Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Plant Sci., 17 December 2020
Sec. Functional Plant Ecology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Role of Flower Color in Angiosperm Evolution View all 30 articles

Floral Color Diversity: How Are Signals Shaped by Elevational Gradient on the Tropical–Subtropical Mountainous Island of Taiwan?

Updated
  • 1Department of Life Science, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 2Institute of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 3School of Media and Communication, RMIT University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 4Department of Entomology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan

Pollinators with different vision are a key driver of flower coloration. Islands provide important insights into evolutionary processes, and previous work suggests islands may have restricted flower colors. Due to both species richness with high endemism in tropical–subtropical environments, and potentially changing pollinator distributions with altitude, we evaluated flower color diversity across the mountainous island of Taiwan in a comparative framework to understand the cause of color diversity. We sampled flower color signaling on the tropical–subtropical island of Taiwan considering altitudes from sea level to 3300 m to inform how over-dispersion, random processes or clustering may influence flower signaling. We employed a model of bee color space to plot loci from 727 species to enable direct comparisons to data sets from continental studies representing Northern and Southern Hemispheres, and also a continental mountain region. We observed that flower color diversity was similar to flowers that exist in mainland continental studies, and also showed evidence that flowers predominantly had evolved color signals that closely matched bee color preferences. At high altitudes floras tend to be phylogenetically clustered rather than over-dispersed, and their floral colors exhibited weak phylogenetic signal which is consistent with character displacement that facilitated the co-existence of related species. Overall flower color signaling on a tropical–subtropical island is mainly influenced by color preferences of key bee pollinators, a pattern consistent with continental studies.

Introduction

Floral color is a key functional trait that affects plant–pollinator interactions (Oberrath and Böhning Gaese, 1999; Bradshaw and Schemske, 2003; Ômura and Honda, 2005), contributing to biodiversity and structure of plant communities (Sargent and Ackerly, 2008; Schemske et al., 2009). The variation of floral color diversity (FCD) may reflect how plant–pollinator interactions and evolutionary history shape color signaling (Weevers, 1952; Chittka and Menzel, 1992; McEwen and Vamosi, 2010). Whilst there has been increased interest on the studies of FCD for continental floras around the world (Arnold et al., 2009a,b; Dyer et al., 2012; Shrestha et al., 2014; Gray et al., 2018), relevant studies of island floras are rarely reported (Makino and Yokoyama, 2015; Shrestha et al., 2016).

Island data can provide important insights for a better understanding the global patterns of floral color resulting from plant–pollinator interactions, and how threats like species invasion, habitats loss and/or pollinator extinction (Cox and Elmqvist, 2000; Brooks et al., 2002; van Kleunen et al., 2015) may impact environments. Compared to continental environments, islands can provide an assessable model with insular nature to study the FCD (Emerson and Gillespie, 2008). Interestingly, previous studies report that oceanic island floras [e.g., Macquarie Island (MI) (Shrestha et al., 2016), Juan Fernández Islands (JFI) (Bernardello et al., 2001) and sub-Antarctic Campbell Island (Lord et al., 2013) display simple and inconspicuous colors and have low FCD. Even on larger oceanic islands, floras in New Zealand (NZ) mountains consist of constrained flower color signals, exhibiting much lower FCD (Bischoff et al., 2013b) when compared to its adjacent continental island of Australia (Webb and Kelly, 1993; Dyer et al., 2012). It’s worth noting that the low species richness of these oceanic islands, e.g., MI (37 angiosperm species, de Salas and Baker (2015) and JFI (152 species), may more or less determine their low FCD, as plant communities with larger species richness would often, but not always, have greater FCD (Arnold et al., 2009b; Shrestha et al., 2014; Bergamo et al., 2018; Gray et al., 2018).

Oceanic island plants were mainly colonized from continents within a short evolutionary history and often lacks guilds of pollinators, thus low FCD was expected. Unlike oceanic islands, Taiwan is an atypical island, i.e., isolation with partial reconnection during glaciation and it has been repeatedly in contact with the Asian continent (Voris, 2000), which allows plants to readily migrate into Taiwan (Hsieh, 2002; Huang, 2011; Li et al., 2013). Moreover, Taiwan is situated on the transition zone between subtropical and tropical region, potentially contributing to a greater FCD, as biota have long been thought to exhibit greater color diversity in tropics than high latitudes (Wilson and von Neumann, 1972; Adams et al., 2014). The islands of Taiwan are located at the boundary between the Holarctic and Paleotropical floristic kingdoms indicating the diverse evolutionary origins of local flora (Hsieh, 2002). Indeed, Taiwan harbors greater species richness (up to 3,500 flowering species) and higher specific endemism (26.1% of native plants) than other oceanic islands (Hsieh, 2002), we thus might expect a greater FCD in Taiwan, especially with respect to other island studies.

In addition to having high species richness, Taiwan also has mountainous topography, with steep altitudinal gradients up to 3952 m a.s.l. and accessible flowering plants to 3300 m a.s.l. within a relatively short distance. This creates altitudinal vegetation zones from tropical/foothill lowland evergreen broad-leaved forest to subtropical evergreen and temperature deciduous mountain forest, and to mountain peaks as cool temperate upper-montane coniferous and subalpine boreal forest or alpine tundra (Li et al., 2013). The percentage of flowering plant endemism in Taiwan therefore increases dramatically along altitude from 15% at sea level to reach 60% among most mountain peaks (Hsieh, 2002). This provides us with an appealing opportunity to incorporate how floral colors are shaped along altitudinal gradients of the tropical–subtropical island, as previous works have studied the variation of FCD along altitudes in several continents (Arnold et al., 2009b; Shrestha et al., 2014; Gray et al., 2018), but to the best of our knowledge this has not been tested on an island.

Considering mountainous environments, Bergamo et al. analyzed 71 plant species in 700–1800 m a.s.l. and found the FCD was greater in low altitudes than that in middle and high altitudes (Bergamo et al., 2018), whereas Shrestha et al. (2014) analyzed 107 plant species in Nepal and found a converse pattern, i.e., greater the FCD in subalpine region (3000–4100 m, n = 61) than subtropical regions (900–2000 m, n = 46). Interestingly, in the Himalayan mountains of Nepal there is also a high diversity of bumblebees and some other insects (Thapa, 2000; Williams et al., 2010) that act as pollinators of flowering plants (Shrestha et al., 2014; Paudel et al., 2015, 2016, 2018). These studies suggested that the species richness (represented by sample sizes) may be a main factor influencing FCD, and altitude as a factor may only mediate probability of species richness depending upon temperature range in a particular region of the world. To contribute to our understanding of the complexity of FCD and climatic conditions due to changing altitude, our work included a large sample size in each altitude group (>140 species) and covered an entire altitude gradient over which flowering plants exist in Taiwan (from 0 to 3300 m). This enables formally testing if species richness is a key factor to determine the FCD in an environment.

In addition, floral color assembly (FCA) demonstrates the similarity of floral colors between species within a given region, i.e., if sympatric floras tend to display similar colors (cluster) or divergent colors (over-dispersion). When compared to FCD, an investigation of FCA can enable interpretations of how species evolutionary history and plant–pollinator interaction shape the floral colors within different plant communities (Gumbert et al., 1999; McEwen and Vamosi, 2010; Makino and Yokoyama, 2015).

Several studies used null models based on random assembly to evaluate the pattern of FCA, and revealed that FCA was non-random in most plant communities and highly shaped by the plant–pollinator interaction (de Jager et al., 2011; Muchhala et al., 2014; Makino and Yokoyama, 2015; Ohashi et al., 2015; Kemp et al., 2019), although evidence of random assembly also exists for the continental island of Australia (Shrestha et al., 2019b). Competition for pollinator visitations was the major selection force to shape the FCA of plant communities in high latitudes and high altitudes, which often caused floral color over-dispersion (Muchhala et al., 2014; van der Kooi et al., 2016; Bergamo et al., 2018), but there is also some evidence of clustering at high altitudes (Bergamo et al., 2018). As both number and abundance of pollinators are thought to generally decrease with increasing altitudes (Warren et al., 1988; Bingham and Orthner, 1998), but see Shrestha et al. (2014) for a different effect in the Himalayan mountains, it is plausible this may affect FCA.

At a global scale large islands have relatively more bee species (ca. 0.0011–0.0420 spp. per square kilometer, and Taiwan 0.005) than are present on continental land masses (ca. 0.0004–0.0015) (Buchmann, 1994). Bees are often more effective in pollination than other insects, e.g., bees deliver more pollen in each visitation, and visit more flowers per minute than the flies (Bingham and Orthner, 1998; Bischoff et al., 2013a) as well as often exhibiting flower constancy (Chittka et al., 1999). In our current study, we thus not only investigated the floral colors in the human color category, but also translated their reflectance spectra into color loci in sensory color space of the bee (bee hexagon) which permits a robust interpretation of how important bee pollinators perceive floral color signals (Chittka, 1992; Dyer, 2006; Kemp et al., 2019; Shrestha et al., 2019b). This advanced technique is possible because for bees there is detailed knowledge about their phylogenetically conserved trichromatic vision including receptors, neural processing and calibration with behavioral data (Chittka, 1992; Dyer et al., 2011; Garcia et al., 2017; Shrestha et al., 2019a,b). The bee color hexagon model was used to quantify the FCD (Shrestha et al., 2014, 2016), and was also used to evaluate the FCA by applying the mean pair-wise distance (MPD) of the color loci (McEwen and Vamosi, 2010; Makino and Yokoyama, 2015; Shrestha et al., 2019b).

Studies of floras from both Southern and Northern Hemispheres consistently exhibited a predominant direction of color loci in bee hexagon (Chittka et al., 1994; Dyer et al., 2012; Shrestha et al., 2014, 2019b), which overlapped with the short-wavelength “blue” color preferences of several bee species that have been formally tested (Giurfa et al., 1995; Raine and Chittka, 2005; Dyer et al., 2016). Such distinct color preferences likely exist due to the overlap of the phylogenetically conserved color photoreceptors in bees from around the world (Peitsch et al., 1992; Giurfa et al., 1995; Raine and Chittka, 2005; Morawetz et al., 2013; Dyer et al., 2016), and strongly suggest that the bee color preferences are the major selection force that shape the floral colors in regions where bees are common pollinators (Shrestha et al., 2019b). Indeed, when bees are not the pollinator of flowers then very different distributions of flower colors are observed (Lunau et al., 2011; Shrestha et al., 2013, 2016, 2019a; Burd et al., 2014; Camargo et al., 2019).

The multiple origins of Taiwan flora and complex island habitats thus may generate unique floral colors as flowers adapt to different pollinators along elevation gradients. We therefore would like to test whether flower color diversity is comparable to these adjacent continent and islands. We also would like to ask whether flower color is constrained for attracting limited pollinators on islands or diverges for competing pollinator visiting. Our null position is that flower color in Taiwan is not significantly different to mainland continental studies. Thus, in the current study, we firstly evaluate whether the floras in tropical–subtropical island Taiwan exhibited low FCD consistent with other island studies, or if the favorable conditions of a tropically located mountain island may promote higher FCD consistent with, or even greater than, mainland continental studies. We secondly investigate if there is evidence of variation of FCD considering three defined different altitudinal zones in TW, and evaluate if any observed variation might be dependent upon species richness. For our analyses we consider the evidence or whether FCA of plants in TW are over-dispersed, random or clustered based on low, mid and high-altitude plants using the phylogenetic-informed analysis, whereby we could interpret the possible driving force and ecological process accounting for the patterns of FCA. Finally, we use a comparative framework to evaluate if bee color preferences are also a likely factor influencing the distribution of floral colors in Taiwan, as has been recently demonstrated in other parts of the world.

Materials and Methods

Study Area and Sample Collection

The main island of Taiwan lies in East Asia located in between 21.916107 N - 25.086991 N, 120.787456 E - 121.899446 E with a land area of 35,808 square kilometers. The island is surrounded by South and East China Seas, and at the nearest point is 130 km from any other major continental land mass (Figure 1). The main island lies in a complex tectonic region of Yangtze Plate to the west and north, the Okinawa Plate on the north-east, and the Philippine Mobile Belt on the east and south, formed approximately four to five million years ago (Wu, 1978; Biq et al., 1985). Moreover, many high mountains in Taiwan (over 100 peaks over 3000 m a.s.l.) have created topographically isolated habitats and fast-changing climatic zones (from tropical low land forest to alpine tundra) along elevation changes. The multiple origins of Taiwan flora and complex island habitats thus may have potentially generated very unique FCD in different habitats along elevation. However, very little was known about Taiwanese or even Asian flower coloration with respect to animal pollinator color vision prior to our study.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Location of Taiwan in red square (A), and the maps of 18 sampling sites from three altitudinal regions in Taiwan (B). Maps are prepared using package Maps in R version 3.4.4.

We selected our study sites in the Taiwanese main island and two nearby offshore islands named Green Island and Orchid Island. We classified these sites into different altitudinal regions, i.e., low (0–900 m a.s.l.), middle (1500–2200 m a.s.l.) and high-altitude regions (2800–3300 m a.s.l.) (Supplementary Table S1). Sample sites in low-altitude comprise multiple types of vegetation zones including coastal, tropical and subtropical mountain zonal forest; middle sites are dominated by subtropical evergreen cloud forest, while the high-altitude sample site contained high-mountain coniferous woodlands and forests (Li et al., 2013). We chose a total of 18 sample sites from three altitudinal regions: 11 sites in low, 6 sites in middle, and 1 site in high-altitude (details in Supplementary Table S1). These sampling sites cover major altitudinal vegetation zones in Taiwan. We mostly choose the National Parks in different altitudinal regions, which were well-studied and represent all vegetation types of the study region. The number of sites tested reflected availability in respective National Parks. We sampled one high-altitudinal site covering a 10 km2 mountain range of several higher than 3200 m mountain peaks at the Taroko national park. All flower samples were native to Taiwan and were collected from March 2016 to September 2017, covering two periods of the peak blooming (Tai, 2018). Our dataset comprised of a total of 727 native flowering species (399 species in low-altitude, 186 species in middle-altitude, and 142 species in high-altitude) [see details Supplementary Table S2: species list and altitude, Dryad digital repository (Tai et al., 2020)] including ca 33% endemic species to Taiwan. Some example flowers collected at different altitudinal regions is given in Figure 2. We also used the data from Australia (Burd et al., 2014), Nepal (Shrestha et al., 2014), and Japan (Makino and Yokoyama, 2015) to enable a comparative framework to other regions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Examples of representative native flowering species from low (A–D), middle (E–H) and high (I–L) altitudes of Taiwan. (A) Rhododendron formosanum; (B) Rhododendron oldhamii; (C) Titanotrichum oldhamii; (D) Cirsium japonicum var. australe; (E) Odontochilus bisaccatus; (F) Paris polyphylla var. stenophylla; (G) Stellaria arisanensis; (H) Conandron ramondioides; (I) Parnassia palustris; (J) Pedicularis verticillata; (K) Gentiana scabrida var. punctulata; (L) Aconitum fukutomei.

Floral Color Measurement

Floral colors were measured using an Ocean Optics spectrophotometer (Ocean Optics Inc., USB-4000+, United States) with a UV-VIS-NIR light source (Ocean Optics Inc., DH-2000-BAL, United States) and a quartz fiber-optic probe (Ocean Optics Inc., Lab-grade Reflection Probes, United States). The reflectance spectra were measured from 300 to 700 nm (see Supplementary Method S1) and then processed by the software OCEAN VIEW (Ocean Optics Inc., United States). Additional details for the measurement of reflectance spectra were given in Supplementary Method S1 and Supplementary Figure S1 for the details of marker point comparison to show the evidence of bees are the primary pollinator of flowering plants at low, middle, and high altitudes in Taiwan.

Hymenopteran Color Space Modeling and Descriptors of Floral Colors

To analyze the FCD as perceived by bees, we used the bee color hexagon model which is widely accepted in relevant studies (Chittka, 1992; Chittka and Menzel, 1992; Dyer et al., 2012; Shrestha et al., 2014, 2019b; Kantsa et al., 2018; Kemp et al., 2019). In this model, the reflectance spectrum of each species is translated into a locus in a two-dimensional plane of the hexagon color space (Chittka, 1992) by employing photoreceptor sensitivities for a bee (350, 440, and 540 nm) (Dyer, 1999), standard D65 illumination (Judd et al., 1964) corrected for photon flux and assuming the visual system was adapted to the leaf-green background (reflectance spectra of the leaves for all samples in our study, see Supplementary Method S1). We categorized hexagon color space in to six sub-sectors (BLUE: B; BLUE GREEN: BG; GREEN: G; UV-GREEN: UG; ULTRAVIOLET: UV; UV-BLUE: UB) following the method used in Chittka et al. (1994). Each color locus in bee hexagon was also specified as Polar co-ordinate (a certain vector angle θ and vector magnitude r) or Cartesian co-ordinate (a certain x and y) (Chittka et al., 1994).

To quantitatively compare the overall FCD among different geographic regions and different altitudinal regions, we calculated the area of the minimum convex polygon (MCP) which encapsulate all color loci using the function ‘Polygon’ in R package SP (version no. 3.4.4).

For descriptors of floral spectral signals considering bee color vision, color hue can be defined as the vector angle θ of color loci in bee hexagon, while color contrast can be defined as the vector magnitude r from the achromatic center of color space that represents background information to the locus of a color (Chittka, 1992). Color distance is the Euclidean distance (Cartesian co-ordinate) between two color loci in bee hexagon (Chittka, 1992; Garcia et al., 2017).

Phylogenetic Tree

To access phylogenetic-informed analysis for our three altitudinal group data, we constructed the phylogenetic tree for all species using Phylomatic 3.0 (Webb and Donoghue, 2005), which was widely used in the multispecies analysis (Stournaras et al., 2013; Shrestha et al., 2014; Zanne et al., 2014; Ohashi et al., 2015). We used Wikström et al. (2001, 2015) to calibrate the many of the nodes and used other published references whereas our genus label phylogeny were based on Soltis et al. (2011) (see details in Supplementary Table S3). In the output phylogenetic tree, relationship among all family level and some genera level clades (e.g., in Rosaceae and Fabaceae) were left as unresolved polytomies (Figure 3). A phylogenetic distance of each pairwise species was computed with function ‘cophenetic.phylo’ in R package APE. The output tree Appendix A available in Dryad Digital Repository (Tai et al., 2020).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Phylogenetic relationship of the species in our samples. Terminal branches were painted as the floral color of each species in human vision, which was generated from the reflectance spectrum with function ‘spec2rgb’ of R package PAVO. Solid circles at the tip represents the altitude region of the species (red: low-altitude, green: middle-altitude, blue: high-altitude).

Floral Color Assembly (FCA)

We used mean-pairwise distance (MPD) for the color loci in bee hexagon, which we termed MPDcolor, to evaluate the pattern of FCA in low, middle and high altitudes. First, we calculated the actual mean pairwise distance (actual MPDcolor) for all color loci of each altitude. This actual MPDcolor will be compared with the mean of nulls MPDcolor (indicating random assembly), which is generated from randomly resampling the color loci from species pool for 1,000 times. Species pool comprises all samples in our study and the sample size for each resampling is identical to the species number within that region.

The pattern of FCA is determined by the comparison between actual MPDcolor and the mean of nulls MPDcolor. We computed all MPDcolor and its significance p-value with function ‘ses.mpd’ in R package PICANTE (Kembel et al., 2010) (details provided in Supplementary Method S2).

Phylogenetic Structure

We used mean-pairwise distance (MPD) to evaluate the pattern of phylogenetic structure of species in low, middle and high altitudes, except that phylogenetic distance rather than color distance is the variable analyzed here (thus termed MPDphylo). The pattern of phylogenetic structure was determined by the same procedure of comparison between actual MPDphylo and the mean of nulls MPDphylo as that of FCA described above. That the actual MPDphylo was lower than the mean of nulls MPD infers species in that region tend to be closely related to each other (phylogenetic cluster). On the contrary, if actual MPDphylo was larger than the mean of nulls MPDphylo this would infer that species tend to be distantly related to each other (phylogenetic over-dispersion). Likewise, higher p-value indicates stronger phylogenetic over-dispersion, while lower p-value indicates phylogenetic cluster. We used the PICANTE function ses.mpd for these calculations (Kembel et al., 2010). In addition, we also calculated mean nearest taxon distance (MNTDphylo) to evaluate the phylogenetic structure and details for these metrics are given in the Supplementary Method S2.

Phylogenetic Signal

We examined phylogenetic signal with the commonly used Blomberg’s K in previous studies (Blomberg et al., 2003; Muchhala et al., 2014; Shrestha et al., 2014). We calculated the Blomberg’s K values of the two key floral color descriptors: color hue (Shrestha et al., 2014), and color contrast (referred to as saturation in some studies) in bee hexagon (Lunau, 1990; Kantsa et al., 2017) of low, mid and high-altitude floras in our study using the function ‘phylosig’ in R package PHYTOOLS (Revell, 2012). We also performed Mantel test to evaluate the phylogenetic signal (Muchhala et al., 2014; Shrestha et al., 2014; Bergamo et al., 2018). Additional details of the calculations for Blomberg’s K and Mantel test are given in the Supplementary Method S3.

Results

Among 727 flowering plant species, the most common floral color in human vision was white (45%), followed by yellow (18%), pink (13%), and purple (11%) whereas species with green (4%), blue (3%), red (3%), and other/orange (3%) flowers were less frequent in Taiwan. Our analysis of their reflectance spectra showed that almost all white flowers are UV-absorbed white (e.g., Figure 4b), consistent with previous studies (Inouye and Pyke, 1988; Dyer, 1996; Kevan et al., 1996). Spectra of yellow flowers could be categorized into two types (Dyer, 1996) based on the presence of UV (67%, e.g., Figure 4a) and absence UV (33%) reflection.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Example reflectance spectra of two native species in our study (a) Odontochilus bisaccatus (dash yellow curve, human yellow) and (b) Clematis montana (black line curve, human white).

Floral Color Diversity (FCD)

We used bee color space to evaluate the pattern of FCD in Taiwan (TW) and compared the patterns to that of the continental island of Australia (AUS), the mountains of Nepal (NPL) and to Japan (JPN) (temperate continental islands) (Figures 5A–D). Color loci of floras in TW covered a broad range in bee hexagon (Figure 5A), which was comparable to that of continental floras from AUS (Figure 5C) and mountains of NPL (Figure 5D). The Area of MCP (representing FCD) of color loci in TW was 0.558 (hexagon squared units) which was larger than that (0.498) for AUS, but lower than that (0.783) for both NPL and JPN 0.588 (details Supplementary Table S5), thus suggesting overall a somewhat similar pattern to what has been previously observed.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Bee color hexagon showing the floral color loci of species from four separate geographic regions. (A) 727 species of Taiwan; (B) 212 species of Japan (Makino and Yokoyama, 2015); (C) 201 species of Australia (Dyer et al., 2012; Burd et al., 2014); (D) 107 species of Nepal (Shrestha et al., 2014). We recalculated the color loci in bee hexagon of Japanese samples based on the published reflectance spectra in Makino and Yokoyama (2015).

The color loci of floras in TW concentrated to two distinct clusters in bee hexagon. One cluster has the greatest frequency toward the bee-BG vertex of bee hexagon, which can be termed ‘White Arm’ because most floral colors inside were human white (Figure 5A). Similarly, the other cluster lay toward bee-UG vertex can be termed ‘Yellow Arm’ as it was made up of human yellow flowers (Figure 5A). We also found similar ‘White and Yellow Arms’ in the pattern of JPN (Figure 5B), and these colors are also frequent flower hues from AUS (Figure 5C) and the mountains of NPL (Figure 5D), although the “arm” is less distinct.

We evaluated the variation of FCD between low, middle, and high altitudes in Taiwan by comparing the area of MCP of the color loci in bee hexagon (Figure 6). We found the area of MCP of low-altitude (0.546) was larger than that in high-altitude (0.304) and almost twice as that of middle-altitude (0.270). It was possible that the unequal sample size between different altitudes (from low to high: n = 399, 186 vs. 142) may bias the areas of MCP. We thus conducted permutation test to resolve this argument, and the result revealed that the areas of MCP of middle and high altitudes persisted to be lower than that of low-altitude (Supplementary Method S4 and Supplementary Figure S2). Thus, this result suggested that floras of low-altitude in Taiwan exhibited greater FCD than that of middle and high altitudes.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Bee color hexagon showing floral color loci of species from low, middle, and high altitudes in Taiwan. (A) 399 species from low-altitude, (B) 186 species from middle-altitude, and (C) 142 species from high-altitude.

Color Hexagon Sector

The most predominant peak of absolute frequency for floral color loci of all Taiwanese floras (i.e., from low to high altitudes) in bee hexagon sector occurred around 60°, which lies on the bee’s dominant preference regions (Figure 7). In addition, when compared to the global floral color distribution, Taiwanese data exhibited no other distinct peaks outside the bee preference regions.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Absolute frequency distribution of floral color loci in 10° sector of bee color hexagon as proposed by Chittka et al. (1994). Distributions for species from low, middle and high altitudes in Taiwan (yellow, green and blue symbols and lines) are plotted, and compared to the global floral color distribution in hexagon color space (inset). The inset figure shows the distribution of species across the Australian continent (brown symbols and line; data from Dyer et al., 2012; Shrestha et al., 2019b), along sub-tropical and sub-alpine transects of the Nepalese Himalayas (blue symbols and line; data from Shrestha et al., 2014) and Israel (green symbols and line; data from Chittka et al., 1994). The dotted rectangle encloses the bee-blue and bee-blue–green sectors of the color space (30°–90°, see inset) where honeybees (Giurfa et al., 1995), bumblebees (Raine and Chittka, 2005) and the stingless bee Tetragonula carbonaria (Dyer et al., 2016) all demonstrate their strongest innate color preference.

Floral Color Assembly (FCA)

In order to test if floral colors in Taiwan show evidence of either over-dispersion or clustering, we compared the actual mean pairwise distance (MPDcolor) of the color loci in bee hexagon to the nulls MPDcolor based on random assembly. We found the actual MPDcolor of high-altitude species (0.245) was larger than that (0.227) of the mean of nulls MPDcolor (random assembly), and its p-value was 0.930 (Table 1). This suggested that the floral colors of high-altitude species may have diverged from each other, i.e., floral color over-dispersion. On the other hand, floral colors of middle-altitude species are significantly clustered (p = 0.040), whereas MPDcolor of low-altitude species appeared to be best explained by a random assembly (p = 0.599) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of floral color assembly (FCA) for three altitudes zones in Taiwan considering bee visit species and other insect visit species in high-altitude.

Given that not all plant species in high-altitude were pollinated by bees as other insect pollinators like flies are relatively more frequent in cooler environments (Totland, 1993; Arnold et al., 2009b; Lord et al., 2013; Paudel et al., 2016; Shrestha et al., 2016), we specifically evaluated the FCA of the species visited by either bees or other insects (see Supplementary Method S5). If the calculated MPDcolor and relative p-value increase, this indicates that high-altitude plant species associating with the bees or other insects exhibit more divergent floral colors. Indeed, we found that the MPDcolor and relative p-values of both bee visit (MPDcolor = 0.260, p = 0.950) and insect visit (MPDcolor = 0.267, p = 0.980) species in high-altitude has increased (Table 1), showing that floral colors exhibited stronger over-dispersed within high-altitude species visited by bee and insect group.

Phylogenetic Structure

To clarify if relatedness of species accounted for the floral color over-dispersion within high-altitude plant community, we analyzed the comparative phylogenetic structure of species in the low, mid and high-altitude zones by calculating the mean pairwise distance (MPDphylo) and mean nearest taxon distance (MNTDphylo) (details in Table 2). Both actual MPDphylo and MNTDphylo for species in low-altitude were close to the mean of nulls MPDphylo and MNTDphylo (indicative of random assembly), and the p-values (0.639 and 0.645) also suggested that the phylogenetic structure of low-altitude species approached to random assembly (Table 2). The MPDphylo for middle-altitude species was close to the mean of nulls MPDphylo (p = 0.540), whereas the MNTDphylo was much lower than the mean of nulls MNTDphylo (p = 0.098) (Table 2). These revealed that middle-altitude species were randomly assembled across the entire phylogenetic tree, but clustered toward the tips of the phylogenetic tree. Further, we found the MPDphylo value of high-altitude species (260.6) was lower than the mean of nulls MPDphylo (267.0) (p = 0.174), this indicated that high-altitude species are more closely related to each other across the entire phylogenetic tree (Table 2). Additionally, actual MNTDphylo of high-altitude species was significantly lower than that of the mean of nulls MNTDphylo (p = 0.004) (Table 2), which supported the evidence that high-altitude species were significantly clustered toward the tips of the phylogenetic tree.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of the phylogenetic structure of low, middle and high-altitude species in Taiwan.

Phylogenetic Signal

The K values for two color descriptors (color hue, color contrast) that are known to be key factors in flower signaling were significantly deviated from random expectation (K = 0) and lower than that expected by Brownian motion evolution (K = 1) at low and high-altitude (Table 3). As all K values of low and high-altitudes (0.275∼0.454) were closer to zero than one, this suggested that the key descriptor values were more divergent to each other among closely related species in low and high-altitudes (i.e., weak phylogenetic signal). Moreover, Mantel test of correlation supported this weak phylogenetic signal at high altitude (r = 0.008, p = 0.417). This suggested that floral colors are liable, thus these closely related species do not necessarily have similar floral colors and may adapt to optimal pollinator in a particular environment. Interestingly, K value for color hue (K = 0.629) of middle altitude species did not deviate from 1 (Table 3), indicating floral colors of closely related species in middle altitude are more similar as that expected by Brownian motion evolution. Consistently, the Mantel test detected a relatively stronger correlation between phylogenetic distance and color distance of the middle altitude species (r = 0.125, p = 0.005). These data thus revealed a stronger phylogenetic signal, i.e., closely related species in middle-altitude plant community shared more similar floral colors than that in a low and high-altitude plant community.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Phylogenetic signal for the floral color descriptors of species in low, mid, and high-altitude of Taiwan.

Discussion

How flowers evolved color signals in different environments is important for understanding the plant–pollinator interactions that have occurred in the past and driven evolutionary processes (Fenster et al., 2004), and what might happen in the future with changing climatic conditions and/or pollinator distributions (Hegland et al., 2009; Shrestha et al., 2018). Based on large scales studies, species in tropical regions have been proposed to exhibit more diverse colors compared to the higher latitudes, and this may result from greater strength of biotic interaction in tropics to accelerate the evolution of coloration (Schemske et al., 2009; Dalrymple et al., 2015). However, previous island studies away from the tropics tend to show low FCD.

In the current study, we considered the uniquely placed island of Taiwan and used a hexagon color space model to plot flower loci and subsequently calculate FCD as the minimum convex polygon (MCP) encapsulating all loci. Our data revealed that the overall FCD of TW was 0.558, which is greater than AUS (0.498), but less than JPN (0.588) or NPL (0.783). Thus, despite the species richness of a mountainous tropical island with close proximity to mainland plant sources that we hypothesized should lead to high color diversity within Taiwan, it appears the data falls in the middle of FCD values for our comparative country data sets. We found that ca. 33% of the floras in our collected TW samples are endemic species, which have MCP (0.400) (Supplementary Table S4) and accounted for 72% of the overall FCD in TW, suggesting that flower color signaling is very consistent to our comparative data sets from different parts of the world. We also observed that within TW there was evidence of changes in color signaling with increasing altitude as considering low (0–900 m a.s.l.), middle (1500–2200 m a.s.l.) and high-altitude regions (2800–3300 m a.s.l.) the respective FCD values were 0.546 (399 species), 0.270 (186 species) and 0.304 (142 species) (Figure 6). Such a change did not appear to be caused simply by the number of species since at high-altitude there was a higher FCD, but actually a lower number of plant species compared to the mid-altitude region.

Taiwan is an atypical island as its low isolation level allows plant species to readily migrate to TW from subtropical Asia (e.g., China) and tropical Asia (e.g., Philippines) (Hsieh, 2002; Huang, 2011), and there is ample habitat diversity to host a range of potential pollinators. The co-shared floras covered 95% of the total FCD in TW (Supplementary Table S4). Our results suggest that observed differences in FCD at different altitudes may be mediated by lineage diversity, rather than plant species richness. The evidence for this is threefold. Firstly, we found FCD in low altitudes was greater than that in middle and high altitudes. This was because some particular color floras belonging to the families, e.g., Aristolochiaceae, Zingiberaceae, Fabaceae and Rubiaceae (Mussaenda) (Figure 3), were common in low altitudes but less frequent or absent in middle and high altitudes, and the color loci the flowers of these plants were often distributed toward the outer regions of hexagon color space resulting in an increase the FCD at low altitudes (Figure 6). Secondly, the phylogenetic structure of low-altitude floras was of over-dispersed, whilst the phylogenetic structure of high-altitude floras showed evidence of being clustered (Table 2). This implies that lineages of low-altitude floras contain more diverse groups than that of high-altitude floras do. Thirdly, the results of our permutation test revealed that the greater FCD in low altitude was neither due to the larger sample size, nor species richness (Supplementary Figure S2). When considering lineage diversity as the key explanation for why FCD changed with altitude, our finding is consistent with the findings of previous studies in Norway and Nepal as the lineage diversity could be represented by the amount of family and genera. The greater FCD in low altitudes of Norway was linked to greater lineage diversity (24 families, 47 genera) than in high altitudes (13 families, 14 genera) (Bergamo et al., 2018) (Supplementary Table S5). Similarly, as lineages are more diverse in flowering samples from high altitude (25 families, 45 genera) than that from low altitude (17 families, 38 genera) of Nepal, greater FCD was instead observed in high altitude (Shrestha et al., 2014) (Supplementary Table S5). Taken together these comparative findings suggest that lineage diversity is a key factor mediating FCD.

Lázaro et al. (2008) reported that flowers of white and yellow appearance to a human observer are represented by a more diverse range of plant species when considering the total number of plant species in an environment. Color trapping experiments in alpine Australia (Pickering and Stock, 2003) found that white and yellow flowers were the top two choices for multiple pollinators. Interestingly we also found that human white (bee BLUE-GREEN) and yellow (bee UV-GREEN) flowers were more dominant in Taiwan (Figure 5A). White and yellow flowers are also dominant on other islands like the temperate island of Japan (Makino and Yokoyama, 2015), some islands and mountains of southern Hemisphere (Webb and Kelly, 1993; Bernardello et al., 2001; Bischoff et al., 2013b), and these colors are also the most frequent flower hues on larger continental studies (Dyer, 1996; Arnold et al., 2009a; Dyer et al., 2012; Shrestha et al., 2014). An exception case is the study of MI (Shrestha et al., 2016) where no bees have ever been present, and the flowers of MI show a distinctive long wavelength yellowish color that fits with known long wavelength color preferences of flies; the only animal pollination vector on MI. Similarly, orchids on mainland Australia that are pollinated by flies show a distinctive long-wavelength reflection, whilst bee-pollinated orchids from the same community environment display color signals close to the spectral profiles of bee pollinated flowers around the world (Shrestha et al., 2019a). Thus, color preferences of insect pollinators may be a driving factor of flower color signaling. Such spectra fit with measured color preferences in major bee genera like Apis sp. and Bombus sp., as well as phylogenetically separated bee species like the stingless bee Tetragonula carbonaria (Figure 7). Interestingly, such color preferences have been observed to be biologically relevant considering the foraging decisions of naïve bees visiting real flowers (Dyer et al., 2019), providing a plausible explanation for how insect preferences do influence plant fitness and flower color signaling (Giurfa et al., 1995; Shrestha et al., 2020). In addition, floral color loci in the three altitudinal regions of TW we tested consistently and predominantly fall in the sector of bee color space preferred by bees (Figure 7). The increased FCD at higher compared to middle altitudes in TW suggests that the increased frequencies of alternative pollinators like flies may promote some diversification of flower color signaling, and future work could seek to test plant–pollinator networks to understand how signaling may be influenced by different insects with communities (Kantsa et al., 2017; Shrestha et al., 2019a,b).

Floral color over-dispersion has been reported for some alpine plant communities around the world (McEwen and Vamosi, 2010; Shrestha et al., 2014). Our result was in agreement with McEwen and Vamosi (2010) who found four of five alpine plant communities in Canada exhibited a trend towards floral color over-dispersion. Similarly, Shrestha et al. (2014) reported floral colors were significantly over-dispersed in high altitudes, whilst tending to be clustered at low altitudes in Nepal. Our results are also consistent with previous studies which reported the bees have driven the floral color over-dispersion within alpine plant communities in Nepal (Shrestha et al., 2014) and contribute to the floral color diversification of sympatric Pedicularis sp. in Hengduan Mt. (China) (Eaton et al., 2012).

Based on our phylogenetic-informed analysis, we found high altitude floras more closely related to each other than expected by the random assembly (i.e., phylogenetic cluster) (Table 2). However, floral color of these related species actually exhibited higher divergence than that predicted by neutral Brownian motion evolution (i.e., weak phylogenetic signal) (Table 3). Consistently, a community-based study in Nepal (Shrestha et al., 2014), and a clade-based study in Andean Solanaceae (Muchhala et al., 2014) also reported that floral color over-dispersion was mainly attributed to the divergent floral colors among sympatric closely related species.

Previous studies suggest that floral color over-dispersion most typically occurs through either character displacement or competitive exclusion (Emerson and Gillespie, 2008; Sargent and Ackerly, 2008), although it is often difficult to distinguish these two ecological processes at community level. In the current study if floral color over-dispersion had resulted from competitive exclusion, we will expect that closely related species are not likely to co-exist as they would compete with each other, thus the phylogenetic structure of the community should tend to be over-dispersed (Cavender-Bares et al., 2004; Sargent and Ackerly, 2008). On the other hand, if character displacement occurred, closely related species should evolve divergent floral colors perhaps due to reduce competition for pollinators, therefore a weak phylogenetic signal is expected (Revell et al., 2008; Muchhala et al., 2014). We found that the high altitudes floras tend to be phylogenetically clustered rather than over-dispersed (Table 2), and their floral colors exhibited weak phylogenetic signal (Table 3) which is consistent with character displacement that facilitated the co-existence of similar species. In addition, our comparative results suggest that bees appear to be the major pollinator in Taiwan consistent with other large studies around the world (Supplementary Figure S1) where the most frequently observed flower colors fit with the innate preferences of bees (Figure 7). Thus results from a large tropical–subtropical island reveal flower color evolution around the world appears to have most frequently followed a similar trajectory.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. Dryad Dig. Repos. https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.63xsj3v08.

Author Contributions

E-CY, C-NW, MS, and AD developed the concept. K-CT, E-CY, and C-NW collected the data. MS and AD contributed data from Australia and Nepal. K-CT, MS, AD, and C-NW did data analysis and drafted the final version of the manuscript. All authors contributed to review the manuscript.

Funding

C-NW received funding from the Ministry of Science and Technology (MOST-109-2313-B-002-019), Taiwan. E-CY received funding from the Ministry of Science and Technology (MOST, 107-2313-B-002-038-MY3), Taiwan. AD received funding from the Australian Research Council Discovery Projects grant DP160100161 to support MS.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Taroko National Park and Yangmingshan National Park for the permissions to conduct field works and providing student research awards to K-CT. We are grateful to all staff in alpine station of the Endemic Species Research Institute (ESRI), Council of Agriculture, particularly Dr. Tsai-Wen Hsu and Dr. Chao-Ching Huang for recommendations on alpine species samplings and Dr. Chien-Der Yao for providing accommodation during the field work. We are also thankful for Fushan Research Center, Kenting National Park Headquarters, Sunlinksea Forest Recreation Area, Taipei Botanical Garden and Hengchun Tropical Botanic Garden, Taiwan Forestry Research Institute (TFRI) for providing native plant species collections for sampling. We are also grateful to Mr. Kai-Hsiu Chen, Mr. Jing-Yi Lu, Mr. Hong-Wen Ma, Mr. Pei-Bo Chen for help with sample collection and identification of plant species. We acknowledge Dr. Chun-Lin Huang for suggestions on phylogenetic signal analysis. We also acknowledge Dr. Babu R. Paudel for the comments in earlier versions of the manuscript. We are thankful to Professor Montserrate Arista for editing the previous version of the manuscript. We thank Dr. Yu-Shan Hung for discussions.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpls.2020.582784/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1 | Marker point calculation.

Supplementary Figure 2 | Frequency distribution of the areas of MCP in permutation test.

Supplementary Table 1 | Detailed information of the sampling sites.

Supplementary Table 2 | Details of the list of the 727 flowering plant species in our current study, including the altitude and location is provided is “.csv” file.

Supplementary Table 3 | Studies used to provide subfamilial topology in the phylogenetic tree.

Supplementary Table 4 | Summary of the percentage for species in our sample co-sharing with other geographic region.

Supplementary Table 5 | FCD for Nepal, Norway, Japan, and Australia.

Supplementary Method 1 | Details of floral color measurement.

Supplementary Method 2 | Details of MPDcolor and MNTDphylo for phylogenetic structure.

Supplementary Method 3 | Details of the Blomberg’s K and Mantel test.

Supplementary Method 4 | Procedure of permutation test.

Supplementary Method 5 | Identification of flower visitors of high-altitude species.

References

Adams, J. M., Kang, C., and June-Wells, M. (2014). Are tropical butterflies more colorful? Ecol. Res. 29, 685–691. doi: 10.1007/s11284-014-1154-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, S. E. J., Le Comber, S. C., and Chittka, L. (2009a). Flower color phenology in European grassland and woodland habitats, through the eyes of pollinators. Israel J. Plant Sci. 57, 211–230. doi: 10.1560/ijps.57.3.211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, S. E. J., Savolainen, V., and Chittka, L. (2009b). Flower colours along an alpine altitude gradient, seen through the eyes of fly and bee pollinators. Arthropod Plant Interact. 3, 27–43. doi: 10.1007/s11829-009-9056-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergamo, P. J., Telles, F. J., Arnold, S. E. J., and de Brito, V. L. G. (2018). Flower colour within communities shifts from overdispersed to clustered along an alpine altitudinal gradient. Oecologia 188, 223–235. doi: 10.1007/s00442-018-4204-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernardello, G., Anderson, G. J., Stuessy, T. F., and Crawford, D. J. (2001). A survey of floral traits, breeding systems, floral visitors, and pollination systems of the angiosperms of the Juan Fernández Islands (Chile). Bot. Rev. 67, 255–308. doi: 10.1007/BF02858097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bingham, R. A., and Orthner, A. R. (1998). Efficient pollination of alpine plants. Nature 391, 238–239. doi: 10.1038/34564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biq, C.-C., Shyu, C.-T., Chen, J.-C., and Boggs, S. (1985). “Taiwan: geology, geophysics, and marine sediments,” in The Ocean Basins and Margins, eds A. E. M. Nairn, F. G. Stehli, and S. Uyeda (Boston, MA: Springer), 503–550. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4613-2351-8_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bischoff, M., Campbell, D. R., Lord, J. M., and Robertson, A. W. (2013a). The relative importance of solitary bees and syrphid flies as pollinators of two outcrossing plant species in the New Zealand alpine. Austr. Ecol. 38, 169–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-9993.2012.02389.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bischoff, M., Lord, J. M., Robertson, A. W., and Dyer, A. G. (2013b). Hymenopteran pollinators as agents of selection on flower colour in the New Zealand mountains: salient chromatic signals enhance flower discrimination. N. Z. J. Bot. 51, 181–193. doi: 10.1080/0028825X.2013.806933

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blomberg, S. P., Garland, T., and Ives, A. R. (2003). Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution 57, 717–745. doi: 10.1554/0014-38202003057[0717:TFPSIC]2.0.CO2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, H. D., and Schemske, D. W. (2003). Allele substitution at a flower colour locus produces a pollinator shift in monkeyflowers. Nature 426, 176–178. doi: 10.1038/nature02106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, T. M., Mittermeier, R. A., Mittermeier, C. G., da Fonseca, G. A. B., Rylands, A. B., Konstant, W. R., et al. (2002). Habitat loss and extinction in the hotspots of biodiversity. Conserv. Biol. 16, 909–923. doi: 10.1046/j.1523-1739.2002.00530.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchmann, S. L. (1994). “Diversity and importance of native bees from the Arizona Mexico Madrean Archipelago,” in Biodiversity and Management of the Madrean Archipelago : The Sky Islands of Southwestern United States and Northwestern Mexico, eds L. F. de Bano, G. J. Gottfried, R. H. Hamre, C. B. Edminster, P. F. Ffolliott, and A. Ortega (Tucson, ARI: DIANE Publishing), 301–310.

Google Scholar

Burd, M., Stayton, C. T., Shrestha, M., and Dyer, A. G. (2014). Distinctive convergence in Australian floral colours seen through the eyes of Australian birds. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 281:20132862. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2862

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camargo, M. G., Lunau, K., Batalha, M. A., Brings, S., de Brito, V. L., and Morellato, L. P. (2019). How flower colour signals allure bees and hummingbirds: a community level test of the bee avoidance hypothesis. N. Phytol. 222, 1112–1122. doi: 10.1111/nph.15594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavender-Bares, J., Ackerly, D. D., Baum, D. A., and Bazzaz, F. A. (2004). Phylogenetic overdispersion in floridian oak communities. Am. Natural. 163, 823–843. doi: 10.1086/386375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chittka, L. (1992). The colour hexagon: a chromaticity diagram based on photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponency. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170, 533–543. doi: 10.1007/BF00199331

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chittka, L., and Menzel, R. (1992). The evolutionary adaptation of flower colours and the insect pollinators’ colour vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A 171, 171–181. doi: 10.1007/bf00188925

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chittka, L., Shmida, A., Troje, N., and Menzel, R. (1994). Ultraviolet as a component of flower reflections, and the colour perception of hymenoptera. Vis. Res. 34, 1489–1508. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90151-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chittka, L., Thomson, J. D., and Waser, N. M. (1999). Flower constancy, insect psychology, and plant evolution. Naturwissenschaften 86, 361–377. doi: 10.1007/s001140050636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, P. A., and Elmqvist, T. (2000). Pollinator extinction in the pacific islands. Conserv. Biol. 14, 1237–1239. doi: 10.1046/j.1523-1739.2000.00017.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalrymple, R. L., Kemp, D. J., Flores-Moreno, H., Laffan, S. W., White, T. E., Hemmings, F. A., et al. (2015). Birds, butterflies and flowers in the tropics are not more colourful than those at higher latitudes. Glob. Ecol. Biogeogr. 24, 1424–1432. doi: 10.1111/geb.12368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jager, M. L., Dreyer, L. L., and Ellis, A. G. (2011). Do pollinators influence the assembly of flower colours within plant communities? Oecologia 166, 543–553. doi: 10.1007/s00442-010-1879-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Salas, M. F., and Baker, M. L. (2015). A Census of the Vascular Plants of Tasmania, Including Macquarie Island. Hobart, TAS: Tasmanian Herbarium, Tasmanian Museum and Art Gallery.

Google Scholar

Dyer, A. G. (1996). Reflection of near-ultraviolet radiation from flowers of australian native plants. Austr. J. Bot. 44, 473–488. doi: 10.1071/bt9960473

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyer, A. G. (1999). Broad spectral sensitivities in the honeybee’s photoreceptors limit colour constancy. J. Comp. Physiol. A 185, 445–453. doi: 10.1007/s003590050405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyer, A. G. (2006). Discrimination of flower colours in natural settings by the bumblebee species Bombus terrestris (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Entomol. Gen. 28, 257–268. doi: 10.1127/entom.gen/28/2006/257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyer, A. G., Boyd-Gerny, S., McLoughlin, S., Rosa, M. G. P., Simonov, V., and Wong, B. B. M. (2012). Parallel evolution of angiosperm colour signals: common evolutionary pressures linked to hymenopteran vision. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279, 3606–3615. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.0827

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyer, A. G., Boyd-Gerny, S., Shrestha, M., Garcia, J. E., van der Kooi, C., and Wong, B. B. M. (2019). Colour preferences of Tetragonula carbonaria Sm. stingless bees for colour morphs of the Australian native orchid Caladenia carnea. J. Comp. Physiol. A 205, 347–361. doi: 10.1007/s00359-019-01346-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyer, A. G., Boyd-Gerny, S., Shrestha, M., Lunau, K., Garcia, J. E., Koethe, S., et al. (2016). Innate colour preferences of the Australian native stingless bee Tetragonula carbonaria Sm. J. Comp. Physiol. A 202, 603–613. doi: 10.1007/s00359-016-1101-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyer, A. G., Paulk, A. C., and Reser, D. H. (2011). Colour processing in complex environments: insights from the visual system of bees. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 278, 952–959. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2412

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eaton, D. A. R., Fenster, C. B., Hereford, J., Huang, S.-Q., and Ree, R. H. (2012). Floral diversity and community structure in Pedicularis (Orobanchaceae). Ecology 93, S182–S194. doi: 10.1890/11-0501.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emerson, B. C., and Gillespie, R. G. (2008). Phylogenetic analysis of community assembly and structure over space and time. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 619–630. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenster, C. B., Armbruster, W. S., Wilson, P., Dudash, M. R., and Thomson, J. D. (2004). Pollination syndromes and floral specialization. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 35, 375–403. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.011802.132347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia, J. E., Spaethe, J., and Dyer, A. G. (2017). The path to colour discrimination is S-shaped: behaviour determines the interpretation of colour models. J. Comp. Physiol. A 203, 983–997. doi: 10.1007/s00359-017-1208-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giurfa, M., Núñez, J., Chittka, L., and Menzel, R. (1995). Colour preferences of flower-naive honeybees. J. Comp. Physiol. A 177, 247–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00192415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, M., Stansberry, M. J., Lynn, J. S., Williams, C. F., White, T. E., and Whitney, K. D. (2018). Consistent shifts in pollinator-relevant floral coloration along Rocky Mountain elevation gradients. J. Ecol. 106, 1910–1924. doi: 10.1111/1365-2745.12948

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gumbert, A., Kunze, J., and Chittka, L. (1999). Floral colour diversity in plant communities, bee colour space and a null model. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 266, 1711–1716. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0836

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hegland, S. J., Nielsen, A., Lázaro, A., Bjerknes, A.-L., and Totland, Ø (2009). How does climate warming affect plant-pollinator interactions? Ecol. Lett. 12, 184–195. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2008.01269.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsieh, C.-F. (2002). Composition, endemism and phytogeographical affinities of the Taiwan flora. TAIWANIA 47, 298–310. doi: 10.6165/tai.2002.47(4).298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, S.-F. (2011). Historical biogeography of the flora of Taiwan. J. Natl. Taiwan Museum 64, 33–63.

Google Scholar

Inouye, D. W., and Pyke, G. H. (1988). Pollination biology in the Snowy Mountains of Australia: comparisons with montane Colorado, USA. Austr. J. Ecol. 3, 191–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-9993.1988.tb00968.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Judd, D. B., MacAdam, D. L., Wyszecki, G., Budde, H. W., Condit, H. R., Henderson, S. T., et al. (1964). Spectral distribution of typical daylight as a function of correlated color temperature. J. Optic. Soc. Am. 54, 1031–1040. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.54.001031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantsa, A., Raguso, R. A., Dyer, A. G., Olesen, J. M., Tscheulin, T., and Petanidou, T. (2018). Disentangling the role of floral sensory stimuli in pollination networks. Nat. Commun. 9, 1041–1041. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03448-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantsa, A., Raguso, R. A., Dyer, A. G., Sgardelis, S. P., Olesen, J. M., and Petanidou, T. (2017). Community-wide integration of floral colour and scent in a Mediterranean scrubland. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 1502–1510. doi: 10.1038/s41559-017-0298-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kembel, S. W., Cowan, P. D., Helmus, M. R., Cornwell, W. K., Morlon, H., Ackerly, D. D., et al. (2010). Picante: R tools for integrating phylogenies and ecology. Bioinformatics 26, 1463–1464. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btq166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, J. E., Bergh, N. G., Soares, M., and Ellis, A. G. (2019). Dominant pollinators drive non-random community assembly and shared flower colour patterns in daisy communities. Ann. Bot. 123, 277–288. doi: 10.1093/aob/mcy126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kevan, P., Giurfa, M., and Chittka, L. (1996). Why are there so many and so few white flowers? Trends Plant Sci. 1, 280–284. doi: 10.1016/1360-1385(96)20008-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lázaro, A., Hegland, S. J., and Totland, Ø (2008). The relationships between floral traits and specificity of pollination systems in three Scandinavian plant communities. Oecologia 157, 249–257. doi: 10.1007/s00442-008-1066-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C.-F., Chytrý, M., Zelený, D., Chen, M.-Y., Chen, T.-Y., Chiou, C.-R., et al. (2013). Classification of Taiwan forest vegetation. Appl. Veg. Sci. 16, 698–719. doi: 10.1111/avsc.12025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, J. M., Huggins, L., Little, L. M., and Tomlinson, V. R. (2013). Floral biology and flower visitors on subantarctic Campbell Island. N. Z. J. Bot. 51, 168–180. doi: 10.1080/0028825X.2013.801867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lunau, K. (1990). Colour saturation triggers innate reactions to flower signals: flower dummy experiments with bumblebees. J. Comp. Physiol. A 166, 827–834. doi: 10.1007/BF00187329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lunau, K., Papiorek, S., Eltz, T., and Sazima, M. (2011). Avoidance of achromatic colours by bees provides a private niche for hummingbirds. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 1607–1612. doi: 10.1242/jeb.052688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makino, T. T., and Yokoyama, J. (2015). Nonrandom composition of flower colors in a plant community: mtually different co-flowering natives and disturbance by aliens. PLoS One 10:e0143443. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143443

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, J. R., and Vamosi, J. C. (2010). Floral colour versus phylogeny in structuring subalpine flowering communities. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 277, 2957–2965. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0501

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morawetz, L., Svoboda, A., Spaethe, J., and Dyer, A. G. (2013). Blue colour preference in honeybees distracts visual attention for learning closed shapes. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 199, 817–827. doi: 10.1007/s00359-013-0843-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muchhala, N., Johnsen, S., and Smith, S. D. (2014). Competition for hummingbird pollination shapes flower color variation in andean Solanaceae. Evolution 68, 2275–2286. doi: 10.1111/evo.12441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberrath, R., and Böhning Gaese, K. (1999). Floral color change and the attraction of insect pollinators in lungwort (Pulmonaria collina). Oecologia 121, 383–391. doi: 10.1007/s004420050943

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohashi, K., Makino, T. T., and Arikawa, K. (2015). Floral colour change in the eyes of pollinators: testing possible constraints and correlated evolution. Funct. Ecol. 29, 1144–1155. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ômura, H., and Honda, K. (2005). Priority of color over scent during flower visitation by adult Vanessa indica butterflies. Oecologia 142, 588–596. doi: 10.1007/s00442-004-1761-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paudel, B. R., Burd, M., Shrestha, M., Dyer, A. G., and Li, Q.-J. (2018). Reproductive isolation in alpine gingers: how do co-existing Roscoea (R. purpurea and R. tumjensis) conserve species integrity? Evolution 72, 1840–1850. doi: 10.1111/evo.13546

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paudel, B. R., Shrestha, M., Burd, M., Adhikari, S., Sun, Y.-S., and Li, Q.-J. (2016). Coevolutionary elaboration of pollination-related traits in an alpine ginger (Roscoea purpurea) and a tabanid fly in the Nepalese Himalayas. N. Phytol. 211, 1402–1411. doi: 10.1111/nph.13974

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paudel, B. R., Shrestha, M., Dyer, A. G., Zhu, X.-F., Abdusalam, A., and Li, Q.-J. (2015). Out of Africa: evidence of the obligate mutualism between long corolla tubed plant and long-tongued fly in the Himalayas. Ecol. Evol. 5, 5240–5251. doi: 10.1002/ece3.1784

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peitsch, D., Fietz, A., Hertel, H., de Souza, J., Ventura, D. F., and Menzel, R. (1992). The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their receptor-based colour vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170, 23–40. doi: 10.1007/BF00190398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickering, C. M., and Stock, M. (2003). Insect colour preference compared to flower colours in the Australian Alps. Nordic J. Bot. 23, 217–223. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-1051.2003.tb00384.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raine, N. E., and Chittka, L. (2005). Colour preferences in relation to the foraging performance and fitness of the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Uludag Bee J. 5, 145–150.

Google Scholar

Revell, L. J. (2012). phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol. Evol. 3, 217–223. doi: 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2011.00169.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revell, L. J., Harmon, L. J., and Collar, D. C. (2008). Phylogenetic signal, evolutionary process, and rate. Syst. Biol. 57, 591–601. doi: 10.1080/10635150802302427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sargent, R. D., and Ackerly, D. D. (2008). Plant–pollinator interactions and the assembly of plant communities. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2007.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schemske, D. W., Mittelbach, G. G., Cornell, H. V., Sobel, J. M., and Roy, K. (2009). Is there a latitudinal gradient in the importance of biotic interactions? Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 40, 245–269. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.39.110707.173430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrestha, M., Burd, M., Garcia, J. E., Dorin, A., and Dyer, A. G. (2019a). Colour evolution within orchids depends on whether the pollinator is a bee or a fly. Plant Biol. 21, 745–752. doi: 10.1111/plb.12968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrestha, M., Dyer, A. G., Garcia, J. E., and Burd, M. (2019b). Floral colour structure in two Australian herbaceous communities: it depends on who is looking. Ann. Bot. 124, 221–232. doi: 10.1093/aob/mcz043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrestha, M., Dyer, A. G., Bhattarai, P., and Burd, M. (2014). Flower colour and phylogeny along an altitudinal gradient in the Himalayas of Nepal. J. Ecol. 102, 126–135. doi: 10.1111/1365-2745.12185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrestha, M., Dyer, A. G., Boyd-Gerny, S., Wong, B. B. M., and Burd, M. (2013). Shades of red: bird-pollinated flowers target the specific colour discrimination abilities of avian vision. New Phytol. 198, 301–310. doi: 10.1111/nph.12135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrestha, M., Garcia, J. E., Bukovac, Z., Dorin, A., and Dyer, A. G. (2018). Pollination in a new climate: assessing the potential influence of flower temperature variation on insect pollinator behaviour. PLoS One 13:e0200549. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200549

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrestha, M., Garcia, J. E., Burd, M., and Dyer, A. G. (2020). Australian native flower colours: does nectar reward drive bee pollinator flower preferences? PLoS One 15:e0226469. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrestha, M., Lunau, K., Dorin, A., Schulze, B., Bischoff, M., Burd, M., et al. (2016). Floral colours in a world without birds and bees: the plants of Macquarie Island. Plant Biol. 18, 842–850. doi: 10.1111/plb.12456

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soltis, D. E., Smith, S. A., Cellinese, N., Wurdack, K. J., Tank, D. C., Brockington, S. F., et al. (2011). Angiosperm phylogeny: 17 genes, 640 taxa. Am. J. Bot. 98, 704–730.

Google Scholar

Stournaras, K. E., Lo, E., Böhning-Gaese, K., Cazetta, E., Dehling, D. M., Schleuning, M., et al. (2013). How colorful are fruits? Limited color diversity in fleshy fruits on local and global scales. N. Phytol. 198, 617–629. doi: 10.1111/nph.12157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tai, K.-C. (2018). Pollinator Preference and Pollinator-Mediated Competition United to Shape the Floral Color Assembly Along Altitudes in Taiwan. Master thesis. Taipei: National Taiwan University.

Google Scholar

Tai, K.-C., Shrestha, M., Dyer, A. G., Yang, E.-C., and Wang, C.-N. (2020). Data from: Floral colour diversity in the tropical-subtropical mountainous island of Taiwan. Dryad Dig. Repos. doi: 10.5061/dryad.63xsj3v08 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapa, V. K. (2000). An Inventory of Nepal Insect. Volumn III (Hemiptera, Hymenoptera, Coleoptera & Diptera). Kathmandu: IUCN.

Google Scholar

Totland, Ø (1993). Pollination in alpine Norway: flowering phenology, insect visitors, and visitation rates in two plant communities. Can. J. Bot. 71, 1072–1079. doi: 10.1139/b93-124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Kooi, C. J., Pen, I., Staal, M., Stavenga, D. G., and Elzenga, J. T. M. (2016). Competition for pollinators and intra-communal spectral dissimilarity of flowers. Plant Biol. 18, 56–62. doi: 10.1111/plb.12328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Kleunen, M., Dawson, W., Essl, F., Pergl, J., Winter, M., Weber, E., et al. (2015). Global exchange and accumulation of non-native plants. Nature 525, 100–103. doi: 10.1038/nature14910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voris, H. K. (2000). Maps of Pleistocene sea levels in Southeast Asia: shorelines, river systems and time durations. J. Biogeogr. 27, 1153–1167. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2699.2000.00489.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, S. D., Harper, K. T., and Booth, G. M. (1988). Elevational distribution of insect pollinators. Am. Midland Nat. 120, 325–330. doi: 10.2307/2426004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, C. J., and Kelly, D. (1993). The reproductive biology of the New Zealand flora. Trends Ecol. Evol. 8, 442–447. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(93)90007-C

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, C. O., and Donoghue, M. J. (2005). Phylomatic: tree assembly for applied phylogenetics. Mol. Ecol. Notes 5, 181–183. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-8286.2004.00829.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weevers, T. (1952). Flower colours and their frequency. Acta Bot. Neerland. 1, 81–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1438-8677.1952.tb00002.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wikström, N., Kainulainen, K., Razafimandimbison, S. G., Smedmark, J. E. E., and Bremer, B. (2015). A revised time tree of the asterids: establishing a temporal framework for evolutionary studies of the coffee family (Rubiaceae). PLoS One 10:e0126690. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126690

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wikström, N., Savolainen, V., and Chase, M. W. (2001). Evolution of the angiosperms: calibrating the family tree. Proc. R. Soc. B 268, 2211–2220. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1782

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, P. H., Ito, M., Matsumura, T., and Kudo, I. (2010). The bumblebees of the Nepal Himalaya (Hymenoptera : Apidae). Insecta Matsumurana 66, 115–151.

Google Scholar

Wilson, M. F., and von Neumann, R. A. (1972). Why are Neotropical birds more colourful than North American birds? Avicult. Magaz. 78, 141–147.

Google Scholar

Wu, F. T. (1978). Recent tectonics of taiwan. J. Phys. Earth 26, S265–S299. doi: 10.4294/jpe1952.26.Supplement_S265

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanne, A. E., Tank, D. C., Cornwell, W. K., Eastman, J. M., Smith, S. A., FitzJohn, R. G., et al. (2014). Three keys to the radiation of angiosperms into freezing environments. Nature 506, 89–92. doi: 10.1038/nature12872

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: flowers, bee vision, phylogeny, community, altitude, tropical–subtropical, island

Citation: Tai K-C, Shrestha M, Dyer AG, Yang E-C and Wang C-N (2020) Floral Color Diversity: How Are Signals Shaped by Elevational Gradient on the Tropical–Subtropical Mountainous Island of Taiwan? Front. Plant Sci. 11:582784. doi: 10.3389/fpls.2020.582784

Received: 13 July 2020; Accepted: 25 November 2020;
Published: 17 December 2020.

Edited by:

Boris Rewald, University of Natural Resources and Life Sciences Vienna, Austria

Reviewed by:

André Rodrigo Rech, Universidade Federal dos Vales do Jequitinhonha e Mucuri, Brazil
Mascha Bischoff, Darmstadt University of Technology, Germany

Copyright © 2020 Tai, Shrestha, Dyer, Yang and Wang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mani Shrestha, c2gubWFuaUBnbWFpbC5jb20=; bWFuaS5zaHJlc3RoYUBybWl0LmVkdS5hdQ==; Chun-Neng Wang, bGVhZnlAbnR1LmVkdS50dw==

ORCID: King-Chun Tai, orcid.org/0000-0002-2851-982X; Mani Shrestha, orcid.org/0000-0002-6165-8418; Adrian G. Dyer, orcid.org/0000-0002-2632-9061; En-Cheng Yang orcid.org/0000-0002-5793-5427; Chun-Neng Wang, orcid.org/0000-0003-2583-6544

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.