their differential salience with respect to what they expect to gain from those behaviors (i.e., positive outcome expectancies, positive OE) versus what they expect to lose as a result of those behaviors (i.e., negative outcome expectancies, negative OE) (17). For example, young adults' gambling behaviors could be predicted by their anticipation of positive gambling outcomes even though gambling behaviors have been found to be negatively correlated with negative OE (18). Researchers also found consistent positive relationships between positive OE and Internet usage (19, 20). However, the results regarding negative OE are far from consistent (13, 21, 22). Some researchers have found a negative correlation between negative OE and Internet usage (19), while in other studies, negative OE failed to demonstrate a significant negative effect on Internet use (21, 22).
These contradictory findings might be because most previous research used self-reported questionnaires, which assessed participants' explicit, conscious thoughts about the possible outcomes of Internet usage. However, unlike goal-directed actions, addictive behaviors may not depend on awareness but instead may be directly triggered by conditioned stimuli with no recruitment of higher cognitive processes being involved in the initiation of such behaviors (23). It has been shown that the initiation of drug seeking may depend solely upon implicit mechanisms, without an individual's explicit intention or choice processes being involved in initiating such behavior (23–25). Although research is still rare regarding the possible relationship between OE and behaviors of IGD, a recent study has found that gamers with IGD hold different explicit and implicit OE (26). Specifically, young gamers with IGD reported negative explicit OE, while they still associated Internet gaming more closely with positive outcomes in the Go/No-go association task (27), suggesting that such gamers with IGD have relatively positive implicit Internet gaming outcome expectancies (OEs) (26). In addition, only implicit Internet gaming OEs were found to be significantly correlated with the number of years that gamers with IGD persist in playing games (26). In this sense, the unconscious anticipation of the possible outcomes of playing Internet games may play an important role in predicting Internet gaming impulsivity. Thus, in order to clarify the effect of OE on IGD, the current study focuses on evaluating the impact of implicit OE on Internet gaming impulsivity among players of IGD.
Although, to our knowledge, no empirical study has examined the specific effects of altering implicit Internet gaming OEs on IGD, several research papers regarding substance abuse have already attempted to produce interventions with respect to alcohol consumption by directly manipulating implicit alcohol consumption OEs (28, 29). Some studies have demonstrated that changing implicit cognition can effectively reduce people's alcohol consumption (30). For example, when college students were semantically primed with positive outcomes such as “funny”, “relax”, or “talkative”, such positive priming resulted in significantly higher rates of beer consumption; whereas when students were semantically primed with negative outcomes such as “sick”, “dizzy”, or “problem”, such negative priming resulted in significantly lower rates of beer consumption (31). In contrast, when participants were asked to consciously change their OEs toward drinking (e.g., when participants were asked to reword their positive OE of “Alcohol makes me worry less” to “I seem to be worried less when I'm out with my friends, regardless of whether or not I have been drinking”), their rate of alcohol consumption failed to change (32). Previous research also used Evaluative Conditioning (EC) (33, 34) in changing implicit attitudes or beliefs. During the EC paradigm, participants were forced to associate target terms with positive or negative stimuli implicitly, and thereby get the positive or negative attribute. It has been documented that EC can have a significant effect in creating and changing implicit attitudes and beliefs (35, 36). Once an implicit attitude has been created or changed by EC, such changes can last at least 24 hours (37, 38). In fact, EC has already been used successfully to intervene in problem behaviors such as unhealthy eating habits (39). However, since the EC paradigm has not been adopted in studies looking to provide interventions for IGD it is not known whether implicit Internet gaming OE can also be changed through the use of EC. Nor do we know whether changes in implicit OE will have an influential effect on the Internet gaming impulsivity of problematic Internet gamers.
In summary, the current study attempted to address several limitations of past studies. First, in order to clarify the effect of Internet gaming OE on Internet gaming impulsivity, we distinguished implicit OE from explicit OE. The present study merely focuses on the unconscious beliefs about the possible outcomes of playing Internet games among players with IGD. The Go/No-go association task (27) was adopted in evaluating the implicit OE of players with IGD (26). Second, limited existing research regarding implicit Internet gaming OE only used correlation analysis in exploring the relationship between implicit Internet gaming OE and IGD. In the present study, we went further by examining the possible causal effects of changing implicit Internet gaming OE on Internet gaming impulsivity among players with IGD. Specifically, the current study first adopted the EC paradigm to enhance the association of Internet gaming with negative outcomes and to thereby increase the negativity of implicit Internet gaming OEs among students with IGD. After the manipulating effect was indicated, the study further discussed the effects of increasing the negativity of implicit Internet gaming OE on the Internet gaming impulsivity of gamers with IGD.
Methods
Participants
Ninety male college students from three universities in Beijing completed the Chinese Internet Addiction Scale (CIAS) (40). Twenty-five of them participated in the pilot study. Of the remaining 65 students, 39 met the following two criteria and were categorized as players with IGD: first, their scores on CIAS were higher than 63, an indicator considered most likely to diagnose one as an Internet addict (41); second, their online behaviors fulfilled all of the following three criteria: (1) their main reason for accessing the Internet was to play Internet games; (2) the games they played belong to the game types categorized by the National Internet Developmental Situation Report of China (42); and (3) they had spent approximately 20 or more hours playing Internet games each week over the past two months. Among the 39 participants, 29 (74.4%) were undergraduates and 10 (25.6%) were postgraduate students. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University where the study was conducted. Students volunteering for the study also signed consent forms before participating in the study.
Measures
Implicit Internet Gaming OEs
We used the Go/No-go Association Task (GNAT) (27) to assess the implicit Internet gaming OEs of study participants. Implicit Internet gaming OEs were represented by the association that people had between Internet gaming words and positive or negative outcomes during the GNAT (26, 27). In the GNAT (27), the strength of such associations was assessed by the degree to which items belonging to the target category and attribute (e.g., fruit and good) could be distinguished from distracter items that did not belong to those concepts (e.g., bad). Two conditions were included in the GNAT. One condition required the simultaneous identification of stimuli that represented the target category (e.g., fruit) and an attribute (e.g., good); a second condition required the simultaneous identification of stimuli that represented the same target category (e.g., fruit) and an alternative attribute (e.g., bad). In both conditions, participants' response latencies were recorded as the dependent variable. The extent to which fruit is associated with good versus bad was reflected in the response latencies involved in associating fruit with one evaluative attribute (e.g., good) as opposed to the other (e.g., bad).
In the present study, players with IGD completed a word-based GNAT task, which presented 50 stimuli words with three to four Chinese characters. These same stimuli were used and proven to be effective in Hou and Fang's study (26). The category words used are Internet gaming words, and the attribute words used are positive or negative outcome words. Specifically, there were 10 Internet gaming words that are neutral nouns (e.g., game currency “游戏币 you2 xi4 bi4”), 10 direct positive and negative outcome words that could be obtained directly from the game itself (e.g., level up “升级了 sheng1 ji2 le”; lose game “输比赛shu1 bi3 sai4”), and 10 indirect positive and negative outcome words that might reflect the real life situations of individuals after they had played Internet games (e.g., happy “高兴的 gao1 xing4 de”; poor academic results “学业差 xue2 ye4 cha4”) (See details in Appendix 1 in the Supplementary Material). The GNAT included four blocks, with 50 items in each block. Participants were asked to associate Internet gaming words with the appropriate outcome words in each block. The effects were calculated using the average response time (in milliseconds) of instances where participants correctly associated Internet gaming words with the appropriate outcome words within each block. Formal study started only after five practice trials to make sure that participants had fully understood the instructions for completing the task.
Internet Gaming Impulsivity
Internet gaming impulsivity was assessed using the delay discounting paradigm (15). The delay discounting paradigm is an amount-adjusting computerized choice procedure. Participants were asked to make a series of choices between an immediate smaller reward and a delayed larger reward. The data in delay discounting typically fits Mazur's (42) hyperbolic equation: V = A/(1 + kD). A larger k value represents greater discounting, meaning that a delayed reward loses subjective value more quickly over time. Thus, the k value is used as an index of impulsivity (Figure 1)
In the classical delay discounting paradigm, the reward used was money (43). Later, researchers studying addictive behaviors changed the rewards to stimuli that participants were addicted to (44). Accordingly, in the present study, hours of playing Internet games, instead of money, were used as rewards in the delay discounting paradigm. All the participants with IGD were asked to estimate the duration of time spent playing Internet games that would be equivalent to receiving a given amount of money before the formal experiment began (44). The magnitude of the reward given was the number of hours of playing Internet games that was deemed to be equivalent to $100 by each participant. The delays used in the study were 2 days, 1 month, and half a year.
Specifically, participants would read the following instructions in the first session:
“I want you to imagine that you have a choice between receiving some money and receiving a duration of time to play Internet games. For the following statement, please fill in the number of hours that would make the two choices equally attractive to you:
Receiving RMB 100 right now would be just as attractive as receiving ::_ hours to play Internet games.”
During the second session, participants were asked to make a series of choices between a standard larger, but later, (LL) reward option and an adjusted smaller, but sooner, (SS) reward option. The magnitude of the SS option was adjusted across trials until an indifference point was determined. The formal study started only after five practice trials had been conducted to make sure that participants had fully understood the instructions involved in completing the task. The equation of V = A/(1 + kD) was used to estimate the discounting rate “k” as the index of Internet gaming impulsivity.
The Manipulation of Implicit Internet Gaming OEs
An EC paradigm was used to strengthen the association between Internet gaming and negative outcomes and, thereby, to increase the negativity of implicit Internet gaming OE (35, 36). During the EC paradigm, participants were forced to associate Internet gaming words with negative outcome words implicitly. Specifically, both direct and indirect negative outcomes of playing Internet games were paired with Internet gaming words, and these word pairs were presented in each trial (see details in Figure 2). Each Internet gaming word was randomly presented 20 times. Given that there are 10 Internet gaming words and 20 negative-outcome words (10 direct and 10 indirect negative outcome words), there were 200 pairings involving Internet gaming words and negative-outcome words. To make participants concentrate on the experiment, both common-life words and positive-outcome words also appeared in pairs randomly (Figure 3). This entire intervention described above takes approximately 20 minutes to complete.
Procedures
The whole procedure included both pre-tests and post-tests of implicit Internet gaming OE and of impulsivity, and the process used to manipulate participants' implicit Internet gaming OE was based on EC. GNAT was used to investigate implicit Internet gaming OE. A delay discounting paradigm was used to evaluate Internet gaming impulsivity. After the pre-test, players with IGD took a 10-minute rest and then proceeded to the EC program. A post-test was implemented 24 hours later using the same methods as those used in the pre-test. The entire computer-based procedure was supported by an application written in the C++ programming language.
Statistical Analysis
We checked the changes in the implicit Internet gaming OE of players with players with IGD and the changes in Internet gaming impulsivity under the delay discounting paradigm using repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). All the k values in the delay discounting paradigm were calculated by using a natural log transformation, which normalized the distributions (45). Finally, paired t-test analysis was used to test the changes in Internet gaming impulsivity between the effective and non-effective manipulating groups of implicit Internet gaming OE. All analyses were performed with IBM SPSS Statistics v20 for Windows.
Results
Manipulation Check of the Changes in Implicit Internet Gaming OEs
We first checked the manipulating effect of the changes in implicit Internet gaming OE among students with IGD. The mean scores of the latent response time for associating Internet gaming words with positive and negative outcome words in the pre-test (T1) and the post-test (T2) are listed in Table 1.
Table 1 The mean scores of latent response times obtained when associating Internet gaming words with positive and negative outcome words (SD).
We estimated whether the implicit Internet gaming OE became more negative after performing the EC paradigm. Specifically, we compared the strength of association that players with IGD exhibited when associating Internet gaming words with positive and negative outcomes before and after performing the EC paradigm. From the descriptive analysis mentioned above (see Table 1), before EC (T1), the latent response time was shorter when players with IGD associated Internet games with direct positive outcomes than with direct negative outcomes (DP < DN). After EC (T2), the latent response time was shorter when players with IGD associated Internet games with direct negative outcomes than with direct positive outcomes (DP > DN). In addition, the latent response time obtained when players with IGD associated Internet gaming words with indirect positive and negative outcomes was similar in T1. However, in T2, the latent response time obtained when players with IGD associated Internet gaming words with indirect negative outcomes was faster than that with indirect positive outcomes (IP > IN).
We then conducted repeated measures ANOVA with testing time (T1 vs. T2), the attribute of OE (positive vs. negative), and direct or indirect outcomes as within-subject independent variables and defining the latent response time of associating Internet gaming words with outcome words to be the dependent variable. The results showed that the main effect of testing time was significant, F (1, 38) = 11.75, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.24; the interaction between testing time and the attribute of implicit Internet gaming OE was also significant, F (1,38) = 11.31, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.23. A planned paired t-test showed that in T1, DP associations was stronger than DN, t (38) = –2.79, p < 0.01, while in T2, DN associations was stronger than DP, t (38) = 2.11, p < 0.05 (see Table 1). Such findings suggest that in T1, players with IGD associated Internet gaming more closely with direct positive outcomes, whereas after participating in the EC paradigm, players with IGD associated Internet gaming more closely with direct negative outcomes. The attribute of implicit direct Internet gaming OE changed from positive to negative. On the other hand, there was no significant difference between IP and IN in T1, t (38) = –0.24, p = 0.81, but after conducting the EC paradigm, IN associations were significantly stronger than IP associations in T2, t (38) = 2.58, p < 0.01 (see Table 1). This result suggests that in T1, the strength with which players with IGD associated Internet gaming with indirect positive outcomes and with negative outcomes had no significant difference. However, after participating in the EC paradigm, players with IGD associated Internet gaming more closely with indirect negative outcomes than with indirect positive outcomes. Thus, after the EC manipulation, the attribute of indirect implicit Internet gaming OE changed from neutral to negative. Taking into account both direct and indirect implicit Internet gaming OE together, the EC paradigm effectively increased the negativity of implicit Internet gaming OE among students with IGD.
The Influential Effects of Increasing the Negativity of Implicit Internet Gaming OE on Internet Gaming Impulsivity
We then estimated the influential effect of changing implicit Internet gaming OE on Internet gaming impulsivity among players with IGD. Specifically, we asked whether participants demonstrated lower impulsivity under the delay discounting paradigm after participating in EC. We first compared players' Internet gaming impulsivity before and after participating in the EC paradigm. The results showed that at all delay levels, the k value decreased in T2 compared with that in T1, and the k value also decreased with the increase of the delay time (see Table 2). Repeated ANOVA showed that the main effect of testing time was significant, F (1, 38) = 7.02, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.16; the main effect of delay level was significant, F(2, 76) = 25.18, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.40; and the interaction between testing time and delay level was not significant, F(2, 76) = 0.43, p = 0.67, η2 = 0.01. Such findings suggest that after the negativity of implicit Internet gaming OE was increased by participating in the EC paradigm, Internet gaming impulsivity was also reduced significantly in T2 as compared to T1. In addition, Internet gaming impulsivity decreased significantly with the increase of delay levels (see Table 2).
Furthermore, we asked whether such changes in Internet gaming impulsivity the result of the increased negativity of implicit Internet gaming OE were. We first categorized the participants into two groups depending on whether their attribute of implicit Internet gaming OE was effectively changed to negative by the EC paradigm. Specifically, participants whose attribute of implicit Internet gaming OE turned from positive to negative after participating in the EC paradigm were placed in the effective manipulating group. Participants whose attribute of implicit Internet gaming OE did not change or changed from negative (T1) to positive (T2) after participating in the EC paradigm, were placed in the non-effective manipulating group. When considering direct and indirect implicit Internet gaming OE separately, there were 16 participants in the effective manipulating group of direct OE and 23 participants in the non-effective manipulating group of direct OE. With respect to indirect implicit Internet gaming OE, there were 15 participants in the effective manipulating group of indirect OE and 24 participants in the non-effective manipulating group of indirect OE. The mean scores for the k values of different delay levels in both groups are shown in Table 3.
Table 3 The mean scores for k values of participants in the effective manipulating groups and the non-effective manipulating groups for both direct and indirect implicit Internet gaming OE.
Paired t-tests (see Table 3) showed that when the delay level was 2 days, in the effective manipulating group of both direct and indirect implicit OE, the k value significantly decreased in T2 compared with that in T1. However, in the non-effective manipulating group of both direct and indirect implicit OE, the k value showed no significant difference between T2 and T1. Similarly, when the delay level was one month, in the effective manipulating group of both direct and indirect implicit OE, the k value significantly decreased in T2 compared with that in T1. However, in the non-effective manipulating group of both direct and indirect implicit OE, the k value showed no significant difference between T2 and T1. However, when the delay level was half a year, none of effective manipulating group or non-effective manipulating group groups showed significant changes in k value between T1 and T2.
In summary, after participating in the EC paradigm, players with IGD associate Internet gaming more closely with negative outcomes than with positive ones, which suggests a significant increase in the negativity of implicit Internet gaming OE. Accordingly, Internet gaming impulsivity also decreased after players with IGD participated in the EC paradigm. Finally, this decrease in Internet gaming impulsivity was probably due to the increasingly negative implicit Internet gaming OE.
Discussion
The present study used the EC paradigm to increase the negativity of implicit Internet gaming OE among students with IGD examined the influential effects of such an increase on Internet gaming impulsivity among those same students. The results first supported that after participating in the EC paradigm, students with IGD associate Internet gaming words more closely with negative outcomes than with positive ones, suggesting an increase in the negativity of implicit Internet gaming OE. Moreover, Internet gaming impulsivity among participants decreased after the negativity of participants' implicit OE became stronger. Our study is one of the initial efforts aimed at demonstrating the effect of changing implicit Internet gaming OE on Internet gaming impulsivity among players with IGD based on experimental tasks. The findings of the current study also demonstrated the possibility for inducing a short-term change in Internet gaming impulsivity within a laboratory setting.
The Effectiveness of Changing Implicit Internet Gaming OEs Using the EC Paradigm
Although research has shown that EC was effective in changing people's attitudes and beliefs (34, 46), little is known about whether EC can be successfully applied in changing attitudes of players with IGD about the outcomes of playing Internet games. Our study was among the initial efforts to demonstrate that EC can change implicit OEs among young people with IGD. Specifically, after associating Internet gaming stimuli with negative outcomes using the EC paradigm, implicit Internet gaming OE among players with IGD became negative. This finding was consistent with previous studies regarding evaluative conditioning, which found that when target stimuli are associated with negative/positive stimuli, the target stimuli's valence will often change in accordance with the negative/positive stimuli it has been paired with (36, 47–49). Accordingly, EC has proven effective in changing people's implicit attitude toward target objects such as fruit, black people, or food (27, 39). Our study expanded on previous research by successfully applying the EC paradigm to changing the valence of implicit attitudes toward a kind of addictive behavior (Internet gaming).
The Influential Effect of Increasing the Negativity of Implicit OEs on Internet Gaming Impulsivity
While exploring the mechanisms of addiction, research regarding substance abuse has suggested that drug seeking and drug taking habits were reinforced by Pavlovian conditioning. Drug-associated stimuli in the environment act as conditioned reinforcers that facilitate the development of incentive habits (50, 51). Such incentive habit processes may facilitate the transition to compulsivity, the hallmark of addiction (52). Based on this finding, we asked, “If Internet gaming stimuli were associated with negative outcomes, would the impulsivity associated with IGD thereby be inhibited?” Such a hypothesis was supported by the results of the current study. Like substance abuse, the impulsivity of players with IGD may also be reinforced by Pavlovian conditioning. Hence, after participating in EC, where IGD players associated Internet gaming stimuli more closely with negative outcomes, their Internet gaming impulsivity significantly decreased. In addition, participants whose implicit OE toward Internet gaming were significantly changed to be negative showed a significant decrease in their Internet gaming impulsivity, whereas people whose implicit OE of Internet gaming were not effectively changed to be negative showed no change in Internet gaming impulsivity. Such findings provide behavioral evidence about the causal relationship between implicit OEs and Internet gaming impulsivity. Our study on implicit Internet gaming OE also contributed to the understanding of why young people with IGD are unable to resist playing Internet games even when they clearly know (explicitly) the negative outcomes of such excessive Internet gaming.
The present study also has practical implications. Previous researchers have applied implicit methods when intervening in problem behaviors such as smoking or alcohol consumption (53–57). The EC paradigm has proven to be effective in providing interventions for phobias, sexual, and food-related problem behaviors (39, 58). However, to our knowledge, our study was among the first to show how one might change the implicit OEs of students' with IGD using the EC paradigm. In addition, we also proved that Internet gaming impulsivity can be inhibited within laboratory settings for at least 24 hours by increasing the negativity of people's implicit Internet gaming OE. It would be worthwhile for future studies to incorporate implicit methods into intervention programs that seek to control the Internet gaming impulsivity of players with IGD.
Limitations and Future Directions
The present study has several limitations. First, the players with IGD in our study were all college students. Admittedly, our findings may not be generalizable to other groups of people. In future studies, the mechanism underlying these findings may need further discussion with respect to players with IGD of different ages or with different educational backgrounds. Second, the post-test in our study was taken 24 hours after the conducting the EC paradigm. Thus, our study only showed that participating in the EC paradigm may change participants' implicit OEs within a short period of time. The long-lasting effects of EC on the changing implicit OE of participants needs to be verified in future studies by conducting a longitudinal study. Third, the current research used a task-based paradigm to change implicit OEs and thus inhibit Internet gaming impulsivity among players with IGD only in laboratory settings; however, whether the paradigm has long lasting interventional effects on the impulsivity of players with IGD is not known. Future studies may establish a more complete intervention program to be used outside laboratory settings, and such studies may also further examine the possibility of intervening in players with IGD by changing implicit OE.
Conclusion
This is the first study used behavioral experiments to explore the relationship between implicit Internet gaming OEs and Internet gaming impulsivity. The present study found that when the negativity of implicit Internet gaming OE was increased, the Internet gaming impulsivity of gamers with IGD could be significantly decreased. This study indicated that a possible way to reduce the Internet gaming impulsivity of gamers with IGD is to increase the negativity of their implicit OEs with respect to Internet gaming.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Beijing Normal University, China. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
SH conceived of the presented idea, planned the experiments, and took the lead in writing the manuscript. XF provided funding acquisition, project administration, and resources. NZ performed the analytic calculations and contributed to the interpretation of the results. PC contributed to sample preparation and data collection.
Funding
This study was supported by The Chinese Natural Science Foundation (Project No. 31170990) and Yangzi-River Professor Supportive Program of the corresponding author XF, and the Key Project of Shenzhen Education Science Plan in 2019 (zdzz19002) of the first author SH.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00336/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD, Pontes HM. Chaos and confusion in DSM-5 diagnosis of Internet Gaming Disorder: Issues, concerns, and recommendations for clarity in the field. J Behav Addict (2017) 6(2):103–9. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.062
2. China Internet Network Information Center. (2019). The 43th survey report: Statistical report on Internet development in China. Retrieved from http://www.cnnic.net.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/hlwtjbg/201902/P020190318523029756345.pdf.
3. Rehbein F, Kliem S, Baier D, Mößle T, Petry NM. Prevalence of Internet gaming disorder in German adolescents: Diagnostic contribution of the nine DSM-5 criteria in a state-wide representative sample. Addiction (2015) 110(5):842–51. doi: 10.1111/add.12849
4. Gentile DA, Choo H, Liau A, Sim T, Li D, Fung D. Pathological video game use among youths: a two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics (2011)127(2):319–29. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1353
5. Griffiths MD, Van Rooij AJ, Kardefelt-Winther D, Starcevic V, Király O, Pallesen S, et al. Working towards an international consensus on criteria for assessing Internet gaming disorder: A critical commentary on Petry et al.(2014). Addiction (2016) 111(1):167–75. doi: 10.1111/add.13057
6. Kwon JH, Chung CS, Lee J. The effects of escape from self and interpersonal relationship on the pathological use of Internet games. Community Ment Health J (2011) 47(1):113–21. doi: 10.1007/s10597-009-9236-1
7. Weinstein A, Livny A, Weizman A. New developments in brain research of internet and gaming disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2017), 75314–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.040
8. World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases, 11th revision (ICD-11); 2018. Geneva: World Health Organization (2018).
9. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).
10. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Drug addiction: updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annu Rev Psychol (2016), 6723–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457
11. Sim T, Gentile DA, Bricolo F, Serpelloni G, Gulamoydeen F. A conceptual review of research on the pathological use of computers, video games, and the Internet. Int J Ment Health Addict (2012) 10(5):748–69. doi: 10.1007/s11469-011-9369-7
12. Wu AM, Chen JH, Tong K-K, Yu S, Lau JT. Prevalence and associated factors of Internet gaming disorder among community dwelling adults in Macao, China. J Behav Addict (2018) 7(1):62–9. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.12
13. Meerkerk G-J, van den Eijnden RJ, Franken I, Garretsen H. Is compulsive internet use related to sensitivity to reward and punishment, and impulsivity? Comput Hum Behav (2010) 26(4):729–35. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.01.009
14. Zhou ZH, Yuan GZ, Yao JJ, Li C, Cheng Z-H. An event-related potential investigation of deficient inhibitory control in individuals with pathological Internet use. Acta Neuropsychiat (2010) 22(5):228–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2010.00444.x
15. Tian M, Tao R, Zheng Y, Zhang H, Yang G, Li Q. Internet gaming disorder in adolescents is linked to delay discounting but not probability discounting. Comput Hum Behav 201880:59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.018
16. Fromme K, Katz EC, Rivet K. Outcome expectancies and risk-taking behavior. Cogn Ther Res (1997) 21(4):421–42. doi: 10.1023/A:1021932326716
17. Gillespie MA, Derevensky J, Gupta R. II. The utility of outcome expectancies in the prediction of adolescent gambling behaviour. J Gambling Issues (2007) 19:69–85. doi: 10.4309/jgi.2007.19.4
18. Wickwire EM, Whelan JP, Meyers AW. Outcome expectancies and gambling behavior among urban adolescents. Psychol Addictive Behav (2010) 24(1):75–88. doi: 10.1037/a0017505
19. LaRose R, Mastro D, Eastin MS. Understanding Internet usage: A social-cognitive approach to uses and gratifications. Soc Sci Comput Rev (2001) 19(4):395–413. doi: 10.1177/089443930101900401
20. Wu JYW, Ko HC, Wong TY, Wu LA, Oei TP. Positive outcome expectancy mediates the relationship between peer influence and Internet gaming addiction among adolescents in Taiwan. Cyberpsychol Behavior Soc Networking (2016) 19(1):49–55. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0345
21. Eastin MS. Teen Internet use: Relating social perceptions and cognitive models to behavior. CyberPsychol Behav (2005) 8(1):62–75. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2005.8.62
22. Lin MP, Ko HC, Wu JYW. The role of positive/negative outcome expectancy and refusal self-efficacy of Internet use on Internet addiction among college students in Taiwan. CyberPsychol Behav (2008) 11(4):451–7. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.0121
23. Belin D, Belin-Rauscent A, Murray JE, Everitt BJ. Addiction: failure of control over maladaptive incentive habits. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2013) 23(4):564–72. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.025
24. Belin D, Belin-Rauscent A, Everitt BJ, Dalley JW. In search of predictive endophenotypes in addiction: insights from preclinical research. Genes Brain Behav (2016) 15(1):74–88. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12265
25. Tiffany ST. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev (1990) 97(2):147–68. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.2.147
26. Hou S, Fang X. The explicit and implicit outcome expectancies of Internet games and their relationships with Internet gaming behaviors among college students. Comput Hum Behavior (2014), 39346–55. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.07.028
27. Nosek BA, Banaji MR. The go/no-go association task. Soc Cogn (2001) 19(6):625–66. doi: 10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886
28. Pieters S, van der Vorst H, Engels RCME, Wiers RW. Implicit and explicit cognitions related to alcohol use in children. Addictive Behav (2010) 35:471–8. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.12.022
29. Reich RR, Below MC, Goldman MS. Explicit and implicit measures of expectancy and related alcohol cognitions: A meta-analytic comparison. Psychol Addictive Behav (2010) 24:13–25. doi: 10.1037/a0016556
30. Carpenter KM, Martinez D, Vadhan NP, Barnes-Holmes D, Nunes EV. Measures of attentional bias and relational responding are associated with behavioral treatment outcome for cocaine dependence. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse (2012), 38146–54. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2011.643986
31. Carter JA, McNair LD, Corbin WR, Black DH. Effects of priming positive and negative outcomes on drinking responses. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol (1998) 6(4):399–405. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.6.4.399
32. Corbin WR, McNair LD, Carter JA. Evaluation of a treatment-appropriate cognitive intervention for challenging alcohol outcome expectancies. Addictive Behav (2001) 26(4):475–88. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4603(00)00138-6
33. De Houwer J, Baeyens F, Field AP. Associative learning of likes and dislikes: Some current controversies and possible ways forward. Cogn Emotion (2005), 19161–74. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000265
34. De Houwer J, Thomas S, Baeyens F. Association learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychol Bull (2001) 127(6):853–69. doi: 10.1037//D033-29O9.127.6.853
35. Dijksterhuis A. I like myself but I don't know why: enhancing implicit self-esteem by subliminal evaluative conditioning. J Pers Soc Psychol (2004) 86(2):345–55. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.345
36. Gibson B. Can evaluative conditioning change attitudes toward mature brands? New evidence from the Implicit Association Test. J Consumer Res (2008) 35(1):178–88. doi: 10.1086/527341
37. Dasgupta N, Greenwald AG. On the malleability of automatic attitudes: combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. J Pers Soc Psychol (2001) 81(5):800–14. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.800
38. Hammerl M, Grabitz H-J. Human evaluative conditioning without experiencing a valued event. Learn Motivation (1996) 27(3):278–93. doi: 10.1006/lmot.1996.0015
39. Hollands GJ, Prestwich A, Marteau TM. Using aversive images to enhance healthy food choices and implicit attitudes: An experimental test of evaluative conditioning. Health Psychol (2011) 30(2):195–203. doi: 10.1037/a0022261
40. Chen S-H, Weng LJ, Su YJ, Wu H, Yang P. Development of a Chinese Internet addiction scale and its psychometric study. Chin J Psychol (2003) 45(3):279–94. doi: 10.1037/t44491-000
41. Ko C-H, Yen JY, Chen SH, Yang MJ, Lin HC, Yen CF. Proposed diagnostic criteria and the screening and diagnosing tool of Internet addiction in college students. Compr Psychiatry (2009) 50(4):378–84. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.05.019
42. China Internetwork Information Center (CNNIC). (2005). Retrieved from http://www.cnnic.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/200906/P020120709345359935070.pdf/.
43. Mazur JE. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. Commons, ML.; Mazur, JE.; Nevin, JA (1987) p. 55–73. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
44. Rachlin H, Raineri A, Cross D. SUBJECTIVE PROBABILITY AND DELAY. J Exp Anal Behav (1991) 55(2):233–44. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(09)80011-9
45. Baker F, Johnson MW, Bickel WK. Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. J Abnormal Psychol (2003) 112(3):382–92. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.382
46. Hofmann W, De Houwer J, Perugini M, Baeyens F, Crombez G. Evaluative conditioning in humans: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull (2010) 136(3):390–421. doi: 10.1037/a0018916
47. De Houwer J, Baeyens F, Vansteenwegen D, Eelen P. Evaluative conditioning in the picture–picture paradigm with random assignment of conditioned stimuli to unconditioned stimuli. J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Processes (2000) 26(2):237–42. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.26.2.237
48. Olson MA, Fazio RH. Reducing automatically activated racial prejudice through implicit evaluative conditioning. Pers Soc Psychol Bull (2006) 32(4):421–33. doi: 10.1177/0146167205284004
49. Sweldens S, Corneille O, Yzerbyt V. The role of awareness in attitude formation through evaluative conditioning. Pers Soc Psychol Rev (2014) 18(2):187–209. doi: 10.1177/1088868314527832
50. Arroyo M, Markou A, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Acquisition, maintenance and reinstatement of intravenous cocaine self-administration under a second-order schedule of reinforcement in rats: effects of conditioned cues and continuous access to cocaine. Psychopharmacology (1998) 140(3):331–44. doi: 10.1007/s002130050774
51. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Second-order schedules of drug reinforcement in rats and monkeys: measurement of reinforcing efficacy and drug-seeking behaviour. Psychopharmacology (2000) 153(1):17–30. doi: 10.1007/s002130000566
52. Belin D, Belin-Rauscent A, Murray JE, Everitt BJ. Addiction: failure of control over maladaptive incentive habits. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2013) 23(4):564–72. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.025
53. Attwood AS, O'sullivan H, Leonards U, Mackintosh B, Munafò MR. Attentional bias training and cue reactivity in cigarette smokers. Addiction (2008) 103(11):1875–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02335.x
54. Field M, Duka T, Eastwood B, Child R, Santarcangelo M, Gayton M. Experimental manipulation of attentional biases in heavy drinkers: do the effects generalise? Psychopharmacology (2007) 192(4):593–608. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0760-9
55. Field M, Duka T, Tyler E, Schoenmakers T. Attentional bias modification in tobacco smokers. Nicotine Tobacco Res (2009) 11(7):812–22. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp067
56. Schoenmakers T, Wiers RW, Jones BT, Bruce G, Jansen AT. Attentional re-training decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. Addiction (2007) 102(3):399–405. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01718.x
57. Schoenmakers TM, de Bruin M, Lux IF, Goertz AG, Van Kerkhof DH, Wiers RW. Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modification training in abstinent alcoholic patients. Drug Alcohol Depend (2010) 109(1-3):30–6. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.11.022
Keywords: problem Internet gaming, evaluative conditioning, delay discounting, college students, Go/No-Go Association task
Citation: Hou S, Fang X, Zhou N and Cai P (2020) Effects of Increasing the Negativity of Implicit Outcome Expectancies on Internet Gaming Impulsivity. Front. Psychiatry 11:336. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00336
Received: 03 February 2020; Accepted: 03 April 2020;
Published: 29 April 2020.
Edited by:
Xiaochu Zhang, University of Science and Technology of China, ChinaReviewed by:
Guangheng Dong, Zhejiang Normal University, ChinaZongkui Zhou, Central China Normal University, China
Qi Chen, South China Normal University, China
Copyright © 2020 Hou, Fang, Zhou and Cai. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Xiaoyi Fang, ZmFuZ3h5QGJudS5lZHUuY24=