- 1Institute for Cognitive Science Studies, Tehran, Iran
- 2Iranian National Center for Addiction Studies, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
- 3Institute for Cognitive and Brain Sciences (ICBS), Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran
- 4Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
- 5Centre of Precision Rehabilitation for Spinal Pain (CPR Spine), School of Sport, Exercise and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, United States
Background: There is a trade-off between drug-related impulsive process and cognitive reflective process among ex-drug abusers. The present study aimed to investigate the impulsive effects of methamphetamine-related stimuli on working memory (WM) performance by manipulating WM load in abstinent ex-methamphetamine users.
Methods: Thirty abstinent ex-methamphetamine users and 30 nonaddict matched control participants were recruited in this study. We used a modified Sternberg task in which participants were instructed to memorize three different sets of methamphetamine-related and non–drug-related words (three, five, or seven words) while performing a secondary attention-demanding task as an interference.
Results: Repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that reaction times of abstinent ex-methamphetamine users increased during low WM load (three words) compared to the control group (p = 0.01). No significant differences were observed during high WM loads (five or seven words) (both p’s > 0.1). Besides, reaction times of the experimental group during trials with high interference (three, five, or seven words) were not significantly different compared to the control group (p > 0.2).
Conclusion: These findings imply that increasing WM load may provide an efficient buffer against attentional capture by salient stimuli (i.e., methamphetamine-related words). This buffer might modify the effect of interference bias. Besides, presenting methamphetamine-related stimuli might facilitate the encoding phase due to bias toward task-relevant stimuli. This finding has an important implication, suggesting that performing concurrent demanding tasks may reduce the power of salient stimuli and thus improve the efficiency of emotional regulation strategies.
Introduction
Methamphetamine, which is an extremely addictive neurotoxic drug, is the second most used illegal drug after cannabis (1). Prevalence of methamphetamine abuse is 1.2 million people in the United States and 17.2 million people around the world (2). Chronic use of methamphetamine has been associated with multiple physical health problems (e.g., cardiovascular disease), mental health problems (e.g., depression) (3–5), and daily functioning problems (e.g., impulsivity) (6, 7), which can also affect the brain and neurocognitive functions (8–10).
Addiction to methamphetamine—similar to addiction to other substances—is often resistant to conventional interventions (11). Therefore, a critical need exists to address additional and appropriate interventions such as nonpharmacological approaches. In line with this, theoretical models and empirical evidence support a role for the modulation of addiction with cognitive-based approaches (11–17). For example, dual-process models of addiction suggest that addictive behaviors are affected by the dominance of drug-related impulsive processes over the reflective processes (13, 18, 19). Several studies have shown that the drug-related impulsive process is spontaneous, fast, and relatively unconscious, while the reflective process is deliberate, slow, and conscious (13, 18, 19).
There is a trade-off between the drug-related impulsive process and reflective process (11, 13, 15). The drug-related impulsive process is affected by the repeated abuse of drugs (20). Impulsive behaviors in addiction are referred to as behaviors that are associated with selecting an immediate reward, making risky decisions (21), generating memory impairment (22), and showing bias toward salient drug stimuli (12, 23, 24). For example, methamphetamine-related stimuli can involuntarily catch the attention of methamphetamine users (i.e., attentional bias). Attentional bias toward methamphetamine-related stimuli can increase the effect of subjective craving, which may contribute to relapse (25, 26). However, the drug-related impulsive process can be modulated by the reflective system (14, 26). Working memory (WM), which is considered as the main part of the reflective process, can modulate the drug-related impulsive process (14, 26, 27). WM is a temporary storage system that can actively maintain information and manipulate stored information (28). WM is involved in the modulation of the processing of irrelevant information by attentional mechanisms (i.e., the reflective process) (29). However, WM processes can negatively be influenced by emotionally salient stimuli like those related to drugs (18). As a result, the bias toward emotionally salient stimuli can lead to deficits in WM performance (18). Therefore, it is important to understand how WM can modulate the attention given to methamphetamine-related stimuli and vice versa.
Given that WM performance might be impaired in methamphetamine users (10, 30) and in abstinent methamphetamine users, it is plausible that the ability to apply attentional control over methamphetamine-related stimuli is reduced as a result of impaired WM performance. For example, a systematic review on methamphetamine use and cognitive function reported that cognitive domains (e.g., WM performance, attention, cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, decision making) in methamphetamine users were decreased compared to the control group (31). This reduced cognitive performance was associated with deficits in the brain measures, including lower metabolism, gray matter density, fractional anisotropy, and activation (31). For example, the study of abstinent methamphetamine users showed that WM performance (during a one-back cued response, one- back, two-back, and one-increment tasks) is decreased in abstinent methamphetamine users compared to control group (32). In this study, abstinent methamphetamine users showed increased brain activity in left occipital and right posterior parietal lobe compared to control group, while they showed decreased activity in bilateral putamen/insular cortex and right lateral compared to control group (32). Another study showed a correlation between performance on the delayed recall and increased metabolism in the thalamus in abstinent methamphetamine users compared to the control group (33). Another study also reported a correlation between performance on the word-recall task and hippocampal volume, which was smaller in the abstinent methamphetamine users than in the control group (34). These studies have indicated a decreased cognitive function in methamphetamine users in several domains, including WM performance (31).
Effective cognitive control over addiction encompasses more than simply disengaging attention from methamphetamine stimuli; it is also necessary to maintain attention toward nonmethamphetamine information (14, 35). WM allows us to maintain and prioritize relevant information in the face of irrelevant information (28). Evidence supported the role of WM, and the corresponding processes, in the control of attention (29). To understand effective cognitive control over addiction to methamphetamine, we first need to know the trade-off between the top-down effect of WM in attentional control (reflective process) over methamphetamine-related stimuli and the bottom-up effect of attentional bias in WM (impulsive process) (18).
Studies revealed that automatic attentional mechanisms (i.e., impulsive processes) are not independent of the available processing resources (29, 36). However, investigating the effect of WM capacity (i.e. the ability to actively store information despite ongoing processing, which is an indicator of limited cognitive resources) on the interaction between the reflective process and drug-related impulsive processes is a missing piece in the literature (37–43). Many studies have examined the effect of attentional bias in drug-dependent populations versus control groups (23–24, 25, 44–47). However, according to our knowledge, no study to date has investigated the interactive effect of both bias and WM capacity in abstinent ex-methamphetamine users versus a control group. Therefore, the current study investigated the effect of bias and load on WM maintenance in different ways: first, by showing drug-related words, which are task-relevant stimuli that can facilitate the encoding process; next, by applying an interference task, which can disturb the process of rehearsal and needs to be inhibited; and finally, by increasing WM loads, which can result in greater rehearsal demands. Investigating the effect of these WM manipulations independently in combination with WM load can help determine factors that might contribute to dual-process models of methamphetamine addiction and may lead to the development of effective assessment tools and interventions.
Methods
Ethics Approval
All experimental procedures corresponded to the standards set by the latest revision of the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the ethical committee of the Institute for Cognitive Sciences Studies, Tehran, Iran. All participants provided written informed consent, acknowledging their right to withdraw from the experiment without prejudice.
Showing methamphetamine cues to participants may increase the possibility of relapse. Concerning this important ethical issue, we used methamphetamine-related words instead of real substances. In addition, participants were monitored in the following weeks for any signs of drug craving, and they also had access to psychological interventions to manage potential drug cravings.
Participants
Thirty abstinent ex-methamphetamine users (all men, 20–47 years old, experimental group) and 30 participants without a history of addiction or drug abuse (all men, 20–50 years old, control group) were recruited in the current study (Table 1). The experimental group was recruited from former methamphetamine-dependent users who were admitted to Vardij Abstinence-Based Residential Centre, Karaj, Iran. This treatment center specializes in amphetamine-type stimulant dependence and is located in a rural area near Tehran—a part of the therapeutic network belonging to Rebirth Society Organization (a nonprofit charity). The abstinent ex-methamphetamine users in this center were relatively homogeneous, and only men were admitted. Participants in the control group (all males) were recruited from employees of Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran. They reported no history of drug abuse. Both groups were right-handed and were matched for age (20–50 years) and educational level (<12 years of school. Inclusion criteria for the experimental group included having a history of methamphetamine abuse in the past 12 months prior to entering the treatment center (methamphetamine dependence based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition criteria). The most common mean of drug administration was smoking. Subjects had to be abstinent from any drugs except cigarettes for at least a week before the experiment, with confirmation by urine testing.
Exclusion criteria for both the experimental group and the control group included any current or past major clinical neurological disorders, central nervous system–effective medication intake, or any major clinical psychiatric disorders (in Axis I, except substance-related disorders). We excluded data of two participants from the experimental group and data of two participants from the control group because of their inaccurate responses to the cognitive task.
The Modified Sternberg Task With Interference
To test WM performance of participants, we adopted a modified Sternberg task with interference. The task was designed with MATLAB (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States) and used the Psychtoolbox ran on a Microsoft Windows 7 operating system. The modified Sternberg task fits in the category of a complex span task. It consisted of three steps: memorizing a list of words (encoding step), performing a secondary task (as an interference step), and selecting the memorized word among presented words (retrieval step). In order to obtain different levels of WM load, the Sternberg task included a list of either three, five, or seven words (Figure 1) (19).
Figure 1 Modified Sternberg task with interference.This Figure is an example of one trial with different responses at different steps of the modified Sternberg task with interference. The modified Sternberg task with interference consisted of three steps. The first step shows a working memory set with three Persian words, out of which one is a methamphetamine-related word (e.g., methamphetamine) and two are non–drug-related words (e.g., cart and scissors). This step is considered as an encoding step. The second step illustrates the secondary task in which the color of the words can change to green or blue (interference step). Subjects have to respond to the color of the word by pressing the corresponding button on the response box (e.g., left for green, right for blue). During the last step, called the pairing step, the participants have to choose the correct word from the working memory set by using the response box (according to their position on the screen; recall step). Numbers represent the display time of the words on the screen (Figure is modified from (48).
We selected a list of words, that were validated in a previous study based on their mean of craving and emotional valence (49). This list consisted of 24 Persian words: 12 were selected randomly from a list of methamphetamine-related words (i.e., experimental; ex: methamphetamine, drugs), and 12 were selected randomly from a list of non–drug-related words (i.e., neutral; ex: scissors, carriage). All words had two syllables with a maximum of four letters. They were presented with the same font in white color on a black background screen.
Proceeding of the Modified Sternberg Task With Interference
The first step (i.e., WM set) consisted of the presentation of a list of three, five, or seven words (encoding step). Participants had to memorize the presented word list. Words were presented randomly according to methamphetamine-related or non–drug-related content. For example, in the 3-word memory set, there were either two methamphetamine-related words and one non–drug-related word or vice versa. In the 5-word WM set, there were either three methamphetamine-related words and two non–drug-related words or three non–drug-related stimuli and two methamphetamine-related words. Each word was presented for 750 ms with an interstimulus interval of 500 ms. Two fixation crosses (++) were presented in the center of the screen to signal the end of this step (Figure 1, left panel).
The second step consisted of a secondary task (as an interference step). In this step, four words were presented one after the other. Two of these four words were methamphetamine-related words, and two were non–drug-related. These words were new and different from the words used in the memory set step. Each word was presented for 500 ms, after which time the font color was changed randomly to either blue or green (750 ms). Participants were asked to indicate which font color was used for the words by pressing the corresponding button on the response box (left button for green, right for blue). After the participant’s response, a black slide was presented for 250 ms, and the next word appeared. One-third of the trials were null; instead of word stimuli, an empty black screen was presented. The total duration of the secondary task was 6 seconds (Figure 1, middle panel).
In the third step (i.e., pairing step), each word from the first step was presented once along with a word from the secondary task (ST trial) or a novel list (NL) of methamphetamine-related and non–drug-related words, which had not been presented in that trial, for 3,000 ms. The participant’s task was to choose the word that was presented in the memory set by pressing the corresponding key on the response box (i.e., right button for the word on the right side of the screen, and left button for the word on the left) as fast and as accurate as possible. After the presentation of each pair and response by the participant, the screen was replaced by a black slide for 500 ms, and the next pair appeared on the screen. After all words from the memory set were presented, a single fixation cross was presented in the center of the screen, and the next trial was started. The intertrial time interval was set to be between 800 and 1,200 ms (Figure 1, right panel). This pairing step is referred to as retrieval step. A black screen was presented for 500 ms after each probe. At the end of the three probes, a black screen was presented randomly for 800 or 1,200 ms (Figure 1, right panel).
Second words (i.e., incorrect words) during the pairing step were selected randomly considering the following restrictions: at least one methamphetamine-related word and one non–drug-related word were required to be among the words. Second words in each probe had a 50% chance of being randomly selected from the ST step of its respective trial (i.e., high interference trials). The remaining second words were again randomly selected among methamphetamine-related and non–drug-related words (NL) not previously presented in its respective trial (i.e., low interference trials).
Regarding the mentioned rules for the presentation of both words, the display in the pairing step included the situations as below:
A. methamphetamine-related words (WM set) + methamphetamine-related words (ST);
B. methamphetamine-related words (WM set) + methamphetamine-related words (NL);
C. methamphetamine-related words (WM set) + non–drug-related words (ST);
D. methamphetamine-related words (WM set) + non–drug-related words (NL);
E. non–drug-related words (WM set) + methamphetamine-related words (ST);
F. non–drug-related words (WM set) + methamphetamine-related words (NL);
G. non–drug-related words (WM set) + non–drug-related words (ST);
H. non–drug-related words (WM set) + non–drug-related words (NL).
Experimental Procedure
All subjects participated in one session. The experimental procedure was explained clearly to them at the beginning of the session. Basic demographic information, drug abuse, treatment history, and high-risk behaviors of each subject were recorded during a structured interview by an expert drug counselor. After signing the consent form, participants sat in front of a 13-inch laptop screen at a 60-cm viewing distance in a room with dimmed light to increase their focus on the screen.
The experimental procedure had two different phases: a training phase and a test phase. The goal of the training phase was to learn how to perform the Sternberg task. The training task was designed similarly to the main one, but with different words compared to the main experiment (all of them non–drug-related). After it was sure that participants knew how to perform the task, they proceeded to the testing phase.
Overall, participants performed three conditions, including a condition of three WM words consisting of 72 trials, a condition of five WM words also consisting of 72 trials, and a condition of seven WM words consisting of 72 trials (Figure 1). All 24 words appeared equally in the probe; they were also paired with second words (incorrect words) in all types of pairings (i.e., methamphetamine-related words, non–drug-related words, and non–drug-related words from the NL). Each of the 24 words was repeated 27 times during 72 trials. The sequence of words was counterbalanced between participants.
Statistical Analysis
Data analysis was conducted using SPSS 21 and Statistica v13 (Dell Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA). All results are expressed as the mean ± standard error of the mean, and the statistical threshold was set to p ≤ 0.05. A priori hypotheses were tested with post hoc analysis (Tukey test) and planned contrasts. The data from trials with null stimuli were excluded from all statistical tests. To analyze the reaction time (RT), trials with incorrect responses were excluded from relevant statistical tests.
Bias Caused by Difference Sources
Potential bias, caused by the methamphetamine-related words on the performance of experimental participants, was from different sources and should be separated in the current task paradigm.
i. The first bias we considered was the summation of WM interference bias and WM bias during different WM loads (three, five, or seven words). This score was defined as 1/2 * (RT (E − G) + RT (F − H) + RT (G − C) + RT (H − D)). Repeated-measures ANOVA was used to calculate this first bias: the three first bias scores during the different WM loads (three, five, or seven words) were considered as a within-subject factor, and subject group (experimental, control) was considered as the between-subject factor.
ii. The second bias we referred to was the WM interference bias. This score was defined as (RT(E) + RT(F))/2 − (RT(G) + RT(H)/2. Repeated-measures ANOVA tests were used to obtain this second score; three second bias scores during the different WM loads (three, five, or seven words) were considered as a within-subject factor, and subject group (experimental, control) was considered as the between-subject factor.
iii. The third bias we referred to was the WM bias. This score was defined as (RT(G) + RT(H)/2 − (RT(C) + RT(D))/2. Repeated-measures ANOVA tests were used in order to calculate this score: three third bias scores during the different WM loads (three, five, or seven words) were considered as a within-subject factor, and subject group (experimental, control) was considered as the between-subject factor.
The Effect of Different WM Loads on the Performance of Participants During High Interference Trials
The performance of participants during high interference trials (i.e. the high interference effect caused by the words from the secondary task) was measured during different WM loads (three, five, or seven words). High interference trials included trials from A, C, E, and G conditions. To test the effect of presenting different WM loads (three, five, or seven words) on the performance of participants, two separate repeated-measures ANOVA tests were performed on RTs and accuracy of participants during high interference trials. In this analysis, RTs and accuracy during different WM loads (three, five, or seven words) were considered as a within-subject factor, and subject group (experimental, control) was considered as the between-subject factor.
Results
Bias Caused by Difference Sources
The First Bias
Repeated-measures ANOVA test showed a significant interaction effect between WM load and subject group on the first bias of RTs (F(2, 116) = 3.76, p = 0.02). Planned contrasts analysis revealed that mean scores for the first bias RTs of the methamphetamine user group significantly increased during the performance of the 3-word WM compared to the control group (p = 0.01). However, no significant difference was observed in mean RTs during the 5- and 7-word WM sets (both p’s > 0.1) (Figure 2).
Figure 2 The first bias.The summation of reaction times of working memory interference bias and working memory bias during different working memory loads (three, five, or seven words). Error bars indicate standard error of the mean **p ≤ 0.01; ***p ≤ 0.01.
Planned comparisons based on our priory hypothesis revealed that mean scores for the first bias RTs of the control group were significantly increased during the performance of the 3-word WM set compared to the 5- and 7-word memory sets (both p’s < 0.001). However, no significant difference was observed in mean scores for the first bias RTs of the control group when comparing the 5-word WM sets to the 7-word sets (p > 0.2). Additionally, no significant difference was observed in mean scores for the first bias RTs of the experimental group during the 3-word WM set compared to the 5- and 7-word memory sets (p’s > 0.2) (Figure 2).
The Second Bias
Repeated-measures ANOVA test showed no significant interaction effect between WM load and subject group on the second bias of RTs of the experimental group compared to the control group (F(2, 116) = 1.97, p = 0.14). Planned comparisons based on our priory hypothesis revealed that mean scores for the second bias RTs of the control group were not significantly changed during the performance of the 3-word WM set compared to the 5- and 7-word memory sets (both p’s > 0.2). No significant difference was observed in mean scores for the second bias RTs of the control group when comparing the 5-word WM sets to the 7-word sets (p > 0.2). Additionally, mean scores for the second bias RTs of the experimental group were not significantly changed during the performance of the 3-word WM set compared to the 5- and 7-word memory sets (both p’s > 0.1). No significant difference was observed in mean scores for the second bias RTs of the experimental group when comparing the 5-word WM sets to the 7-word sets (p > 0.05).
The Third Bias
Repeated-measures ANOVA test showed no significant interaction effect between WM load and subject group on the third bias RTs of the experimental group compared to the control group (F(2, 116) = 0.81, p = 0.44). Planned contrasts analysis revealed that mean scores for the third bias RTs of the control group were not significantly changed during the performance of the 3-word WM set compared to the 5- and 7-word memory sets (both p’s > 0.1). No significant difference was observed in mean scores for the third bias RTs of the control group when comparing the 5-word WM sets to the 7-word sets (p > 0.2). Additionally, mean scores for the third bias RTs of the experimental group were not significantly changed during the performance of the 3-word WM set compared to the 5- and 7-word memory sets (both p’s > 0.2). No significant difference was observed in mean scores for the second bias RTs of the experimental group when comparing the 5-word WM sets to the 7-word sets (p > 0.05).
The Effect of Different WM Loads on the Performance of Participants During High Interference Trials
Reaction Times During Trials With High Interference
Repeated-measures ANOVA test showed no significant interaction effects between WM load and subject group on the RT of high interference condition (F(2, 116) = 0.47, p = 0.62). However, priori hypotheses were tested with planned contrasts, and the type I error rate was controlled for using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Planned contrasts analysis revealed that the mean RTs of the methamphetamine user group significantly decreased during the performance of the 3-word WM set compared to performing the 5- and 7-word WM sets (p’s < 0.001). However, no significant difference was observed in mean RTs during the 5-word WM set compared to mean RTs when performing the 7-word WM sets (p > 0.2). The same results were also found in the control group. Mean RTs of the control group during performance of the 3-word WM set compared to the 5- and 7-word memory sets were significantly decreased (p’s < 0.001), but no significant difference was observed in mean RTs when comparing the 5-word WM sets to the 7-word sets (p > 0.2) (Figure 3).
Figure 3 Working memory performance during high interference (reaction times).Mean reaction times of abstinent ex-methamphetamine users and nonaddict control group for the recognition of words from the 3-word working memory sets were compared to the 5- and 7-word working memory sets during trials with high interference. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean ***p ≤ 0.001.
Accuracy During Trials With High Interference
Repeated-measures ANOVA test showed no significant interaction effects between WM load and subject group on the accuracy of high interference condition (F(2, 116) = 2.91, p = 0.058). However, priori hypotheses were tested with planned contrasts, and the type I error rate was controlled for using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Planned contrasts analysis revealed that the mean accuracy of the methamphetamine user group significantly increased during the performance of the 3-word WM set compared to performing the 5- and 7-word WM sets (p’s < 0.001). However, no significant difference was observed in mean accuracy during the 5-word WM set compared to mean accuracy when performing the 7-word WM sets (p > 0.2). Mean accuracy of the control group during performance of the 3-word WM set compared to the 5- and 7-word memory sets was significantly increased (p’s < 0.001). Besides, mean accuracy was significantly increased when comparing the 5-word WM sets to the 7-word sets (p = 0.03) (Figure 4).
Figure 4 Working memory performance during high interference (accuracy).Mean accuracy of abstinent ex-methamphetamine users and nonaddict control group in recognition of words from the 3-word working memory sets was compared to the five- and seven-word working memory sets during trials with high interference. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean ***p ≤ 0.001 and *p ≤ 0.05.
Demographic information is summarized in Table 1.
Discussion
The novel finding of the current study is that abstinent ex-methamphetamine users compared to a nonaddict group showed a bias toward methamphetamine-related stimuli only in in low WM load conditions (3-word WM sets). These results suggest that increasing the load of WM might reduce the effect of interference. In addition, there was no statistically significant difference in WM performance between all three WM load conditions during trials with high interference between both groups. These findings suggest that increasing the load of WM shields the effect of interference. Besides, attentional bias toward methamphetamine-related stimuli, which were presented during the encoding phase of WM, may contribute to optimal WM performance and may increase the availability of the shared cognitive resources.
Bias Caused by Difference Sources
The First Bias
The results showed that abstinent ex-methamphetamine users showed a bias (i.e., the summation of WM interference bias and WM bias) during low WM load (three words) task performance but not during high load WM (five and seven words) compared to the nonaddict group. The impulsive process may trigger cognitive biases such as attentional bias for drug-related stimuli (14, 15, 18). Studies showed that the use of drugs develops a specific reward system in the brain by releasing dopamine in mesolimbic brain areas, which in turn enhance learning by conditioning (26, 50–52). Attentional bias toward drug-related stimuli results in prolonging the disengagement of attention from those stimuli, leading to increased RTs (20, 51, 53). However, following the views that WM protects bias toward distractors, we expected to find a modulation over distraction (i.e. methamphetamine words) under higher WM loads (29).
The Second and Third Bias
Results from the current study showed no significant differences between groups for the second and third biases (i.e., WM interference bias, WM bias). These results can best be considered with some possible explanations:
i. Abstinent ex-methamphetamine users are probably motivated to quit or stay abstain from drugs (54, 55). This motivation leads individuals to develop avoidance strategies to cope with tempting stimuli, which meanwhile might be effortful for them (54, 56). For example, heavy alcohol drinkers showed an attentional bias toward alcohol-related stimuli, while abstaining alcohol-dependent individuals avoided such stimuli (24, 57, 2, 55). Besides, studies have also indicated that presenting drug-related stimuli long enough (e.g., 500 ms) can give patients enough time to use efficient avoidance strategies; therefore, the effect of attentional bias might be reversed and away from drug-related stimuli (24, 57).
ii. The cognitive effort, which has been closely coupled with concepts of attention, difficulty in concentration, and motivation, can also explain current results (58). Cognitive effort can modulate the cognitive resources dedicated to a particular task (59). Indeed, to accomplish more demanding tasks, we have to exert more effort, which leads to a reduced effect of distractors (59, 60). Although we did not measure cognitive effort during the task, abstinent ex-methamphetamine users might invest more effort to perform WM task.
iii. Drug-related stimuli might elicit an attentional bias toward those stimuli, resulting in an interruption in the performance during the ongoing task. However, it is also possible that drug-related stimuli can elicit a motor response to provide fast and necessary reactions, resulting in avoiding the effect of distractions (61, 62). Besides, according to the literature on anxiety, the shorter RTs for threat stimuli in threat-neutral pairs could indicate an attentional bias away from the threat (61–63). Therefore, it is postulated that salient methamphetamine-related stimuli might lead to increased anxiety, resulting in quicker responses.
iv. The reflective process can moderate the impact of the impulsive process by emphasizing the effect of WM capacity (top-down process) (14, 17, 18, 64–67). For example, studies have supported the moderating effect of WM capacity on alcohol abuse (27, 64). These results indicated that individuals with a lower WM capacity show strong correlations between implicit alcohol associations and the use of alcohol (14, 27, 64). Although traditional models of impulse control have emphasized the adverse effect of increasing cognitive load on self-regulation, emotion-related studies have supported the idea that increased cognitive load can inhibit feelings of temptation (68–71). Regarding this issue, attention toward an emotional target is automatic (i.e., fast and involuntary), but it is also resource-dependent (71–73). It means that an increased cognitive load may lead to a decrease in the motivation to process task-irrelevant stimuli despite their saliency and associated feelings of temptation (71, 74). For example, categorizing the gender of angry faces compared to happy faces—as an index of selective attention to threatening information—was slower during the mental rehearsal of a one-digit number (low cognitive load) compared to the rehearsal of an eight-digit number (high cognitive load) (74). The bottom line is that there is bias variability in the addiction literature, which makes the basic mechanisms still unclear, but this bias might reflect variations in top-down cognitive control (47, 62, 75). In addition to the emotion-related studies, the WM theory proposed by Andrade et al. (76) also supports the current findings. This theory suggested that retrieving information from WM requires WM capacity, but if the capacity of WM (resource) is occupied during memory reactivation, the emotionality and saliency of new information will be decreased, which will result in updating that information into a less emotional form (67). For example, studies have shown that using a high WM load task (visual-spatial task) during the retrieval of drug-related information could decrease cigarette (77) and food cravings (67, 78).
In summary, studies have indicated that WM and attention processes recruit similar neural networks and share common cognitive resources (79–81). In our case, attentional bias to the methamphetamine-related stimuli, particularly during high WM loads, may bring gain in WM performance (particularly during the encoding phase) and may increase the availability of cognitive resources. The result of the first bias during a task with low WM load indicated that attention was directed toward salient methamphetamine-related stimuli. On the other hand, performing tasks with high WM loads inhibited the effect of bias in abstinent ex-methamphetamine users. Regarding the results of the second and third bias considered, we suggest that presenting methamphetamine-related stimuli and increasing loads of WM were helpful for the experimental group to inhibit the effect of interference. In line with the dual-process models of addiction, which is focused on the trade-off between impulsive and reflective processes, these findings suggest that WM engagement and increased WM load improved avoidance strategy, possibly through reflective processes (71).
The Effect of Different WM Loads on the Performance of Participants During High Interference Trials
Our findings showed that increasing WM load resulted in increased RTs and decreased accuracy in both the abstinent ex-methamphetamine and nonaddict groups. These results were supported by previous studies showing that increasing WM load could decrease WM performance (82–85). However, there was no significant difference between groups, which contradicted our hypothesis.
There are contradictory findings regarding the effect of methamphetamine on WM performance. On the one hand, some studies have revealed overall cognitive deficits in the domains of verbal memory, WM, executive function, and social cognition in methamphetamine users (9, 10, 20, 86–89). For example, one study indicated that chronic methamphetamine users showed a deficit in some CogState battery domains (i.e., evaluated seven cognitive domains including WM) and poor psychological well-being (88). Studies have also revealed that methamphetamine users had deficits in brain function in areas including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during performing cognitive tasks that assess executive function (WM) (20, 87). Activity in this brain area can support WM performance and allocation of attentional resources (36, 90, 91).
However, in line with our results, some studies have shown no significant difference in WM performance of abstinent ex-methamphetamine compared to the nonaddict group (30). For example, Boileau et al. (92) asked methamphetamine users and subjects from a control group to perform different cognitive tasks including WM, attention/psychomotor function, and immediate and delayed memory tasks. Their results showed that there was no significant difference in WM performance between both groups, but that in attention/psychomotor function and delayed memory tasks, methamphetamine users showed a deficit. Another study employed attention/psychomotor function tasks (e.g., Stroop), learning/memory tasks, WM tasks, response inhibition tasks, and set-shifting/executive function tasks for both methamphetamine users and a control group (93). The findings indicated no significant difference between groups for all cognitive tasks. Similar results were observed in other studies as well (94, 95); for a review of this topic, see Hart et al. (30).
The use of different kinds of WM tasks in different experiments might explain these contradictory findings. In our study, we utilized the modified Sternberg task, in which methamphetamine-related stimuli were presented during the encoding phase of WM. These salient methamphetamine-related stimuli might cause attentional bias leading to attentional capture and eventually contributing to better performance despite having an interference bias. Indeed, attentional bias to methamphetamine-related stimuli might highlight those stimuli in WM, resulting in enhanced WM performance. Also, type of distractors might be an important factor to explained contradictory findings. For example, the amount of physical separation between targets and distractors might modulate the effects of load on distraction (29).
The availability of cognitive resources for optimal task performance and inhibiting the effect of interference is critical, particularly when WM is highly loaded or saturated (29). Our findings suggest that cognitive resources might be available as they are not dominated by task demands, resulting in optimal performance. Besides, bias toward the methamphetamine-related words (which were task-relevant information) might facilitate the WM process.
Limitations and Future Directions
Only male participants were recruited in the present study to minimize the effect of potentially confounding factors. One noteworthy and currently unexplored direction for future studies might be to examine gender differences. Moreover, participants with the mean of nearly 17-day abstinence from methamphetamine use were recruited. We chose this sample to assess the effect of short time abstinence from methamphetamine on WM performance. However, it is still unclear what may be the effect of long-term abstinence from methamphetamine use on cognitive function. For example, one study revealed that enhanced performance on tests of verbal memory and executive function was observed after approximately 6 months of abstinence from methamphetamine use. In line with this idea, some studies have examined the role of duration of abstinence from methamphetamine use on cognitive function (96, 97). They showed that prolonged greater duration of abstinence from methamphetamine use resulted in better cognitive performance (96–98). Future studies might consider the effect of long-term abstinence from methamphetamine use on WM biases and WM capacity. In addition, this study did not include a sample of active methamphetamine users who do not want to quit drug use, due to the difficulty in performing the modified Sternberg task. Future studies might add this group to compare the effect of motivation to quit on WM performance between abstinent and active groups. Due to the size of the center and the limited-time permission we have for our study, we could not recruit more participants. In several tests, we realized that the power of analysis is below the optimal level, and for some interactions, there was only a trend toward significance. In the future studies hiring complex tasks, more participants should be recruited to have enough power to run all the required analyses properly. This study sought to examine the neurobiological substrates of the interaction between WM bias, WM capacity, and interference effect using a complex span task in methamphetamine users.
Conclusion
We investigated the impulsive effects of methamphetamine-related stimuli on WM performance in abstinent ex-methamphetamine users. The experimental group demonstrated bias toward methamphetamine-related stimuli during a task with low WM load (three words) but not while performing tasks with higher WM loads (five and seven words). This result suggests that increasing WM load may provide an efficient buffer against attentional capture by salient stimuli (i.e., methamphetamine-related words). In line with this findings, investigating the effect of increasing WM load on the performance of abstinent ex-methamphetamine users (i.e., WM capacity) showed that increasing WM load had no significant effect on WM performance of abstinent ex-methamphetamine users compared with the control group. These findings suggest that increasing WM loads modified the impact of the interference bias. Besides, presenting methamphetamine-related stimuli facilitated their encoding due to bias toward task-relevant stimuli.
This finding has an important implication, suggesting that performing concurrent demanding tasks may reduce the power of salient stimuli and thus improve the efficiency of emotion regulation strategies. Further investigation on the interactions between WM interference bias, WM bias, and WM capacity may lead to the development of better tools and alternative therapies, including WM training, for the treatment of addiction.
Ethics Statement
All experimental procedures corresponded to the standards set by the latest revision of the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the ethical committee of the Institute for Cognitive Sciences Studies, Tehran, Iran. All participants provided written informed consent, acknowledging their right to withdraw from the experiment without prejudice.
Author Contributions
ZD contributed to all aspects of the research. HE contributed to experimental design and data interpretation. HP contributed to experimental design and data interpretation. AK contributed to all aspects of the research.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Dr. Javad Salehi Fadardi for statistical consulting, Mehrshad Golesorkhi for his assistance in data collection, and Drs. Reinout W. Wiers and Thomas E. Gladwin for helpful comments on the manuscript.
Abbreviations
WM, Working Memory; RTs, Reaction Times.
References
1. Degenhardt L, Hall W. Extent of illicit drug use and dependence, and their contribution to the global burden of disease. Lancet (2012) 379(9810):55–70. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61138-0
2. Degenhardt L, Mathers B, Guarinieri M, Panda S, Phillips B, Strathdee SA, et al. Meth/amphetamine use and associated HIV: Implications for global policy and public health. Int J Drug Policy (2010) 21(5):347–58. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2009.11.007
3. Meredith CW, Jaffe C, Ang-Lee K, Saxon AJ. Implications of chronic methamphetamine use: a literature review. Harv Rev Psychiatry (2005) 13(3):141–54. doi: 10.1080/10673220591003605
4. Lecomte T, Mueser KT, MacEwan W, Thornton AE, Buchanan T, Bouchard V, et al. Predictors of persistent psychotic symptoms in persons with methamphetamine abuse receiving psychiatric treatment. J Nerv Ment Dis (2013) 201(12):1085–9. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000059
5. Fassbender C, Lesh TA, Ursu S, Salo R. Reaction time variability and related brain activity in methamphetamine psychosis. Biol Psychiatry (2015) 77(5):465–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.07.028
6. Semple SJ, Patterson TL, Grant I. A comparison of injection and non-injection methamphetamine-using HIV positive men who have sex with men. Drug Alcohol Depend (2004) 76(2):203–12. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.05.003
7. Semple SJ, Zians J, Grant I, Patterson TL. Impulsivity and methamphetamine use. J Subst Abuse Treat (2005) 29(2):85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.05.001
8. Panenka WJ, Procyshyn RM, Lecomte T, MacEwan GW, Flynn SW, Honer WG, et al. Methamphetamine use: A comprehensive review of molecular, preclinical and clinical findings. Drug Alcohol Depend (2013) 129(3):167–79. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.11.016
9. Luo YL, Bian JW, Zheng ZJ, Zhao L, Han S, Sun XH, et al. Effects of methamphetamine abuse on spatial cognitive function. Sci Rep (2018) 8(1):5502. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23828-y
10. Potvin S, Pelletier J, Grot S, Hebert C, Barr AM, Lecomte T. Cognitive deficits in individuals with methamphetamine use disorder: A meta-analysis. Addict Behav (2018) 80:154–60. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.01.021
11. Gladwin TE, Wiers CE, Wiers RW. Cognitive neuroscience of cognitive retraining for addiction medicine: From mediating mechanisms to questions of efficacy. Prog Brain Res (2016) 224:323–44. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.07.021
12. Wiers RW, Cox WM, Field M, Fadardi JS, Palfai TP, Schoenmakers T, et al. The search for new ways to change implicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2006) 30(2):320–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00037.x
13. Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RC, Sher KJ, et al. Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review and a model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (2007) 86(2):263–83. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021
14. Lavigne AM, Wood MD, Janssen T, Wiers RW. Implicit and Explicit Alcohol Cognitions: The Moderating Effect of Executive Functions. Alcohol Alcohol (2017) 52(2):256–62. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agw066
15. Wiers CE, Gladwin TE, Ludwig VU, Gropper S, Stuke H, Gawron CK, et al. Comparing three cognitive biases for alcohol cues in alcohol dependence. Alcohol Alcohol (2017) 52(2):242–8. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agw063
16. Wiers CE, Wiers RW. Imaging the neural effects of cognitive bias modification training. Neuroimage (2017) 151:81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.041
17. Zerhouni O, Begue L, Comiran F, Wiers RW. Controlled and implicit processes in evaluative conditioning on implicit and explicit attitudes toward alcohol and intentions to drink. Addict Behav (2018) 76:335–42. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.08.026
18. Stacy AW, Wiers RW. Implicit cognition and addiction: a tool for explaining paradoxical behavior. Annu Rev Clin Psychol (2010) 6:551–75. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131444
19. Gladwin T, Wiers RW. How do alcohol cues affect working memory? Persistent slowing due to alcohol-related distracters in an alcohol version of the Sternberg task. Addict Res Theory (2012) 20(4):284–90. doi: 10.3109/16066359.2011.614977
20. Hoffman WF, Moore M, Templin R, McFarland B, Hitzemann RJ, Mitchell SH. Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (2006) 188(2):162–70. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0494-0
21. Duarte NA, Woods SP, Rooney A, Atkinson JH, Grant I. Working memory deficits affect risky decision-making in methamphetamine users with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Psychiatr Res (2012) 46(4):492–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.006
22. Casaletto KB, Obermeit L, Morgan EE, Weber E, Franklin DR, Grant I, et al. Depression and executive dysfunction contribute to a metamemory deficit among individuals with methamphetamine use disorders. Addict Behav (2015) 40:45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.08.007
23. Cox WM, Hogan LM, Kristian MR, Race JH. Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of alcohol abusers’ treatment outcome. Drug Alcohol Depend (2002) 68(3):237–43. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(02)00219-3
24. Field M, Cox WM. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend (2008) 97(1-2):1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.030
25. Anderson BA. What is abnormal about addiction-related attentional biases? Drug Alcohol Depend (2016) 167:8–14. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.08.002
26. Brooks SJ, Wiemerslage L, Burch KH, Maiorana SA, Cocolas E, Schioth HB, et al. The impact of cognitive training in substance use disorder: the effect of working memory training on impulse control in methamphetamine users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2017) 234(12):1911–21. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4597-6
27. Thush C, Wiers RW, Ames SL, Grenard JL, Sussman S, Stacy AW. Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. Drug Alcohol Depend (2008) 94(1-3):116–24. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.10.019
28. Baddeley A. Working Memory. In: Theories, Models, and Controversies., vol. 1-29. ANNUAL REVIEWS (2012). doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
29. SanMiguel I, Corral M-J, Escera C. When Loading Working Memory Reduces Distraction: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence from an Auditory-Visual Distraction Paradigm. J Cognit Neurosci (2008) 20(7):1131–45. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20078
30. Hart CL, Marvin CB, Silver R, Smith EE. Is cognitive functioning impaired in methamphetamine users? A critical review. Neuropsychopharmacology (2012) 37(3):586–608. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.276
31. Sabrini S, Wang GY, Lin JC, Ian JK, Curley LE. Methamphetamine use and cognitive function: A systematic review of neuroimaging research. Drug Alcohol Depend (2019) 194:75–87. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.08.041
32. Chang L, Ernst T, Speck O, Patel H, DeSilva M, Leonido-Yee M, et al. Perfusion MRI and computerized cognitive test abnormalities in abstinent methamphetamine users. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging (2002) 114(2):65–79. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(02)00004-5
33. Gene-Jack Wang MD, Nora D. Volkow MD, Linda Chang MD, Miller E, Mark Sedler MD, Hitzemann R, et al. Partial Recovery of Brain Metabolism in Methamphetamine Abusers After Protracted Abstinence. Am J Psychiatry (2004) 161(2):242–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.242
34. Thompson PM, Hayashi KM, Simon SL, Geaga JA, Hong MS, Sui Y, et al. Structural abnormalities in the brains of human subjects who use methamphetamine. J Neurosci (2004) 24(26):6028–36. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0713-04.2004
35. Kessler K, Pajak KM, Harkin B, Jones B. A working memory bias for alcohol-related stimuli depends on drinking score. Psychol Addict Behav (2013) 27(1):23–31. doi: 10.1037/a0028664
36. Awh E, Vogel EK, Oh SH. Interactions between attention and working memory. Neurosci (2006) 139(1):201–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.023
37. Rosen VM, Engle RW. Working Memory Capacity and Suppression. J Mem Lang (1998) 39(3):418–36. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2590
38. Kane MJ, Bleckley MK, Conway AR, Engle RW. A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. J Exp Psychol Gen (2001) 130(2):169–83. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.2.169
39. Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. J Exp Psychol Gen (2003) 132(1):47–70. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47
40. Barrett LF, Tugade MM, Engle RW. Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychol Bull (2004) 130(4):553–73. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.553
41. Unsworth N, Engle RW. Simple and complex memory spans and their relation to fluid abilities: Evidence from list-length effects. J Mem Lang (2006) 54(1):68–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.06.003
42. Heitz RP, Schrock JC, Payne TW, Engle RW. Effects of incentive on working memory capacity: behavioral and pupillometric data. Psychophysiology (2008) 45(1):119–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00605.x
43. Unsworth N, Redick TS, Heitz RP, Broadway JM, Engle RW. Complex working memory span tasks and higher-order cognition: a latent-variable analysis of the relationship between processing and storage. Memory (2009) 17(6):635–54. doi: 10.1080/09658210902998047
44. Townshend JM, Duka T. Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2001) 157(1):67–74. doi: 10.1007/s002130100764
45. Field M, Wiers RW, Christiansen P, Fillmore MT, Verster JC. Acute alcohol effects on inhibitory control and implicit cognition: implications for loss of control over drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2010) 34(8):1346–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01218.x
46. Miller MA, Fillmore MT. The effect of image complexity on attentional bias towards alcohol-related images in adult drinkers. Addiction (2010) 105(5):883–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02860.x
47. Gladwin TE. Attentional bias variability and cued attentional bias for alcohol stimuli. Addict Res Theory (2017) 25(1):32–8. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2016.1196674
48. Gladwin TE, Uyl TE, Fregni FF, Wiers RW. Enhancement of selective attention by tDCS: interaction with interference in a Sternberg task. Neurosci Lett (2012) 512(1):33–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.01.056
49. Ekhtiari H, Alam-Mehrjerdi Z, Hassani-Abharian P, Nouri M, Farnam R, Mokri A. Examination and evaluation of craving-inductive verbal cues among Persian-speaking methamphetamine abusers. [Examination and evaluation of craving-inductive verbal cues among Persian-speaking methamphetamine abusers.]. Adv. Cognit Sci (2010) 12(2):69–82.
50. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev (1993) 18(3):247–91. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P
51. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol (2003) 54:25–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237
52. Smith JL, Mattick RP, Jamadar SD, Iredale JM. Deficits in behavioural inhibition in substance abuse and addiction: a meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend (2014) 145:1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.08.009
53. Petersen SE, Posner MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci (2012) 35(1):73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
54. Galla BM, Duckworth AL. More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. J Pers Soc Psychol (2015) 109(3):508–25. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000026
55. Brevers D, Bechara A, Kilts CD, Antoniali V, Bruylant A, Verbanck P, et al. Competing motivations: proactive response inhibition toward addiction-related stimuli in quitting-motivated individuals. J Gambl Stud (2018) 34(3):785–806. doi: 10.1007/s10899-017-9722-2
56. Hofmann W, Friese M, Strack F. Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspect Psychol Sci (2009) 4(2):162–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01116.x
57. Noel X, Colmant M, Van Der Linden M, Bechara A, Bullens Q, Hanak C, et al. Time course of attention for alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholic patients: the role of initial orienting. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2006) 30(11):1871–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00224.x
58. Westbrook A, Braver TS. Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach. Cognit Affective Behav Neurosci (2015) 15(2):395–415. doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0334-y
59. Lavie N, Hirst A, de Fockert JW, Viding E. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. J Exp Psychol Gen (2004b) 133(3):339–54. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339
60. Sarter M, Gehring WJ, Kozak R. More attention must be paid: the neurobiology of attentional effort. Brain Res Rev (2006) 51(2):145–60. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.11.002
61. Mathews A, Mogg K, Kentish J, Eysenck M. Effect of psychological treatment on cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. Behav Res Ther (1995) 33(3):293–303. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)E0022-B
62. Townshend JM, Duka T. Avoidance of alcohol-related stimuli in alcohol-dependent inpatients. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2007) 31(8):1349–57. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00429.x
63. Brown HM, Eley TC, Broeren S, Macleod C, Rinck M, Hadwin JA, et al. Psychometric properties of reaction time based experimental paradigms measuring anxiety-related information-processing biases in children. J Anxiety Disord (2014) 28(1):97–107. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.11.004
64. Grenard JL, Ames SL, Wiers RW, Thush C, Sussman S, Stacy AW. Working memory capacity moderates the predictive effects of drug-related associations on substance use. Psychol Addict Behav (2008) 22(3):426–32. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.3.426
65. Piechatzek M, Indlekofer F, Daamen M, Glasmacher C, Lieb R, Pfister H, et al. Is moderate substance use associated with altered executive functioning in a population-based sample of young adults? Hum Psychopharmacol (2009) 24(8):650–65. doi: 10.1002/hup.1069
66. Boendermaker WJ, Gladwin TE, Peeters M, Prins PJM, Wiers RW. Training working memory in adolescents using serious game elements: pilot randomized controlled trial. JMIR Serious Games (2018) 6(2):e10. doi: 10.2196/games.8364
67. Kaag AM, Goudriaan AE, De Vries TJ, Pattij T, Wiers RW. A high working memory load prior to memory retrieval reduces craving in non-treatment seeking problem drinkers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2018) 235(3):695–708. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4785-4
68. Wegner DM. Ironic processes of mental control. Psychol Rev (1994) 101(1):34–52. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.101.1.34
69. Ward A, Mann T. Don’t mind if I do: disinhibited eating under cognitive load. J Pers Soc Psychol (2000) 78(4):753–63. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.4.753
70. Kim SY, Kim MS, Chun MM. Concurrent working memory load can reduce distraction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102(45):16524–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505454102
71. Van Dillen LF, Papies EK, Hofmann W. Turning a blind eye to temptation: how cognitive load can facilitate self-regulation. J Pers Soc Psychol (2013) 104(3):427–43. doi: 10.1037/a0031262
72. Van Dillen LF, Derks B. Working memory load reduces facilitated processing of threatening faces: an ERP study. Emotion (2012) 12(6):1340–9. doi: 10.1037/a0028624
73. Van Dillen LF, van der Wal RC, van den Bos K. On the role of attention and emotion in morality: attentional control modulates unrelated disgust in moral judgments. Pers Soc Psychol Bull (2012) 38(9):1222–31. doi: 10.1177/0146167212448485
74. Van Dillen LF, Koole SL. How automatic is “automatic vigilance”? The role of working memory in attentional interference of negative information. Cogn Emot (2009) 23(6):1106–17. doi: 10.1080/02699930802338178
75. Iacoviello BM, Wu G, Abend R, Murrough JW, Feder A, Fruchter E, et al. Attention bias variability and symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress (2014) 27(2):232–9. doi: 10.1002/jts.21899
76. Andrade J, Kavanagh D, Baddeley A. Eye-movements and visual imagery: a working memory approach to the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder. Br J Clin Psychol (1997) 36(Pt 2):209–23. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1997.tb01408.x
77. May J, Andrade J, Panabokke N, Kavanagh D. Visuospatial tasks suppress craving for cigarettes. Behav Res Ther (2010) 48(6):476–85. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.02.001
78. Kemps E, Tiggemann M. Hand-held dynamic visual noise reduces naturally occurring food cravings and craving-related consumption. Appetite (2013) 68:152–7. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.05.001
79. Fougnie D, Marois R. Attentive tracking disrupts feature binding in visual working memory. Vis cogn (2009) 17(1-2):48–66. doi: 10.1080/13506280802281337
80. Kiyonaga A, Egner T. Working memory as internal attention: Toward an integrative account of internal and external selection processes. Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20(2):228–42. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0359-y
81. Dowd EW. Memory-Based Attentional Guidance: A Window to the Relationship between Working Memory and Attention. Dissertation/Thesis, ProQuest Dissertations Publishing (2016)
82. Lavie N, Hirst A, de Fockert JW, Viding E. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. J Exp Psychol: Gen (2004a) 133(3):339–54. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339
83. Hester R, Garavan H. Working memory and executive function: The influence of content and load on the control of attention. Mem Cognit (2005) 33(2):221–33. doi: 10.3758/BF03195311
84. Klemen J, Buchel C, Buhler M, Menz MM, Rose M. Auditory working memory load impairs visual ventral stream processing: toward a unified model of attentional load. J Cogn Neurosci (2010) 22(3):437–46. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21204
85. Simon SS, Tusch ES, Holcomb PJ, Daffner KR. Increasing working memory load reduces processing of cross-modal task-irrelevant stimuli even after controlling for task difficulty and executive capacity. Front Hum Neurosci (2016) 10:380–0. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00380
86. Scott JC, Woods SP, Matt GE, Meyer RA, Heaton RK, Atkinson JH, et al. Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: a critical review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev (2007) 17(3):275–97. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9031-0
87. Dean AC, Groman SM, Morales AM, London ED. An evaluation of the evidence that methamphetamine abuse causes cognitive decline in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology (2012) 38:259. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.179
88. Zhong N, Jiang H, Du J, Zhao Y, Sun H, Xu D, et al. The cognitive impairments and psychological wellbeing of methamphetamine dependent patients compared with health controls. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2016) 69:31–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2016.04.005
89. Sabrini S, Wang GY, Lin JC, Ian JK, Curley LE. Methamphetamine use and cognitive function: A systematic review of neuroimaging research. Drug Alcohol Depend (2018) 194:75–87. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.08.041
90. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci (2001) 24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
91. Barcelo F, Escera C, Corral MJ, Periáñez JA. Task switching and novelty processing activate a common neural network for cognitive control. J Cognit Neurosci (2006) 18(10):1734–48. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1734
92. Boileau I, Rusjan P, Houle S, Wilkins D, Tong J, Selby P, et al. Increased vesicular monoamine transporter binding during early abstinence in human methamphetamine users: Is VMAT2 a stable dopamine neuron biomarker? J Neurosci (2008) 28(39):9850–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3008-08.2008
93. McCann UD, Kuwabara H, Kumar A, Palermo M, Abbey R, Brasic J, et al. Persistent cognitive and dopamine transporter deficits in abstinent methamphetamine users. Synapse (2008) 62(2):91–100. doi: 10.1002/syn.20471
94. Johanson CE, Frey KA, Lundahl LH, Keenan P, Lockhart N, Roll J, et al. Cognitive function and nigrostriatal markers in abstinent methamphetamine abusers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2006) 185(3):327–38. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0330-6
95. Silber BY, Croft RJ, Papafotiou K, Stough C. The acute effects of d-amphetamine and methamphetamine on attention and psychomotor performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2006) 187(2):154–69. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0410-7
96. Salo R, Nordahl TE, Galloway GP, Moore CD, Waters C, Leamon MH. Drug abstinence and cognitive control in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. J Subst Abuse Treat (2009) 37(3):292–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2009.03.004
97. Simon SL, Dean AC, Cordova X, Monterosso JR, London ED. Methamphetamine dependence and neuropsychological functioning: evaluating change during early abstinence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs (2010) 71(3):335–44. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.335
Keywords: addiction, dual-process models, working memory bias, working memory interference bias, working memory capacity, abstinent ex-methamphetamine users
Citation: Deldar Z, Ekhtiari H, Pouretemad HR and Khatibi A (2019) Bias Toward Drug-Related Stimuli Is Affected by Loading Working Memory in Abstinent Ex-Methamphetamine Users. Front. Psychiatry 10:776. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00776
Received: 01 January 2019; Accepted: 26 September 2019;
Published: 22 October 2019.
Edited by:
Scott J. Moeller, Stony Brook Medicine, United StatesCopyright © 2019 Deldar, Ekhtiari, Pouretemad and Khatibi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ali Khatibi, YWxpLmtoYXRpYmlAZ21haWwuY29t