Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 25 May 2018
Sec. Public Mental Health

Relationship Between Leaving Children at Home Alone and Their Mental Health: Results From the A-CHILD Study in Japan

  • 1Department of Global Health Promotion, Tokyo Medical and Dental University (TMDU), Tokyo, Japan
  • 2Japan Support Center for Suicide Countermeasures, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Tokyo, Japan
  • 3Department of Social Medicine, National Research Institute for Child Health and Development, Tokyo, Japan

Leaving children at home alone is considered a form of “neglect” in most developed countries. In Japan, this practice is not prohibited, probably because this country is considered to have relatively safe communities for children. The impact of leaving children at home alone on their mental health is a controversial issue, and few studies have examined it to date. The aim of this study was to examine the impact of leaving children aged 6 or 7 years at home alone on their mental health, focusing on both the positive and negative aspects; that is, resilience, difficult behavior, and prosocial behavior. Data from the Adachi Child Health Impact of Living Difficulty (A-CHILD) study were used. The caregivers of all children in the first grade in Adachi City, Tokyo, were targeted, of whom 80% completed the questionnaire (n = 4,291). Among the analytical sample which comprises those who completed both exposure and outcome variables (n = 4,195), 2,190 (52.2%) children had never been left at home alone, 1,581 (37.7%) children were left at home alone less than once a week, and 424 (10.1%) children were left at home alone once a week or more. Child resilience was measured using the Children's Resilient Coping Scale, and difficult behavior (emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer relationship problems) and prosocial behavior using the Strength and Difficulty Questionnaire. Multivariate regression analyses were performed to examine the dose-response association between leaving children at home alone and child mental health, followed by propensity-score matching as a pseudo-randomized controlled trial to reduce potential confounding. The results showed that leaving children at home alone once a week or more, but not less than once a week, was associated with total difficulties scores, especially conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer relationship problems. These findings indicate that leaving children at home alone should be avoided in Japan, as is recommended in North America.

Introduction

Leaving children at home alone is considered a form of “neglect” in most developed countries (1). In the United States, some states have laws that relate to the minimum age of children who can be left at home alone in order to protect them from neglect, accidents, and crime, or they provide guidelines that can assist parents in their decision on leaving their children at home alone (2, 3). For example, the Illinois law stipulates that children aged less than 14 years old should not be left at home alone (4). In Maryland, the law permits parents to leave children aged 8 years or above alone1.

However, the impact of leaving children at home alone is controversial. If children are mature enough to stay safely at home, being alone might provide them with unique opportunities to enhance their independence, responsibility, and confidence (5, 6). Moreover, older adolescents are more likely to report positive responses to the question on being at home alone, such as having time to read a book and do homework without distraction, and having the opportunity to invite friends over or to go out with friends (7).

On the other hand, the majority of the preceding studies on this topic report negative responses among adolescents. Many studies in North America have shown that adolescents left at home alone feel lonely, have worries, and experience fear, and are also at risk of antisocial behavior such as truancy, stealing, and drinking (68). Mertens et al. (9) also showed that middle-grade students, aged 12–14 years in the United States who were left at home alone for 3 h or more tended to show higher levels of depression, behavior problems, low self-esteem, and low academic efficacy.

Surprisingly, few studies have examined the impact of leaving preadolescent children at home alone (10, 11). A low prevalence of young children being left at home alone in developed countries has been reported (12), possibly because of the law or guidelines that prohibit young children being left at home alone. In their study of 206 children in the first to fourth grades in the United States, Marshall et al. (11) indicated that leaving children at home alone was associated with behavioral problems, especially in low-income families. However, the sample size of that study was small, and the impact of leaving young children at home alone on their development needs to be investigated in a study with a larger sample size.

In contrast to North America, Japan has no law or guidelines regarding leaving children at home alone, including supervision by older siblings, probably because leaving children at home alone may not be the Japanese norm. In comparison to the Western culture, the Japanese parenting style might be more likely to be interdependent, rather than independent [e.g., (13)], which promotes the development of autonomy (14). This assumption is supported by the finding that about 95% of children in the first and second grades always have a dinner with parents that is, most children are not left alone until dinner time (15).

Another possible reason for this lack of law or guidelines is that community living offers a relatively safer neighborhood for children in Japan (16), as shown by its lowest homicide rate among Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) countries (17). A safe living environment is one of the factors that influence the parental decision to leave children at home alone (18), as revealed in an empirical study of 5- to 7-year-old children (12). In a previous study that examined Japanese parents' perception of risk to elementary school children, more than 40% of them perceived leaving a child home alone as “safe” or “slightly safe” and about 90% accepted leaving children at home alone (19). Rather than homicide rate among neighborhoods, the perception of neighborhood safety or social capital [e.g., (20)], both developed based on social trust among neighbors, may be more feasible to assess the impact of neighborhood considering the low number of homicide in Japan. To date, no previous study has revealed a direct association between the impact of social capital and leaving children at home alone.

Social capital is defined as “the resources available to individuals or groups through their social connections” (21). In Japan, which is known to have rich social capital [as reported in narrative studies; (22)], higher social capital has been found to promote better parenting (23). Further, social capital is positively associated with child mental health [e.g., (2426)]. Therefore, social capital may confound the association between leaving children at home alone and their mental health, and thus, it is needed to elucidate the impact of leaving children at home alone on their mental health by the level of social capital. Additionally, income, siblings, maternal education, or caregiver's mental health are associated with leaving children at home alone (8, 27) and child mental health (28, 29). Therefore, we need to assess the effect of social capital, income, siblings, maternal education, and caregiver's mental health.

Our research was conducted as a large project called the Adachi Child Health Impact of Living Difficulty (A-CHILD) study, which examined the health and living environment of children in the first grade in all elementary schools (69 schools) in Adachi City, Tokyo. Adachi City, known as a deprived area in Tokyo (e.g., the unemployment rate was 7.1% in 2010 (30), which is higher than Japan's average of 3.1% in 2016 (31) is keen on tackling with child poverty, which made the current study feasible. Participants were young Japanese children, and the present study has gathered more data than that in a previous study conducted in the United States (11). In the present study, we aimed to examine the association between leaving young children aged 6 or 7 years at home alone and their mental health, focusing on both positive and negative aspects; that is, resilience, difficult behavior, and prosocial behavior, adjusting for the effects of social capital, status of household, and caregiver's mental health.

Methods

Participants

We used data from the A-CHILD study performed in 2015. The survey covered all 69 public elementary schools in Adachi City, Tokyo, Japan. In 2015, self-reported questionnaires with anonymous unique IDs were distributed to 5,355 children in the first grade in elementary school, aged 6–7 years. Children took the questionnaire back home, caregivers entered their responses, and children submitted the filled out response sheet to their school. A total of 4,467 participants returned the questionnaire (response rate = 83.4%). Among respondents, 4,291 participants provided informed consent (valid response rate = 80.1%). Among valid respondents, 96 participants were excluded as explanatory variable, outcome variables, and social capital were missing (Figure 1). Among analytical sample, 90.8 and 7.4% were mothers and fathers, respectively. The A-CHILD protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee in National Center for Child Health and Development (No. 1187).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Requirement flow chart.

Measurements

Explanatory Variable

Leaving children at home alone

The caregivers were asked how often their children stayed at home alone, more than one hour during weekday, which was rated as 1 (never), 2 (one to eleven times a year), 3 (one to three times a month), and 4 (once a week or more). In this study, 0 (never) was categorized as 0 (never left at home alone), 2 (one to eleven times a year) to 3 (one to three times a month) were categorized as 1 (less than once a week), and 4 (once a week or more) was categorized as 2 (once a week or more).

Outcome Variables

Resilience

Child resilience was assessed using the Children's Resilient Coping Scale (CRCS), which has eight items developed by Japanese experts to suit the Japanese context based on previous studies related to resilience (3235) and coping (3638). One study showed that the CRCS has high internal consistency (Chronbach's alpha = 0.80) and sufficient validity (39). The eight items of the CRCS are 1) speaks positively about their future 2) tries to do their best 3) able to take teasing or mean comments well 4) knows how to properly greet others 5) able to get ready for school, study, and do his/her chores without directions 6) seeks appropriate advice when necessary 7) able to give up on things they want or do things that they do not like to do for better future outcomes, and 8) able to ask questions to learn about what they do not understand. For these items, respondents (i.e., caregivers) rated child resilience/coping behaviors using a scale of 0 (never) to 4 (very frequently). Total score was converted into 0–100 to help interpretation of coefficients from statistical analysis, and a higher total score indicates higher level of resilience.

Difficult behavior and prosocial behavior

The Japanese version of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire [SDQ; (40)] was translated from the English version of the SDQ (41). The SDQ, which is an others-reported tool, has 25 items and consists of five subscales: emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, and prosocial behavior. The total difficulties score is calculated using the sum of four subscale scores (emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer relationship problems). Respondents rated the items on a scale of 0 (not true) to 2 (certainly true). In this study, total scores (i.e., total difficulties score, scores of emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, and prosocial behavior) were converted into 0–100 to help interpretation of coefficients from statistical analysis. Higher scores for total difficulties, emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer relationship problems indicate that children have more difficulties, while a higher prosocial behavior score means more prosocial behavior. In this study, the Cronbach's alpha for total difficulties score and prosocial behavior score were 0.78 and 0.70 respectively.

Demographic Data

Respondents

We also assessed the respondents, regardless of whether the person was the mother, father, or any other caregiver.

Child Characteristic

The parents or caregivers were asked about the child's sex (boy or girl).

Family Background

The caregivers were asked about the child's siblings (younger sibling, older sibling, or no siblings), whether they were living with grandparents (yes or no), marital status (married or unmarried), mother's age (< 30, 30–34, 35–39, 40–44, or >45 years), maternal education (high school or less, some college education, college education or higher, or other/unknown), maternal occupation (full-time/part-time job, self-employed, or not working), annual household income (< 500,000, 500,000–999,999, 1,000,000–1,999,999, 2,000,000–2,999,999, 3,000,000–3,999,999, 4,000,000–4,999,999, 5,000,000–5,999,999, 6,000,000–7,499,999, 7,500,000–9,999,999, or ≥ 10,000,000 yen, or unknown).

Parent's or Caregiver's Mental Health

The respondents' anxiety and mood status was assessed using the Japanese version of Kesseler 6 [K6; (42)]. Scores on this tool range from 0 to 24 and estimated cut-off points is a score of 4/5 (43). Higher scores indicate frequent problems on psychological distress. In this study, the Cronbach's alpha for the tool was 0.89.

Social Capital

Respondents rated three items on a scale of 1 (true) to 5 (false). The items were “my community can be trusted,” “my community is cohesive,” and “neighbors in my community help each other.” These items have been used in an earlier study (23, 44). In the present study, 1 (true) to 2 (somewhat true) were categorized as 1 (high social capital), while 3 (cannot say) to 5 (false) were categorized as 0 (low social capital). The Cronbach's alpha for the tool was 0.86.

Ethics

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the National Center for Child Health and Development (approval number: 1147).

Statistical Analysis

First, multivariate regression analyses were conducted to examine the impact of leaving children at home alone on child mental health, adjusting child's sex, living with an older sibling, living with a younger sibling, living with grandparents, maternal age, maternal education, marital status, maternal occupation, income, K6 score, and social capital (Model 1). Moreover, multivariate regression analyses, including interaction term (interaction between leaving children at home alone and social capital), were conducted to examine whether social capital moderates the association between leaving children at home alone and their mental health (Model 2). To deal with missing data in covariates (child's sex, maternal age, maternal education, marital status, maternal occupation, income, and K6 score), multiple imputation was performed using complete data, i.e., status of leaving children at home alone, CRCS score, SDQ total difficulties, prosocial behavior, emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems sores, social capital status, and interaction term of leaving children at home alone and social capital (100 imputed datasets).

Second, propensity-score (PS) matching was used to examine the differences in the characteristics between the two conditions when estimating the impact of leaving children at home alone on child mental health. Thus, we conducted PS matching between children who were never left at home alone and those left at home alone less than once a week, and between children never left at home alone and those left at home alone once a week or more. The possible confounders were demographic variables such as child's sex, having a younger sibling, having an older sibling, living with grandparents, maternal age, marital status, maternal education, maternal occupation, income, K6 score, and social capital, as listed in Table 3. These confounders were selected based on previous studies (8, 27). In PS matching, missing data of confounders were used as dummy variable. Adjusting for these possible confounders, the PS model was estimated using a logistic regression model. PS matching used the following algorithm: 1:1 optimal match with calipers up to 0.01 and no replacement. Before and after PS matching, the balance in the possible confounders in the two conditions within the matched pairs was assessed using the standardized bias. Of the children who were left at home alone less than once a week, 76.8% (n = 1,214) were matched to similar children who were never left at home alone. Of the children who were left at home alone once a week or more, 89.4% (n = 379) were matched to similar children who were never left at home alone. Using the matched pairs, regression analyses were conducted to examine the impact of leaving children at home alone on resilience (CRCS), total difficulties, emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, and prosocial behavior (SDQ). The data were analyzed using STATA version 13.1.

Results

Children Left at Home Alone

Table 1 shows the distribution of the CRCS, SDQ scores, and characteristics by status of leaving children at home alone. In the present study, 2,190 children (52.2%) had never been left at home alone, 1,581 children (37.7%) were left at home alone less than once a week, and 424 children (10.1%) were left at home alone once a week or more among 4,195 children in Adachi City. Children left at home alone less than once a week were less likely to have younger sibling (p < 0.001), more likely to have older sibling (p < 0.001), less likely to live with grandparents (p < 0.001), and their caregiver showed higher K6 score (p < 0.01), compared with those who had never been left at home alone. Similarly, children left home alone once a week or more less likely to have younger sibling (p < 0.001), more likely to have older sibling (p < 0.001), less likely to live with grandparents (p < 0.001), their caregivers were more likely to be unmarried (p < 0.001), their mothers were more likely working full-time or part-time (p < 0.001), their annual household income were less likely to be low (i.e., less than 3 million yen) (p = 0.06), and their caregiver showed higher K6 score (p < 0.01) compared with those who had never been left at home alone.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Distribution of characteristics without multiple imputation.

Multivariate Regression Analysis

Table 2 shows the coefficients of leaving children at home alone for CRCS and SDQ scores by multivariate regression analysis using all data. In terms of the CRCS, in crude model, leaving children at home alone once a week or more showed significantly lower resilience (β = −2.73, 95% CI = −4.34 to −1.13) compared with never leaving children at home alone, and further adjustment of potential confounders, including social capital, the association remain significant and point estimate was similar (β = −2.45, 95% CI = −4.09 to −0.80). Further, leaving children at home alone less than once a week was not significantly associated with resilience in crude model (β = −0.46, 95% CI = −1.45 to 0.54) and after adjustment of potential confounders (β = −0.91, 95% CI = −1.93 to 0.10) (Model 1). As shown in Model 2, social capital did not significantly moderate the association between leaving children at home alone and resilience (p for interaction term = 0.12), suggesting that high or low social capital did not differentiate the association between leaving children at home alone and resilience.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of regression analyses (n = 4,195).

In terms of the SDQ, leaving children at home alone once a week or more was positively associated with the total difficulties score in crude model (β = 2.17, 95% CI = 0.79–3.55) and confounder-adjusted model (β = 2.22, 95% CI = 0.85–3.59) (Model 1). Because the point estimate did not attenuate toward null, potential confounders may not have confounded the association. However, conduct problems (β = 2.61, 95% CI = 0.62–4.60), hyperactivity/inattention (β = 3.38, 95% CI = 0.93–5.83), and peer relationship problems scores (β = 2.69, 95% CI = 0.89–4.49) showed substantial reduction of coefficients in leaving children at home alone once a week or more (Model 1). On the contrary, leaving children at home alone (both less than once a week and once a week or more) was not associated with emotional symptoms (β = 0.19, 95% CI = −1.83 to 2.20) and prosocial behavior scores (β = −1.36, 95% CI = −3.59 to 0.86). Interaction in terms of leaving children at home alone and social capital were not significant in all scores of the SDQ (Model 2). (all p for interaction term > 0.07), suggesting that high or low social capital did not differentiate the association between leaving children at home alone and SDQ score.

Propensity-Score Matching

As shown in Table 1, characteristics were significantly different depending on the frequency of leaving children at home alone and can be considered to have confounded the association between leaving children at home alone and CRCS and SDQ. Thus, we conducted PS matching under two conditions (i.e., children who had never been left at home alone vs. children who had been left at home alone less than once a week, children who had never been left at home alone vs. children who had been left at home alone once a week or more). Table 3 shows the distribution of characteristics after PS matching. The covariate balance within the matched pairs was improved because the standardized bias of almost all covariates was less than 5%. Though some covariates had a standardized bias of more than 5%, the standardized bias of matched pairs decreased from that of the unmatched pairs or the results of their t-test were not significant.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Distribution of characteristics after propensity-score matching.

Table 4 shows the coefficients of leaving children at home alone for CRCS and SDQ scores using fixed effect regression model. There was no significant association between leaving children at home alone less than once a week and never left children at home alone on both CRCS and SDQ scores. In contrast, leaving children at home alone once a week or more was positively associated with the total difficulties (β = 2.67, 95% CI = 0.76–4.59), conduct problems (β = 2.88, 95% CI = 0.14–5.61), hyperactivity/inattention (β = 3.46 95% CI = 0.02 to 6.91), and peer relationship problems scores (β = 3.38, 95% CI = 1.04–5.72) compared with children who had never been left at home alone. However, resilience (β = −2.10, 95% CI = −4.35 to 0.14), emotional symptoms (β = 0.98, 95% CI = −1.68 to 3.63), and prosocial behavior scores (β = −0.77, 95% CI = −3.78 to 2.25) were not significantly associated. These results remained unchanged when the participant was not the mother (data not shown).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Coefficient of leaving children at home alone for the CRCS and the SDQ scores after propensity score matching.

Discussion

This is the first study to examine the association between leaving children at home alone and their mental health in a large cohort of Japanese children aged 6 or 7 years, focusing on both positive and negative aspects. Moreover, this study took into account the effect of social capital more than that of common confounders.

In terms of the association between leaving children at home alone and child mental health, the results indicated that children left at home alone frequently (i.e., once a week or more) was associated with only negative outcomes, specifically, the total difficulties, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer relationship problems scores, although children left at home alone occasionally (i.e., less than once a week) showed no impact on their mental health among 6 or 7 years children in Japan. The current findings are consistent with those of previous studies that revealed that leaving children at home alone increased the risk of accident or crime by children themselves (45), possibly because leaving children at home alone may escalate to further neglect toward children (7). Moreover, this study adds to the literature that there would be no impact of leaving children at home alone on the positive aspects of child mental health.

In this study, we found the impact of leaving children at home alone once a week or more on total difficulties, especially conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer relationship problems. Previous studies showed that poor parent-child relationship is related to child conduct problems [e.g., (46)], hyperactivity/inattention [e.g., (47)], and peer relationship problems (48, 49). Therefore, leaving children at home alone might be associated with parent-child relationships. Further study is needed to examine the mediating role of both community environment and parent-child relationship. Moreover, conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention are often classified as “externalizing” problems (50). In terms of peer relationship, poor peer relationship predicts externalizing behavior and internalizing behaviors (51, 52). Hence, it is likely to be more difficult for parents to detect children internalizing problems (i.e., emotional problems) than externalizing problems (46), which might be caused by children's need for attention from the parents.

To interpret the current findings, it might be helpful to understand the cultural difference between the Japanese and Western cultures, which are characterized by interdependence and independence, respectively (13). Interdependence is a social orientation that is more likely to emphasize on harmony or relatedness, which is more likely found in Japan. Independence is a social orientation that is more likely to emphasize on self-direction, autonomy, and self-expression, which can be observed more in the Western culture. These differences may affect the parenting style in these cultures; Japanese parents are less likely to encourage autonomy and children's personal choice (53), while American parents are more likely to emphasize on individualism or autonomy [e.g., (54)]. Additionally, Japanese parents might expect interdependence with the community, which can be related with social capital (55). Our findings indicated that the negative impact of leaving children at home alone on child mental health remained even though we took into account of the effect of social capital. That is, we showed the adverse effect of leaving children at home alone in Japan, where autonomy is less likely focused in parenting, and the level of interdependence in the community, measured as the social capital, had no impact on this association.

There are several limitations to this study. First, a causal relationship between leaving children at home alone and child mental health cannot be revealed because it was a cross-sectional study. A previous study suggested that the parent–child relationship might deteriorate if the child is left at home alone (6). It is necessary to examine the long-term effects of leaving children at home alone through a longitudinal study. Second, all variables in this study were assessed only by the parent or caregiver. Ideally, the SDQ and resilience should be assessed not only by caregivers, but also by school teachers. Further, the absence of the responses of children themselves might induce a measurement error. Moreover, there are some limitations in accurately detecting the variables related to child mental health using only parental assessment. However, we should be cautious when using the self-rating SDQ with children younger than 11 years (56). It is necessary to understand child behavior as accurately as possible using the teacher-rated SDQ. Third, the participants of this study were children in Adachi City, Tokyo, which is an urban area in Japan. The association between leaving children at home alone and child mental health may differ depending on regional characteristics, public safety, culture, including concept of values, and so on. Especially, these results might not be applicable to other areas such as rural towns. However, child poverty is expected to become a more serious problem in Japan (57). We need to examine the impact of leaving young children at home alone in Adachi City continually and in other areas. Fourth, PS matching may increase the imbalance of covariates and may lead to a selection bias (58). In this study, we checked the imbalance and confirmed that it may not have affected the result of the study.

In conclusion, we found that leaving children at home alone may be linked to child conduct problems or hyperactivity problems, and it was not associated with other positive aspects of mental health. Leaving children at home alone should not be recommended in Japan, similar to the recommendation in North America.

Author Contributions

TF conceived the study, TF, MO, AI and TK managed study and collected data, SD wrote first draft, TF finalized the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the Health Labour Sciences Research Grant, Comprehensive Research on Lifestyle Disease from the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare (H27-Jyunkankito-ippan-002), and Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 16H03276 and 16K21669), St. Luke's Life Science Institute Grants, and the Japan Health Foundation Grants.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

1. ^State of Maryland Family Law 5-801. Confinement in dwelling, building, enclosure, or motor vehicle. Provided by Anne Arundel County Department of Social Services, Annapolis, Maryland. Contact: Madelyn Williams, Protective Supervisor, [410] 269-4701

References

1. Zielewski EH, Malm K, Geen R. Children Caring for Themselves and Child Neglect: When do They Overlap? Washington, DC: The Urban Institute (2006).

Google Scholar

2. Family Defense Center. When can Parent Let Children be Alone? Child Neglect Policy and Recommendations in the Age of Free Range and Helicopter Parenting. Chicago, IL: Family Defense Center (2015).

3. Ruiz-Casares M, Radic I. Legal Age for Leaving Children Unsupervised Across Canada. CWRP Information Sheet #144E. Montreal, QC: McGill University, Centre for Research on Children and Families (2015).

Google Scholar

4. Illinois 720 I11. Comp. Stat. Ann., 5/12-21.5 (West Supp, 1998). As Reported in National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect, National Clearinghouse on Child Abuse and Neglect, National Center for Prosecution of Child Abuse. Crimes: Criminal Neglect and Abandonment. Child Abuse and Neglect State Statutes Series. Vol. V, no.32. Washington, DC: U.S Department of Health and Human Services, National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect (1997).

5. Reiley D, Steinberg J. Four popular stereotypes about children in self-care: implications for family life educators. Fam Relat. (2004) 53:95–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2004.00013.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Ruiz-Casares M, Rousseau C. Between freedom and fear: children's views on home alone. Br J Soc Work (2010) 40:2560–77. doi: 10.1093/bjsw/bcq067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Ruiz-Casares M, Rousseau C, Currie JL, Heymann J. ‘I holed on to my teddiy bear really tight’: children's experiences when they are home alone. Am J Orthopsychiatry (2012) 82:97–103. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2011.01141.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Aizer A. Home alone: supervision after school and child behavior. J Public Econ. (2004) 88:1835–48. doi: 10.1016/S0047-2727(03)00022-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Mertens SB, Flowers N, Mulhall PF. Research on middle school renewal: should middle grades students be left alone after school? Middle Sch J. (2003) 34:57–61. doi: 10.1080/00940771.2003.11494517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Mahoney JL, Lord H, Carryl E. An ecological analysis of after-school program participation and the development of academic performance and motivational attributes for disadvantaged children. Child Dev. (2005) 76:811–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00879.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Marshall NL, Coll CG, Marx F, McCartney K, Keefe N, Ruh J. After-school time and children's behavioral adjustment. Merrill-Palmer Q. (1997) 43:497–514.

Google Scholar

12. Casper LM, Smith KE. Dispelling the myths: self-care, class, and race. J Family Issues (2002) 23:716–27. doi: 10.1177/0192513X02023006002

CrossRef Full Text

13. Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol Rev. (1991) 98:224–53. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Miura A, Fujiwara T, Osawa T, Anme T. Inverse correlation of parental oxytocin levels with autonomy support in toddlers. J Child Fam Stud (2015) 24:2620–5. doi: 10.1007/s10826-014-0064-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Imperial Gift Foundation Boshi-Aiiku-Kai. Almanac of Data on Japanese Children 2017. Tokyo: Chuoh Publishing (2017).

16. Mori N, Armada F, Willcox C. Walking to school in Japan and childhood obesity prevention: new lesson from an old policy. Res Pract. (2012) 102:2068–73. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300913

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. OECD. How's Life? 2015: Measuring Well-Being. Paris: OECD Publishing (2015).

18. Bianchi SM, Casper LM. American Families (Population Bulletin Vol. 55 No. 4). Washington, DC: Population Reference Bureau (2000).

19. Igarashi H, Koukudo S, Matsumoto K. Communication of parents and children and Safety/Danger recognition to the protector in elementary school. Jpn J Hum Growth Dev Res. (2009) 42:1–10. doi: 10.5332/hatsuhatsu.2009.42_1

CrossRef Full Text

20. Bennett T. Factors related to participation in neighborhood watch schemes. Br J Criminol. (1989) 29:207–18. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bjc.a047831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Kawachi I, Subramanian SV, Kim D. Social Capital and Health. New York, NY: Springer (2008).

22. Fukuyama F. Social Capital. Tanner Lecture on Human Values. Oxford: Brasenose College (1997).

23. Fujiwara T, Yamaoka Y, Kawachi I. Neighborhood social capital and infant physical abuse: a population-based study in Japan. Int J Ment Health Syst. (2016) 10:13. doi: 10.1186/s13033-016-0047-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Caughy MO, O'Campo PJ, Muntaner C. When being alone might be better: neighborhood poverty, social capital, child mental health. Soc Sci Med. (2003) 57:227–37. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00342-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Curtis LJ, Dooley MD, Phipps SA. Child well-being and neighbourhood quality: evidence from the Canadian National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth. Soc Sci Med. (2004) 58:1917–27. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Linden JVD, Drukker M, Gunther N, Feron F, Os JV. Children's mental health service use, neighbourhood socioeconomic deprivation, and social capital. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2003) 38:507–14. doi: 10.1007/s00127-003-0665-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Kerrebrock N, Lewit EM. Children in self-care. Future Child. (1999) 9:151–60. doi: 10.2307/1602715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kahn RS, Brandt D, Whitaker RC. Combined effect of mothers' and fathers' mental health symptoms on children's behavioral and emotional well-being. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2004) 158:721–9. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.8.721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Najman JM, Williams GM, Nikles J, Spence SUE, Bor W, O'callaghan M, et al. Mothers' mental illness and child behavior problems: cause-effect association or observation bias? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2000) 39:592–602. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200005000-00013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Adachi city. The Population of the Adachi and Household (2010). (2014). Available online at: http://www.city.adachi.tokyo.jp/somu/ku/aramashi/documents/kokusei2.pdf [in Japanese]

31. OECD. Unemployment Rate (indicator). Paris: OECD Publishing (2017).

32. Luthar SS, Cicchetti D, Becker B. The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Dev. (2000) 71:543–62. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Luthar SS, Zelazo LB. Research on resilience: an integrative review. In: Luthar SS, editor. Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2003). p. 510–49.

Google Scholar

34. Masten AS. Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. Am Psychol. (2001) 56:227. doi: 10.1037//0003-066X.56.3.227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Windle G. What is resilience? A review and concept analysis. Rev Clin Gerontol. (2011) 21:152–69. doi: 10.1017/S0959259810000420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Compas BE, Connor-Smith JK, Saltzman H, Thomsen AH, Wadsworth ME. Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychol Bull. (2001) 127:87. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.87

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Compas BE, Jaser SS, Dunbar JP, Watson KH, Bettis AH, Gruhn MA, et al. Coping and emotion regulation from childhood to early adulthood: points of convergence and divergence. Aust J Psychol. (2014) 66:71–81. doi: 10.1111/ajpy.12043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Lazarus RS. Coping theory and research: past, present, and future. Psychosom Med. (1993) 55:234–47. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199305000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Doi S, Fujiwara T, Ochi M, Isumi A, Kato T. Association of sleep habits with behavior problems and resilience of 6- to 7-year-old children: Results from the A-CHILD study. Sleep Med. (2018) 45:62–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Matsuishi T, Nagano M, Araki Y, Tanaka Y, Iwasaki M, Yamashita Y, et al. Scale properties of the Japanese version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): a study of infant and school children in community samples. Brain Dev. (2008) 30:410–5. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2007.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. (1977) 38:581–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Furukawa TA, Kawakami N, Saitoh M, Ono Y, Nakane Y, Nakamura Y, et al. The performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and K10 in the world mental health survey Japan. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. (2008) 17:152–8. doi: 10.1002/mpr.257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Sakurai K, Nishi A, Kondo K, Yanagida K, Kawakami N. Screening performance of K6/k10 and other screening instruments for mood and anxiety disorders in Japan. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2011) 65:434–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2011.02236.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: a multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science (1997) 277, 918–24.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

45. Peterson L. Latchkey children's preparation for self-care: overestimated, under-rehearsed, and unsafe. J Clin Child Psychol. (1989) 18:36–43.

Google Scholar

46. Stadelmann S, Perren S, Wyl AV, Klitzing KV. Associations between family relationships and symptoms/strengths at kindergarten age: what is the role of children's parental representations? J Child Psychol Psychiatr. (2007) 48:996–1004. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01813.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hawes DJ, Dadds MR, Frost ADJ, Russell A. Parenting practices and prospective levels of hyperactivity/inattention across early- and middle- childhood. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2013) 35:273–82. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9341-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Healy KL, Sanders MR, Iyer A. Parenting practices, children's peer relationships and being bullied at school. J Child Fam Stud. (2015) 24:127–40. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9820-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Kerns KA, Klepac L, Cole A. Peer relationships and preadolescents' perceptions of security in the child-mother relationship. Dev Psychol. (1996) 32:457. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.3.457

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Goodman A, Lamping DL, Ploubidis GB. When to use broader internalizing and externalizing subscales instead of the hypothesized five subscales on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): data from British parents, teachers and children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2010) 38:1179–91. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hymel S, Rubin KH, Rowden L, LeMare L. Children's peer relationships: longitudinal prediction of internalizing and externaliziing problems from middle to late childhood. Child Dev. (1990) 61:2004–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb03582.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Laird RD, Jordan KY, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. Peer rejection in childhood, involvement with antisocial peers in early adolescence, and the development of externalizing behavior problems. Dev Psychopathol. (2001) 13:337–54. doi: 10.1017/S0954579401002085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Chao R, Tseng V. Parenting of Asians. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of Parenting: Social Conditions and Applied Parenting, Vol 4. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (2002). p. 59–93.

Google Scholar

54. Harwood RL, Miller JG, Irizarry NL. Culture and Attachment: Perceptions of the Child in Context. New York, NY: Guilford Press (1997).

Google Scholar

55. Nahapiet J, Ghoshal S. Social capital, intellectual capital, and the organizational advantage. Acad Manage Rev. (1998) 23:242–66.

56. Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatr. (2000) 177:534–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.6.534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

57. OECD Doing Better for Children. Paris: OECD Publishing (2009).

58. Iacus SM, King G, Porro G, Katz JN. Causal inference without balance checking: coarsened exact matching. Polit Anal. (2012) 20:1–24. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpr013

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: leaving children at home alone, resilience, difficult behavior, prosocial behavior, Japan

Citation: Doi S, Fujiwara T, Isumi A, Ochi M and Kato T (2018) Relationship Between Leaving Children at Home Alone and Their Mental Health: Results From the A-CHILD Study in Japan. Front. Psychiatry 9:192. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00192

Received: 26 October 2017; Accepted: 23 April 2018;
Published: 25 May 2018.

Edited by:

Masoumeh Dejman, Johns Hopkins University, United States

Reviewed by:

Reinhold Kilian, Universität Ulm, Germany
Erich Flammer, ZfP Südwürttemberg, Germany

Copyright © 2018 Doi, Fujiwara, Isumi, Ochi and Kato. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Takeo Fujiwara, ZnVqaXdhcmEuaGx0aEB0bWQuYWMuanA=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.