Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychiatry, 05 July 2019
Sec. Psychopathology

Revisiting Imagery in Psychopathology: Why Mechanisms Are Important

  • School of Psychology, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland

Humans have an exceptional capacity to mentally simulate sensory information of stimuli that are not physically present. This high-level cognitive ability—known as mental imagery—is a multisensory process that exploits internal representations of perception and/or action in working memory (1). Imagery mechanisms are fundamental to the computational functioning of the brain, facilitating predictive processing (2) and guiding future behavior (3). If imagined prospective possibilities are grounded in appropriate mental representations, successful behavior may ensue (3), but if not, psychopathological tendencies may arise (e.g., suicidal “flashforwards”) (4). Whether used explicitly (consciously and voluntarily) or implicitly (unconsciously and/or involuntarily), imagery can simulate (absent) sensorimotor information to improve memory and enhance skills (5). However, when imagery is intrusive or uncontrolled, it may signal psychopathology. Involuntary and/or distressing imagery plays a ubiquitous role in the etiology and maintenance of many mental disorders (6) such as OCD (7), PTSD (8), and suicidality (4). Dysfunctional imagery also contributes to maladaptive interpersonal patterns [e.g., Ref. (9)] and reduced capacity for a positive future-orientated outlook (10).

Although considerable progress has been made in understanding imagery processes in healthy populations, less is known about these in the clinical domain. Specifically, the mechanisms driving the development, maintenance, and/or treatment of dysfunctional imagery in psychopathology remain largely unresolved (11). So the present paper explains why research on imagery mechanisms is important in understanding certain clinical conditions. Drawing on research on motor imagery (MI), we highlight imagery-related mechanisms involved in clinical disorders with motor components (e.g., compulsions in OCD). It is beyond our scope to investigate imagery content in clinical disorders. Instead, we focus on plausible mechanisms underlying imagery processes in psychopathology. We begin by outlining key characteristics of MI before discussing possible mechanisms driving intrusive images in certain clinical conditions. Implications for treatment are also considered.

Advances in the Science of Motor Imagery (MI)

MI is a dynamic mental process that involves the performance of actions in the mind while inhibiting motor output (12). It is characteristically accompanied by subjective visual and/or kinesthetic sensations—one “sees” and/or “feels” one’s body move (13). So patients’ MI experiences involve a realness that often results in more emotional arousal than would be experienced otherwise (14). Further, MI is associated with psychophysiological responses (15), which suggest active processing of motor representations. Heightened motorsensory, psychophysiological, and emotional information during MI may mask conceptual processing of events and increase the likelihood and frequency of intrusive imagery (16). Interestingly, psychophysiological responses can provide an adjunct measure of symptom severity in PTSD (17). The systematic use of MI is associated with changes at neural and behavioral levels (18). Such findings highlight a potential mechanism by which dysfunctional imagery might be maintained. If negative imagery continues over time, neural motor systems will be shaped accordingly and associated maladjusted behaviors strengthened. Finally, first-person kinesthetic MI (i.e., where we feel our own body perform the action) activates primary motor cortical networks and facilitates corticospinal excitability (19). The presence of such neural activity during MI strengthens a sense of body ownership and controlled agency (20). Accordingly, this imagery perspective may empower patients by providing them with a sense of control over their bodily experiences.

Motor simulation theory (MST; 12, 21) postulates that MI is functionally equivalent to the physical execution of action because the two movement types share brain systems and functional mechanisms [see Ref. (22)]. During MI, similar motor representations to those involved during action execution are activated, and a putative simulation mechanism enables offline/cognitive performance of the represented action (12). Thus, MI primes the motor system in anticipation of acting. The key difference between MI and executed action is that the former must employ inhibitory mechanisms to prevent overt movement (21). This theory is supported by evidence that MI and actual action rely on similar neuroanatomical structures and activate overlapping neural networks [see Ref. (23)]. However, the overlap is not perfect (24), and MI activates additional ventral neural circuits linking the prefrontal and parietal cortices—thought to facilitate conscious cognitive control (25). Cognitive control is important in imagery-related psychopathology particularly given the experienced realness of images (10). Without such control, patients may be susceptible to relapse. Imagery likely involves several levels of cognition from low-level unconscious simulation to high-level conscious MI and even meta-cognitive awareness. Interestingly, research demonstrates that compulsions or irresistible urges to act (as observed in OCD) are linked to impaired attentional capacity (7). Furthermore, interventions (e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy) that increase neural connectivity in the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices (networks crucial for inhibitory control) are associated with greater resistance to compulsion (26). Accordingly, if used appropriately and with patients with good imagery ability, imagery-related treatments may strengthen activity in networks mediating conscious cognitive control.

Progress has also been made in understanding the specific cognitive systems supporting and constraining MI (22). Attentional systems are important during MI because images are formed and consciously manipulated in working memory. Supervisory attentional mechanisms bias the selection of specific goal-compatible action schemata held in long-term memory, integrating the various action components involved (12). Attentional systems also help to guide imagined actions towards completion, supervising lower-level (fast and automatic) cognitive simulation processes that anticipate sensory consequences of the action (12). Importantly, meta-cognitive processes (i.e., people’s knowledge and awareness of, and control over their own cognitive processes) (27) may supervise this entire process, ensuring control over the imagined action and disengagement of the simulation mechanism. So meta-cognitive/imagery processes may represent a valuable target for future imagery-related interventions because they may facilitate the cognitive control required to manage intrusive images [e.g., Ref. (28)]. Furthermore, meta-awareness of momentary attentional bias towards emotional information leads to greater regulation of subsequent behavior. By contrast, inadequate meta-awareness may prolong dysregulated attentional behavior (29). Another cognitive process underpinning MI is inhibition. Although MI and executed action share functional mechanisms, MI must engage inhibition to prevent overt movement (21). This inhibition can be implemented with minimal attentional effort at the action intention stage (19, 30) or by actively maintaining a balance between the production and inhibition of motor commands (31). The extent of attentional effort involved in inhibition has clinical relevance as it may alter the outcomes of the treatment; the frequency of, and/or control over, imaginal intrusions; or the patient’s experience of the treatment itself (32). Future research could fruitfully investigate the role of inhibitory processes in imagery-related disorder intervention. Recent studies show that “practice” at inhibiting intrusions through repeated experience of suppressing trauma may reduce the impact of unwanted memories through enhanced inhibitory control (33). Such practice seems particularly important in OCD, which is characterized by an overwhelming urge to perform compulsive acts (7). In summary, MI mechanisms involve attention, working memory, and long-term memory. The next section will relate such neurocognitive mechanisms of MI to intrusive imagery in clinical disorders.

MI and Intrusive Negative Images

Intrusive imagery is an undesirable and involuntary experience of negative and/or distressing images invading conscious awareness (34). These images are typically persistent, detailed, and dynamic and involve not only visual imagery but also motor and autonomic arousal elements, many of which are unconscious (35). Intrusive imagery occurs across a range of clinical disorders, including depression and suicidality (11), OCD (36), and PTSD (8). Psychotherapeutic treatment for these difficulties often involves some form of imaginal exposure and/or imagery rescripting, often as part of cognitive behavioral regimes (37). Imagery rescripting involves altering toxic memories/representations to create, via imagery, an alternative (more beneficial) mental representation of an event (38). Although the vividness and/or frequency of intrusive imagery may decline after such treatment (39, 40), the mechanisms producing such change remain unclear (33, 37). This section will attempt to address this issue. Specifically, what is the role of MI in generating and driving (motor-related) intrusive imagery in clinical disorders and the positive treatment outcomes observed following imagery rescripting interventions (41)?

According to MST, MI primes the motor system in anticipation of action execution (21). Although MI is underpinned by many cognitive systems, it is the fast and unconscious simulation that seems to be its key mechanism (21). So intrusive negative images might take hold when conscious control is weakened. As continued priming of neural motor systems may induce brain plasticity and reorganization (18), MI may contribute to the maintenance of imagery-related psychopathology. Therefore, early intervention for clinical disorders with imagery dysfunction may be important (34). If imagery-related neural plasticity occurs to a similar extent in imagined as in physical execution of action, then where actions are inappropriate (e.g., in OCD), MI might offer a viable route to altering maladaptive behavioral patterns. For example, habitual compulsive acts “ensure the persistence of obsessions” in OCD (p. 49, 7). So MI training might assist in the control, alteration, or diversion of compulsive behavior via inhibitory processes and attentional control (28). The attentional mechanisms supporting motor simulation/imagery are also relevant to imagery rescripting interventions because during rescripting they may help to create and bias a competitive retrieval preference for a new, less harmful (motor) memory of a traumatic event (38). Regarding higher-level control, research indicates that most individuals have a capacity for metacognitive processing (29). Interventions focusing on meta-cognition in mental health treatments is not new (28), and some, for example, in the interpersonal therapy domain [e.g., Ref. (42)], have demonstrated improvements in life quality. However, consideration of meta-cognitive processes in imagery-related intervention has largely been neglected to date [e.g., Ref. (32)]. So far, imagery interventions like rescripting (43) have largely focused on altering the meaning of events and associated images/memories (40). However, as previously suggested, they might also have a potentially powerful influence on altering the (uncontrolled) overt and covert behaviors of patients. Although re-scripting may help to relieve the initial distress associated with negative images, its long-term use requires a deeper understanding of relevant imagery mechanisms. Overall, MI is a complex psychological construct, supported and constrained by several cognitive processes, and thus, its association with imagery dysfunction and clinical disorder requires nuanced exploration and application at both content and function levels.

Concluding Comments

This article highlights recent theoretical progress in imagery research and identifies some possible MI mechanisms driving intrusive negative imagery experiences in psychopathology. MST provides a potentially valuable theoretical basis for investigating, understanding, and treating maladapted imagery (with motor components) across clinical disorders. The simulation mechanism driving MI—and the higher-order cognitive systems that support it—represent potential key targets for future intervention. Very often, imaginal exposure therapy and re-experiencing are associated with distress and high dropout rates, and preferred imagery techniques tend to involve imagery rescripting (39). Therefore, developing enhanced rescripting interventions that take into consideration some of the underlying imagery mechanisms outlined above (attentional, inhibitory, and meta-cognitive processes) offer promising future directions. In summary, the present opinion paper aims to encourage dialogue and promote knowledge transfer between cognitive psychological/neuroscientific and clinical domains.

Author Contributions

Both authors contributed equally to this article.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Keogh R, Pearson J. The perceptual and phenomenal capacity of mental imagery. Cognition (2017) 162:124–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bubic A, von Cramon DY, Schubotz RI. Prediction, cognition and the brain. Front Hum Neurosci (2010) 4(25):1–15. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Seligman MEP, Railton P, Baumeister RF, Sripada C. Homo prospectus. New York: Oxford University Press (2016).

Google Scholar

4. Van Bentum JS, Sijbrandij M, Huibers MJH, Huisman A, Arntz A, Holmes EA, et al. Treatment of intrusive suicidal imagery using eye movements. Int J Environ Res Public Health (2017) 14(7):E714. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14070714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Di Rienzo F, Debarnot U, Daligault S, Saruco E, Delpuech C, Doyon J, et al. Online and offline performance gains following motor imagery practice: a comprehensive review of behavioural and neuroimaging 160 studies. Front Hum Neurosci (2016) 10:315. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hackmann A, Bennett-Levy J, Holmes EA. Oxford guide to imagery in cognitive therapy. New York: Oxford University Press (2011). doi: 10.1093/med:psych/9780199234028.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fineberg N, Apergis-Schoute A, Vaghi M, Banca P, Gillan C, Voon V, et al. Mapping compulsivity in the DSM-5 obsessive compulsive and related disorders: cognitive domains, neural circuitry, and treatment. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol (2018) 21(1):42–58. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyx088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Brewin CR, Gregory JD, Lipton M, Burgess N. Intrusive images in psychological disorders: characteristics, neural mechanisms, and treatment implications. Psychol Rev (2010) 117(1):210–32. doi: 10.1037/a0018113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Dimaggio G, Montano A, Popolo R, Salvatore G. Metacognitive interpersonal therapy for personality disorders: a treatment manual. New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis (2015). doi: 10.4324/9781315744124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Blackwell SE. Mental imagery: from basic research to clinical practice. J Psychother Integr (2018). doi: 10.1037/int0000108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Holmes EA, Blackwell SE, Heyes SB, Renner F, Raes F. Mental imagery in depression: phenomenology, potential mechanisms, and treatment implications. Annu Rev Clin Psychol (2017) 12:249–80. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-092925

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Jeannerod M. Motor cognition: what actions tell the self. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2006). doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198569657.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Moran A, Guillot A, MacIntyre T, Collet C. Re-imagining motor imagery: building bridges between cognitive neuroscience and sport psychology. Br J Psychol (2012) 103(2):224–47. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02068.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Mathews A, Ridgeway V, Holmes EA. Feels like the real thing: imagery is both more realistic and emotional than verbal thought. Cogn Emot (2013) 27(2):217–29. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.698252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Papadelis C, Kourtidou-Papadeli C, Bamidis P, Albani M. Effects of imagery training on cognitive performance and use of physiological measures as an assessment tool of mental effort. Brain Cogn (2007) 64(1):74–85. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ng RMK, Krans J, Holmes EA. Mental imagery and psychopathology: Examples of post-traumatic stress disorder and bipolar disorder. In: Lacey S, Lawson R, editors. Multisensory imagery. Springer Science and Business Media (2013). p. 365–84. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5879-1_19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Castro-Chapman PL, Orr SP, Berg J, Pineles SL, Yanson J, Salomon K. Heart rate reactivity to trauma-related imagery as a measure of PTSD symptom severity: examining a new cohort of veterans. Psychiatry Res (2018) 261:574–80. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Debarnot U, Sperduti M, Di Rienzo F, Guillot A. Experts bodies, experts minds: how physical and mental training shape the brain. Front Hum Neurosci (2014) 8:280. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Lebon F, Ruffino C, Greenhouse I, Labruna L, Ivry RB, Papaxanthis C. The neural specificity of movement preparation during actual and imagined movements. Cereb Cortex (2019) 29(2):689–700. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Fossataro C, Bruno V, Giurgola S, Bolognini N, Garbarini F. Losing my hand. Eur J Neurosci (2018) 48(6):2272–87. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Jeannerod M. Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage (2001) 14(1):S103–S109. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. O’Shea H, Moran A. Does motor simulation theory explain the cognitive mechanisms underlying motor imagery? A critical review. Front Hum Neurosci (2017) 11:7. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00072

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Hardwick RM, Caspers S, Eickhoff SB, Swinnen SP. Neural correlates of action: comparing meta-analyses of imagery, observation, and execution. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2018) 94:31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Zabicki A, De Haas B, Zentgraf K, Stark R, Munzert J, Krüger B. Imagined and executed actions in the human motor system: testing neural similarity between execution and imagery of actions with a multivariate approach. Cereb Cortex (2017) 27(9):4523–36. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Vry M, Saur D, Rijntjes M, Umarova R, Kellmeyer P, Schnell S, et al. Ventral and dorsal fiber systems for imagined and executed movement. Exp Brain Res (2012) 219(2):203–16. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3079-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Moody TD, Morfini F, Cheng G, Sheen C, Tadayonnejad R, Reggente N, et al. Mechanisms of cognitive-behavioral therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder involve robust and extensive increases in brain network connectivity. Transl Psychiatry (2017) 7(9):e1230. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Keulers EHH, Birkisdóttir MB, Falbo L, de Bruin A, Stiers PLJ. Age-related differences in task-induced brain activation is not task specific: multivariate pattern generalization between metacognition, cognition and perception. Neuroimage (2019) 188:309–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wells A, Colbear JS. Treating posttraumatic stress disorder with metacognitive therapy: a preliminary controlled trial. J Clin Psychol (2012) 68(4):373–81. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Ruimi L, Hadash Y, Zvielli A, Amir I, Goldstein P, Bernstein A. Meta-awareness of dysregulated emotional attention. Clin Psychol Sci (2018) 6(5):658–70. doi: 10.1177/2167702618776948

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. O’Shea H, Moran A. To go or not to go: pupillometry elucidates inhibitory mechanisms in motor imagery. J Cogn Psychol (2018) 30(4):466–83. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2018.1461104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Scheil J, Liefooghe B. Motor command inhibition and the representation of response mode during motor imagery. Acta Psychol (2018) 186:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Siegesleitner M, Strohm M, Wittekind CE, Ehring T, Kunze AE. Effects of imagery rescripting on consolidated memories of an aversive film. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry (2019) 62:22–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hulbert JC, Anderson MC. What doesn’t kill you makes you stronger: psychological trauma and its relationship to enhanced memory control. J Exp Psychol Gen (2018) 147(12):1931–49. doi: 10.1037/xge0000461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Iyadurai L, Visser RM, Lau-Zhu A, Porcheret K, Horsch A, Holmes EA, et al. Intrusive memories of trauma: a target for research bridging cognitive science and its clinical application. Clin Psychol Rev (2019) 69:67–82. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Hellawell SJ, Brewin CR. A comparison of flashbacks and ordinary autobiographical memories of trauma: cognitive resources and behavioural observations. Behav Res Ther (2002) 40(10):1143–56. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00080-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Moritz S, Purdon C, Jelinek L, Chiang B, Hauschildt M. If it is absurd, then why do you do it? The richer the obsessional experience, the more compelling the compulsion. Clin Psychol Psychother (2017) 25(2):210–6. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Schnyder U, Ehlers A, Elbert T, Foa EB, Gersons BPR, Resick PA, et al. Psychotherapies for PTSD: What do they have in common? Eur J Psychotraumatol (2015) 6(1). doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v6.28186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Wheatley J, Hackmann A. Using imagery rescripting to treat major depression: theory and practice. Cogn Behav Pract (2011) 18(4):444–53. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2010.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Morina N, Lancee J, Arntz A. Imagery rescripting as a clinical intervention for aversive memories: a meta-analysis. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry (2017) 55:6–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Arntz A. Imagery rescripting as a therapeutic technique: review of clinical trials, basic studies, and research agenda. J Exp Psychopathol (2012) 3(2):189–208. doi: 10.5127/jep.024211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Hagenaars MA, Arntz A. Reduced intrusion development after post-trauma imagery rescripting: an experimental study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry (2012) 43:808–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Gordon-King K, Schweitzer RD, Dimaggio G. Metacognitive interpersonal therapy for personality disorders: the case of a man with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder and avoidant personality disorder. J Contemp Psychother (2019) 49:39–47. doi: 10.1007/s10879-018-9404-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Smucker MR, Dancu C, Foa EB, Niederee JL. Imagery rescripting: a new treatment for survivors of childhood sexual abuse suffering from posttraumatic stress. J Cogn Psychother (1995) 9:3–17. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.9.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: motor imagery, intrusive imagery, obsessive compulsive disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, motor simulation theory

Citation: O’Shea H and Moran A (2019) Revisiting Imagery in Psychopathology: Why Mechanisms Are Important. Front. Psychiatry 10:457. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00457

Received: 27 March 2019; Accepted: 11 June 2019;
Published: 05 July 2019.

Edited by:

Éric Laurent, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, France

Reviewed by:

Giancarlo Dimaggio, Centro di Terapia Metacognitiva Interpersonale (CTMI), Italy

Copyright © 2019 O’Shea and Moran. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Helen O’Shea, aGVsZW4uby1zaGVhQHVjZGNvbm5lY3QuaWU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.