Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 28 September 2018
Sec. Addictive Disorders

Parkinson's Disease: Impulsivity Does Not Cause Impulse Control Disorders but Boosts Their Severity

\r\nJuan Marín-Lahoz,,,Juan Marín-Lahoz1,2,3,4Javier Pagonabarraga,,*&#x;Javier Pagonabarraga1,2,3*Saul Martinez-Horta,,Saul Martinez-Horta1,2,3Ramn Fernandez de Bobadilla,Ramón Fernandez de Bobadilla5,6Berta Pascual-Sedano,,,Berta Pascual-Sedano1,2,3,6Jesús Prez-Prez,,Jesús Pérez-Pérez1,2,3Alexandre Gironell,,Alexandre Gironell1,2,3Jaime Kulisevsky,,,*&#x;Jaime Kulisevsky1,2,3,6*
  • 1Movement Disorders Unit, Neurology Department, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Biomedical Research Institute (IIB-Sant Pau), Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red sobre Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas (CIBERNED), Madrid, Spain
  • 4Institute de Neurociencies Universitat Autonoma de Barcelona, Cerdanyola del Vallès, Spain
  • 5Centro Neurológico de Atención Integral, Navarra, Spain
  • 6Estudios de Ciencias de la Salud, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, Barcelona, Spain

Introduction: Impulse control disorders (ICDs) are a common complication of Parkinson's disease (PD) receiving dopamine agonist (DAA) Impulsivity is considered an underlying mechanism but evidence of this relationship is scarce. To explore the relationship between impulsivity and the presence and severity of ICD in PD.

Methods: Prospective cross-sectional study of consecutive PD outpatients. Patients with dementia or previously known ICDs were excluded. Two measures of impulsivity were assessed: Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) for impulsiveness trait (main exposure) and commission errors in the Continuous Performance Test (CE) for motor inhibition. Main outcomes were diagnosis of ICD based on a comprehensive clinical interview and severity of ICD based on the Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders.

Results: Of 100 patients (mean [SD] age, 67.2 [8.8], 54 male), 31 had ICD. Patients with ICDs were 5.3 years younger (p = 0.01), used more frequently dopamine agonist (p = 0.02), alcohol (p = 0.009) and tobacco (p = 0.02). They were not more impulsive on BIS-11 (56 vs. 58, p = 0.23, adjusted p = 0.46) and CE (p = 0.96). No relationship was found between dopaminergic medications and impulsivity or ICD severity. Among patients with ICD, impulsivity was correlated with ICD severity (BIS-11 r = 0.33, p = 0.001, adjusted p = 0.002, CE r = 0.53, p = 0.006). Multivariate regression analysis confirmed the independent predictive role of both measures.

Conclusions: Impulsivity is not associated with increased prevalence of ICD in PD but it is strongly linked to ICD severity. When considering dopamine replacement therapy, assessment of impulsivity may be a useful approach to detect those patients at risk of severe forms of ICD.

Introduction

Impulse control disorders (ICDs) are a common neuropsychiatric complication of Parkinson's disease (PD). By definition, ICDs refer to pathological behaviors characterized by failure to resist an impulse, drive or temptation to perform an act that is harmful. Usually, the affected individual experiences pleasure, gratification, or relief at the time of committing the act (1). Common ICDs in PD include dysfunctional behaviors related to gambling, sex, food intake, shopping, and hobbies. In the context of PD, these and other ICD-related behaviors are increasingly regarded as behavioral addictions (24).

ICDs are uncommon in the general population and in untreated PD patients (57). Among PD treatments, dopamine agonists (DAA) are strongly associated with ICDs (5). Patients receiving DAA for a disorder other than PD also have a high risk of ICDs (8, 9). However, other risk factors are important for their development and phenomenology as most patients taking DAA will not develop ICDs, and the best clinical-genetic predictive models for the development of ICD symptoms explains only part of the risk (4).

Not only ICD frequency is worrisome, but also for their range of severity. Severity may vary from extremely disruptive addictions causing bankruptcy, divorce, or even criminal prosecution (10, 11) to mild addictive symptoms—usually related to increased creativity or productivity—that may be even perceived as positive for patients' functionality (12, 13). It is also worth noting that 13–39% of patients with ICDs do not experience improvement or remission of the addictions after dopamine agonists withdrawal (3, 1416). It is therefore important not only to study risk factors for the development of ICDs, but to identify the variables responsible for different prognosis and severity.

Impulsivity is a psychological construct characterized by poor control of thoughts and actions with a propensity to react fast over urges and environmental demands despite potential negative consequences. The definition of impulsivity shares obvious aspects with that of ICDs, and impulsivity is usually considered to have a causal relation with ICDs in PD. Higher scores on the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11), a self-reported questionnaire with semi-quantitative responses, have been observed in PD patients with and without ICDs (17, 18). Other modes of impulsivity, such as deficit of motor inhibition, have also been studied (18). Yet, studies on the influence of impulsivity for the development and clinical manifestations of ICDs in PD are scarce (17, 19).

In the present study, we aimed to explore the relation between impulsivity and frequency and severity of ICDs in PD. We assessed impulsivity and ICDs in PD patients who were taking dopaminergic drugs. Only incident cases of ICDs were included to avoid confounding factors such as changes in dopaminergic medication and a bias toward chronic ICDs. If impulsivity was a true risk factor for ICDs in PD we would expect higher levels of impulsivity in patients with ICDs.

Materials and Methods

Consecutive PD outpatients followed at the Movement Disorders Unit at Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau were invited to participate. Inclusion criteria were idiopathic PD, active treatment with a dopaminergic agent and last follow up at the same center including ICDs evaluation within the last 6 months. Exclusion criteria were: any other neurological condition, history of brain surgery, dementia according to Movement Disorder Society PD-dementia criteria (20), inability to perform the proposed tasks, use of dopamine antagonists, unstable medical or psychiatric conditions (depression and anxiety under effective and stable treatment were not excluded), and presence of ICD in the previous follow up. Excluding patients with previous ICDs was chosen to get measures of the exposures as close to ICD inception as possible and to avoid bias generated by medication changes and chronicity.

Patients were informed about the study during follow-up visits. If they agreed to participate in the study, they returned for the study evaluation. Patients were not excluded if ICDs were suspected or diagnosed the day they were informed about the study. In such cases, no changes in medication were made before the study protocol was completed (always within a week). All the participants were evaluated by a neurologist trained in movement disorders and a neuropsychologist experienced in PD. All the patients gave written informed consent and the study protocol was approved by the clinical research ethics committee at Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau. All the study was conducted according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. All the evaluations were performed on medication.

ICDs Diagnosis and Rating

The presence of ICDs was assessed through a comprehensive clinical interview. ICDs were considered present when the related behavior was dysfunctional according to the components of addiction proposed by Brown (21) and modified by Griffiths (22). This model considers six components that comprise addiction regardless of the involvement of drug use: salience, mood modification, tolerance, withdrawal symptoms, conflict, and relapse. At least 4 of the 6 components needed to be present to consider a behavior as ICD. To diagnose a patient with more than one ICD, each ICD had to be unrelated to the others and considered dysfunctional.

To obtain a semiquantitative measure of severity in patients with a diagnosis of ICD, we used the short version of the questionnaire for impulsive-compulsive disorders in PD (QUIPs) (23) (score range 0–13), the Minnesota Impulsive Disorders Interview (MIDI) (24) (score range 0–56), and the number of different ICDs. The number of ICDs was based on the clinical interview. ICD diagnosis and QUIPs score were considered main outcomes and the other ICD related variables were considered exploratory. The investigator rating the main outcomes was blinded to impulsivity measures.

Impulsivity Evaluation

Impulsivity was evaluated using two different approaches, the BIS-11 and the PEBL Continuous Performance Test (PCPT). The BIS-11 was designed to assess the personality trait of impulsiveness (25). It has 30 self-reported items grouped in three subtraits: motor, attentional, and nonplanning. We used BIS-11 as a subjective estimator for impulsivity. The PCPT is a continuous performance task programmed in the Psychology Experiment Building Language (26) based on the Conners Continuous Performance Task II (27). The PCPT was designed to assess motor inhibition and sustained attention. Participants have to press a key in response to any capital letter (except X) that appears on the screen. At the same time, they must refrain from responding to lures (any X that appears). Targets are much more common than lures, creating a tendency to respond to lures, an inhibition failure. Commission error rate (CE)—failure to avoid responding to lures—was used as an indirect but objective measure of impulsivity (28). The investigator rating impulsivity measures was blinded to patient outcomes (presence and severity of ICDs).

PD-Related Variables

We recorded the motor part of Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS-III) (29), Hoehn and Yahr stage (H&Y), age at PD onset, PD duration, and current medical treatment. We calculated the Levodopa equivalent daily dose (LEDD) and the amount of LEDD corresponding to dopamine agonists (agonist-LEDD) according to previously reported conversion factors (30). We also recorded personal use of legal drugs (caffeine, nicotine and alcohol), use of illegal drugs and both personal and family history of alcohol and illegal drugs use disorders.

Data Analysis

Patients with and without ICD (ICD+/ ICD-) were compared. The chi-squared test was used for discrete variables. When this was inappropriate, the Fischer exact test was used. For most quantitative variables we used mean and Student's t-test and for those which did not comply with parametric assumptions we used median and Mann–Whitney U-test. We used Pearson correlations in ICD+ patients to analyze the relationship between ICD severity and other variables. Linear regression was used to assess whether impulsivity independently explained ICD severity and to control for motor state (UPDRS III) when comparing medication doses between groups. Logistic regression was used to assed which variables were independently associated with ICD diagnosis. The level of significance was set as p < 0.05, 2-sided. Main objectives were the comparison of BIS-11 score between ICD+ and ICD- and the correlation between BIS-11 with severity among ICD+ (measured by QUIPs). Multiple comparison adjustment was performed for these two tests by Bonferroni (using the number of tests performed for impulsivity and ICD diagnosis and for impulsivity and ICD severity). The other statistical analysis were considered exploratory and significance was not adjusted. Confidence intervals (CI) are reported at 95% level. All the statistical analyses were conducted using R version 3.1.3 (31).

Results

ICD Frequency

One hundred consecutive PD patients (54 male, age mean ± SD 67 ± 9, education 11 ± 5, age at PD diagnosis 61 ± 9) were recruited and ICDs were diagnosed in 31. Thirty-eight patients had a positive score in QUIPs and 19 in MIDI.

The behaviors causing ICDs were hobbism/punding (n = 15), binge-eating (n = 14), pathological hypersexuality (n = 5), and compulsive shopping (n = 5). Hobbism/punding behaviors were tidying (n = 6), board games (n = 3), social networking (n = 3), repairing (n = 2), computer assisted edition (n = 2), and one case each of compulsive reading, doll handcraft, dancing, and walk-about. Eleven patients had several unrelated ICDs. None had pathological gambling or dopamine dysregulation syndrome.

ICD+ patients were 5.25 years younger at the time of the study (CI 1.24–9.25, p = 0.01) and at PD onset (p = 0.017). They were also more frequently receiving treatment with a DAA (59.4 vs. 83.9%, CI 4.7–44.1%, p = 0.02). No differences were found regarding time since PD diagnosis, Hoehn & Yahr stage, or UPDRS III status (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Clinical and behavioral description of the sample.

Most patients in the sample were taking DAA: 41 used pramipexole, 11 ropinirole, and 15 rotigotine (among ICD+ patients 16, 4 and 6, respectively). No patients used more than one DAA. Average agonist-LEDD was higher in ICD+ patients (p = 0.012) but this difference was due to the higher proportion of DAA use in the ICD+ group (59.4% among ICD-, 83.9% among ICD+; chi-square test p = 0.02). Among patients taking DAA, no differences were found in agonist-LEDD between ICD+ and ICD- groups (p = 0.17). No difference was found after adjusting for UPDRS III (p = 0.22). Levodopa dose was similar in both groups (p = 0.35) and remained similar after adjusting for UPDRS III (p = 0.07). MAO-B inhibitors and amantadine use did not differ between groups (p = 0.96 and p = 0.22, respectively). LEDD showed a trend toward significance, with higher doses in ICD+ patients (p = 0.08). However, after controlling for UPDRS III this difference became clearly significant (p = 0.008), indicating that for a comparable degree of motor severity, ICD+ patients were taking higher LEDD.

No differences between ICD- and ICD+ groups were found either in BIS-11 (56.35 vs. 58.33, unadjusted p = 0.23, adjusted p = 0.46,) or in CE (p = 0.96) (Table 1). Neither BIS-11 nor CE, were related to LEDD, DAA use or agonist-LEDD (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation matrix of impulsivity and dopaminergic medication.

None of the patients had begun to consume legal or illegal drugs after PD diagnosis. No association was found between the current amount of alcohol intake and ICDs (p = 0.46). However, current alcohol use was associated to ICD (41.5 vs. 12.9%, OR = 5.42, CI = 1.64 - 23.61, p = 0.003), and patients with no history of alcohol use had a significantly lower prevalence of ICDs (6.7 vs. 34.5%, OR = 0.13, CI = 0.01-0.95, p = 0.03). Most of the participants did not remember the age of first alcohol use and therefore it was not analyzed.

Current tobacco use was associated with ICDs (OR = 5.17, CI = 1.05–34.44, p = 0.02), although < 10% of our sample were current smokers and the average tobacco consumption did not differ between groups (p = 0.25). Lifetime tobacco consumption was more common (52%) but its association with ICDs was not statistically significant (OR = 2.09, CI = 0.81–5.61, p = 0.09). However, patients who had begun smoking at 18 years old or younger were more likely to present ICDs than older first time smokers (OR = 7.18, CI 1.29–76.3, p = 0.01) and ICD+ patients had started smoking 4 years earlier on average (15.5 vs. 19.7 y.o., p < 0.001). Current and previous coffee intake was not related to ICDs (p = 0.73 and p = 0.67, respectively).

No patient was currently using illegal drugs and previous use was not significantly related to ICD diagnosis (OR = 3.26, CI = 0.51–23.87, p = 0.19). No association was found between ICDs and family history of drug or alcohol abuse (OR = 2.48, CI = 0.76–8.02, p = 0.08). Only one of the 18 patients who had never consumed alcohol on a regular basis and were not current smokers had ICDs (Fischer exact test p = 0.01).

Multiple logistic regression showed that only current alcohol consumption and age were independently associated with ICD diagnosis (Supplementary material).

ICD Severity

Among ICD+ we studied correlations patients between impulsivity measures and ICD severity measures. BIS-11 and QUIPs correlated significantly (r = 0.33, unadjusted p = 0.001, adjusted p = 0.002). We also found positive, significant correlations between each impulsivity measure and each severity measure, a correlation matrix is shown in Table 3. However, no correlation was found between BIS-11 and PCPT commission error rate (Pearson's r = 0.03, p = 0.75). Other variables related to ICD frequency were not statistically associated to severity measures (Supplementary Material).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation matrix of impulsivity and ICD severity among ICD+.

We performed multiple linear regression analysis to study whether each impulsivity estimator independently explained QUIPs in ICD+ patients. As the other tested variables were not statistically associated with QUIPs, we included as independent variables the ones that were associated with ICD presence except for age of smoking onset (because 32% of the ICD+ patients had never smoked) and history of alcohol use (because only one ICD+ patient had never used it). QUIPs score was the predicted value. We also used bidirectional stepwise regression to select the predictors. Both impulsivity measures—BIS-11 and commission error rate—significantly predicted QUIPs in the “all in” model. Current smoking was also a significant predictor. These were also the only variables selected by bidirectional stepwise regression (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Multiple linear regression models using QUIPs as the dependent variable among ICD+.

Discussion

The present results show a complex interaction between DAA and impulsivity with the presence and severity of ICDs in PD patients. Contrary to our hypothesis impulsivity was not significantly higher in patients with ICD. However it was associated with higher severity of ICDs. This suggests that the role of impulsivity in ICD presence may be weak or nonexistent but it has an important role regulating ICD severity. The use of DAA, as previously shown, is associated with the presence of ICDs, but not with impulsivity or with ICD severity. Therefore DAA seem to have a critical role in ICD inception but not in their severity.

The fact that LEDD was higher in ICD+ only after controlling for motor severity might indicate that ICDs are related to an imbalance of dopaminergic activity between dorsal and ventral striatum. Higher doses of dopaminergic medication to control motor symptoms could promote overdosing of the ventral striatum (32), but our study is not designed to address this hypothesis.

Other studies have found higher levels of impulsivity in ICD+ patients (17). This discrepancy with our findings may be due to different study designs. Case-control studies select previously diagnosed patients who are more likely to have more severe ICDs (33). Systematic screening of consecutive patients permits to identify less conspicuous but relevant cases. Therefore, our sample, which excluded patients with previously known ICDs, is more likely to be enriched with less severe addictive behaviors. Both approaches are valid and useful to serve different purposes. The selection bias in case-control studies increases the probability to rule out suspected differences because the expected difference between groups is higher. Cross sectional studies such as the present study tend to produce more representative results. Another plausible explanation for the lack of significant differences in impulsivity between ICD+ and ICD- patients is based on the type of ICDs found in our sample. None of our patients had pathological gambling, while other studies analyzing risk factors for ICDs in PD included almost exclusively patients with gambling, a condition known to be highly related to elevated impulsivity (34).

The double dissociation exhibited by impulsivity and dopamine agonist use suggests that dopamine agonist do not cause PD-ICDs by means of fostering impulsivity. Impulsivity promotes the expression of the disorder not restraining the behaviors that constitute it. Conversely, the existence of an impulse able to constitute a disorder, would be caused by the imbalance generated by dopaminergic medication in the reward system (35). Accordingly, impulsive behaviors not generally linked to reward, such as reckless driving or domestic violence are quite rare in PD patients. Nonetheless, a minimum grade of impulsivity may be required as a perfect self-control would preclude any addictive behavior.

As previously reported (5), we found the use of legal drugs are greatly associated to ICDs. Drug use is linked to both reward imbalance and impulsivity, not shedding light on the discussed dissociation. In this sample, alcohol consumption was associated with the presence while tobacco was associated with both presence and severity of ICDs. The use of both drugs precedes ICDs development in this study, therefore they act as risk factors. History of alcohol and tobacco use is easily available information and probably not usually taken into account prior to DAA prescription as it has not been used in predictive models (4).

Our study has several limitations and strengths. The first limitation is that QUIP short has not been properly validated as a measure of ICD severity. However, it is considered sensitive, reliable, easy to answer accurately and able to capture ICD severity (23). A rating scale has been validated for ICD severity, the QUIP rating scale (36). This scale is closely related to QUIP short. To overcome this limitation, MIDI and the number of ICDs have also been studied yielding similar statistical correlations. Second, the design does not probe that impulsivity antecedes ICDs. Prospective studies are necessary to confirm causality. The strengths of our study are: (1) the assessment of two unrelated modes of impulsivity, showing that they were independently related to ICD severity; (2) the use of behavioral addiction criteria to diagnose ICDs, allowing the use of the same criteria independently of the studied behavior; and (3) a sample of systematically interviewed consecutive outpatients, avoiding selection bias.

Conclusions

In summary, we found impulsivity is more associated with severity of ICDs than with ICD diagnosis. Conversely, we found DAA are associated the diagnosis of ICDs but are not associated with their severity. We also show how previous and current use of legal drugs is strongly related to the appearance and severity of ICDs. Further research is needed to evaluate whether impulsivity and legal drug use should be taken into account before prescribing DAA, and whether treatment strategies focused on decreasing impulsivity (37) in PD patients with ICDs would help to control these behaviors.

Author Contributions

JM-L conception, design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, writing, and editing. JP and JK design, data collection, data interpretation, writing, and editing. RF design and data collection. SM-H data interpretation, writing, and editing. BP-S, AG, and JP-P data collection and editing.

Funding

This work was financially supported by CERCA and CIBERNED funding, and grants from la Marató de TV3 (2014/U/477 and 20142910) and Fondo de Investigaciones Sanitarias del Ministerio de Sanidad y Consumo (PI15/00962 and PI14/02058). JM-L is recipient of a Rio Hortega contract. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. None of the authors have any relationship that could be perceived as a conflict of interest.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Carolyn Newey revised and edited English and scientific language.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00465/full#supplementary-material

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic Criteria From DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: The Association (2000).

2. Potenza MN. Non-substance addictive behaviors in the context of DSM-5. Addict Behav. (2014) 39:1–2. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Cilia R, Benfante R, Asselta R, Marabini L, Cereda E, Siri C, et al. Tryptophan hydroxylase type 2 variants modulate severity and outcome of addictive behaviors in Parkinson's disease. Parkins Relat Disord. (2016) 29:96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kraemmer J, Smith K, Weintraub D, Guillemot V, Nalls MA, Cormier-Dequaire F, et al. Clinical-genetic model predicts incident impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2016). 87:1106–11. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2015-312848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Weintraub D, Koester J, Potenza MN, Siderowf AD, Stacy M, Voon V, etal. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a cross-sectional study of 3090 patients. Arch Neurol. (2010) 67:589–95. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Weintraub D, Papay K, Siderowf A. Screening for impulse control symptoms in patients with de novo Parkinson disease A case-control study. Neurology (2013) 80:176–80. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827b915c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Poletti M, Logi C, Lucetti C, Del Dotto P, Baldacci F, Vergallo A, et al. A single-center, cross-sectional prevalence study of impulse control disorders in parkinson disease: association with dopaminergic drugs. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2013) 33:691–4. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e3182979830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Tippmann-Peikert M, Park JG, Boeve BF, Shepard JW, Silber MH. Pathologic gambling in patients with restless legs syndrome treated with dopaminergic agonists. Neurology (2007) 68:301–3. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000252368.25106.b6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Martinkova J, Trejbalova L, Sasikova M, Benetin J, Valkovic P. Impulse control disorders associated with dopaminergic medication in patients with pituitary adenomas. Clin Neuropharmacol. (2011) 34:179–181. doi: 10.1097/WNF.0b013e3182281b2f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Berger C, Mehrhoff FW, Beier KM, Meinck H-M. Sexual delinquency and Parkinson's disease. Nervenarzt (2003) 74:370–5. doi: 10.1007/s00115-003-1505-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

11. Sobrido MJ, Dias-Silva JJ, Quintáns B. Behavioral disorders in Parkinson's disease. Genetic, pharmacological and medico-legal aspects. Rev Neurol. (2009) 48(Suppl 1):S43–48.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

12. Kulisevsky J, Pagonabarraga J, Martinez-Corral M. Changes in artistic style and behaviour in Parkinson's disease: dopamine and creativity. J Neurol. (2009) 256:816–9. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-5001-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Joutsa J, Martikainen K, Kaasinen V. Parallel appearance of compulsive behaviors and artistic creativity in Parkinson's disease. Case Rep Neurol. (2012) 4:77–83. doi: 10.1159/000338759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Mamikonyan E, Siderowf AD, Duda JE, Potenza MN, Horn S, Stern MB, et al. Long-term follow-up of impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2008) 23:75–80. doi: 10.1002/mds.21770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bastiaens J, Dorfman BJ, Christos PJ, Nirenberg MJ. Prospective cohort study of impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2013) 28:327–33. doi: 10.1002/mds.25291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Zhang S, Dissanayaka NN, Dawson A, O'Sullivan JD, Mosley P, Hall W, et al. Management of impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. (2016) 28:1597–614. doi: 10.1017/S104161021600096X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Voon V, Thomsen T, Miyasaki JM, de Souza M, Shafro A, Fox SH, et al. Factors associated with dopaminergic drug–related pathological gambling in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. (2007) 64:212–6. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.2.212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Nombela C, Rittman T, Robbins TW, Rowe JB. Multiple modes of impulsivity in Parkinson's disease. PLoS ONE (2014) 9:e85747. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sáez-Francàs N, Martí Andrés G, Ramírez N, de Fàbregues O, Álvarez-Sabín J, Casas M, et al. Factores clínicos y psicopatológicos asociados a los trastornos del control de impulsos en la enfermedad de Parkinson. Neurología (2015) 31:231–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nrl.2015.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

20. Emre M, Aarsland D, Brown R, Burn DJ, Duyckaerts C, Mizuno Y, et al. Clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2007) 22:1689–707; quiz 1837. doi: 10.1002/mds.21507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Brown RIF. Some contributions of the study of gambling to the study of other addictions. In: Eadington WR, Cornelius JA, editors. Gambling Behavior and Problem Gambling. Reno, NV: Univ of Nevada Pr (1993). pp. 241–72.

22. Griffiths M. A ‘components' model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J Subst Use (2005) 10:191–7. doi: 10.1080/14659890500114359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Weintraub D, Hoops S, Shea JA, Lyons KE, Pahwa R, Driver-Dunckley ED, et al. Validation of the questionnaire for impulsive-compulsive disorders in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2009) 24:1461–7. doi: 10.1002/mds.22571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Grant JE. Impulse Control Disorders: A Clinician's Guide to Understanding and Treating Behavioral Addictions. 1st ed. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company (2008).

25. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. (1995) 51:768–74.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

26. Mueller ST, Piper BJ. The Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) and PEBL Test Battery. J Neurosci Methods (2014) 222:250–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Homack S. Conners' Continuous Performance Test (2nd ed.; CCPT-II). J Atten Disord. (2006) 9:556–8. doi: 10.1177/1087054705283578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Epstein JN, Erkanli A, Conners CK, Klaric J, Costello JE, Angold A. Relations between continuous performance test performance measures and ADHD behaviors. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2003) 31:543–54. doi: 10.1023/A:1025405216339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Fahn S, Elton R. Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD, Calne D, Goldstein R. editors. Recent Developments in Parkinson's Disease. Florham Park, NJ: Macmillan Healthcare Information (1987). pp.153–63.

Google Scholar

30. Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:2649–53. doi: 10.1002/mds.23429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. R Development Core Team (2008). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online at: http://www.R-project.org

32. Aracil-Bolaños I, Strafella AP. Molecular imaging and neural networks in impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. Parkins Relat Disord. (2016) 22:S101–05. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

33. Kopec JA, Esdaile JM. Bias in case-control studies. A review. J Epidemiol Community Health (1990) 44:179.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

34. Cilia R, Cho SS, van Eimeren T, Marotta G, Siri C, Ko JH, et al. Pathological gambling in patients with Parkinson's disease is associated with fronto-striatal disconnection: a path modeling analysis. Mov Disord. (2011) 26:225–33. doi: 10.1002/mds.23480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Riba J, Krämer UM, Heldmann M, Richter S, Münte TF. Dopamine agonist increases risk taking but blunts reward-related brain activity. PLoS ONE (2008) 3:e2479. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Weintraub D, Mamikonyan E, Papay K, Shea JA, Xie SX, Siderowf A. Questionnaire for impulsive-compulsive disorders in Parkinson's Disease-rating scale. Mov Disord. (2012) 27:242–7. doi: 10.1002/mds.24023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kehagia AA, Housden CR, Regenthal R, Barker RA, Muller U, Rowe J, et al. Targeting impulsivity in Parkinson's disease using atomoxetine. Brain (2014) 137:1986–97. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: impulsivity, impulse control disorders, behavioral addictions, Parkinson's disease, severity

Citation: Marín-Lahoz J, Pagonabarraga J, Martinez-Horta S, Fernandez de Bobadilla R, Pascual-Sedano B, Pérez-Pérez J, Gironell A and Kulisevsky J (2018) Parkinson's Disease: Impulsivity Does Not Cause Impulse Control Disorders but Boosts Their Severity. Front. Psychiatry 9:465. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00465

Received: 20 April 2018; Accepted: 06 September 2018;
Published: 28 September 2018.

Edited by:

Alain Dervaux, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire (CHU) de Amiens, France

Reviewed by:

Mauro Ceccanti, Università degli Studi di Roma La Sapienza, Italy
Carla Cannizzaro, Università degli Studi di Palermo, Italy

Copyright © 2018 Marín-Lahoz, Pagonabarraga, Martinez-Horta, Fernandez de Bobadilla, Pascual-Sedano, Pérez-Pérez, Gironell and Kulisevsky. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Javier Pagonabarraga, jpagonabarraga@santpau.cat
Jaime Kulisevsky, jkulisevsky@santpau.cat

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.