Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 05 September 2018
Sec. Mood Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Resilience and Vulnerability Factors in Response to Stress View all 17 articles

Stressful Life Memories Relate to Ruminative Thoughts in Women With Sexual Violence History, Irrespective of PTSD

  • Behavioral and Systems Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Center for Collaborative Neuroscience, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, United States

More than one in every four women in the world experience sexual violence (SV) in their lifetime, most often as teenagers and young adults. These traumatic experiences leave memories in the brain, which are difficult if not impossible to forget. We asked whether women with SV history experience stronger memories of their most stressful life event than women without SV history and if so, whether strength relates to ruminative and trauma-related thoughts. Using the Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire (AMQ), women with SV history (n = 64) reported this memory as especially strong (p < 0.001), remembering more sensory and contextual details, compared to women without SV history (n = 119). They further considered the event a significant part of their personal life story. The strength of the memory was highly correlated with posttraumatic cognitions and ruminative thoughts, as well as symptoms of depression and anxiety (p's < 0.001, n = 183). A third (33%) of the women with SV history were diagnosed with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), but PTSD alone did not account for the increase in memory strength (p's < 0.001). These data suggest that the experience of SV increases the strength of stressful autobiographical memories, which are then reexperienced in everyday life during posttraumatic and ruminative thoughts. We propose that the repeated rehearsal of vivid stressful life memories generates more trauma memories in the brain, making the experience of SV even more difficult to forget.

Introduction

Sexual violence (SV) against women is common in today's world with numbers upward of 25% (1, 2). Most experiences occur during adolescence and young adulthood, when women are most vulnerable (3). The numbers are also high for women in college. Several years ago, the White House Task Force partnered with the Bureau of Justice to survey SV on nine college campuses. Of nearly 15,000 responders, ~10% reported SV during college (4). Other surveys suggest numbers closer to 25% (5, 6). Percentages are even higher (32%) for women in the same age group but not enrolled in college (6).

Memories of an extremely stressful life event tend to be easily recalled and are generally stronger than memories of normal-day experience (79). They also change with time as the memory is rehearsed and/or avoided (10). When a memory is reactivated, it becomes associated with the new context (11). If the context is safe and/or neutral, the strength of the memory may lessen and its expression can extinguish. This process of “extinction” forms the basis of exposure therapy, the most accepted evidence-based intervention for people with trauma history (12). The memory is not erased from the brain but exposure therapy is effective because cues associated with the event are less likely to elicit the conditioned fear response. However, others theorize that rehearsing the trauma memory can strengthen the memory simply because it is being reactivated and reconsolidated over and over again. Because of these concerns, it is important to understand more fully how someone relives the memory of an extremely stressful life event, such as sexual violence. Does the person remember when and where the event took place more or less than the feelings associated with the event? Does he or she tend to remember discrete sensory details such as the sounds and smells or rather is the memory relived like a movie in sequence? Does he or she feel as if traveling back in time or is the memory experienced in the present moment?

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is a mental illness characterized by persistent trauma-related thoughts after an extremely stressful life event (13). Most individuals who experience SV do not go on to develop PTSD but are at high risk. In fact, of all traumas, rape is the most likely to induce PTSD (13, 14). A diagnosis includes some combination of intrusive thoughts, flashbacks, avoidance behaviors, numbness or hyperarousal, along with disruption of normal life function (15). Although many people with PTSD suffer from symptoms of depression and anxiety (11, 16), trauma memories are the source of most problems (17, 18). According to Rubin and colleagues, “PTSD is defined in large part by changes that occur specifically in autobiographical memory” (19). To assess the strength of these memories, they developed the Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire (AMQ), which asks questions related to the details of an autobiographical memory, typically framed as a negative event in their past. In response to the AMQ, individuals with PTSD recalled negative autobiographical events with greater strength compared to people without PTSD, and the strength of the memory correlated with PTSD symptoms (9, 1921). They interpret these and other findings to suggest that the repeated rehearsal of an intense memory experience may exacerbate PTSD symptoms over time and contribute to one's life story (19). To our knowledge, the AMQ has not been used to assess the strength of stressful life memories in women who experienced the trauma of SV while adolescents and young adults, as it relates to the diagnosis of PTSD.

Rumination is defined as the repeated rehearsal of thoughts. These thoughts are usually autobiographical, negative in nature and about the past. They are also described as uncontrollable and involuntary. Historically, rumination was considered a trait and thus relatively stable. As such, the tendency to ruminate was considered a potential risk factor for PTSD (22, 23). However, more recent data suggest that rumination is malleable and can be decreased significantly by interventions, which target trauma memories (2427). In general, ruminative thoughts have been most often associated with depression, although recent studies, including our own, indicate a strong relationship to trauma (22, 28). But exactly how they relate to trauma is unclear. Minimally, they exacerbate trauma-related thoughts through the repetition of trauma memories (28) and as discussed, each time a memory is retrieved, a new memory is made through its association with the context in which it is expressed. Thus, at a neuroscience level, one could hypothesize that the repetitive and largely involuntary rehearsal of a trauma memory creates yet more memories of the trauma and related memories in the brain. A related theory, known as the “multiple trace theory” was developed by Nadel and Moscovitch to account for the persistence of memories (2931). We extend their theory to suggest that the repeated rehearsal of a vivid autobiographical memory during rumination generates more trauma memories, thereby making the trauma more difficult to “forget.” The purpose of the present study was to investigate the relationship between rumination and the strength of autobiographical memories in women with SV history.

In the present study, we hypothesized that women with SV history would report autobiographical memories of a stressful life event as more intense than women without SV history, even in the absence of PTSD. It was hypothesized that women who ruminate more would also report stronger stressful life memories. We further hypothesized that the experience of SV history rather than the diagnosis of PTSD per se, would influence the expression of ruminative thoughts and intensity of stressful memories. Finally, we hypothesized that the relationship between the stressful life memory and rumination would relate to the numbers of trauma-related cognitions, as well as anxious and depressive symptoms. Adult women who experienced SV during and after puberty were evaluated because most women are assaulted during this time period and because memory processes for events that occur in childhood may be different from those acquired later (3). To test the hypotheses, we relied on statistical analyses of group differences between women with and without SV history as well as correlations among outcomes within individuals.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

One hundred and eighty three college-aged women (Mage = 20 years, SD = 2.67, range 18–39 years; 33.2% Asian, 30.4% European/Caucasian/White; 16.8% African-American/Black/Caribbean; 9.8% Hispanic/Latina; 9.8% more than one race/other/unknown) participated in testing at a northeastern university. Participants were included if they were able and willing to provide written informed consent and excluded if they were over 40 years of age. Less than 10% of the participants were currently prescribed anti-anxiety medications or anti-depressants. Sixty four participants (n = 64) reported experiencing sexual violence, and one hundred nineteen participants (n = 119) did not experience SV and served as controls.

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Institutional Review Board at Rutgers University. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants were assessed for trauma history with a structured clinical interview, completed a series of questionnaires (described below) and then completed the working memory task (total session ~2 h). Afterwards, participants were debriefed and compensated ($20 or research credits).

Materials

All participants were assessed for trauma history with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID; 15) by a doctoral psychology graduate student trained in conducting clinical interviews. Trauma exposure was defined according to the DSM-5 as direct exposure to “an event or events that involved actual or threatened death, serious injury or sexual violation to the self” (32). The age of participant at time of trauma was also recorded and used to select only those participants with trauma history during adolescence or young adulthood for analyses.

The Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire [AMQ; (33)] is a 19-item questionnaire that assesses qualities of an autobiographical memory. Given the breadth of the questionnaire, typically items are scored individually or in clusters, depending on the research question of interest (20). We were especially interested in the sensory details and vividness of details related to the memory rather than the accuracy of the memory itself. Therefore, we did not assess items concerning confidence of the memory or dating the event. We also wanted to protect their privacy and therefore, did not ask them to identify the event. Participants were told to respond to the questionnaire in relation to an autobiographical memory of the most stressful event in your life. We included 12 individual items which assessed the rehearsal of the memory, sensory details (“As I remember the event, I can hear it in my mind” or “see it in my mind”), temporal and spatial details (“As I remember the event, I know its temporal and spatial layout”), emotional intensity (“As I remember the event, I feel the emotions now that I felt then”), and significance (“the memory is significant in my life”). Total scores were calculated, with greater scores representing heightened vividness of details related to a stressful autobiographical memory. This task has been used in the past to distinguish between types of memory systems activated during recall, particularly in participants with PTSD symptoms (7, 19, 20, 34).

The Symmetry Span Task was used to assess working memory (35). Developed by Engle and colleagues, the computer task required participants to make assessments of symmetrical pictures while remembering the temporal order and spatial location of a series of squares individually displayed on a grid. Participants were shown a picture and were asked to respond whether it was symmetrical or not. Immediately following, a 1 × 1 square in a 4 × 4 grid appeared on the screen for 2-s. Pairs of pictures and squares were presented 2–5 times per trial, for 12 trials (3 blocks total). Following stimulus presentations, participants were asked to replicate the location of the squares in the order presented. The task required ~20 min to complete. Data from the working memory task yielded partial and absolute accuracy scores. A partial accuracy score was a sum of all correct responses. An absolute accuracy score was the sum of correct responses in completely correct trials, when the order and location of all squares were correctly identified during the trial (all-or-none system).

The Ruminative Responses Scale [RRS; (36)] is a 22-item questionnaire that assesses thoughts and responses to depressed mood and affect. Examples of RRS items include “thinking about how sad you feel,” “thinking about your shortcomings, failings, and mistakes” or “analyzing events to understand why you feel depressed or unmotivated.” The RRS is scored as a summation of responses (min 22; max 88) as well as according to three subscales: (1) depressive ruminations, which relate to the rehearsal of depressive events, (2) brooding ruminations, which are often non-adaptive and emotion-laden, and (3) reflective ruminations, which are not as maladaptive but self-focused (37, 38).

The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI) is a 33-item questionnaire that assesses altered cognitions related to traumatic life experiences (16). The PTCI is composed of three subscales: thoughts related to oneself (i.e., sees oneself as blameworthy, isolated, unreliable), others (i.e., sees them as untrustworthy), and the world (i.e., sees it as a dangerous place). The purpose of the PTCI is to assess the person's reaction to the event after time has passed and includes items such as “I have to be on guard all the time,” “I feel like I don't know myself anymore,” and “I feel isolated.” We altered the prompt to ask participants about their thoughts and feelings related to the most stressful event of your life. This way, women with no trauma history could also report their thoughts and feelings of a past event. We also wanted to use the same prompt as used for the AMQ. Higher PTCI scores are indicative of more negative posttraumatic cognitions and are generally associated with greater numbers of PTSD symptoms (24).

The Beck Depression Inventory [BDI; (39)] is a 21-item questionnaire that measures symptoms of depression. The Beck Anxiety Inventory [BAI; (40)] is a 21-item questionnaire that measures symptoms of anxiety. For both measures, total scores are computed by adding response choices. Scores greater than 20 on the BDI represent moderate to severe depression and scores greater than 20 on the BAI are consistent with moderate to severe anxiety. Both questionnaires are commonly used to assess cognitions associated with symptoms of depression and anxiety, respectively (4143).

Statistical Analyses

Dependent variables were obtained from self-report scores from the respective questionnaires with independent variables based on violence, PTSD, or trauma history. Independent samples t-tests and analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used to test differences in mental health outcomes (AMQ, Working Memory task, RRS, PTCI, BDI, BAI) between groups without (no-SV, n = 119) and with history of SV (SV, n = 64). The sample was divided further to account for PTSD diagnosis: no-SV, SV-no PTSD (n = 43), SV+PTSD (n = 21). When analyzing groups based on SV history and PTSD diagnosis, ANOVAs and Fisher's least significance difference (LSD) post-hoc test were used to uncover differences in the strength of autobiographical memories and ruminative thoughts between groups.

Relationships among all outcome measures are presented as Pearson rho correlations. Statistical analyses were assessed with SPSS version 24.0 (IBM Corp., 2017) and a significance level of p < 0.05 was used for all analyses, with Bonferroni corrections for the AMQ. Effect sizes are presented as partial eta-squared (η2) in order to quantify meaningful differences in dependent outcomes between women with and without SV history. Typically, partial η2 effect sizes are categorized as 0.01 (small), 0.09 (medium), and 0.25 (large) (44).

Results

Sexual Violence History With and Without PTSD

Approximately one-third of women with SV history (n = 21) met diagnostic criteria for current PTSD according to SCID-5 criteria. Women with SV history regardless of PTSD diagnosis averaged seven current PTSD symptoms (SD = 5, range 0–19). Women with exposure to trauma other than SV (n = 19) averaged one current PTSD symptom.

Autobiographical Memory

The cumulative AMQ score from items 1–12 was calculated for women with SV history vs. women without SV history. Women with SV history reported a significantly stronger memory of their most stressful life event compared to women with no history of SV F(1, 181) = 11.75, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.06 (Figure 1A). Women with SV and PTSD did not report higher AMQ scores than women with SV but no PTSD, F(2, 180) = 6.53, p > 0.05, but they did report higher AMQ scores than no-trauma controls. Women with SV history but no PTSD reported stronger AMQ memory scores compared to women without SV history (p < 0.05).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Women with sexual violence history reported significantly more details of an autobiographical memory of a past stressful event compared to women with no sexual violence history as assessed by the Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire (AMQ) *p < 0.001. (B) Women with sexual violence history reported significantly more details related to seeing the event in their mind, the temporal and spatial layout of the memory as well as the significance the memory played in their life compared to women with no sexual violence history. *p < 0.005, with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Items on the AMQ were analyzed for group differences. To correct for multiple comparisons and avoid Type 1 Errors, Bonferroni corrections were applied to a p-value of 0.05 (0.05/11 comparisons of 12 AMQ items = 0.005). Therefore, a p-value less than 0.005 was necessary to establish significance. Group scores were significantly different for three items (Figure 1B). Women with SV history (1) reported more details related to seeing the event in their mind, t(181) = −3.73, p < 0.001, (2) indicated more details related to the spatial layout of the memory t(181) = −3.35, p < 0.001, and (3) reported the autobiographical memory as significantly more central to their life story, t(181) = −4.03, p < 0.001. Group responses did not differ significantly on the other nine items assessed by the AMQ, p > 0.005, with the Bonferroni correction, although the overall AMQ score was significantly larger in women with SV history.

Working Memory

Data collected for spatial and temporal working memory with the Symmetry Span task were analyzed. Sixty five participants did not complete the task due to technical problems. Women with SV history (n = 48) did not significantly differ from women without SV history (n = 76), on either partial accuracy, t(122) = 1.26, p > 0.05, or absolute accuracy, t(119.93) = 1.29, p > 0.05.

Ruminative Thoughts

Women with SV history reported ~15% more rumination as assessed with the Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS) compared to women without SV history, F(1, 181) = 12.15, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.06 (Figure 2A). RRS subscales for the depressive, brooding and reflective thoughts were analyzed (Figures 2BD). Women with SV history reported 17% more depressive rumination compared to women without SV history, F(1, 181) = 12.51, p < 0.001. Women with SV history reported 11% more brooding compared to women without SV history, F(1, 181) = 5.54, p < 0.001. Additionally, women with SV history also reported 14% more reflection compared to women without SV history, F(1, 181) = 8.32, p < 0.001. Women significantly differed on RRS scores depending on PTSD diagnosis, F(2, 180) = 8.46, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.09. Fisher's LSD post-hoc test revealed that women with SV and PTSD reported more rumination than women with SV but no PTSD, p < 0.05. Women with SV and no PTSD reported more rumination than women without SV, p < 0.05.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Women with sexual violence history reported significantly greater numbers of ruminative thoughts as assessed by the Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS), as well as more (B) depressive, (C) brooding, and (D) reflective subtypes compared to women with no sexual violence history. *p < 0.001.

Trauma-Related Thoughts, Depression and Anxiety

Women with SV history reported 26% more posttraumatic cognitions as assessed on the PTCI compared to women without SV history, F(1, 181) = 38.42, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.18 (Figure 4A). PTCI subscales of self, world and others were analyzed. Women with SV history significantly differed from the no-SV group on all three PTCI subscales of self-blame, negative thoughts about the world and of others (p's < 0.01). Women significantly differed on PTCI scores depending on PTSD diagnosis, F(2, 180) = 24.87, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.22. Fisher's LSD post-hoc test revealed that women with SV and PTSD reported more posttraumatic cognitions than women with SV but no PTSD, p < 0.05. Women with SV and no PTSD reported more posttraumatic cognitions than women without SV, p < 0.01.

Women with SV history reported 44% more depressive symptoms as scored on the BDI compared to women without SV history, F(1, 181) = 25.66, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.12 (equal variances not assumed) (Figure 4B). Women significantly differed on BDI scores depending on PTSD diagnosis, F(2, 180) = 15.34, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.15. Fisher's LSD post-hoc test revealed that women with SV and PTSD reported over 50% more depressive symptoms than women with SV and no PTSD, as well as women without SV, p's < 0.05. Women with SV and no PTSD also had more depressive symptoms than women without SV, p = 0.001.

Women with SV history reported twice as many anxiety symptoms as assessed on the BAI compared to women without SV history, F(1, 181) = 25.09, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.12 (equal variances not assumed) (Figure 4C). Differences in anxiety depended on PTSD diagnosis, F(2, 180) = 16.59, p < 0.001, Partial η2 = 0.16. Fisher's LSD post-hoc test revealed that women with SV and PTSD reported over 70% more anxious symptoms than women with SV and no PTSD, as well as women without SV, p's < 0.05. Women with SV and no PTSD had more anxious symptoms than women without SV, p < 0.01.

Individual Differences

As summarized in Table 1, women who reported more ruminative thoughts (higher RRS scores) reported a greater number of details surrounding an autobiographical memory of a past stressful event and more posttraumatic cognitions (p < 0.001; Figure 3). Relationships among depressive, anxious and posttraumatic symptoms across the entire sample were highly significant (p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Pearson correlations coefficients among all measures.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Recall of an autobiographical memory of a past stressful event as assessed by the Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire (AMQ) and (B) trauma-related thoughts as assessed with the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI) correlated with ruminative thoughts as assessed by the Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS).

Discussion

In this study, we asked whether stressful life memories would be reported as more intense in women with a history of sexual trauma and if so, whether they would relate to posttraumatic cognitions and ruminative thoughts within individuals. We also asked whether these relationships would occur irrespective of a PTSD diagnosis. Researchers have long been interested in questions such as these (20, 34, 4547). The AMQ was developed by Rubin and colleagues to answer some of these questions (33). Through a series of studies, they established traumatic memory recall as part of a basic memory system rather than its own specific system. In other words, the processes through which autobiographical memories of stressful life events are recalled may not be substantively different from the way that other memories are recalled. We used their autobiographical memory questionnaire here to compare the strength of stressful life memories in women with and without SV history and further to compare responses in women who did and did not meet criteria for current PTSD. We predicted that women with SV history would report a stressful life memory as more intense compared to responses from women without SV history and this response would not depend on the concurrent diagnosis of PTSD. As predicted, SV in the past, irrespective of PTSD diagnosis, was associated with a stronger reported memory experience. Thus, SV is sufficient to establish an especially strong autobiographical memory of a very stressful life event upon recall.

In the present study, women with SV history not only reported a stronger stressful life memory, they reported specific details as being especially prevalent (Figure 1A). They were more likely to: (1) see the event in their mind, (2) recall its temporal and spatial layout, and (3) consider the memory a significant part of their life story (Figure 1B). They did not report the “feelings” associated with the memory as more intense, nor did they report more “reliving” of the memory, when compared to women without SV history reflecting on the most stressful event in their life. These differences in memory detail suggest that the memory for a SV event is experienced as watching a movie rather than a body response, per se. Of course, memories for trauma are experienced in the body—the participants just did not report it as more intense. One limitation of this study is that we did not ask participants to identify the event they were reflecting upon and therefore do not necessarily know whether women were recalling the sexually violent experience. Nonetheless, these data suggest that memories for stressful life events are especially vivid in sensory and contextual detail as well as meaningful for women who have had a sexually violent experience.

Working memory is a learning process, which allows the brain to hold and manipulate information in short-term memory, usually in the service of completing a cognitive task or skill (48). Women with SV history performed a working memory task just as well as women without SV history. These findings are generally consistent with others (49). For example, college students with interpersonal trauma history performed just as well on tests of working memory and cognitive flexibility (using digit-span and card-sorting tasks) as students without trauma history (50). Another study compared the recall of autobiographical to nonautobiographical memories in individuals with PTSD. Whereas, autobiographical memories were more disorganized, nonautobiographical memories were not (51). These data and those we report here suggest that trauma and/or violence history is more likely to impact stressful life memories rather than general memory processes.

The psychological mechanisms through which SV and memory interact is not without controversy (45, 52). Many studies point to disorganization of memory (10, 51). Rape victims often describe their memories as emotionally intense but lacking coherence (53), whereas other victims report memories rich in detail. A recent study asked survivors of domestic violence to write a narrative about a traumatic event and a positive experience (54). The trauma narratives were detailed and more coherent than the memories of a positive experience, and were better predictors of PTSD. The underlying psychopathology, if present, is also important. For example, undergraduates with PTSD reported more sensory details of a traumatic event whereas undergraduates with depression recorded fewer details (55). Age also seems to matter. For example, children sometimes dissociate during trauma and as a consequence, may have difficulty remembering what happened (56). We only tested women who had experienced SV during or after puberty. Their responses suggest a strong detailed memory for their most stressful life event, which is consistent with some studies in adults (45, 57).

Ruminations

Ruminations generate intrusive memories which can extend the persistence of PTSD (58, 59). In the present study, women with SV history reported they tend to ruminate about 15% more than women without SV history (Figure 2A). These women also reported significantly more depressive, brooding and reflective ruminations. Rumination was further associated with posttraumatic cognitions, which are indicative of PTSD symptomology. These data are consistent with meta-analyses, indicating positive relationships between rumination and posttraumatic symptoms, even years after the trauma (57, 6063). The mechanisms through which ruminative and posttraumatic thoughts interact are unknown. In one study, individuals with PTSD described their ruminations as uncontrollable thoughts about critical life events (28). In our study, women with SV history and PTSD reported more ruminations than women with SV history and no PTSD, who in turn reported more ruminations than women without SV history. Therefore, PTSD symptoms are associated with a greater tendency to ruminate but exposure to SV on its own is sufficient to increase rumination. Of course, we cannot determine whether the tendency to ruminate predates trauma exposure and consequently exacerbates the development of trauma-related symptoms later in life, though other studies suggest this may be the case (22).

Relations Among Ruminative Thoughts and Stressful Life Memories

In general, participants who reported one outcome in this study were more likely to report another outcome (Table 1). Strong correlations among responses may not be especially surprising because the questions on the various questionnaires do overlap. But even so, the correlations were strong and pervasive. For this study, we focused primarily on the relationship between rumination and stressful life memories (Figure 3). The correlation between RRS and AMQ scores was highly significant across the entire sample, as shown in Table 1; the correlation between RRS and AMQ for SV without PTSD was significant, albeit less strong (r = 0.33; p < 0.05). To our knowledge, no study to date has reported a positive relationship between rumination and autobiographical memory detail in women with sexual trauma exposure but not necessarily experiencing PTSD. To interpret these results, we come back to the “multiple memory trace” hypothesis. Each time a memory is activated in a new context, additional details are incorporated into the memory and a new “hippocampal trace” memory is generated (29, 30, 6466). These learning processes in the hippocampus can interact with stress as well. In one study, people watched a stressful film and later were exposed to cues, which reactivated the memory for the film. Those who did so under stressful conditions (high cortisol) reported more intrusive memories than those who did so under unstressed conditions (67). A similar process may be occurring in women with SV history. According to their self-report surveys, they often rehearse trauma memories. The rehearsals are likely done under stressful conditions because the women are experiencing moderate levels of depression and anxiety. We propose these involuntary rehearsals generate new memories with new contextual details in the brain, which become more difficult to extinguish.

The positive relationship between rumination and memory reported here is different from other reports. Kleim and Ehlers (68) observed less detail for autobiographical memories in individuals with acute stress disorder (68). Moreover, the correlation between memory detail and rumination was negative. There are some key differences between studies. In theirs, memories were provoked with cues using the Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT) and thus different in content from “the most stressful life memory” retrieved during the AMQ. Also, their assessments were within weeks of the trauma and ours were generally much later. But interestingly, they conclude that an overgeneralized memory is not necessarily the result of trauma, per se, but rather a predisposition to depression, which arises as a result of trauma. The literature connecting trauma with autobiographical memory is extensive and implicates other factors such as avoidance behavior and executive control (17, 18, 69). Others studies propose even more complicated relationships (7074). It is difficult to directly compare our results with these because of the methodological differences and because most of our participants did not have PTSD. In general, there does appear to be a strong relationship between rumination and the strength of autobiographical memory recall, but the direction of this relationship depends on the experimental question and procedures, as well as the presence of PTSD and/or other mental disorders such as depression.

Rumination is considered a risk factor for depression, in part because women tend to ruminate more than men do and are more often diagnosed with depression (75, 76). However, rumination per se does not necessarily account for the high incidence of depression in women (37). In one of our recent studies, both men and women with high numbers of depressive symptoms reported more ruminative thoughts, but there were no sex differences in the relationship. With that being said, because women are more likely to ruminate and more likely to experience sexual violence, it is reasonable to assume that these ruminations may exacerbate the symptoms of depression, as previously suggested (68). Of course, not all ruminations are maladaptive and some degree of reflection is necessary in order to learn how to recover from trauma (77).

Trauma-Related Thoughts and Symptoms of Anxiety and Depression

Women with SV history had PTCI scores greater than 120, which is relatively high (Figure 4A), especially because most of them were not diagnosed with PTSD (16). However, participants without trauma history also had relatively high PTCI scores (16). These differences are easily explained. We asked participants to reflect on the most “stressful event” in their lifetime, rather than the most “traumatic event.” We did this in order to compare responses between participants with and without trauma history but obviously, this procedure would increase PTCI scores across groups. Importantly though, women with SV history reported more trauma-related thoughts than women without SV history. And women with SV and PTSD reported more posttraumatic thoughts than women with SV history and no PTSD.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Women with sexual violence history reported significantly more posttraumatic cognitions as assessed by the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI), (B) more depressive symptoms as assessed by the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), and (C) more anxiety symptoms as assessed by the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) compared to women with no sexual violence history. *p < 0.001.

Most women with SV history did not meet diagnostic criteria for a depressive or anxiety disorder, as diagnosed by the SCID. As a group, their BDI scores were consistent with a mild mood disturbance (39) and BAI scores were consistent with mild anxiety (40). Overall, these data indicate a significant yet moderate elevation of anxiety and depressive symptoms in response to SV history. Others report similar findings in college students with violence history (78, 79). These results likely reflect low level activation of the sympathetic nervous system and the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal axis, both of which are associated with the persistent expression of symptoms of depression and anxiety (8084).

Sex Differences

We concentrated on women and not men for several reasons. First, women are four times more likely to experience SV compared to men, and women between ages 12 and 34 are most likely to experience sexual assault (2, 6). Second, college-age women report sexual assault as the most traumatic event of their lifetime when compared to men (53% compared to 11%) (85). And third, women are more likely than men to develop assault-related and stress-related disorders such as PTSD, presumably because they are more frequently exposed to sexually violent events, but also perhaps due to biological differences in stress-related systems (86, 87).

The impact of SV on the human brain is difficult to study, in part because studies must be retrospective and variability between individual experiences is substantial (86, 8890). To meet this need, we developed an animal model known as SCAR, which stands for Sexual Conspecific Aggressive Response. During this procedure, a young female rat in puberty is exposed to an adult sexually-experienced male each day for 30 min. As a result, female rats produced high levels of stress hormones and did not learn as well in standard laboratory training tasks. They also retained fewer new neurons in the hippocampus, a part of the brain necessary for many types of learning (91). These data suggest that the experience of sexual aggression can affect neuronal processes related to learning and memory, even neurogenesis. However, these results must be interpreted with caution; it is not possible to directly compare results from laboratory animal studies to women with SV history because the conditions surrounding the stressful events are different in many ways and levels of organization.

Learning to Recover

Sexual violence against women is an all too common occurrence and recent attention from the media is long overdue (1, 3). But this problem will not go away soon and we must keep our attention focused on prevention and justice for survivors. We must also find new ways to help women learn to recover. The most accepted course of treatment is Prolonged Exposure Therapy (PET) (12, 16, 24, 92), during which the client recollects the trauma memory during interviews, story-writing and even revisiting the traumatic location. After repeated exposure, the strength of the memory often lessens and the conditioned responses of fear and anxiety begin to extinguish (93, 94). Theoretically, the memory becomes less accessible in part because it has been updated and reconsolidated with other safer memories (95). Effective as it is, PET is time-consuming and can be expensive. Also, clients often drop out because it is emotionally painful to rehearse and relive the memory.

We recently developed a novel intervention to help women recover from the trauma of SV (26, 27, 96). The intervention is known as MAP Training because it combines “Mental And Physical” Training. Each session combines 30 min of mental training with silent meditation followed by 30 min of aerobic exercise. After 6 weeks of training, twice a week, women with SV history reported significantly fewer trauma-related cognitions and ruminative thoughts than women with SV history who were trained with meditation alone or exercise alone. Women who completed MAP Training also reported greater self-worth than the other training groups (26). It is unclear how this intervention works to reduce rumination and trauma-related thoughts. Women in the study completed the AMQ as well. Interestingly, scores did not change in women who completed training but increased in women who did not train. These data may suggest that fewer trauma-related and ruminative thoughts lessen the likelihood that the memory will strengthen over time.

Conclusion

Women who experienced SV as adolescents or young adults reported intense memories for stressful life events, irrespective of PTSD. The strength of these memories was highly related to ruminative and trauma-related thoughts within individuals, as were symptoms of depression and anxiety. Theoretically, these data support the idea that ruminative thoughts and vivid memories of stressful life events coexist and are especially prominent in women who have experienced sexual violence. Thus, rumination may be an especially effective target for trauma recovery, because fewer ruminations would produce fewer vivid memories of trauma in the brain.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out with written informed consent from all participants. IRB approval was obtained from Rutgers University for the study.

Author Contributions

TS, EM, and HC designed project. EM and HC carried out the study. TS, EM, and HC analyzed results. TS, EM, and HC wrote the manuscript.

Funding

Brain Health Institute, Rutgers University.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer KL and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation.

Acknowledgments

We want to thank our research assistants Abigail Harriman, Christina Mesa, Katlyn Schroder, Ahua Traore, and Jacqueline Ugwuneri for their help with this project.

References

1. World Health Organization. Global and Regional Estimates of Violence Against Women: Prevalence and Health Effects of Intimate Partner Violence and Non-Partner Sexual Violence. World Health Organization (2013).

2. Kessler RC. Posttraumatic stress disorder: the burden to the individual and to society. J Clin Psychiatry (2000) 61:52–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

3. Greenfeld LA. Sex Offenses and Offenders. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics, Department of Justice (1997).

Google Scholar

4. Krebs C, Lindquist C, Berzofsky M, Shook-Sa B, Peterson K, Planty M, et al. Campus Climate Survey Validation Study Final Technical Report. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics, Department of Justice (2016).

5. Cantor D, Fisher B, Chibnall S, Townsend R, Lee H, Bruce C, et al. Report on the AAU Campus Climate Survey on Sexual Assault and Sexual Misconduct. Rockville, MD: Association of American Universities (2015).

6. Sinozich S, Langton L. Rape and Sexual Assault Victimization Among College-Age Females, 1995–2013. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics, Department of Justice (2014).

7. Rubin DC. Schema-driven construction of future autobiographical traumatic events: the future is much more troubling than the past. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2014) 143:612–30. doi: 10.1037/a0032638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Clark IA, Mackay CE. Mental imagery and post-traumatic stress disorder: a neuroimaging and experimental psychopathology approach to intrusive memories of trauma. Front Psychiatry (2015) 6:104. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rubin DC. A basic-systems approach to autobiographical memory. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2005) 14:79–83. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00339.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Ehlers A, Clark DM. A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther. (2000) 38:319–45. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00123-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. van der Kolk B. Posttraumatic stress disorder and the nature of trauma. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2000) 2:7–22.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

12. Foa EB, Gillihan SJ, Bryant RA. Challenges and successes in dissemination of evidence-based treatments for posttraumatic stress: lessons learned from prolonged exposure therapy for PTSD. Psychol Sci Public Interest (2013) 14:65–111. doi: 10.1177/1529100612468841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Cahill S, Pontoski K. Post-traumatic stress disorder and acute stress disorder I: their nature and assessment considerations. Psychiatry (2005) 2:14–25.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

14. Kessler R, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Benjet C, Bromet E, Cardoso G, et al. Trauma and PTSD in the WHO world mental health surveys. Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2017) 8:1353383. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2017.1353383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. First MB, Williams JBW, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID-5), Research Version. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2015).

16. Foa EB, Ehlers A, Clark DM, Tolin DF, Orsillo SM. The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): development and validation. Psychol Assess. (1999) 11:303–14. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.11.3.303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Williams JMG, Barnhofer T, Crane C, Herman D, Raes F, Watkins E, et al. Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychol Bull. (2007) 133:122–48. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Moore SA, Zoellner LA. Overgeneral autobiographical memory and traumatic events: an evaluative review. Psychol Bull. (2007) 133:419–37. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Rubin DC, Boals A, Berntsen D. Memory in posttraumatic stress disorder: properties of voluntary and involuntary, traumatic and non-traumatic autobiographical memories in people with and without PTSD symptoms. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2008) 137:591–614. doi: 10.1037/a0013165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Rubin DC, Dennis MF, Beckham JC. Autobiographical memory for stressful events: the role of autobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Conscious Cogn. (2011) 20:840–56. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Rubin DC, Feldman ME, Beckham JC. Reliving, emotions, and fragmentation in the autobiographical memories of veterans diagnosed with PTSD. Appl Cogn Psychol. (2004) 18:17–35. doi: 10.1002/acp.950

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Spinhoven P, Penninx B, Krempeniou A, van Hemert A, Elzinga B. Trait rumination predicts onset of post-traumatic stress disorder through trauma-related cognitive appraisals: a 4-year longitudinal study. Behav Res Ther. (2015) 71:101–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Elwood L, Hahn K, Olatunji B, Williams N. Cognitive vulnerabilities to the development of PTSD: a review of four vulnerabilities and the proposal of an integrative vulnerability model. Clin Psychol Rev. (2009) 29:87–100. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Foa EB, Rauch SAM. Cognitive changes during prolonged exposure versus prolonged exposure plus cognitive restructuring in female assault survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2004) 72:879–84. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Resick PA, Nishith P, Weaver TL, Astin MC, Feuer CA. A comparison of cognitive-processing therapy with prolonged exposure and a waiting condition for the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder in female rape victims. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2002) 70:867–79. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.70.4.867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Shors TJ, Chang HYM, Millon EM. MAP Training My BrainTM: meditation plus aerobic exercise lessens trauma of sexual violence more than either activity alone. Front Neurosci. (2018) 12:211. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Alderman BL, Olson RL, Brush CJ, Shors TJ. Mental and Physical (MAP) Training: combining meditation and aerobic exercise reduces depression and rumination while enhancing synchronized brain activity. Transl Psychiatry (2016) 6:1–9. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.225

CrossRef Full Text

28. Hu E, Koucky EM, Brown WJ, Bruce SE, Sheline YI. The role of rumination in elevating perceived stress in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Interpers Violence (2014) 29:1953–62. doi: 10.1177/0886260513511697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Nadel L, Moscovitch M. Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1997) 7:217–27. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80010-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Moscovitch M, Nadel L. Consolidation and the hippocampal complex revisited: in defense of the multiple-trace model. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1998) 8:297–300. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80155-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A, Addis DR, Westmacott R, Grady C, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. J Anat. (2005) 207:35–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00421.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

33. Rubin DC, Schrauf RW, Greenberg DL. Belief and recollection of autobiographical memories. Mem Cogn. (2003) 31:887–901. doi: 10.3758/BF03196443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Rubin DC, Berntsen D, Bohni MK. A memory-based model of posttraumatic stress disorder: evaluating basic assumptions underlying the PTSD diagnosis. Psychol Rev. (2008) 115:985–1011. doi: 10.1037/a0013397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Foster JL, Shipstead Z, Harrison TL, Hicks KL, Redick TS, Engle RW. Shortened complex span tasks can reliably measure working memory capacity. Mem Cogn. (2015) 43:226–36. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0461-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Treynor W, Gonzalez R, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cognit Ther Res. (2003) 27:247–59. doi: 10.1023/A:1023910315561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Shors TJ, Millon EM, Chang HYM, Olson RL, Alderman BL. Do sex differences in rumination explain sex differences in depression? J Neurosci Res. (2017) 95:711–18. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Michl LC, McLaughlin KA, Shepherd K, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. J Abnorm Psychol. (2013) 122:339–52. doi: 10.1037/a0031994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. 1st ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (1996).

40. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1988) 56:893–7. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Muntingh ADT, van der Feltz-Cornelis CM, van Marwijk HWJ, Spinhoven P, Penninx BWJH, van Balkom AJLM. Is the Beck Anxiety Inventory a good tool to assess the severity of anxiety? A primary care study in the Netherlands study of depression and anxiety (NESDA). BMC Fam Pract. (2011) 12:66. doi: 10.1186/1471-2296-12-66

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Viinamäki H, Tanskanen A, Honkalampi K, Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Haatainen K, Kaustio O, et al. Is the Beck Depression Inventory suitable for screening major depression in different phases of the disease? Nord J Psychiatry (2004) 58:49–53. doi: 10.1080/08039480310000798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Clark DA, Steer RA, Beck AT. Common and specific dimensions of self-reported anxiety and depression: implications for the cognitive and tripartite models. J Abnorm Psychol. (1994) 103:645–54. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.103.4.645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (1988).

45. McNally R. Remembering Trauma. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press (2003).

46. van der Kolk BA, Fisler R. Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: overview and exploratory study. J Trauma Stress (1995) 8:505–25. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490080402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Michael T, Ehlers A, Halligan SL, Clark DM. Unwanted memories of assault: what intrusion characteristics are associated with PTSD? Behav Res Ther. (2005) 43:613–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Baddeley A. Working memory. Science (1992) 255:556–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1736359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Landre L, Destrieux C, Andersson F, Barantin L, Quide Y, Tapia G, et al. Working memory processing of traumatic material in women with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2012) 37:87–94. doi: 10.1503/jpn.100167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Twamley EW, Hami S, Stein MB. Neuropsychological function in college students with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2004) 126:265–74. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Jelinek L, Randjbar S, Seifert D, Kellner M, Moritz S. The organization of autobiographical and nonautobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). J Abnorm Psychol. (2009) 118:288–98. doi: 10.1037/a0015633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol Rev. (2000) 107:261–88. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Koss M, Figueredo A, Bell I, Tharan M, Tromp S. Traumatic memory characteristics: a cross-validated mediational model of response to rape among employed women. J Abnorm Psychol. (1996) 105:421–32. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.3.421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Fernández-Lansac V, Crespo M. Quality of memories in women abused by their intimate partner: analysis of traumatic and nontraumatic narratives. J Trauma Stress (2017) 30:80–7. doi: 10.1002/jts.22154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ashbaugh AR, Marinos J, Bujaki B. The impact of depression and PTSD symptom severity on trauma memory. Memory (2018) 26:106–16. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1334801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Brewin CR. The nature and significance of memory disturbance in posttraumatic stress disorder. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2011) 7:203–27. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kensinger EA. Remembering the details: effects of emotion. Emot Rev J Int Soc Res Emot. (2009) 1:99–113. doi: 10.1177/1754073908100432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Birrer E, Michael T. Rumination in PTSD as well as in traumatized and non-traumatized depressed patients: a cross-sectional clinical study. Behav Cogn Psychother. (2011) 39:381–97. doi: 10.1017/S1352465811000087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Valdez CE, Lilly MM. Posttraumatic rumination: content, correlates, and processes. J Clin Psychol. (2017) 73:707–21. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Szabo Y, Warnecke A, Newton T, Valentine J. Rumination and posttraumatic stress symptoms in trauma-exposed adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Anxiety Stress Coping (2017) 30:396–414. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2017.1313835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Murray J, Ehlers A, Mayou RA. Dissociation and post-traumatic stress disorder: two prospective studies of road traffic accident survivors. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci. (2002) 180:363–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.180.4.363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Ehlers A, Mayou RA, Bryant B. Psychological predictors of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. J Abnorm Psychol. (1998) 107:508–19. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.107.3.508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Mayou RA, Ehlers A, Bryant B. Posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents: 3-year follow-up of a prospective longitudinal study. Behav Res Ther. (2002) 40:665–75. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00069-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Nader K, Schafe GE, Le Doux JE. Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature (2000) 406:722–6. doi: 10.1038/35021052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Hupbach A, Gomez R, Hardt O, Nadel L. Reconsolidation of episodic memories: a subtle reminder triggers integration of new information. Learn Mem. (2007) 14:47–53. doi: 10.1101/lm.365707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Ryan L, Nadel L, Keil K, Putnam K, Schnyer D, Trouard T, et al. Hippocampal complex and retrieval of recent and very remote autobiographical memories: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging in neurologically intact people. Hippocampus (2001) 11:707–14. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Cheung J, Garber B, Bryant RA. The role of stress during memory reactivation on intrusive memories. Neurobiol Learn Mem. (2015) 123:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Kleim B, Ehlers A. Reduced autobiographical memory specificity predicts depression and posttraumatic stress disorder after recent trauma. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2008) 76:231–42. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Sumner JA. The mechanisms underlying overgeneral autobiographical memory: an evaluative review of evidence for the CaR-FA-X model. Clin Psychol Rev. (2012) 32:34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Harvey AG, Bryant RA, Dang ST. Autobiographical memory in acute stress disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1998) 66:500–6. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.66.3.500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Kangas M, Henry JL, Bryant RA. A prospective study of autobiographical memory and posttraumatic stress disorder following cancer. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2005) 73:293–9. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. McNally RJ, Lasko NB, Macklin ML, Pitman RK. Autobiographical memory disturbance in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther. (1995) 33:619–30. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(95)00007-K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Schönfeld S, Ehlers A. Overgeneral memory extends to pictorial retrieval cues and correlates with cognitive features in posttraumatic stress disorder. Emotion (2006) 6:611–21. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.4.611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Schönfeld S, Ehlers A, Böllinghaus I, Rief W. Overgeneral memory and suppression of trauma memories in post-traumatic stress disorder. Memory (2007) 15:339–52. doi: 10.1080/09658210701256571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Larson J, Grayson C. Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1999) 77:1061–72. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Jackson B. Mediators of the gender difference in rumination. Psychol Women Q. (2001) 25:37–47. doi: 10.1111/1471-6402.00005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Michael T, Halligan SL, Clark DM, Ehlers A. Rumination in posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress Anxiety (2007) 24:307–17. doi: 10.1002/da.20228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Começanha R, Basto-Pereira M, Maia Â. Clinically speaking, psychological abuse matters. Compr Psychiatry (2017) 73:120–6. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Martinson A, Craner J, Sigmon S. Differences in HPA axis reactivity to intimacy in women with and without histories of sexual trauma. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2016) 65:118–26. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Vreeburg SA, Hoogendijk WJ, van Pelt J, Derijk RH, Verhagen JC, van Dyck R, et al. Major depressive disorder and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2009) 66:617–26. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Appelhof BC, Huyser J, Verweij M, Brouwer JP, van Dyck R, Fliers E, et al. Glucocorticoids and relapse of major depression (dexamethasone/corticotropin-releasing hormone test in relation to relapse of major depression). Biol Psychiatry (2006) 59:696–701. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Burke HM, Davis MC, Otte C, Mohr DC. Depression and cortisol responses to psychological stress: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2005) 30:846–56. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Yoon KL, Joormann J. Stress reactivity in social anxiety disorder with and without comorbid depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (2012) 121:250–5. doi: 10.1037/a0025079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Altamura M, Iuso S, Balzotti A, Francavilla G, Dimitri A, Cibelli G, et al. Salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol responsiveness to stress in first episode, drug-naïve patients with panic disorder. Neurosci Res. (2018). doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2018.03.003. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

85. Vrana S, Lauterbach D. Prevalence of traumatic events and post-traumatic psychological symptoms in a nonclinical sample of college students. J Trauma Stress (1994) 7:289–302. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490070209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Shors TJ, Millon EM. Sexual trauma and the female brain. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2016) 41:87–98. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2016.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Tolin DF, Foa EB. Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: a quantitative review of 25 years of research. Psychol Bull. (2006) 132:959–92. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Wong JY-H, Fong DY-T, Lai V, Tiwari A. Bridging intimate partner violence and the human brain: a literature review. Trauma Violence Abus (2014) 15:22–33. doi: 10.1177/1524838013496333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Herman JL. Trauma and Recovery. New York, NY: BasicBooks (1997).

90. Hegadoren KM, Lasiuk GC, Coupland NJ. Posttraumatic stress disorder part III: health effects of interpersonal violence among women. Perspect Psychiatr Care (2006) 42:163–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6163.2006.00078.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Shors TJ, Tobon K, DiFeo G, Durham DM, Chang HYM. Sexual Conspecific Aggressive Response (SCAR): social interaction between a pubescent female and an adult male disrupts learning to care for offspring. Sci Rep. (2015) 6:18960. doi: 10.1038/srep18960

CrossRef Full Text

92. Rauch SAM, Foa EB, Furr JM, Filip JC. Imagery vividness and perceived anxious arousal in prolonged exposure treatment for PTSD. J Trauma Stress (2004) 17:461–5. doi: 10.1007/s10960-004-5794-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Foa EB, Kozak MJ. Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective information. Psychol Bull. (1986) 99:20–35. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Schnyder U, Ehlers A, Elbert T, Foa EB, Gersons BPR, Resick PA, et al. Psychotherapies for PTSD: what do they have in common? Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2015) 6:28186. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v6.28186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Kwapis JL, Jarome TJ, Ferrara NC, Helmstetter FJ. Updating procedures can reorganize the neural circuit supporting a fear memory. Nature (2017) 42:1688–97. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Shors TJ, Olson RL, Bates ME, Selby EA, Alderman BL. Mental and Physical (MAP) Training: a neurogenesis-inspired intervention that enhances health in humans. Neurobiol Learn Mem. (2014) 115:3–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sexual violence, stress, trauma, PTSD, depression, rumination, memory, fear

Citation: Millon EM, Chang HYM and Shors TJ (2018) Stressful Life Memories Relate to Ruminative Thoughts in Women With Sexual Violence History, Irrespective of PTSD. Front. Psychiatry 9:311. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00311

Received: 30 November 2017; Accepted: 22 June 2018;
Published: 05 September 2018.

Edited by:

Chantal Martin Soelch, University of Fribourg, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Mirko Manchia, Università degli studi di Cagliari, Italy
Katharina Marlene Ledermann, Université de Fribourg, Switzerland

Copyright © 2018 Millon, Chang and Shors. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tracey J. Shors, shors@rutgers.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.