Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 02 May 2017
Sec. Addictive Disorders

The Role of Self-esteem and Fear of Negative Evaluation in Compulsive Buying

  • Department of Education Studies, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy

Compulsive buying is a relatively new addictive disorder that interferes with everyday functioning and may result in serious psychological and financial problems (1). A very few data are currently available regarding this behavioral addiction. This study investigated gender differences in the relationships between contingent self-esteem (CSE), fear of negative evaluation (FNE), and compulsive buying. Participants included 240 Italian adults (170 females, M age = 33.80) who responded to self-report questionnaires. The results showed that women scored higher on CSE and FNE scales than men. No gender differences were found in compulsive buying tendencies. CSE and FNE were positively related to CB. Furthermore, structural equation modeling confirmed the evidence on CSE as a strong predictor of CB for both genders. Interestingly, FNE seems to play a mediating role between CSE and compulsive buying behaviors only for women. These findings highlight the importance of studying self-esteem in compulsive buying tendencies to inquire more deeply into the underlying mechanisms of some compulsive behaviors.

Introduction

Compulsive shopping is a dysfunctional form of excessive and pervading buying (24). The consumer experiences an unstoppable, chronic, and repeated impulse to go shopping and spend money and loses control of this activity (5). The psychosocial consequences of compulsive buying (or shopping) behavior can be summarized as: a great amount of debts, inability to meet payments, legal and financial problems, self-criticism, guilt, and personal suffering [e.g., Ref. (6, 7)].

Results from a meta-analysis of 40 studies from 16 countries reported a prevalence of compulsive buying of 4.9% in the adult population (8) and the age of onset appears to be around 30 (1, 9).

Most clinical studies report that women are much more likely to become compulsive buyers than men [e.g., Ref. (10)] but, in the last decade, scholars have observed no significant differences based on gender (1114).

Compulsive buying is primarily motivated by the relief of anxiety, stress, and unpleasant emotions (1517).

Dysfunctional consumer behavior is often enacted for some perceived psychological advantages (18). In particular, the desire to improve emotions related to self-esteem is a prominent motivation that can predict this form of behavioral addiction (19, 20). Ertelt et al. (21) showed that compulsive buyers suffer from low self-esteem and seek approval from others to compensate it.

The “consumption of objects” may be a coping response to unpleasant emotions deriving from insecurity about individuals’ selves and identities and disappointing self-evaluations (16, 22). Some people may use shopping to improve their appearance, self-confidence, consideration by others, and social relationships (23). Material possessions respond illusorily to the inner security need to compensate feelings of low self-esteem and, at the same time, current consumer culture suggests that a sense of self-confidence and well-being can be purchased. Therefore, the “consumption of objects” may be both a symptom of insecurities and a “coping strategy” to offer relief from problems and to satisfy certain needs (2426).

Some products play a peculiar role for compulsive buyers due to the emotional and social advantages they offer (27). By buying objects, compulsive shoppers try to address to an “ideal self,” improve their social image and express themselves (20), such that perceived self-esteem benefits represent a direct predictor of compulsive buying (18).

Contingent Self-esteem (CSE)

At the same line, Roberts et al. (28), in accordance with the Self-Determination Theory (29, 30), argue that self-esteem varies along a continuum that ranges from true self-esteem to CSE. True self-esteem is a more stable individual characteristic that is not dependent upon meeting external standards or others’ approval. At the other end of the continuum is CSE, where one’s self-esteem is dependent on matching some self-imposed or external standard or meeting some objective. Individuals with a high CSE score may base their self-worth on their physical attractiveness, social conditions, or job performance (31, 32).

In people with high CSE, searching and maintaining positive self-definition becomes the main orientation, expressed also through their behaviors. They direct their efforts to appearing worthy to others and feel that they must achieve something in order to justify their positive feelings toward themselves (29, 33, 34).

Very little research has focused on gender differences in CSE. The few studies indicated that women report greater CSE compared to men (32, 35).

Previous studies have investigated the role of CSE in relation to body image and body dissatisfaction [e.g., Ref. (36)] or in relation to mental health and alcohol use [e.g., Ref. (37, 38)]. To our knowledge, only one previous study (28) has investigated the predictive role of CSE on compulsive buying behavior.

The Fear of Others’ Judgment

In addition, compulsive shoppers, especially women, give more importance to disapproval and are more likely to engage in social comparison than others (39). Indeed, compulsive shoppers are more inclined to base their instable self-esteem on the judgment of others and often purchase things to impress other people (40, 41).

Roberts et al. (28) proposed the mediation role of the fear of negative evaluation (FNE) on CSE and compulsive buying. They confirmed the importance of CSE to predict compulsive buying through the mediation of FNE, in particular for the adult population. Specifically, FNE is defined as a personal trait consisting in apprehension about others’ evaluations, distress over their negative evaluations, and the desire to seek social approval and avoid disapproval (42). To date, it has been considered a cognitive and emotional risk factor for social anxiety [e.g., Ref. (43)] and eating disorders, especially for women [e.g., Ref. (44, 45)]. A previous study investigated the FNE as a potential mediator between social anxiety and disordered eating in females (46). Generally, studies reported that women scored significantly higher on the FNE scale than men [e.g., Ref. (47)]. However, much of the research on women’s social comparison and fear of evaluation has been in the field of body image and attractiveness (48) and does not include compulsive buying.

The main objective of this study is to deepen our knowledge of compulsive buying and gender differences in its underlying mechanisms. Specifically, the aim is to examine if and how CSE predicts compulsive buying through the mediation of FNE in men and women.

Hypotheses

Our first hypothesis concerns the differences between men and women in the variables analyzed. Specifically, in line with the literature previously described, we predict that women will score higher on CB, CSE, and FNE than men (H1).

Our second hypothesis is summarized in the theoretical model presented in Figure 1. More specifically, we expect that, in both men and women, CSE predicts CB (H2). As shown by the literature, CSE is an individual characteristic referring to feelings about oneself that are dependent on matching some standard or living up to others’ expectations; this fragile sense of self is a risk factor for CB in both genders.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The theoretical model.

Our third hypothesis is that FNE fully or partially mediates the relationship between CSE and CB (H3). The prediction is expected to be stronger for women, on account of their greater sensitivity to others’ judgments, their higher social self-consciousness, and their supposed greater FNE and punishment (49).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Two hundred forty subjects [M = 29.2% (70); F = 70.8 (170)] aged between 18 and 61 (mean age = 33.80, SD = 10.61) were recruited online. As regards marital status, 148 (61.7%) were unmarried, 75 (31.2%) were married or living with a partner, 16 (6.7%) were divorced or separated, and 1 (0.4%) was widowed.

Sixteen participants (6.7%) had a primary school or a junior high certificate, 83 (34.6%) had a high school diploma, 72 (30.0%) had graduated in the first cycle degree, 50 (20.8%) had graduated in the second cycle degree, and 19 (7.9%) had a specialization or Ph.D.

With regards socioeconomic status, 44.6% declared they had an annual income of less than 15,000 Euros, 37.9% between 15,000 and 30,000, 9.6% between 30,000 and 45,000, 3.3% between 45,000 and 60,000, and 1.3% more than 60,000.

Procedure

An online survey was constructed using LimeSurvey software (http://www.limesurvey.org). Participants were contacted through social networks (e.g., Facebook) and by e-mail spreading. Participants were asked to answer some questions on purchasing behavior. No personal identifying information was collected. As standard procedure for minimal-risk online survey, the study waived documentation of informed consent, by permitting continued participation to signal consent. The Ethics Commission of the institution where the author work approved this survey, which was conducted in agreement with the ethical norms laid down by the Italian National Psychological Association.

Measures

The questionnaire included a first section regarding age, sex, education level, income, time spent in shopping, average amount spent per shopping trip, credit card usage, and influence of advertising on purchases.

Compulsive Buying

A questionnaire including 13 statements [compulsive buying scale (CBS); (50)], evaluated on a 5-point scale of disagreement/agreement, in its Italian version (5), was used to measure compulsive buying. Sample items include the following: “When I have money, I cannot help but spend part or all of it”; “At times, I have felt somewhat guilty after buying a product, because it seemed unreasonable.” Cronbach’s α = 0.91.

Contingent Self-esteem

An 8-item scale (31) was used to measure CSE (evaluated on a 5-point scale of disagreement/agreement). Although the original scale contains 15 items, we used the version tested by Roberts et al. (28) measuring self-esteem contingencies with regard to gaining others’ approval. Sample items include the following: “My overall feelings about myself are heavily influenced by how much other people like and accept me” and “My overall feelings about myself are heavily influenced by how good I look.” Cronbach’s α = 0.81.

Fear of Negative Evaluation

The brief version of the FNE (51) was used to measure fears of being observed and evaluated by others. We deleted four items according to the version proposed by Roberts et al. (28). Sample items include the following: “I am afraid others will not approve of me” “When I am talking to someone, I worry about what they may be thinking of me.” Subjects rate their agreement with statements on a 5-point scale, where 1 = not very characteristic of me to 5 = very characteristic of me. Cronbach’s α = 0.92.

Analytical Procedures

Data were analyzed using SPSS version 18 (Chi-square and ANOVA analysis) to test Hypothesis 1. Subsequently, the mediation model (Hypothesis 2 and Hypothesis 3) was tested using multiple-step regression analysis by the PROCESS macro for SPSS (52). In particular, we used the so-called “Model 4” (simple mediation model) methodology.

Results

Compulsive Buying

Preliminarily, in order to explore clinical implications, we examined the rates of compulsive shoppers according to the Italian version of the CBS (5) cutoff score (42). In this sample, the CBS scores ranged from 14.35 to 55.35 (M = 30.60, SD = 9.61) and 12.9% (N = 31, M = 6; F = 25) obtained scores below the proposed cutoff for CB. Applying the cutoff score, we compared the compulsive and non-compulsive buyers (non-CBs) on variables of interest.

Chi-square test showed that compulsive buyers do not differ from non-CB on marital status, level of education, or income.

ANOVA analysis (see Table 1) showed that the compulsive buyers have a higher average of time spent shopping (hours per week) than the non-CB. In shopping experiences, the former are more influenced by advertising and have higher scores on CSE and FNE than the latter. However, the two groups did not differ in money spent per shopping trip and in the frequencies of credit card usage.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means and ANOVA differences for compulsive buyers (CBs) and non-compulsive buyers (non-CBs).

Gender Differences in Buying Behaviors and Variables of Interest

Comparing male and female groups, the Chi-square test showed that women do not differ from men on marital status (χ2 = 2.59, df = 4, p < n.s.). With regard to level of education (χ2 = 33.52, df = 6, p < 0.000), the results showed that more females had a first cycle degree (35.3%) than males (17.7%). In addition, there were more males with only a primary school or a junior high certificate (17.2%) than females (1.2%). Women (50%) show a higher quota with a low income (χ2 = 20.88, df = 5, p < 0.001) than men (31.4%), and men are more represented in the range between 45,000 and 60,000 Euros, and more than 60,000 Euros (11.4 vs 1.8%).

As regards the most purchased products, women buy more clothing/shoes than men (61.2 vs 31.4%) and men are more interested in technological objects (22.9 vs 2.4%) and in sports equipment (18.6 vs 4.1%) than women (χ2 = 58.37, df = 9, p < 0.000).

ANOVA analysis comparing genders showed that women were younger than men, used credit cards less frequently, and spent less money per week in shopping activities.

The findings revealed that women have higher scores than men on the CSE and FNE scales; there was no significant difference between genders in the CBS scores and in the media influence on purchasing (see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means and ANOVA differences for gender and shopping behavior.

We then examined Pearson correlations between age, buying behaviors, and constructs of interest (CBS, CSE, and FNE) (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations.

Mediation Analysis

To analyze the hypothetical mediation of FNE on the effect of CSE on compulsive shopping for men and women (Hypothesis 3), two sets of regressions were computed. Multiple regression analyses showed that Set 1 (men) explained 0.37 while Set 2 (women) 0.29 of the variance in compulsive buying.

As can be seen in Figure 2, the significant estimated paths were significant on both sets of regression except that of the mediating effect of FNE on compulsive buying for men. In both sets, CSE positively predicted FNE, whereas FNE positively predicted CB only for women.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Multiple regression analyses predicting compulsive buying for men (Set 1) and women (Set 2). Note: coefficients are unstandardized regression coefficients (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001).

Regarding the mediating effect of FNE, the effect of CSE on compulsive buying was partially mediated by FNE only for women (Set 2: indirect effect B = 0.2110, sobel test z = 2.28, p < 0.02; Set 1: indirect effect B = 0.16, sobel test z = 1.29, p = n.s.).

Discussion

Preliminary results of our investigation into compulsive buying showed that the prevalence in our convenience sample agrees with data reported in the literature concerning Canada and European countries (5, 50, 53).

The time spent on shopping experiences is directly correlated to the CBS score. The result is consistent with McElroy et al. (1) who argued that the dimension of time spent on purchases is one of the proper diagnostic criteria of compulsive buying. Compulsive buyers spend more time on shopping trips than non-CB, but do not spend as much money; data support the hypothesis that it is the experience of browsing rather than purchasing itself that characterizes the disorder (41).

Moreover, in accordance with Maraz et al. (41), CB is not directly related to socioeconomic status or net income. In line with other studies [e.g., Ref. (5)], the findings did not reveal any relation between compulsive buying and credit card usage.

As regard the age influence, the findings reported that age is not related to compulsive shopping or to CSE while the FNE decrease with increased age. About this, Westenberg et al. (54) have argued that individual differences in social evaluation fear may be related to differential level of maturity.

As far as gender differences are concerned, our data partially confirmed the literature previously described suggesting that women scored higher on CSE and FNE scales than men (H1). In agreement with Mueller et al. (55), but nonetheless surprisingly, no significant difference was found between women and men in compulsive buying. Although the vast majority of people with compulsive buying problems have been estimated to be women [e.g., Ref. (56)], the gender differences reported by the literature may be artifactual and a bias could be due to the fact that women are more prone to admitting that they enjoy shopping than men (57).

Consistent with prior research [e.g., Ref. (6, 58, 59)], our findings suggest that appearance-based objects such as clothes and shoes are most frequently purchased by women. Such image-based objects strengthen feelings of self-worth by regulating emotions, gaining social approval, and expressing an idealized self (18).

However, men tend to buy electronic or hardware goods more than women and buying objects such as technological items could be a strategy to signal social status and to impress others also in men. Dittmar et al. (60) reported that men tend to impulsively buy instrumental and leisure items associated with independence and activity, while women tend to purchase symbolic items associated with appearance and emotional aspects of self (e.g., attractiveness) (61, 62). Choosing certain objects to buy can be gender-specific, but the motivation for purchases may be similar, for instance to impress others.

In addition, differently from the literature [e.g., Ref. (63)], we did not find any gender differences in the time spent on shopping trips. Instead, women spend less money and use credit cards less than men. It should be noted that in our sample, the men are on average older and have a higher annual income than women. Moreover, in accordance with the literature (64), male consumer characteristics are different from those of females and men are generally more willing “to take risks with money” than women. Some authors (65) argued that men who shop prefer to see themselves as fulfilling an instrumental need, rather than engaging in shopping per se. They bypass the female relational aspects of shopping experience, looking for bargains, and are more prone to spending in order to pursue business goals.

As hypothesized (H2), the survey findings revealed that CSE impacts on compulsive buying behavior in both genders. CSE proved to be a strong predictor of CB. Theories on self-esteem long held that the sense of self is largely built through social interaction with significant others. Indeed, feelings of self-esteem tend to result from the approval of others. For individuals who view approval as contingent to success, such as getting good grades, winning a game, or living up to standards of physical attractiveness, self-esteem becomes largely dependent on the others’ judgment (32). In agreement with Roberts et al. (28), our model suggested that individuals higher in CSE are more likely to become compulsive shoppers. Specifically, the mechanism underlying compulsive buying may be the process of “symbolic self-completion” (60). Some people need others to acknowledge that they possess a specific self-definition. To feel a sense of completion in his/her own self-definition, an individual must engage in behaviors related to symbols (e.g., possessing and wearing them) insofar as he/she convinces others, and consequently him/herself, which he/she owns the self-definition hoped for.

Some authors (66) have suggested that appearance-related items (e.g., clothes, shoes, and status symbol objects) are often used as symbols in the process of self-completion “because they show others who you are.” Some individuals may feel a chronic state of incompleteness in their self-definitions and may experience the urge to buy objects linked to their desired self-definitions despite the fact that they have purchased too much, resulting in serious problems for their life (60).

Instead, even if CSE predicts FNE in both genders, this variable appears to have no effect on compulsive buying for men. These data do not fully confirm the findings of Roberts et al. (28) and suggest that a certain level of CSE may lead women to consider compulsive shopping as an “antidote” for FNE whereas this is not true for men. Those high in CSE are more likely to buy compulsively, but this relationship is partially mediated by the FNE only in women (H3).

However, the absence of mediation between FNE and CB in men was unexpected. Men with high CSE are more likely to use shopping as a coping strategy but are less affected by the fear of external judgment, or are less prone to admitting it.

To explain this, in accordance with some authors [e.g., Ref. (67)], we assume that one of the most significant differences between men and women is the difference in their self-concept, compatible with gender stereotypes. In particular, males and females differ in regard to how much they define themselves as autonomous agents or they view themselves as connected with others (34). In Western societies, females can develop an “interdependent self-concept,” and conversely, males develop an “independent self-concept,” with reference to self-definitions of being “separated from others,” following “individualistic goals,” and being motivated “to show uniqueness by power over others” (68). Contrary to this, interdependent self-concept, more typical of females, refers to self-definitions such as “connection with others,” where “relationships are perceived as integral parts of one’s being” (34). Some authors argued that women who are more socially self-conscious suffer from greater self-discrepancies (49). Higgins (69) proposed that women who feel there are discrepancies between their actual attributes and their sense of how others think they ought to be also experience increased FNE and punishment, which may result in an unhealthy cycle of self-discrepancy and fear of evaluation (70). Conversely, males show a weaker sensitivity to the opinions of others (71).

One other explanation for the observed gender differences in the mediation role of FNE is the particular pressures faced by women to conform to societal standards. Women are held to more prescriptive and higher social standards compared to men (72), and violating these standards carries more threat of penalty and punishment for women than men (73). FNE may enhance the negative effects of failing to live up to one’s standards, particularly for women.

In summary, the models tested in the present study have contributed to the understanding of psychological processes underlying compulsive buying behavior; in specific, they explained how CSE and the fear of disapproval are involved in this behavioral addiction. The findings suggest that individuals who have a stable sense of worth, and do not depend on others’ evaluation, could theoretically have a barrier against the risk of developing such dysfunctional behavior. Conversely, CSE and FNE represent risk factors for problematic addictions, especially in women. Paradoxically, individuals who base their self-worth on contingencies continue to compare themselves with others, thus setting up a vicious circle of self-disapproval. Compulsive shopping can, therefore, appear as an illusory and counterproductive solution to these negative feelings.

Limitations and Implications

Although this research expands the current knowledge on gender differences in the mechanisms underlying CB, such as CSE and FNE, it presents certain limitations.

The main limitation of the study is that its cross-sectional nature precludes the assessment of cause–effect relationships. That is, although the results suggest statistical predictive effects, these should be more properly tested in a longitudinal model.

Next, the external validity of our findings is limited by the sampling procedure. We used a convenience sample retrieved online that does not allow generalization of the results. The age of the participants is not homogeneous; as FNE is inversely related to age, it would be interesting to test if the mediation model changes for more homogeneous samples (e.g., late adolescence or elderly). Another limitation is the low male response rate. The problem of the low response of men in compulsive buying studies has been recognized in the literature: males are reluctant to respond to inquiries regarding shopping (18).

Furthermore, other psychopathological disorders have not been explored; first of all, depression is a dimension very often associated with pathological purchases that might affect differently men and women as a risk factor for compulsive buying (74). Moreover, comorbidity with other behavioral addictions should be investigated (75).

However, despite these limitations, the results of our study may also have implications for clinical intervention. Identifying predictors and mediators of CB is important for tailoring addiction prevention programs and for improving interventions more suited to individual psychological characteristics. Overall, the findings provide suggestions for future appropriate interventions focused on personality characteristics. These interventions should distinguish between different personality features, taking into account gender differences in CSE. Future research should also focus on men’s and women’s motivations to buy, which could predict or mediate individual vulnerability traits and compulsive buying.

Ethics Statement

APA ethical standards were followed in the conduct of the study.

Author Contributions

The author designed the study, conducted the statistical analysis, and wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer, MN, and handling editor declared their shared affiliation, and the handling editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

Funding

The author did not receive any financial support.

References

1. McElroy SL, Keck JPE, Pope JHG, Smith JM, Strakowski SM. Compulsive buying: a report of 20 cases. J Clin Psychiatry (1994) 55:242–8.

Google Scholar

2. Black DW. Compulsive buying: clinical aspects. Impulse control disorders. In: Aboujaoude E, Koran LM, editors. Impulse Control Disorders. California: Stanford University School of Medicine (2010). p. 5–22.

Google Scholar

3. Faber RJ, O’Guinn TC. A clinical screener for compulsive buying. J Consum Res (1992) 19(3):459–69. doi: 10.1086/209315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Faber RJ, O’Guinn TC, Krych R. Compulsive consumption. Adv Consum Res (1987) 14(1):132–5.

Google Scholar

5. Biolcati R, Melotti G. Contribution to the internal validation of the Italian version of the Compulsive Buying Scale (Valance, d’Astous and Fortier, 1988). BPA Appl Psychol Bull (2013) 266:13–20.

Google Scholar

6. Black DW. A review of compulsive buying disorder. World Psychiatry (2007) 6(1):14–8.

Google Scholar

7. Christenson GA, Faber RJ, Mitchell JE. Compulsive buying: descriptive characteristics and psychiatric comorbidity. J Clin Psychiatry (1994) 55(12):545–6.

Google Scholar

8. Maraz A, Griffiths MD, Demetrovics Z. The prevalence of compulsive buying: a meta-analysis. Addiction (2016) 111:408–19. doi:10.1111/add.13223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Pavarin RM, Biolcati R. Women between normality and dependence: a study of problematic consumption and dependence in women aged 30–50 years. J Subst Use (2015) 20(2):143–50. doi:10.3109/14659891.2013.878759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. O’Guinn TC, Faber RJ. Compulsive buying: a phenomenological exploration. J Consum Res (1989):147–57. doi:10.1086/209204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Koran LM, Faber RJ, Aboujaoude E, Large MD, Serpe RT. Estimated prevalence of compulsive buying behavior in the United States. Am J Psychiatry (2006) 163(10):1806–12. doi:10.1176/ajp.2006.163.10.1806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. MacLaren VV, Best LA. Multiple addictive behaviors in young adults: student norms for the Shorter PROMIS Questionnaire. Addict Behav (2010) 35(3):252–5. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Müller A, Claes L, Georgiadou E, Möllenkamp M, Voth EM, Faber RJ, et al. Is compulsive buying related to materialism, depression or temperament? Findings from a sample of treatment-seeking patients with CB. Psychiatry Res (2014) 216(1):103–7. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Otero-López JM, Villardefrancos E. Prevalence, sociodemographic factors, psychological distress, and coping strategies related to compulsive buying: a cross sectional study in Galicia, Spain. BMC Psychiatry (2014) 14(1):101. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-14-101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. d’Astous A. An inquiry into the compulsive side of “normal” consumers. J Consum Policy (1990) 13(1):15–31. doi:10.1007/BF00411867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Donnelly G, Ksendzova M, Howell RT. Sadness, identity, and plastic in over-shopping: the interplay of materialism, poor credit management, and emotional buying motives in predicting compulsive buying. J Econ Psychol (2013) 39:113–25. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2013.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Ridgway NM, Kukar-Kinney M, Monroe KB. An expanded conceptualization and a new measure of compulsive buying. J Consum Res (2008) 35(4):622–39. doi:10.1086/591108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Dittmar H. Compulsive buying – a growing concern? An examination of gender, age, and endorsement of materialistic values as predictors. Br J Psychol (2005) 96(4):467–91. doi:10.1348/000712605X53533

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Dittmar H, Long K, Bond R. When a better self is only a button click away: associations between materialistic values, emotional and identity-related buying motives, and compulsive buying tendency online. J Soc Clin Psychol (2007) 26(3):334. doi:10.1521/jscp.2007.26.3.334

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Kukar-Kinney M, Ridgway NM, Monroe KB. The role of price in the behavior and purchase decisions of compulsive buyers. J Retailing (2012) 88(1):63–71. doi:10.1016/j.jretai.2011.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ertelt TW, Marino JM, Mitchell JE, Lancaster K. Current status of cognitive-behavioral intervention for compulsive buying disorder. J Contemp Psychother (2009) 39(4):213–20. doi:10.1007/s10879-009-9121-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Kasser T, Ahuvia A. Materialistic values and well-being in business students. Eur J Soc Psychol (2002) 32(1):137–46. doi:10.1002/ejsp.85

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Richins ML. Materialism, transformation expectations, and spending: implications for credit use. J Public Policy Mark (2011) 30(2):141–56. doi:10.1509/jppm.30.2.141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kasser T, Ryan R. Further examining the American dream: differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Pers Soc Psychol Bull (1996) 22:280–7. doi:10.1177/0146167296223006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Roberts JA. Compulsive buying among college students: an investigation of its antedecents, consequences, and implications for public policy. J Consum Aff (1998) 32(2):295–319. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6606.1998.tb00411.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Roberts JA, Pirog SF III. Personal goals and their role in consumer behavior: the case of compulsive buying. J Mark Theory Pract (2004) 12(3):61–73. doi:10.1080/10696679.2004.11658513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Horváth C, Birgelen MV. The role of brands in the behavior and purchase decisions of compulsive versus noncompulsive buyers. Eur J Mark (2015) 49(1/2):2–21. doi:10.1108/EJM-10-2012-0627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Roberts JA, Manolis C, Pullig C. Contingent self-esteem, self-presentational concerns, and compulsive buying. Psychol Mark (2014) 31(2):147–60. doi:10.1002/mar.20683

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Deci EL, Ryan RM. Human autonomy. In: Kernis MH, editor. Efficacy, Agency, and Self-esteem. New York: Springer US (1995). p. 31–49.

Google Scholar

30. Deci EL, Ryan RM. The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol Inq (2000) 11(4):227–68. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Kernis MH. Toward a conceptualization of optimal self-esteem. Psychol Inq (2003) 14(1):1–26. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1401_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Patrick H, Neighbors C, Knee CR. Appearance-related social comparisons: the role of contingent self-esteem and self-perceptions of attractiveness. Pers Soc Psychol Bull (2004) 30(4):501–14. doi:10.1177/0146167203261891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Blom V. Contingent self-esteem, stressors and burnout in working women and men. Work (2012) 43(2):123–31. doi:10.3233/WOR-2012-1366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Marcic R, Grum DK. Gender differences in self-concept and self-esteem components. Stud Psychol (2011) 53(4):373.

Google Scholar

35. Grossbard JR, Lee CM, Neighbors C, Larimer ME. Body image concerns and contingent self-esteem in male and female college students. Sex Roles (2009) 60(3–4):198–207. doi:10.1007/s11199-008-9535-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Bailey SD, Ricciardelli LA. Social comparisons, appearance related comments, contingent self-esteem and their relationships with body dissatisfaction and eating disturbance among women. Eat Behav (2010) 11(2):107–12. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Crocker J. The costs of seeking self-esteem. J Soc Issues (2002) 58(3):597–615. doi:10.1111/1540-4560.00279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Luhtanen RK, Crocker J. Alcohol use in college students: effects of level of self-esteem, narcissism, and contingencies of self-worth. Psychol Addict Behav (2005) 19(1):99. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Giacomantonio M, Mannetti L, Pierro A. Locomoting toward well-being or getting entangled in a material world: regulatory modes and affective well-being. J Econ Psychol (2013) 38:80–9. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2012.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Lejoyeux M, Weinstein A. Compulsive buying. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse (2010) 36(5):248–53. doi:10.3109/00952990.2010.493590

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Maraz A, van den Brink W, Demetrovics Z. Prevalence and construct validity of compulsive buying disorder in shopping mall visitors. Psychiatry Res (2015) 228(3):918–24. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Latimer AE, Martin Ginis KA. The importance of subjective norms for people who care what others think of them. Psychol Health (2005) 20(1):53–62. doi:10.1080/08870440412331300002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Haikal M, Hong RY. The effects of social evaluation and looming threat on self-attentional biases and social anxiety. J Anxiety Disord (2010) 24(3):345–52. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Hinrichsen H, Wright F, Waller G, Meyer C. Social anxiety and coping strategies in the eating disorders. Eat Behav (2003) 4(2):117–26. doi:10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00016-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Gilbert N, Meyer C. Fear of negative evaluation and the development of eating psychopathology: a longitudinal study among nonclinical women. Int J Eat Disord (2005) 37(4):307–12. doi:10.1002/eat.20105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Levinson CA, Rodebaugh TL, White EK, Menatti AR, Weeks JW, Iacovino JM, et al. Social appearance anxiety, perfectionism, and fear of negative evaluation: distinct or shared risk factors for social anxiety and eating disorders? Appetite (2013) 67:125–33. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Duke D, Krishnan M, Faith M, Storch EA. The psychometric properties of the brief fear of negative evaluation scale. J Anxiety Disord (2006) 20(6):807–17. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2005.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Franzoi SL, Vasquez K, Sparapani E, Frost K, Martin J, Aebly M. Exploring body comparison tendencies women are self-critical whereas men are self-hopeful. Psychol Women Q (2012) 36(1):99–109. doi:10.1177/0361684311427028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Calogero RM, Watson N. Self-discrepancy and chronic social self-consciousness: Unique and interactive effects of gender and real–ought discrepancy. Pers Indiv Differ (2009) 46(5–6):642–7. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Valence G, d’Astous A, Fortier L. Compulsive buying: concept and measurement. J Consum Policy (1988) 11(4):419–33. doi:10.1007/BF00411854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Leary MR. A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Pers Soc Psychol Bull (1983) 9:371–5. doi:10.1177/0146167283093007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. Guilford Press (2013).

Google Scholar

53. Hassay DN, Smith MC. Compulsive buying: an examination of the consumption motive. Psychol Market (1996) 13(18):741. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(199612)13:8<741::AID-MAR2>3.0.CO;2-F

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Westenberg PM, Drewes MJ, Goedhart AW, Siebelink BM, Treffers DA. A developmental analysis of self-reported fears in late childhood through mid-adolescence: social-evaluative fears on the rise? J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2004) 45:481–95. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00239.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Mueller A, Mitchell JE, Crosby RD, Gefeller O, Faber RJ, Martin A, et al. Estimated prevalence of compulsive buying in Germany and its association with sociodemographic characteristics and depressive symptoms. Psychiatry Res (2010) 180:137–42. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Schlosser S, Black DW, Repertinger S, Freet D. Compulsive buying: demography, phenomenology, and comorbidity in 46 subjects. Gen Hosp Psychiatry (1994) 16(3):205–12. doi:10.1016/0163-8343(94)90103-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Fattore L, Melis M, Fadda P, Fratta W. Sex differences in addictive disorders. Front Neuroendocrinol (2014) 35(3):272–84. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Dittmar H, Long K, Meek R. Buying on the Internet: gender differences in on-line and conventional buying motivations. Sex Roles (2004) 50(5–6):423–44. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000018896.35251.c7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Harnish RJ, Bridges KR. Compulsive buying: the role of irrational beliefs, materialism, and narcissism. J Ration Emot Cogn Behav Ther (2015) 33(1):1–16. doi:10.1007/s10942-014-0197-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Dittmar H, Beattie J, Friese S. Objects, decision considerations and self-image in men’s and women’s impulse purchases. Acta Psychol (1996) 93(1):187–206. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(96)00019-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Cox J, Dittmar H. The functions of clothes and clothing (dis) satisfaction: a gender analysis among British students. J Consum Policy (1995) 18(2–3):237–65. doi:10.1007/BF01016513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Mueller A, Claes L, Mitchell JE, Faber RJ, Fischer J, de Zwaan M. Does compulsive buying differ between male and female students? Pers Individ Dif (2011) 50(8):1309–12. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Bakewell C, Mitchell VW. Male versus female consumer decision making styles. J Bus Res (2006) 59(12):1297–300. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Areni CS, Kiecker P. Gender differences in competitiveness and risk aversion: theoretical foundations and some preliminary findings. In: Costa JA, editor. Proceedings of the Second Conference on Gender and Consumer Behavior. (Vol. 30), Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Printing Service (1993). 43 p.

Google Scholar

65. Otnes C, McGrath MA. Perceptions and realities of male shopping behavior. J Retailing (2001) 77(1):111–37. doi:10.1016/S0022-4359(00)00047-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Gollwitzer PM, Wicklund RA, Hilton JL. Admission of failure and symbolic self-completion: extending Lewinian theory. J Pers Soc Psychol (1982) 43(2):358. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.43.2.358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Maddux WW, Brewer MB. Gender differences in the relational and collective bases for trust. Group Process Intergr Relat (2005) 8(2):159–71. doi:10.1177/1368430205051065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Cross SE, Madson L. Models of the self: self-construals and gender. Psychol Bull (1997) 122(1):5–37. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Higgins ET. Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychol Rev (1987) 94(3):319–40. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Liss M, Schiffrin HH, Rizzo KM. Maternal guilt and shame: the role of self-discrepancy and fear of negative evaluation. J Child Fam Stud (2013) 22(8):1112–9. doi:10.1007/s10826-012-9673-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Shoaf FR, Scattone J, Morrin M, Maheswaran D. Gender differences in adolescent compulsive consumption. NA Adv Consum Res (1995):22.

Google Scholar

72. Prentice DA, Carranza E. What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: the contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychol Women Q (2002) 26(4):269–81. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.t01-1-00066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Rudman LA, Glick P. Feminized management and backlash toward agentic women: the hidden costs to women of a kinder, gentler image of middle managers. J Pers Soc Psychol (1999) 77(5):1004. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Leite PL, Silva AC. Prevalence study of compulsive buying in a sample with low individual monthly income. Trends Psychiatry Psychother (2015) 37(4):209–12. doi:10.1590/2237-6089-2015-0015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Granero R, Fernández-Aranda F, Steward T, Mestre-Bach G, Baño M, del Pino-Gutiérrez A. Compulsive buying behavior: characteristics of comorbidity with gambling disorder. Front Psychol (2016) 7:625. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: compulsive buying, contingent self-esteem, fear of negative evaluation, gender differences, adult population

Citation: Biolcati R (2017) The Role of Self-esteem and Fear of Negative Evaluation in Compulsive Buying. Front. Psychiatry 8:74. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00074

Received: 03 February 2017; Accepted: 18 April 2017;
Published: 02 May 2017

Edited by:

Luigi Janiri, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy

Reviewed by:

Lucia Romo, Paris West University Nanterre La Défense, France
Marco Di Nicola, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy

Copyright: © 2017 Biolcati. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Roberta Biolcati, r.biolcati@unibo.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.