Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 24 September 2019
Sec. Psychological Therapy and Psychosomatics
This article is part of the Research Topic Placebo and Nocebo Effects in Psychiatry and Beyond View all 39 articles

Placebo Manipulations Reverse Pain Potentiation by Unpleasant Affective Stimuli

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
  • 2Center of Mental Health, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
  • 3Department of Psychology, Education, and Child Studies, University of Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands

According to the motivational priming hypothesis, unpleasant stimuli activate the motivational defense system, which in turn promotes congruent affective states such as negative emotions and pain. The question arises to what degree this bottom–up impact of emotions on pain is susceptible to a manipulation of top–down-driven expectations. To this end, we investigated whether verbal instructions implying pain potentiation vs. reduction (placebo or nocebo expectations)—later on confirmed by corresponding experiences (placebo or nocebo conditioning)—might alter behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of pain modulation by unpleasant pictures. We compared two groups, which underwent three experimental phases: first, participants were either instructed that watching unpleasant affective pictures would increase pain (nocebo group) or that watching unpleasant pictures would decrease pain (placebo group) relative to neutral pictures. During the following placebo/nocebo-conditioning phase, pictures were presented together with electrical pain stimuli of different intensities, reinforcing the instructions. In the subsequent test phase, all pictures were presented again combined with identical pain stimuli. Electroencephalogram was recorded in order to analyze neurophysiological responses of pain (somatosensory evoked potential) and picture processing [visually evoked late positive potential (LPP)], in addition to pain ratings. In the test phase, ratings of pain stimuli administered while watching unpleasant relative to neutral pictures were significantly higher in the nocebo group, thus confirming the motivational priming effect for pain perception. In the placebo group, this effect was reversed such that unpleasant compared with neutral pictures led to significantly lower pain ratings. Similarly, somatosensory evoked potentials were decreased during unpleasant compared with neutral pictures, in the placebo group only. LPPs of the placebo group failed to discriminate between unpleasant and neutral pictures, while the LPPs of the nocebo group showed a clear differentiation. We conclude that the placebo manipulation already affected the processing of the emotional stimuli and, in consequence, the processing of the pain stimuli. In summary, the study revealed that the modulation of pain by emotions, albeit a reliable and well-established finding, is further tuned by reinforced expectations—known to induce placebo/nocebo effects—which should be addressed in future research and considered in clinical applications.

Introduction

The processing of pain is prone to a variety of psychological variables, such as the affective state of an individual [for an overview, see Ref. (1)]. In this vein, it was demonstrated that emotions, induced for instance by a threat manipulation (2) or by emotionally relevant stimuli, modulate pain processing (36). In an earlier study, Kenntner-Mabiala and colleagues presented affective pictures for about 6 s to participants while they applied brief painful electric stimuli and registered pain ratings plus the somatosensory evoked potential (SEP) (7, 8). Results suggest that emotions modulate early pain processing as unpleasant pictures resulted in increased pain ratings and increased amplitudes of the early N1 component of the SEP relative to positive pictures. Other studies indicate that expectations regarding the characteristics of an upcoming pain stimulus also determine the processing of nociceptive stimulation and the resulting pain perception (9, 10). The same is true for placebo and nocebo effects on pain; however, here, expectations focus on the effect of an intervention, which is expected to decrease (placebo) or increase pain (nocebo) (1012). Expectations causing placebo and nocebo effects can be induced by verbal instructions suggesting a pain-modulating effect and/or by the actual experience of pain relief or pain exacerbation (placebo/nocebo conditioning) associated with a certain treatment or—experimental—intervention (1317).

In a recent study, we investigated the respective contribution of expectations and prior experiences on the formation of placebo effects. To this end, we introduced a new, completely psychological placebo manipulation, which ensured that participants had not encountered the placebo agent before and thus had no a priori expectation. We employed a common approach in placebo and nocebo research that is a placebo/nocebo instruction followed by a reinforcing conditioning phase, during which placebos were combined with lower and nocebos with stronger pain stimuli. Three experimental conditions were compared: Participants were either only informed of an analgesic/pro-algesic effect they were about to encounter, or participants actually experienced different levels of pain in a conditioning procedure, or participants received both, an instruction informing about a pain-modulating effect, which received support during a subsequent conditioning phase. We found that the latter condition, i.e., expectation plus concordant conditioning, was capable in modifying subjective and physiological indices of pain, even though the placebo/nocebo manipulation was lacking pharmacological plausibility, since we instructed participants that “watching certain black and white stripe patterns were found to have a pain augmenting/easing effect,” respectively (18). These findings corroborate the critical role of higher-order cognitions for the modulation of pain.

Placebo and nocebo effects, however, are by no means restricted to pain. Significant modifications have been found for various somatic symptoms (12) and also for the perception of emotions. For example, Petrovic and colleagues found that subjective and neuronal responses to unpleasant affective pictures were reduced if participants believed they had received an anxiolytic medication (19). Based on the involved brain areas, the authors assume similar underlying mechanisms in placebo effects altering emotion and pain alike. More recently, Schienle and colleagues (20) demonstrated reduced feelings of disgust paralleled by reduced insular activation when participants thought they took a herbal drug against disgust symptoms. In a related manner, findings from research on reversal learning show placebo- and nocebo-like effects on emotion processing. For instance, threat responses following the presentation of previously established conditioned threat cues (CS+), which were paired with aversive electrical stimuli, are reduced, if participants receive a verbal instruction that the cue is no longer indicative of danger (21). Similarly, although the presentation of emotional facial expressions reliably evokes positive or negative affective responses in an observer, verbal instruction about potential danger being indicated by a certain face category leads to defensive responding irrespective of face valence (e.g., happy or fearful faces announcing an aversive outcome) (12). These results nicely demonstrate that emotional responses can be shaped top–down by cognitive representations of superordinate functions.

Interestingly, placebo and nocebo effects often come along with emotional responses, such as anticipatory anxiety (nocebo) or positive feelings of relief and reward (placebo), which—to some degree—might mediate the modulation of (pain) symptoms (2224). For instance, Aslaksen and colleagues showed that a nocebo instruction suggesting hyperalgesic effects caused by an applied cream led to a pain increase, which was meditated by subjective and physiological indices of stress (25). However, when applying a mere conditioning procedure without explicit placebo or nocebo instructions, the role of negative affect might be less relevant (26). Just recently, Geers and colleagues found that the experimental induction of positive mood by watching a pleasant movie clip was capable to block a pain increase by a verbal nocebo suggestion (27). Despite all these findings, so far, little research explored the interaction of emotions on the one hand and placebo/nocebo manipulations on the other when modulating pain.

In the present study, we aimed at investigating whether the genuine pain-modulating effect of unpleasant affective pictures is sensitive to a placebo or nocebo manipulation. To this end, we compared two groups of participants who received either a placebo or nocebo manipulation related to unpleasant pictures. The nocebo group was instructed that watching unpleasant pictures leads to an increased perception of pain in line with findings from the literature (nocebo instruction), and during a later conditioning procedure, they actually experienced relatively more intense pain stimuli when watching the “nocebo” pictures. The placebo group was told the exact opposite, namely, that unpleasant pictures cause a decreased perception of pain. Thereafter, participants experienced relatively less intense pain stimuli when watching the “placebo” pictures. In addition to pain reports, we measured the electroencephalogram (EEG) that allowed us to analyze neurophysiological correlates of pain perception (N1 and P2 component of the SEP as mentioned earlier) and processing of the emotional pictures by means of visually evoked potentials (28). One component of the visually evoked potential following the presentation of emotional relevant stimuli is the late positive potential (LPP)—a positive signal deflection most prominent at centro-parietal electrode sites—which was found to be a sensitive measure for emotional intensity (arousal) of presented pictures (2931).

We hypothesized that unpleasant picture stimuli generally increase pain processing; however, this effect is modulated by reinforced expectations induced by a placebo/nocebo manipulation. Specifically, we expect that a placebo manipulation (verbal instruction + placebo conditioning) reduces or even reverses the pain-augmenting effect of unpleasant pictures. This might lead to lower pain ratings and SEPs for unpleasant compared with neutral pictures. Further, the placebo manipulation might become evident also in altered neurophysiological correlates of unpleasant affective pictures processing, namely, by a lack of LPP modulation or even higher amplitudes for neutral compared with unpleasant pictures.

Materials and Methods

Sample

Forty-two participants were recruited from the University of Würzburg and received course credit or €20 as compensation. Two participants needed to be excluded due technical problems during data acquisition, leaving 40 participants in the final analysis, 20 participants in the nocebo group (10 females) and 20 participants in the placebo group (10 females). All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, reported no current or prior history of chronic pain, neurological or psychiatric disorders (self-report), and did not take any analgesic medication prior to the experiment. Participants first read detailed instructions about the experiment and signed the informed consent before taking part in the experiment. Participants filled out questionnaires on current positive and negative affect (Positive Affect/Negative Affect Schedule) (32), on state and trait anxiety (State/Trait Anxiety Inventory) (33), on pain catastrophizing (Pain Catastrophizing Scale) (34), on sensitivity for pain (35), on dispositional optimism and pessimism (Life Orientation Test—Revised) (36), and on anxiety of pain related symptoms (Pain Anxiety Symptom Scale) (37). Questionnaire scores of both groups were similar except for state anxiety, which was higher in the placebo group (see Table 1). All procedures were approved by the institutional review board of the medical faculty of the University of Würzburg.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Sample Characteristics.

Visual Stimuli

Participants watched 40 emotional pictures (twice), which were drawn from the International Affective Pictures System (38), comprising 20 neutral (International Affective Pictures System catalog numbers: 2095, 3170, 3180, 3230, 3261, 3500, 3530, 6212, 6256, 9040, 9050, 9163, 9250, 9300, 9321, 9413, 9419, 9901, 9921, and 9925) and 20 unpleasant pictures (2038, 2191, 2383, 2393, 2396, 2514, 2595, 2749, 2850, 2870, 2880, 5390, 5731, 5870, 7002, 7100, 7130, 7493, 7550, and 7590). Pictures were presented for 6 s interleaved by a central fixation cross present for 2–3 s (randomized). Picture order was randomized with the restriction of no more than two consecutive pictures of the same valence. Visual stimuli were projected centrally on a screen of 2 × 3.22 m (Powerwall), at 2.0-m distance from the participant’s chair.

Electrical Pain Stimulation

Electrical pain stimuli were delivered on the left calf of the participants via a surface bar electrode with two stainless steel disk electrodes (8-mm diameter, 30-mm spacing), using a constant-current stimulator (Digitimer DS7A, Digitimer Ltd., Welwyn Garden City, UK). The intensity of the electrical stimulus was adjusted to the participants’ individual pain threshold. During thresholding, participants were asked to rate electrical stimuli of two ascending and two descending series starting from 0 mA applying steps of ±0.5 mA, respectively, on a 11-point scale ranging from 0 (no pain at all) to 10 (unbearable pain). Stimulus intensities rated with a 4 (just noticeable pain) were averaged, and 1 mA was added to the final stimulus intensity to reassure a moderate pain level. The final stimulation intensity was again rated on a 10-point scale (see Table 1). During the experiment, two different stimulation intensities were used, which varied with regard to the number of consecutive single pulses (train length). Low intense stimuli consisted of three square pulses (pulse length 2 ms) and an inter-pulse interval of 4 ms, high intense stimuli instead consisted of 10 square pulses. During the test phase and the threshold procedure, high intense stimuli were delivered, and during the conditioning phase, both low and high intense stimuli were used; see the procedure section for further details.

Electroencephalogram Recording and Evoked Potentials

Electrophysiological data were recorded from 32 active electrodes (ActiCap; Brain Products, Munich, Germany) with a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz, placed according to the international 10–20 system (C3, C4, CP1, CP2, CP5, CP6, Cz, F3, F4, F7, F8, FC1, FC2, FC5, FC6, Fp1, Fp2, Fz, O1, O2, Oz, P3, P4, P7, P8, Pz, T7, T8, TP10). FCz was used as online reference, and data were off-line re-referenced to an average reference. Vertical (above and below the left eye) and horizontal (at the outer canthi of both eyes) electrooculogram was recorded. Electrode impedance was kept below 5 k Ohm, and the online band-pass filter was set to 0.01 to 250 Hz. Data were collected using a Brain-Amp-MR amplifier (Brain Products) and the software Brain Vision Recorder Version 1.05 together with ActiCap Control Software (Brain Products). Off-line EEG analysis was performed using Brain Vision Analyzer Version 2.1 (Brain Products). EEG was filtered (0.1–30 Hz) and corrected for horizontal and vertical ocular artifacts (39). Trials exceeding a transition threshold of 50 µV (sample to sample) or an amplitude criterion of ±100 µV were excluded from further analysis. For the analysis of the picture evoked LPP, epochs registered 100 ms before to 2,000 ms after picture onset were extracted and baseline corrected with reference to the mean baseline interval (100 ms before picture onset). The LPP was scored at the parietal electrode Pz and quantified as mean activity from 700- to 1,000-ms post picture onset, according to visual inspection of the scalp topographies and the literature (28, 31). For the analysis of the SEP following electrical stimulation, epochs registered 100 ms before to 1,000 ms after electrical stimulation (first pulse) were extracted, baseline-corrected, and averaged analog to the procedure of the LPP. Two components of the SEP were analyzed, that is, the N1 and P2, which were scored as mean activity at the Cz electrode in a time window from 75 to 125 ms and 200 to 330 ms, respectively (3, 8). For statistical analysis, all event-related potential components were averaged per participant across all artifact-free picture and pain epochs of the conditioning and test phase, respectively.

Pain Ratings

After each electrical stimulation, pain intensity and unpleasantness ratings were obtained using a digital visual analog scale. Ratings were converted off-line to values between 0 and 100. The scale for pain intensity ratings was labeled “not painful at all” at the left end and “extremely painful” at the right end of the scale, and for pain unpleasantness, the scale ranged from “not unpleasant at all” to “extremely unpleasant.”

Procedure

After arrival, participants were assigned to one of the two experimental groups (nocebo vs. placebo)—taking into account the participants’ gender—following an a priori randomization performed by the experimenter. According to the respective experimental condition, participants were instructed that during the experiment, they would watch a series of unpleasant and neutral pictures, which—in line with recent findings in the literature—very likely would change their perception of concurrently administered painful electrical stimuli. The nocebo group was told that unpleasant pictures would increase the perception of pain, while neutral pictures had no influence on pain at all. The placebo group instead was told that unpleasant pictures would result in a decreased perception of pain compared with neutral pictures, which would leave the perception of pain unchanged. Participants were seated 2.0 m in front of the screen and started the experiment. Unbeknownst to them, the experiment consisted of two parts, the conditioning phase, which was followed without interruption by the test phase. During conditioning, participants of the nocebo group watched neutral pictures and received the low-intensity pain stimuli and unpleasant pictures paired with high-intensity pain stimuli. This association was reversed for participants of the placebo group; here, participants were administered the low-intensity pain stimuli during unpleasant and the high-intensity stimuli during neutral picture presentation. Following the logic of previous placebo manipulation, this procedure should reassure the participants that the instruction they were given in the beginning of the experiment actually hold true and pain perception was modulated accordingly. During the test phase, participants of both groups always received the same, high-intensity pain stimulation, combined with neutral and unpleasant pictures (see Figure 1). After each trial, participants rated the electrical stimulus for pain intensity and unpleasantness. In total, participants completed 80 trials, which is 20 repetitions of unpleasant and neutral pictures per phase. In the end, participants filled out a post experimental survey asking how they evaluate the effect of unpleasant and neutral pictures on pain using a 9-point scale ranging from +4 (very pain increasing) to 0 (no effect on pain) to -4 (very pain reducing). Stimulus presentation was controlled by the software Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA, USA).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Participants were either instructed that watching unpleasant affective pictures would increase pain (nocebo group), or the exact opposite, that watching unpleasant pictures would decrease pain (placebo group) relative to neutral pictures. Afterwards, participants underwent placebo/nocebo conditioning, where unpleasant and neutral pictures were paired with either high- or low-intensity electrical pain stimuli, in line with the placebo or nocebo instruction provided previously. In the following test phase, participants watched the placebo or nocebo and neutral pictures again but received always high-intensity pain stimuli.

Statistical Analysis

Pain ratings (pain unpleasantness, pain intensity) and amplitudes of the SEP components (N1 and P2) were analyzed separately for the conditioning and the test phase. During the conditioning phase, a 2 × 2 repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) including the within-subjects factors Stimulation Level (high vs. low intensity stimulation, irrespective of picture category) and the between-subjects factor Group (nocebo vs. placebo) was applied. During the test phase, pain responses following identical stimulation intensities were analyzed using the within- subjects factor Emotion (unpleasant vs. neutral pictures) and the between-subjects factor Group. LPPs were analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA including the within-subjects factor Emotion (unpleasant vs. neutral pictures), Phase (conditioning vs. test phase) to capture potential changes across the time course of the experiment, and the between-subjects factor Group. Significant interaction was explored using follow-up ANOVAs. The significance level was set to .05 (two-tailed); for follow-up ANOVAs, a corrected alpha of p < .025 was considered. As a measure of effect size, we report partial η². Normal distribution of the analyzed data can be assumed for 93% of the variables (Shapiro–Wilk’s tests), due to the robustness of the repeated measures ANOVA against violations of data normality (40); its usage seems appropriate in the present case.

Results

Pain Ratings—Conditioning Phase

Analysis of pain intensity ratings revealed a significant main effect of Stimulation LevelF(1, 38) = 152.54, p < .001, ηp² = .80, as a result of higher pain ratings following more intense electrical stimulation. The interaction of Stimulation Level × Group was only marginally significant, F(1, 38) = 3.49, p = .07, ηp² = .08, presumably indicating a more pronounced differentiation between the two stimulation intensity for the placebo group. The factor Group was significant, F(1, 38) = 9.36, p = .004, ηp² = .20, due to higher pain ratings in the placebo compared with the nocebo group (M = 46.54 vs. M = 30.64), see Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Mean pain intensity (left) and unpleasantness (right) ratings (+SEM) in the conditioning phase separately for stimulus intensity (high vs. low) and experimental group (nocebo vs. placebo). All within group comparisons and the between factor were significant (p < .05).

Analysis of pain unpleasantness ratings returned a similar picture, participants clearly differentiated between the two different pain stimuli as indicated by the significant main effect of Stimulation Level, F(1, 38) = 113.96, p < .001, ηp² = .75, however the interaction of Group x Stimulation was not significant, F(1, 38) = 0.01, p = .99, ηp² < .01. Again, participants in the placebo group reported higher pain in general, F(1, 38) = 10.72, p = .002, ηp² = .22, (M = 47.32 vs. M = 31.12), see Figure 2.

Pain Ratings—Test Phase

Analysis of pain intensity ratings revealed a marginal significant main effect of EmotionF(1, 38) = 3.66, p = .06, ηp² = .09, which was further qualified by a significant interaction of Emotion × Group, F(1, 38) = 11.72, p < .001, ηp² = .24. Participants in the placebo group rated pain during neutral pictures significantly higher than during unpleasant pictures, F(1, 19) = 11.97, p = .003, ηp² = .39, while the same comparison failed significance in the nocebo group, F(1, 19) = 1.41, p = .25, ηp² = .07. The factor Group was also significant, F(1, 38) = 6.94, p = .01, ηp² = .15, resulting from generally higher pain ratings in the placebo group (M = 50.17 vs. M = 35.07), see Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Mean pain intensity (left) and unpleasantness (right) ratings (+SEM) in the test phase separately for picture category (neutral vs. unpleasant) and experimental group. All within group comparisons—except for pain intensity ratings of the nocebo group—and the between factor were significant (p < .05).

Analysis of pain unpleasantness ratings showed no main effect of Emotion F(1, 38) = 0.46, p = .50, ηp² = .01; however, the interaction of Emotion × Group was significant, F(1, 38) = 31.67, p < .001, ηp² = .45. Separate ANOVAs for each group revealed a significant main effect of Emotion for both the nocebo F(1, 19) = 22.10, p < .001, ηp² = .54 and the placebo groups, F(1, 19) = 11.13, p = .003, ηp² = .37. However, while participants in the nocebo group rated pain stimuli higher during unpleasant compared with neutral pictures (M = 40.92, vs. M = 31.17), participants in the placebo group showed the exact opposite pattern, namely, higher pain unpleasantness ratings while seeing neutral (M = 58.15) compared with unpleasant pictures (M = 50.50). Again, the placebo group showed generally higher pain unpleasantness ratings compared to the nocebo group, F(1, 38) = 11.89, p = .001, ηp² = .24, (M = 54.33 vs. M = 36.45), see Figure 3.

Somatosensory Evoked Potentials—Conditioning Phase

As expected, during the conditioning phase, the physically more intense pain stimuli resulted in elevated SEP amplitudes. This was true for the early N1 component, F(1, 38) = 73.14, p < .001, ηp² = .66, and the subsequent P2, F(1, 38) = 19.11, p < .001, ηp² = .34. For both components, neither the interaction [N1, F(1, 38) = 0.70, p = .41, ηp² = .02; P2, F(1, 38) = 0.91, p = .35, ηp² = .02] nor the factor Group reached significance [N1, F(1, 38) = 1.14, p = .29, ηp² = .03; P2, F(1, 38) = 0.43, p = .52, ηp² = .01], see Figure 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 The SEPs at the Cz electrode elicited by electrical pain stimuli during the conditioning phase. In the nocebo group (left), unpleasant pictures (red line) were paired with high-intensity pain stimuli, and in the placebo group, unpleasant pictures (green line) were paired with low-intensity pain stimuli. The gray lines represent neutral pictures, combined with either high- or low-intensity stimuli. The N1 (mean activity 75–125 ms) and the P2 (200–330 ms) components were significantly increased for high- compared with low-intensity pain stimuli in both experimental groups. All within group comparisons p < .05.

Somatosensory Evoked Potentials—Test Phase

Analysis of N1 amplitudes during the test phase—when pain stimuli had always the same intensity—revealed neither a significant main effect of Emotion F(1, 38) = 0.01, p = .93, ηp² < .01 nor a significant interaction F(1, 38) = 0.47, p = .50, ηp² = .01. The between factor was marginally significant, F(1, 38) = 3.27, p = .08, ηp² = .08, likely due to more pronounced amplitudes in the placebo (M = -6.33) compared with nocebo group (M = -3.71). The P2 component similarly revealed no significant effect of Emotion F(1, 38) = 2.75, p = .11, ηp² < .07; however, the interaction of Group × Emotion was significant, F(1, 38) = 7.44, p = .01, ηp² < .16. Separate ANOVAs for each group showed a significant main effect of Emotion only for the placebo group, F(1, 19) = 6.64, p = .02, ηp² = .26, due to higher mean amplitudes following neutral (M = 14.02) compared with unpleasant pictures (M = 11.89). The same analysis returned a nonsignificant main effect of Emotion F(1, 19) = 1.04, p = .32, ηp² = .05 for the nocebo group¸ see Figure 5.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5 (A) The SEPs at the Cz electrode elicited by electrical pain stimuli during the test phase. The N1 (75–125 ms) component showed no modulation by picture category or across groups. The P2 (200–330 ms) component instead was significantly decreased for electrical stimuli paired with unpleasant pictures in the placebo group only. (B) Scalp topography 220–300 ms for the difference of neutral and unpleasant pictures, separately for each group.

Visually Evoked Potentials During Conditioning and Test Phases

The 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA for the analysis of the visually evoked LPPs revealed a significant main effect Phase F(1, 38) = 15.50, p = .001, ηp² = .29, which was the result of higher LPP amplitudes during the test compared with the conditioning phase (M = 1.68 vs. M = 3.00). Furthermore, the significant main effect of EmotionF(1, 38) - 6.79, p = .01, ηp² = .15, was further qualified by a close to significant two-way interaction of Emotion × Group, F(1, 38) - 3.95, p = .054, ηp² = .09. Follow-up ANOVAs separately for each group revealed a significant main effect of Emotion F(1, 19) - 13.39, p = .002, ηp² = .41 for the nocebo group, which is the result of elevated LPP amplitudes for unpleasant compared with neutral pictures. Interestingly, for the placebo group, the factor Emotion was far from being significant, F(1, 19) - 0.16, p = .70, ηp² < .01, see Figure 6. No other main effect or interaction reached significance, all ps > .22.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6 (A) The LPPs at the Pz electrode averaged across conditioning and test phases, separately for neutral and unpleasant pictures and split by experimental group. The LPP between 700 and 1,000 ms (marked time window) was higher for unpleasant compared with neutral pictures in the nocebo group only (p = .002). (B) Scalp topography 700–1,000 ms for the difference of unpleasant and neutral pictures, separately for each group.

Discussion

In the current study, we addressed the question whether a placebo/nocebo manipulation does alter the pain-enhancing effect of emotions elicited by unpleasant picture stimuli, and if so, neurophysiological correlates of emotions processing were changed accordingly. Results demonstrate lower pain ratings for unpleasant pictures introduced as placebo compared with neutral control pictures. Further, in the placebo group only, negative pictures led to reduced P2 amplitudes of the SEP. In the nocebo group, in line with classical findings, unpleasant pictures led to more pronounced LPP amplitudes than neutral pictures. In the placebo group instead, pleasant (placebo) and neutral (control) pictures led to similar neurophysiological responses, suggesting that the placebo manipulation already affected the processing of the emotional stimuli and, consequently, processing of the pain stimuli.

Pain Modulation by Pictures Indicating Placebo Hypoalgesia or Nocebo Hyperalgesia

Pain ratings and neurophysiological pain responses during the conditioning phase of the experiment demonstrated a clear differentiation between the two stimulus intensities, which suggests a successful manipulation of the participants’ actual experience in line with the idea of reinforced expectations often used in placebo/nocebo designs (16, 18, 41). The high-intensity stimuli were rated as more painful and unpleasant and evoked larger amplitudes of the N1 and P2 components of the SEP in both groups. Regarding the pain intensity ratings, the difference between neutral and unpleasant pictures tended to be even stronger in the placebo group, which is a first hint for a critical role of top–down-driven expectations rather than invariant effects of emotions on pain: following the concept of motivational priming (5, 42), one might have expected the pain-increasing effect of unpleasant pictures to be enhanced in the nocebo group. Instead, the placebo manipulation led to an even more pronounced differentiation, suggesting a more prominent role of the reinforced expectations than of the unpleasant pictures. The generally elevated pain ratings in the placebo group might to some degree be also the consequence of the experimental manipulation, but see the limitation section for further discussion.

The results from the test phase demonstrate even more clearly the interplay of our placebo/nocebo manipulation and the modulation of pain by emotions. Participants of the nocebo group demonstrated the well-known pain-augmenting effect of unpleasant pictures (43). The placebo group, however, reveals a completely reversed pattern. Here, the unpleasant pictures, introduced as having a pain-easing effect, led to significantly reduced pain intensity and unpleasantness ratings—of physically identical pain stimuli—compared with the neutral pictures. This indicates that the placebo expectation in combination with the conditioning procedure was able to reverse the pain-increasing effect of negative emotions, reflecting an efficient top–down control of pain processing.

There is a long-standing debate regarding the role of expectancy and learning, i.e., conditioning underlying the formation of placebo effects [see for instance (44, 16)], and some even question whether placebo effects per se are anything but conditioning effects and suggest to drop the concept in general (45). With regard to our present results, it is hard to tell whether the instruction in the beginning of the experiment or the placebo acquisition phase contributed to greater extent to the final placebo effect. A previous study by our group showed that in case of so to say psychologically mediated placebo/nocebo agents, both aspects are crucial. It might be interesting to test whether the present findings would replicate if only a conditioning procedure or placebo instruction was applied. Although research on expectancy effects on psychological pain modulation is rather sparse, the effect of placebo and nocebo expectations was repeatedly shown for pharmacological pain interventions. For instance, it was demonstrated that the same dosage of pain medication is more effective if it is administered in a so-called open fashion—that is, a patient is well aware of receiving a medication, which generates a robust expectation for analgesia—in contrast to a hidden application without explicit knowledge of the patient (46). On the contrary, a nocebo expectation is capable to abolish the effectiveness of a highly potent analgesic medication (47). Accordingly, the present findings suggest that the same might be true if the pain-modifying mechanism at question is based on psychological processes, here emotion-based pain modulation.

Placebo Expectations and Emotion Processing

Analysis of the LPPs, elicited by the picture stimuli, showed that participants in the nocebo group clearly differentiated between neutral and unpleasant pictures in line with previous studies on neurophysiological correlates of affective picture processing, which demonstrated a preferential processing of threatening stimuli (30, 31, 48, 49). However, the placebo group failed to exhibit discriminative LPPs for neutral versus unpleasant pictures, which might be due to an integration of emotional picture content and their alleged effect on pain. We suppose that the placebo manipulation changed the functional representation of the unpleasant pictures, since according to the instruction, those were now indicative for a positive outcome, which likely rendered them as less threatening. In accordance with this interpretation, Bradley and colleagues found that physiological responses following the presentation of emotional pictures change if picture valence—positive vs. negative—operates as a cue for threat vs. safety, respectively (50). Threat cues, provoked stronger physiological defense reactions, irrespective of the emotional picture content. In a similar paradigm where positive and unpleasant pictures alternatingly served as threat or safety cues, analysis of the LPPs demonstrated elevated amplitudes for pictures indicating potential danger (51). Altogether, these findings suggest that affective picture processing and emotional responding is susceptible to a top–down-driven modulation of motivational/functional significance. These results are further in line with a finding from research on emotion regulation, demonstrating that changing the meaning of an emotional relevant scene for instance by applying an alternative interpretation (reappraisal) leads to altered subjective and neurophysiological responses following picture processing (30, 52, 53).

Neurophysiological Pain Responses While Watching Pictures Indicating Placebo or Nocebo

SEPs during the test phase demonstrated no modulation of the early N1 component, neither in the nocebo group nor in the placebo group. However, previous studies found a significant modulation of the N1 solely for the comparison of unpleasant with positive pictures (3, 8). Accordingly, the contrast between neutral and unpleasant pictures was likely not strong enough, which might explain the lacking N1 modulation, especially in the nocebo group. Similarly, studies on placebo effects measuring neurophysiological responses to short laser beams found no modulation of early components of the LEP (54, 55). The P2 component instead was modulated by the picture category, but only in the placebo group, such that unpleasant compared with neutral (control) pictures led to a significantly reduced amplitude. This is in line with earlier findings demonstrating that emotional compared with neutral pictures reduce the P2 following electrical stimuli (3, 8). Furthermore, studies investigating placebo effects on LEPs found that a placebo manipulation reduced the P2 component or N2/P2 complex, respectively (54, 55). Given that participants in the placebo group showed little differentiation between the picture categories as indicated by similar LPP amplitudes, this might demonstrate an interference of emotion processing by the placebo manipulation. We conclude that the reduction of the P2 component likely is driven more strongly by a placebo effect than by the arousing content of the pictures. In the nocebo group, however, the instructed pain-augmenting effect of unpleasant pictures did not provoke any conflict between picture content (negative) and functional significance (negative). Here, in line with previous studies on nocebo-like cueing effects reporting elevated LEPs (56), the experimental manipulation probably led to an increase of the P2 component during unpleasant picture presentation, which compensated the expected P2 decrease by high-arousing pictures found previously. Yet, the nocebo effect apparently was not strong enough to produce a significant potentiation of the P2 by unpleasant nocebo pictures, exceeding the responses following the neutral control pictures.

Limitations

Although the ratio of female and male subjects was equal within and across groups, due to the small total sample, a moderation of the reported findings by the participants’ gender cannot be excluded. Future studies should incorporate larger sample sizes to explore gender effects in more detail and to control for the sometimes-high variability in placebo and nocebo designs. Furthermore, even though the experimental groups varied only very little with regard to the individual pain threshold and later on administered pain stimuli, participants of the placebo group reported higher pain intensity and unpleasantness ratings, in general, despite similar SEPs amplitudes. Results of the post-experimental ratings—where participants of the placebo group indicated a relative pain-increasing effect of neutral pictures compared with the nocebo group—might be suggestive for an overall overestimation of pain in the placebo group, leading to elevated pain ratings, see Table 1. However, evidence for this interpretation is inconclusive and might be corroborated in future studies, obtaining measures of the participant’s expectation already in the beginning of the experiment. In a similar vein, we decided against trial-by-trial affective ratings of the emotional picture stimuli. This might have been informative with regard to the findings from the visual evoked potentials but, at the same time, led to excessive length of the whole experiment. Future studies should complement physiological affective responses by subjective measures of emotion and expand the stimulus set by a positive valence category. With regard to state affect, participants in the nocebo group presented somewhat higher anxiety scores, which may result from the nocebo instruction. The difference in state anxiety might have influenced the present findings; however, mean scores of both groups indicate very moderate levels of state anxiety. Lastly, the bar electrode used in the present design might have led to muscle contraction artifacts contaminating SEP findings. Given the very similar stimulation intensities between groups, artifacts might not explain group differences. The problem of potential artifacts could be addressed in futures studies for instance by using ring electrodes (57).

Conclusion and Outlook

The present study demonstrated an interaction of emotions and reinforced expectations on pain processing. We showed that a placebo manipulation (verbal instruction + placebo conditioning) is able to modulate and even reverse the genuine pain-increasing effect of unpleasant pictures. We assume that the placebo manipulation altered the processing of emotional pictures themselves, such that unpleasant pictures, expected to exert a positive effect on pain, were perceived as less arousing. This interpretation is in line with previous research demonstrating the modulatory influence of threat manipulations on physiological correlates of emotion processing (51, 58). These findings underline the important role of higher order expectations on pain processing and the effectiveness of psychological placebo effects as shown previously (18).

These processes deserve further explorations in future studies, investigating the interaction of placebo/nocebo expectations with other well-established emotional and cognitive factors impacting pain. For instance, it might be worthwhile to investigate whether a nocebo expectation of, e.g., pain exacerbation caused by highly demanding cognitive tasks, actually hampers the pain decrease following manipulations of attention allocation (59, 60). The same might be true for the modulation of pain by emotion regulation strategies such as reappraisal or suppression (61). A placebo vs. nocebo manipulation suggesting high vs. low effectiveness of pain regulation might block or even potentiate its pain-modifying capacities.

Data Availability

Data is available upon request.

Ethics Statement

All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the institutional review board of the medical faculty of the University of Würzburg.

Author Contributions

PR and MW designed the study. CM collected the data. PR analyzed the data. PR, MW, CM, and PP wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) to project number 44541416-TRR 58 (B01), and to the Research Group ‘’Emotion and Behavior,’’ FOR 605, Wi2714/3-2. This publication was funded by DFG and the University of Würzburg in the funding programme “Open Access Publishing.”

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to LHY for valuable inspirations during the preparation of the manuscript.

References

1. Bushnell M. C.Ceko M.Low L. A. (2013). Cognitive and emotional control of pain and its disruption in chronic pain. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci, 14(7), 502–11. doi: 10.1038/nrn3516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Reicherts P., Gerdes A. B., Pauli P., Wieser M. J. (2013). On the mutual effects of pain and emotion: Facial pain expressions enhance pain perception and vice versa are perceived as more arousing when feeling pain. Pain, 154(6), 793–800. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kenntner-Mabiala R, Pauli P. Affective modulation of brain potentials to painful and nonpainful stimuli. Psychophysiology (2005) 42(5):559–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00310.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Reicherts P, Gerdes AB, Pauli P, Wieser MJ. On the mutual effects of pain and emotion: Facial pain expressions enhance pain perception and vice versa are perceived as more arousing when feeling pain. Pain (2013) doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.02.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rhudy JL. Emotional Modulation of Pain. In: Neuroscience of Pain, Stress, and Emotion. Elsevier: Amsterdam. (2016). p. 51–75. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-800538-5.00003-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Rhudy JL, Williams AE, McCabe KM, Russell JL, Maynard LJ. Emotional control of nociceptive reactions (ECON): Do affective valence and arousal play a role? Pain (2008) 136(3):250–61. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.06.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bromm B, Lorenz J. Neurophysiological evaluation of pain. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1998) 107(4):227–53. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00075-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Kenntner-Mabiala R, Andreatta M, Wieser MJ, Mühlberger A, Pauli P. Distinct effects of attention and affect on pain perception and somatosensory evoked potentials. Biol Psychol (2008) 78(1):114–22. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Wiech K, Lin CS, Brodersen KH, Bingel U, Ploner M, Tracey I. Anterior insula integrates information about salience into perceptual decisions about pain J Neurosci (2010) 30(48):16324–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2087-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Wiech K, Ploner M, Tracey I. Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends Cogn Sci (2008) 12(8):306–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Colloca L, Klinger R, Flor H, Bingel U. Placebo analgesia: psychological and neurobiological mechanisms. Pain (2013) 154(4):511–4. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Enck P, Bingel U, Schedlowski M, Rief W. The placebo response in medicine: minimize, maximize or personalize? Nat Rev Drug Discov (2013) 12(3):191–204. doi: 10.1038/nrd3923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Benedetti F, Pollo A, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Vighetti S, Rainero I. Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. J Neurosci (2003) 23(10):4315–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04315.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Brascher A. K., Witthoft M., Becker S. (2018). The underestimated significance of conditioning in Placebo Hypoalgesia and Nocebo Hyperalgesia. Pain Res Manag 2018:6841985. doi: 10.1155/2018/6841985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Nocebo hyperalgesia: how anxiety is turned into pain. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol (2007) 20(5):435–9. doi: 10.1097/ACO.0b013e3282b972fb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Colloca L, Miller FG. How placebo responses are formed: a learning perspective. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci (2011) 366(1572):1859–69. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Voudouris NJ, Peck CL, Coleman G. The role of conditioning and verbal expectancy in the placebo response. Pain (1990) 43(1):121–8. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(90)90057-K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Reicherts P, Gerdes ABM, Pauli P, Wieser MJ. Psychological placebo and nocebo effects on pain rely on expectation and previous experience. J Pain (2016) 17(2):203–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Petrovic P, Dietrich T, Fransson P, Andersson J, Carlsson K, Ingvar M. Placebo in emotional processing– induced expectations of anxiety relief activate a generalized modulatory network. Neuron (2005) 46(6):957–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Schienle A, Uebel S, Schongassner F, Ille R, Scharmuller W. Disgust regulation via placebo: an fMRI study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (2014) 9(7):985–90. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Costa VD, Bradley MM, Lang PJ. From threat to safety: instructed reversal of defensive reactions. Psychophysiology (2015) 52(3):325–32. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Vighetti S, Asteggiano G. The biochemical and neuroendocrine bases of the hyperalgesic nocebo effect. J of Neurosci (2006) 26(46):12014–22. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2947-06.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Flaten MA, Aslaksen PM, Lyby PS, Bjørkedal E. The relation of emotions to placebo responses. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci (2011) 366(1572):1818–27. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta J-K. Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2008) 65(2):220–31. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Aslaksen PM, Zwarg ML, Eilertsen HI, Gorecka MM, Bjorkedal E. Opposite effects of the same drug: reversal of topical analgesia by nocebo information. Pain (2015) 156(1):39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.0000000000000004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Babel P, Bajcar EA, Adamczyk W, Kicman P, Lisinska N, Swider K, et al. Classical conditioning without verbal suggestions elicits placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia. PLoS One (2017) 12(7):e0181856. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Geers AL, Close S, Caplandies FC, Vogel CL, Murray AB, Pertiwi Y, Handley IM, Vase L. Testing a positive affect induction to reduce verbally induced nocebo hyperalgesia in an experimental pain paradigm. Pain. 2019. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Olofsson JK, Nordin S, Sequeira H, Polich J. Affective picture processing: An integrative review of ERP findings. Biol Psychol (2008) 77(3):247–265. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Cuthbert BN, Schupp HT, Bradley MM, Birbaumer N, Lang PJ. Brain potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biol Psychol (2000) 52(2):95–111. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00044-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Hajcak G, MacNamara A, Olvet DM. Event-Related Potentials, Emotion, and Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review. Dev Neuropsychol (2010) 35(2):129–55. doi: 10.1080/87565640903526504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Schupp HT, Cuthbert BN, Bradley MM, Cacioppo JT, Ito T, Lang PJ. Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology (2000) 37(2):257–61. doi: 10.1017/S0048577200001530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Krohne HW, Egloff B, Kohlmann CW, Tausch A. Investigations with a German version of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Diagnostica (1996) 42(2):139–56. doi: 10.1037/t49650-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD. Das State-Trait-Angstinventar. Weinheim: Beltz Test (1981).

Google Scholar

34. Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess (1995) 7(4):524–32. doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.7.4.524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ruscheweyh R, Marziniak M, Stumpenhorst F, Reinholz J, Knecht S. Pain sensitivity can be assessed by self-rating: Development and validation of the Pain Sensitivity Questionnaire. Pain (2009) 146(1-2):65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Glaesmer H, Hoyer J, Klotsche J, Herzberg PY. The German version of the Life-Orientation-Test (LOT-R) for dispositional optimism and pessimism. Z Gesundheitspsychol. (2008) 16(1):26–31. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Walter B, Hampe D, Wild J, Vaitl D. Die Erfassung der Angst vor Schmerzen: eine modifizierte deutsche Version der Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale(PASS–D) [Assessment of anxiety of pain: a modified German version of the Pain and Anxiety Symptom Scale (PASS–D). Schmerz (2002) 15 (suppl1):83

Google Scholar

38. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida (2008).

Google Scholar

39. Gratton G, Coles MG, Donchin E. A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1983) 55(4):468–84. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Blanca MJ, Alarcon R, Arnau J, Bono R, Bendayan R. Non-normal data: is ANOVA still a valid option? Psicothema (2017) 29(4):552–7. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2016.383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Atlas LY, Wager TD. How expectations shape pain. Neurosci Letters (2012) 520(2):140–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Lang PJ. The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. Am Psychol (1995) 50(5):372–85. doi: 10.1037//0003-066X.50.5.372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Rhudy JL, Bartley EJ, Williams AE, McCabe KM, Chandler MC, Russell JL, et al. Are there sex differences in affective modulation of spinal nociception and pain? J Pain (2010) 11(12):1429–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Stewart-Williams S, Podd J. The placebo effect: Dissolving the expectancy versus conditioning debate. Psychol Bull (2004) 130(2):324–40. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. De Houwer J. A functional-cognitive perspective on the relation between conditioning and placebo research. Int Rev Neurobiol (2018) 138:95–111. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2018.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Colloca L, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Benedetti F. Overt versus covert treatment for pain, anxiety, and Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol (2004) 3(11):679–84. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00908-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Bingel U, Wanigasekera V, Wiech K, Ni Mhuircheartaigh R, Lee MC, Ploner M, et al. The effect of treatment expectation on drug efficacy: imaging the analgesic benefit of the opioid remifentanil. Sci Transl Med (2011) 3(70):70ra14. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3001244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Amrhein C, Mühlberger A, Pauli P, Wiedemann G. Modulation of event-related brain potentials during affective picture processing: a complement to startle reflex and skin conductance response? Int J of Psychophysiol (2004) 54(3):231–40. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Reicherts P, Wieser MJ, Gerdes ABM, Likowski KU, Weyers P, Muhlberge A, et al. Electrocortical evidence for preferential processing of dynamic pain expressions compared to other emotional expressions. Pain (2012) 153(9):1959–64. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Bradley MM, Moulder B, Lang PJ. When good things go bad: the reflex physiology of defense. Psychol Sci (2005) 16(6):468–73. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01558.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bublatzky F, Schupp HT. Pictures cueing threat: brain dynamics in viewing explicitly instructed danger cues. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (2012) 7(6):611–22. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Moser JS, Hajcak G, Bukay E, Simons RF. Intentional modulation of emotional responding to unpleasant pictures: an ERP study. Psychophysiology (2006) 43(3):292–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00402.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Thiruchselvam R, Blechert J, Sheppes G, Rydstrom A, Gross JJ. The temporal dynamics of emotion regulation: an EEG study of distraction and reappraisal. Biol Psychol (2011) 87(1):84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Colloca L, Tinazzi M, Recchia S, Le Pera D, Fiaschi A, Benedetti F, et al. Learning potentiates neurophysiological and behavioral placebo analgesic responses. Pain (2008) 139(2):306–14. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Wager TD, Matre D, Casey KL. Placebo effects in laser-evoked pain potentials. Brain Behav Immun (2006) 20(3):219–30. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2006.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Lorenz J, Hauck M, Paur RC, Nakamura Y, Zimmermann R, Bromm B, et al. Cortical correlates of false expectations during pain intensity judgments—a possible manifestation of placebo/nocebo cognitions. Brain Behav Immun (2005) 19(4):283–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2005.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Mouraux A, Iannetti GD, Plaghki L. Low intensity intra-epidermal electrical stimulation can activate Adelta-nociceptors selectively. Pain (2010) 150(1):199–207. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.04.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Bublatzky F, Guerra P, Alpers GW. Verbal instructions override the meaning of facial expressions. Sci Rep (2018) 8(1):14988. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33269-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Buhle, Stevens BL, Friedman JJ, Wager TD. Distraction and Placebo. Psychol Sci (2012) 23(3):246–53. doi: 10.1177/0956797611427919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Buhle JT, Wager TD. Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task. Pain (2010) 149(1):19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hampton AJD, Hadjistavropoulos T, Gagnon MM, Williams J, Clark D. The effects of emotion regulation strategies on the pain experience: a structured laboratory investigation. Pain (2015) 156(5):868–79. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: placebo and nocebo effects, emotion processing, psychological pain modulation, late positive potential, somatosensory evoked potential

Citation: Reicherts P, Pauli P, Mösler C and Wieser MJ (2019) Placebo Manipulations Reverse Pain Potentiation by Unpleasant Affective Stimuli. Front. Psychiatry 10:663. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00663

Received: 26 February 2019; Accepted: 16 August 2019;
Published: 24 September 2019.

Edited by:

Katja Weimer, University of Ulm, Germany

Reviewed by:

Per M. Aslaksen, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Norway
Florian Bublatzky, Central Institute for Mental Health, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Reicherts, Pauli, Mösler and Wieser. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Philipp Reicherts, cGhpbGlwcC5yZWljaGVydHNAdW5pLXd1ZXJ6YnVyZy5kZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.