Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Physiol. , 01 April 2025

Sec. Exercise Physiology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2025.1544286

Effects of sleep deprivation on sports performance and perceived exertion in athletes and non-athletes: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Yan KongYan Kong1Beibei YuBeibei Yu1Guangming GuanGuangming Guan2Yang WangYang Wang3Hui He
Hui He3*
  • 1Department of Exercise Physiology, Beijing Sport University, Beijing, China
  • 2DeRucci Healthy Sleep Co., Ltd., Beijing, China
  • 3China Institute of Sport and Health Science, Beijing Sport University, Beijing, China

Background: Sleep deprivation can significantly affect sports performance and the perception of fatigue. However, the impact of sleep deprivation on sports performance remains a subject of ongoing debate across different populations.

Objectives: This study aimed to investigate the effects of sleep deprivation on sports performance and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) in different groups, as well as how different types of sleep deprivation affect these aspects.

Methods: This systematic review followed the PRISMA guidelines (PROSPERO CRD42023492792). Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) and randomized crossover studies published in any language or up to any date were eligible based on the P.I.C.O.S. criteria. The systematic search included databases such as PubMed, Cochrane, Embase, Web of Science, and EBSCO, covering studies up to September 2024. The Cochrane RoB 2 tool was used to assess the risk of bias. Meta-analysis was conducted using either a fixed-effect model or a random-effects model. This study conducted subgroup analyses based on different populations, types of sleep deprivation, and testing times.

Results: This review includes 45 studies, comprising 16 on aerobic endurance (AE) performance, 8 on anaerobic endurance (AnE) performance, 23 on explosive power (EP), 10 on maximum force (MF), 4 on speed, 4 on skill control, and 12 on rating of perceived exertion (RPE). The results indicate that sleep deprivation significantly impaired AE in athletes [SMD = −0.66; 95% CI (−1.28, −0.04); P = 0.04], as well as EP [SMD = −0.63; 95% CI (−0.94, −0.33); P < 0.00001], MF [SMD = −0.35; 95% CI (−0.56, −0.14); P = 0.001], speed [SMD = −0.52, 95% CI (−0.83, −0.22); P = 0.0008], skill control [SMD = −0.87; 95% CI (−1.7, −0.04); P = 0.04], and RPE [SMD = 0.39; 95% CI (0.11, 0.66); P = 0.006]. Additionally, AE was also reduced in healthy non-athletes [SMD = −1.02; 95% CI (−1.84, −0.21); P = 0.01]. During the sleep deprivation process, early sleep deprivation (PSDE) significantly reduced EP [SMD = −1.04; 95% CI (−1.58, −0.5); P = 0.0002], MF [SMD = −0.57; 95% CI (−0.94, −0.19); P = 0.003], speed [SMD = −0.78; 95% CI (−1.35, −0.2); P = 0.008], and RPE [SMD = 0.6; 95% CI (0.17, 1.02); P = 0.006]. Late sleep deprivation (PSDB) impacted speed [SMD = −0.57; 95% CI (−1.15, 0.01); P = 0.05], skill control [SMD = −2.12; 95% CI (−3.01, −1.24); P < 0.00001], and RPE [SMD = 0.47; 95% CI (0.02, 0.92); P = 0.04]. Overall, total sleep deprivation primarily affected AE [SMD = −0.56; 95% CI (−1.08, −0.05); P = 0.03]. In terms of testing phases, p.m. tests had a significant impact on AE [SMD = −1.4; 95% CI (−2.47, −0.34); P = 0.01], EP [SMD = −0.68; 95% CI (−1.06, −0.31); P = 0.0004], MF [SMD = −0.3; 95% CI (−0.51, −0.09); P = 0.005], skill control [SMD = −2.12; 95% CI (−3.01, −1.24); P < 0.00001], and RPE [SMD = 0.72; 95% CI (0.20, 1.24); P = 0.007]. In contrast, a.m. tests primarily affected speed [SMD = −0.81; 95% CI (−1.52, −0.1); P = 0.03] and RPE [SMD = 0.44; 95% CI (0.01, 0.86); P = 0.04].

Conclusion: Sleep deprivation significantly impairs athletes' performance across various domains, including AE, MF, speed, and skill control, while also exacerbating RPE. In contrast, although sleep deprivation also negatively affects the AE of healthy non-athletes. Furthermore, PSDE appears to have a more pronounced effect on sports performance overall. Additionally, performance assessments conducted in the p.m. have been shown to further impact sports performance. These findings are crucial for understanding how sleep deprivation impacts both athletes and non-athletes, particularly in the context of training and competitive settings.

1 Introduction

Sleep is a fundamental human activity during which critical metabolic and cognitive processes, including memory consolidation, occur (Samuels, 2008; Filipas et al., 2021a). However, various real-life factors can reduce sleep duration, leading to both acute and chronic sleep deprivation (Ferrara and De Gennaro, 2001; Lopes et al., 2023). Sleep deprivation is characterized by a consistent reduction or loss of sleep, which can be categorized into total sleep deprivation and partial sleep deprivation. Total sleep deprivation involves the complete elimination of sleep for a specified duration (typically at least one night), resulting in prolonged wakefulness (Reynolds and Banks, 2010). Partial sleep deprivation refers to a reduction in sleep duration below an individual’s typical baseline or the amount required to maintain optimal performance (Reynolds and Banks, 2010). It can be further classified into two types: late sleep deprivation (PSDB) and early sleep deprivation (PSDE) (Gong et al., 2024). Sleep deprivation negatively affects work productivity (Kecklund and Axelsson, 2016), exacerbates fatigue (Goel, 2017), and impairs the performance of daily activities (Vanderlinden et al., 2020). Furthermore, sleep deprivation may serve as a stimulus influencing pre-exercise perceptual difficulties (Reilly, 1983; Magnuson et al., 2023). Consequently, sleep deprivation may elevate ratings of perceived exertion (RPE). Athletes are particularly vulnerable to sleep disturbances due to pre-competition stress, intense training regimens, and the demands of competing across time zones (Juliff et al., 2015; Nedelec et al., 2018). This can lead to impairments in subsequent motor performance (Lastella et al., 2014). Healthy non-athletes may also experience the effects of sleep deprivation due to factors such as life stress. Sleep deprivation impairs athletic performance, including muscle strength, speed (Souissi et al., 2013; Honn et al., 2019; Souissi et al., 2020), as well as overall bodily functions (Craven et al., 2022), and reduces aerobic performance (Craven et al., 2022; Lopes et al., 2023). Sleep deprivation leads to a decline in athletic performance, resulting in impaired neuromuscular coordination, increased injury risk, and delayed recovery in both athletes and non-athletes (Charest and Grandner, 2020). The differential effects of sleep deprivation on sports performance and RPE in athletes versus non-athletes remain unclear, particularly in relation to training level. One study found that PSDE had a more pronounced effect on athletes' selective attention than PSDB (Zerouali et al., 2010). However, it remains unclear how different types of sleep deprivation affect sports performance and RPE in athletes and healthy non-athletes.

Several meta-analyses investigating sleep deprivation as an intervention have compared its effects on exercise performance (Pilcher and Huffcutt, 1996; Gao et al., 2019; Craven et al., 2022; Lopes et al., 2023; Gong et al., 2024). However, the conclusions drawn from these analyses regarding the impact of sleep deprivation on various types of sports performance remain inconclusive. A recent meta-analysis reported that sleep deprivation has a moderate impact on endurance performance (Lopes et al., 2023). In contrast, Craven et al. (2022) found that the extent of sleep deprivation did not influence endurance performance. Acute sleep restriction has been shown to reduce explosive power, speed, and other athletic performance metrics in athletes (Gong et al., 2024). However, a crossover study found that acute sleep restriction had no effect on tennis players' ability to perform repeated sprints (Vitale et al., 2021). While recent meta-analyses have explored the effects of total and partial sleep deprivation on aerobic performance across individuals with different training states (Lopes et al., 2023), as well as the impact of various acute sleep deprivation types on athletic performance (Gong et al., 2024), these studies do not fully capture the effects of sleep deprivation on various aspects of sports performance, nor the differential effects across populations. Furthermore, studies have identified sleep deprivation as a contributing factor to motor perception difficulties (Reilly, 1983; Wang et al., 2021). Motor perception difficulties can also reduce athletes' willingness and efficiency to continue performing (Brotherton et al., 2019a; Roberts et al., 2019b). However, it remains unclear whether this effect extends to non-athletes. Therefore, an updated meta-analysis is needed to examine the effects of sleep deprivation on sports performance and RPE in both athletes and non-athletes, as well as the impact of different types of sleep deprivation on various aspects of sports performance.

The originality of this study lies in its comprehensive investigation of the effects of sleep deprivation on both athletic performance and RPE, with a specific focus on comparing athletes and healthy non-athletes. Unlike previous studies, this systematic review and meta-analysis aims to provide a comprehensive summary of the evidence regarding the impact of sleep deprivation on sports performance and perceived fatigue in both athletes and healthy non-athletes. The secondary aim of this study was to examine the effects of different types of sleep deprivation on sports performance and RPE. We hypothesized that sleep deprivation would have a more pronounced impact on performance and RPE in non-athletes compared to athletes, due to their lower baseline fitness levels and less developed coping mechanisms. Furthermore, we hypothesized that different types of sleep deprivation would have differential effects on specific aspects of performance.

2 Methods

2.1 Registration

The systematic review and meta-analysis was reported according to Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) (Shamseer et al., 2015). The study plan was registered in the PROSPERO International Prospective Systematic Review Registry (registration number: CRD42023492792).

2.2 Literature search strategy

We searched PubMed, Cochrane, Web of Science and EBSCO databases for experimental studies from inception until publication on 21 September 2024, and the Embase database for relevant studies published in English on 22 September 2024, using the following PICOS criteria: (P) population: human; (I) interventions: sleep deprivation, sleep restriction, or insufficient sleep; (C) control group: normal sleep; (O) outcome measures: aerobic endurance performance, anaerobic endurance performance, explosive power, maximum force, speed, skills control (such as serving accuracy), rating of perceived exertion (RPE), etc.; and (S) study type: experimental studies. To identify relevant studies, we systematically reviewed the reference lists of the selected articles. Additionally, we conducted forward and backward citation searches to ensure a comprehensive coverage of the literature. Forward citation searches involved tracking citations of included studies, while backward citation searches involved examining the reference lists of these studies to identify additional relevant publications. This dual approach was used to minimize the risk of missing pertinent studies (Supplementary Table S1).

2.3 Exclusion and inclusion criteria

Studies meeting the following criteria were eligible for inclusion: (1) Research subjects: athletes and non-athletes (athletes, defined as professionals engaged in sports, including elite athletes, amateur athletes and university athletes; non-athletes: healthy subjects except athletes); (2) Experimental research: the study included participants who underwent at least one type of sleep deprivation protocol, such as total sleep deprivation (TSD) (at least one night without sleep) (Banks and Dinges, 2007), partial sleep deprivation at the beginning of the night (PSDB) (falling asleep later than usual, e.g., starting sleep at 3 a.m.), partial sleep deprivation at the end of the night (PSDE) (waking up earlier than the normal waking time, e.g., getting up at 3 a.m.), and partial sleep deprivation of unspecified type (partial). Whether it is a randomized controlled trial or crossover trials. Total sleep deprivation is the complete elimination of sleep gained during a specific period of time (Smithies et al., 2021). Partial sleep deprivation refers to a modest reduction in the amount of sleep you get for one or more nights (getting ∼2–6 h of sleep per night) (Smithies et al., 2021); (3) outcome measures: reporting of at least one of the following exercises related to sporting performance and perception of fatigue: aerobic endurance performance, anaerobic endurance performance, explosive power, maximum force, speed, skills control (such as serving accuracy), RPE, etc.; (4) The sleep deprivation condition must have been compared to a control condition involving a night with habitual sleep time; (5) Only studies published in peer-reviewed journals and written in English were included in our analyses. There was no limit to the studies’ publication date.

We excluded studies that (1) involved unhealthy people, such as those with sleep disorders or other medical conditions that could affect sleep or performance; (2) allowed sleep deprivation to be conducted in conjunction with physical exercise, cognitive tasks, caloric restriction, stimulants or sedatives (e.g., caffeine, L-tryptophan, or modafinil), or taking drugs or placebos were conducted only in one study group; (3) sports performance was measured after a period of recovery sleep (sleep latency tests were not considered ‘recovery sleep’); (4) did not provide outcome measures; (5) were duplicate publications; (6) were literature review papers, letters to the editor, abstracts published in conference proceedings; (7) were animal model studies; (8) sports performance data were not adequately reported (i.e., mean ± standard deviation [SD] was not reported or could not be derived) and contacted by means such as email with no results; or (9) an inter-subject experimental design is allowed, with no baseline measurements after “normal sleep”; (10) Observational study.

2.4 Data extraction

All the retrieved studies were imported into the EndNote literature management software. The initial de-duplication was performed using EndNote software, followed by a manual check to ensure accuracy. Two investigators screened the literature based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Studies were excluded first by reading the title and abstract and then following a detailed full-text review.

The studies were screened independently by two researchers. Given the objective nature of the screening process and the clear criteria used, no formal inter-rater reliability assessment (e.g., Cohen’s kappa) was conducted. Any disagreements that arose were addressed through open discussion, where the researchers shared their views and reasoning. Consensus was reached after multiple rounds of review and exchange, with the third author stepping in for a final ruling in cases where an agreement could not be reached. The extracted content included (1) basic information about the included studies (such as the name of the first author and publication date), (2) participants’ baseline characteristics (such as age, gender, and number of participants in the experimental and control groups), (3) intervention measures of the experimental group (such as duration and method of sleep intervention), and (4) relevant outcome measures, including aerobic endurance, anaerobic endurance, maximum power, explosive power, speed, skill control, and perceived exertion (RPE), were extracted as part of the outcome data. To ensure the comparability of the data across studies, we adopted a standardized extraction format. For example, the RPE score was recorded using the Borg RPE Scale, following established methods in the literature. Standardized test protocols were also applied to sports performance indicators. Given the diversity of sports performance metrics, any variations in the measurement of the same performance outcomes across different studies were included for comparison. The extracted data were managed using Excel, with all data extraction forms numbered according to the study’s author and publication year, and stored in Excel to ensure data integrity and traceability.

2.5 Risk of bias

Two reviewers independently assessed the methodological quality of the included studies. A third reviewer was consulted if disagreements persisted. The methodological quality of RCTs and cross-over studies was evaluated by the Cochrane Collaboration risk of bias (RoB) 2.0 tool (Sterne et al., 2019).

2.6 Data synthesis and statistical analysis

For each outcome, the pre-post changes in the experimental and control groups were also pooled to estimate the effects. The outcome measures in this study were continuous variables, and the standard MD (SMD) and its 95% confidence interval (CI) were used as the effect measures. The SMD was used as the effect index. Between-study heterogeneity was measured via I2 statistics and the Cochran’s Q test (Higgins et al., 2003). The Cochran manual recommends that if I2 < 50% or Q test P > 0.1 and the heterogeneity between different study groups was small, the fixed effect model was used to combine the effect size. If I2 ≥ 50% or Q test P ≤ 0.1, both of which indicate a high degree of heterogeneity, effect sizes were pooled using a random effects model that accounted for between-study variation and weighted each study accordingly. Publication bias was assessed by examining the funnel plot and Egger’s test. If publication bias was indicated, the effect of bias on the obtained results was assessed using the trim-and-fill method (Weinhandl and Duval, 2012). Sensitivity analyses were performed to test the robustness of the pooled results by deleting trials that assessed the risk of bias. The quantitative synthesis of the data was performed using Review Manager5.4 or Stata16.0 software. We analyzed different types of exercise performance and perceived fatigue separately to examine how sleep deprivation affects each aspect of performance and fatigue. To explore how different factors influence the effects of sleep deprivation on athletic performance, subgroup analyses will be conducted based on the following criteria: participant type (athletes vs healthy non-athletes), sleep deprivation type (TSD vs PSDB vs PSDE vs P), and testing time (a.m. vs p.m.).

3 Results

3.1 Results of the literature search

A preliminary examination identified 18,127 potentially eligible articles, with 18,104 sourced from database searches and 23 from reference lists. After removing 4,583 duplicates, 13,544 articles remained for further screening. Through the process of title and abstract screening, 12,231 articles were excluded. An additional 85 articles were excluded after full-text review, of which 13 lacked full-text or data, and attempts to contact the authors for clarification went unanswered. The remaining 45 articles were included for quantitative synthesis. (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of the study selection process.

3.2 Characteristics of the included studies

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the included trials. A total of 670 participants were included; the participants were athletes in 28 studies and healthy non-athletes in 17 studies. Seven studies included female participants, and two study did not specify the ratio of male to female. Data from seven studies were included twice due to differences in sleep intervention duration, population, test methods, and other reasons. The age of the participants was 15–40 years old, and most of them were young people. The sleep deprivation protocols applied in the studies predominantly involved single or dual forms of sleep deprivation (TSD, PSDB, PSDE), with certain studies incorporating a washout period to eliminate the influence of the previous form of deprivation in cases where two forms were used. The control group across all studies adhered to a normal sleep routine.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Basic characteristics of the included studies.

To enhance precision in exploring the impact of acute sleep deprivation on diverse athletic abilities and facilitate data integration, this study classified indicators based on their energy supply characteristics and neuromuscular working methods during index tests (Table 2) (Gong et al., 2024).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Types and main characteristics of sports performance and fatigue indicators included in the literature.

3.3 Risk bias assessment results

The methodological quality of the included randomized crossover studies and randomized controlled trials (RCTs) is summarized in Figures 2, 3. Among the 42 included crossover studies, 9 have a high risk of bias, 15 have a moderate risk, and 18 have a low risk. Among the three included RCTs, one has a high risk of bias, while two have a low risk of bias. In the crossover studies, the common sources of bias include: (1) unclear randomization process; (2) absence of blinding of assessors; and (3) unclear whether sufficient time was allowed between the first and second phases. The common sources of bias in the RCTs are unclear randomization processes.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Methodological assessments of cross-over trials by the RoB 2.0 tool.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Methodological assessments of randomized controlled trials by the RoB 2.0 tool.

3.4 Synthesis of the results

3.4.1 Aerobic endurance performance

Figure 4 and Supplemetnary Figures S1, S2 Fig present the effects of sleep deprivation on aerobic endurance performance. Using random-effects pooled effect sizes of 16 studies, sleep deprivation significantly reduced aerobic endurance performance compared with control participants [SMD = −0.76; 95% CI (−1.27, −0.25); P = 0.003; I2 = 84%]. Subgroup analysis indicated that sleep deprivation had a statistically significant effect on aerobic endurance performance of athletes [SMD = −0.66; 95% CI (−1.28, −0.04); P = 0.04] and healthy non-athletes [SMD = −1.02; 95% CI (−1.84, −0.21); P = 0.01]. Moreover, TSD [SMD = −0.56; 95% CI (−1.08, −0.05); P = 0.03] caused a decrease in aerobic endurance performance. However, PSDB [SMD = −0.57; 95% CI (−1.22, 0.08); P = 0.09], PSDE [SMD = −0.24; 95% CI (−1.08, 0.6); P = 0.58] and Partial [SMD = −8.19; 95% CI (−19.66, 3.28); P = 0.16] had no effect on aerobic endurance performance. During the test period, the p.m. tests [SMD = −1.4; 95% CI (−2.47, −0.34); P = 0.01] exerted a more pronounced impact on aerobic endurance performance compared to the a.m. tests [SMD = −0.53; 95% CI (−1.12, 0.06); P = 0.08], which did not yield any statistically significant effects on aerobic endurance performance.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Forest plot of the effect of sleep deprivation on aerobic endurance performance in athletes and healthy non-athletes.

3.4.2 Anaerobic endurance performance

The effect of sleep deprivation on anaerobic endurance performance are shown in Figure 5 and Supplemetnary Figures S3, S4. Eight articles assessing the effects of sleep deprivation on anaerobic endurance performance were pooled for analysis. Pooled effect estimates were performed using random effects models. The combined effect size SMD was −0.08 [95% CI (−0.38, 0.22); P = 0.6; I2 = 50%], indicating that the SMD of anaerobic endurance performance was not statistically significant. Subgroup analysis showed that sleep deprivation had no significant effect on the anaerobic endurance performance of athletes [SMD = −0.1, 95% CI (−0.51, 0.32); P = 0.65] and healthy non-athletes [SMD = 0.01, 95% CI (−0.38, 0.4); P = 0.95]. Upon subgroup analysis of different sleep deprivation types, it was revealed that TSD [SMD = 0, 95% CI (−0.42, 0.42); P = 0.99], Partial [SMD = 0.25; 95% CI (−0.89, 1.38); P = 0.67], PSDB [SMD = −0.14; 95% CI (−0.63, 0.35); P = 0.58], and PSDE [SMD = −0.08; 95% CI (−0.68, 0.51); P = 0.79] had no significant impact on anaerobic endurance performance. In subgroup analysis by test time, a.m. tests [SMD = 0.01; 95% CI (−0.31, 0.33); P = 0.95] and p.m. tests [SMD = −0.17; 95% CI (−0.89, 0.54); P = 0.64] test had no significant effect on anaerobic endurance performance.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Forest plot of the effect of sleep deprivation on anerobic performance in athletes and healthy non-athletes. am: ante meridiem; pm: post meridiem; MP: Wingate mean power.

3.4.3 Explosive power

The effect of sleep deprivation on explosive power are shown in Figure 6 and Supplemetnary Figures S5, S6. A pooled analysis of 23 articles evaluating the effects of sleep deprivation on explosive power was conducted. The random effects model was used to estimate the combined effects. The combined effect size SMD was −0.46 [95% CI (−0.7, −0.21); P = 0.0002; I2 = 69%], indicating that the SMD of explosive power was statistically significant. Subgroup analysis indicated that sleep deprivation intervention significantly altered explosive power in athletes [SMD = −0.63; 95% CI (−0.94, −0.33); P < 0.00001], but not of healthy non-athletes [SMD = −0.02; 95% CI (−0.27, 0.24); P = 0.9]. Among various types of deprivation, PSDE [SMD = −1.04; 95% CI (−1.58, −0.5); P = 0.0002] were found to significantly diminish explosive performance. In contrast, TSD [SMD = −0.18; 95% CI (−0.38, 0.01); P = 0.07], PSDB [SMD = −0.19; 95% CI (−0.52, 0.15); P = 0.27] and the Partial [SMD = 0.12; 95% CI (−0.77, 1.01); P = 0.79] did not exhibit any significant impact on explosive performance. In terms of test period, the p.m. tests [SMD = −0.68; 95% CI (−1.06, −0.31); P = 0.0004] markedly altered explosive power, whereas the a.m. tests [SMD = −0.28; 95% CI (−0.6, 0.04); P = 0.08] had no significant influence on this aspect of physical performance.

Figure 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Forest plot of the effect of sleep deprivation on explosive power in athletes and healthy non-athletes. am, ante meridiem; pm, post meridiem; CMJ, Counter Movement Jump; SJ, Squat Jump; VJ, Vertical Jumps; PP, Wingate peak power; SMBT, seated medicine-ball throw.

3.4.4 Maximum force

The effect of sleep deprivation on maximum force are shown in Figure 7 and Supplemetnary Figures S7, S8. A pooled analysis of 10 articles evaluating the effects of sleep deprivation on maximum force was conducted. The fixed effects model was used to estimate the combined effects. The combined effect size SMD was −0.24 [95% CI (−0.4, −0.09); P = 0.002; I2 = 0%], indicating that the SMD of maximum force was statistically significant. Subgroup analysis indicated that sleep deprivation intervention significantly altered maximum force in athletes [SMD = −0.35; 95% CI (−0.56, −0.14); P = 0.001], but not of healthy non-athletes [SMD = −0.1; 95% CI (−0.34, 0.14); P = 0.42]. Among various types of deprivation, PSDE [SMD = −0.57; 95% CI (−0.94, −0.19); P = 0.003] were found to significantly diminish maximum force performance. In contrast, TSD [SMD = −0.16; 95% CI (−0.39, 0.06); P = 0.15] and PSDB [SMD = −0.19; 95% CI (−0.46, 0.08); P = 0.17] did not exhibit any significant impact on maximum force performance. In terms of test period, the p.m. tests [SMD = −0.3; 95% CI (−0.51, −0.09); P = 0.005] markedly altered maximum force, whereas the a.m. tests [SMD = −0.17; 95% CI (−0.4, 0.06); P = 0.15] had no significant influence on this aspect of physical performance.

Figure 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Forest plot of the effect of sleep deprivation on maximum force in athletes and healthy non-athletes. am, ante meridiem; pm, post meridiem; MVC, Maximal Isometric Voluntary Contraction; HG, Handgrip Strength; ILMS, isometric leg-muscles strength; IBMS, isometric back-muscles strength.

3.4.5 Speed

The effect of sleep deprivation on speed performance are shown in Figure 8 and Supplemetnary Figures S9, S10. Four articles assessing the effects of sleep deprivation on speed performance were pooled for analysis. Pooled effect estimates were performed using random effects models. The combined effect size SMD was −0.58 [95% CI (−0.95, −0.2); P = 0.003; I2 = 61%], indicating that the SMD of speed performance was statistically significant. Subgroup analysis showed that sleep deprivation had a significant effect on the speed performance of athletes [SMD = −0.52, 95% CI (−0.83, −0.22); P = 0.0008], but not of healthy non-athletes [SMD = −0.68; 95% CI (−1.98, 0.03); P = 0.31]. Upon subgroup analysis of different sleep deprivation types, it was revealed that TSD [SMD = −0.59; 95% CI (−1.54, 0.36); P = 0.22] did not exert any significant influence on speed performance, while PSDE [SMD = −0.78; 95% CI (−1.35, −0.2); P = 0.008] and PSDB [SMD = −0.57; 95% CI (−1.15, 0.01); P = 0.05] significantly reduced speed performance. In the subgroup analysis by test time, the a.m. tests [SMD = −0.81; 95% CI (−1.52, −0.1); P = 0.03] markedly altered speed performance, whereas the p.m. tests [SMD = −0.27; 95% CI (−0.63, 0.1); P = 0.15] had no significant influence on this aspect of physical performance.

Figure 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Forest plot of the effect of sleep deprivation on speed performance in athletes and healthy non-athletes.

3.4.6 Skill control

The effect of sleep deprivation on skill control are shown in Figure 9 and Supplemetnary Figures S11, S12. Using random-effects pooled effect sizes of four studies, sleep deprivation significantly reduced skill control performance compared with control participants [SMD = −0.87; 95% CI (−1.7, −0.04); P = 0.04; I2 = 90%]. Subgroup analysis showed that sleep deprivation had a significant effect on the skill control of athletes [SMD = −0.87; 95% CI (−1.7, −0.04); P = 0.04]. Upon subgroup analysis of different sleep deprivation types, it was revealed that TSD [SMD = 0; 95% CI (−0.32, 0.33); P = 0.98] had no significant impact on skill control. Conversely, Partial [SMD = −0.83; 95% CI (−1.55, −0.11); P = 0.02], PSDB [SMD = −2.12; 95% CI (−3.01, −1.24); P < 0.00001] were found to significantly degrade skill control. In a subgroup analysis based on trial time, a.m. tests [SMD = −0.11; 95% CI (−0.49, 0.26); P = 0.55] did not exhibit any significant influence on skill control. In contrast, p.m. tests [SMD = −2.12; 95% CI (−3.01, −1.24); P < 0.00001] was found to have an impact on skill control.

Figure 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Forest plot of the effect of sleep deprivation on skill control performance in athletes and healthy non-athletes.

3.4.7 Rating of perceived exertion (RPE)

The effect of sleep deprivation on RPE are shown in Figure 10 and Supplemetnary Figures S13, S14. By employing random-effects meta-analysis on data from 12 studies, it was determined that sleep deprivation significantly increased the RPE compared to the control group [SMD = 0.51; 95% CI (0.2, 0.82); P = 0.001; I2 = 62%]. Subgroup analyses indicated that while sleep deprivation heightened subjective exertion in athletes [SMD = 0.39; 95% CI (0.11, 0.66); P = 0.006], it did not have a similar effect on healthy non-athletes [SMD = 1.23; 95% CI (−0.11, 2.58); P = 0.07]. Upon subgroup analysis of different sleep deprivation types, it was revealed that TSD [SMD = 0.37; 95% CI (−0.72, 1.45); P = 0.51] had no significant impact on RPE. Conversely, Partial [SMD = 0.62; 95% CI (0.06, 1.18); P = 0.03], PSDB [SMD = 0.47; 95% CI (0.02, 0.92); P = 0.04], and PSDE [SMD = 0.6; 95% CI (0.17, 1.02); P = 0.006] were found to significantly increase RPE. In the time-based subgroup analysis, both a.m. tests [SMD = 0.44; 95% CI (0.01, 0.86); P = 0.04] and p.m. tests [SMD = 0.72; 95% CI (0.20, 1.24); P = 0.007] increased RPE.

Figure 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Forest plot of the effect of sleep deprivation on rating of perceived exertion in athletes and healthy non-athletes. am, ante meridiem; pm, post meridiem; MP, Wingate mean power.

3.5 Sensitivity analysis

To confirm the robustness of the results, sensitivity analyses were performed separately for aerobic endurance performance, anaerobic endurance performance, explosive power, maximum force, speed, skills control, RPE. The results revealed that the overall results were robust and stable in sensitivity analyses of aerobic endurance performance, anaerobic endurance performance, explosive power, maximum force, speed, skills control, RPE after deleting each study (Supplementary Figures S15–S21).

3.6 Evolution of publication bias

The outcomes of more than 10 RCTs were evaluated for publication bias. No obvious asymmetric distribution was observed in the funnel plot of explosive power, maximum force, speed and RPE, and the Egger’s test showed no publication bias in the correlation analysis between explosive power (t = −2.02, P = 0.052), maximum force (t = 0.03, P = 0.978), speed (t = −2.03, P = 0.07) and RPE (t = 1.24, P = 0.229). However, publication bias was noted in the correlation analysis of aerobic endurance performance (t = −2.31, P = 0.032). Aerobic endurance performance results were found to be stable after the four virtual articles were supplemented using the trim-and-fill method, indicating that this publishing bias did not affect the estimation (Supplemetnary Figures S22–S27).

4 Discussion

In this systematic review and meta-analysis, we synthesize the latest experimental studies on the effects of sleep deprivation on athletic performance and perception of fatigue. Our findings suggest that sleep deprivation significantly reduces the athletes' explosive power, maximum power, speed performance, and motor control, and increases their RPE. For healthy non-athletes, lack of sleep can negatively affect their aerobic endurance performance. However, we observed that sleep deprivation had no significant effect on anaerobic endurance levels in athletes and non-athletes. In addition, our systematic review shows that TSD impairs aerobic endurance performance. PSDE reduces explosive power and maximum force. Both PSDE and PSDB affect speed performance and RPE. PSDB can impair skill control. Regarding the timing of the test, the p.m. assessment had substantial negative effects on aerobic endurance performance, explosive power, maximum force, and skill control. Both a.m. and p.m. tests were associated with an increase in RPE.

4.1 Analysis of the effects of sleep deprivation on different types of sports performance

Sleep deprivation exerts a profound and varied impact on sports performance, with different performance indicators being affected to varying extents. The effect size analysis of this study revealed that sleep deprivation had the most significant adverse effects on skill control and aerobic endurance performance, with effect sizes of −0.87 and −0.76, respectively. This was followed by a decline in speed performance and an increase in RPE, with effect sizes of −0.58 and 0.51, respectively. Finally, there was a reduction in explosive power and maximal strength, with effect sizes of −0.46 and −0.24, respectively. Notably, the effect of sleep deprivation on anaerobic endurance was not statistically significant.

The study found that sleep influences various physiological factors, including glucose metabolism (VanHelder et al., 1993), energy metabolism (Penev, 2007), hormone secretion (Leproult and Van Cauter, 2010), and immune function (Santos et al., 2007). The deterioration in skill control motor performance following sleep deprivation is closely linked to impairments in executive functions, including working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility (Cheng et al., 2021). Empirical evidence indicates that sleep deprivation leads to prolonged reaction times, increased error rates, and reduced behavioral accuracy (Pallesen et al., 2017; Filipas et al., 2021b). From a neurological perspective, the underlying mechanisms may involve decreased activation of attention and salience networks, which are essential for cognitive resource allocation, and alterations in antagonistic brain networks. This dysfunction is often linked to lower testosterone levels and altered dopamine regulation, as supported by animal models (Filipas et al., 2021b). These findings highlight the complex relationship between sleep deprivation, cognitive function, and neural activity, underscoring the critical role of adequate sleep in preserving optimal cognitive and motor performance (Gong et al., 2024). Aerobic endurance performance is characterized by lower intensity, longer duration (typically more than 30 min), and energy supply predominantly derived from aerobic oxidation (Vondra et al., 1981; Kong et al., 2002). Speed is characterized by high-intensity work, longer duration, and energy primarily derived from glycolysis. Consequently, the availability of energy reserves and the ability to resynthesize energy during exercise are key factors influencing both aerobic endurance and speed performance. Sleep deprivation increases energy expenditure and depletes muscle and liver glycogen, impairing the body’s ability to resynthesize energy substrates during exercise (Skein et al., 2011a). Moreover, sleep is essential for muscle recovery, primarily due to its influence on hormone secretion (Dáttilo et al., 2020). Hormones associated with aerobic endurance performance, such as growth hormone and insulin-like growth factor, are altered following sleep deprivation (Lack and Wright, 2007; Piovezan et al., 2015). As a result, both aerobic endurance and speed performance are impaired following sleep deprivation.

The pronounced impact on explosive power and maximal strength can be attributed to alterations in the RPE, a measure of the subjective effort athletes experience during physical exertion. When athletes perceive the effort required to perform a task as exceeding their maximum capacity, their motivation and efficiency to sustain performance typically decrease (Roberts et al., 2019a). Sleep deprivation can distort the body’s perception of fatigue, leading to heightened subjective fatigue during exercises of comparable intensity (Vitale et al., 2021). This increased fatigue is especially apparent in high-intensity exercises compared to medium-intensity ones (Oliver et al., 2009). Consequently, sleep deprivation affects not only physical capacity but also the mental perception of effort, both of which can significantly influence sports performance and recovery. A study found that mental fatigue had a minor effect on anaerobic performance (Wei et al., 2024), possibly because anaerobic endurance tests require specific power patterns and completion times, resulting in an insignificant total effect size of sleep deprivation on anaerobic endurance performance.

4.2 Analysis of the effects of sleep deprivation on sports performance in athletes and healthy non-athletes

The effects of sleep deprivation on athletic performance vary across different populations. The effect size analysis in this study revealed that sleep deprivation significantly impaired athletes' aerobic endurance, explosive power, maximal force, speed, skill control, and RPE, with effect sizes of −0.66, −0.63, −0.35, −0.52, −0.87, and 0.39, respectively. Simultaneously, it negatively affects the aerobic performance of healthy non-athletes, with an effect size of −1.02.

The effects of sleep deprivation on athletes are closely linked to the specific demands of their sport disciplines. In sports that require aggressive and defensive maneuvers against opponents, athletes must possess high levels of physical fitness, including explosiveness, speed, well-developed cognitive functions, sharp decision-making skills, and quick reflexes (Mori et al., 2002; Chaabène et al., 2012; Di Corrado et al., 2014; Daaloul et al., 2019). In contrast, sports that require high precision and skill place a greater emphasis on advanced cognitive performance (Filipas et al., 2021a). Healthy non-athletes, who do not engage in such physically demanding activities regularly, rely less on these physiological parameters in their daily routines. Therefore, explosive power, maximal strength, speed, skill control, and RPE may be more significantly negatively affected in athletes after sleep deprivation than in non-athletes. Regarding aerobic exercise, sleep deprivation increases energy expenditure, making aerobic performance in both athletes and non-athletes susceptible to its detrimental effects.

4.3 Analysis of the influence of TSD, PSDE, PSDB on different sports performance

Different aspects of sports performance are influenced by distinct types of sleep deprivation. TSD exerts a more significant effect on aerobic performance (−0.56), while partial sleep deprivation disproportionately affects explosive power, maximal strength, speed, skill control, and RPE. Specifically, explosive power, maximal force, and speed were primarily affected by PSDE (effect sizes −1.04, −0.57, and −0.78, respectively). Skill control was primarily affected by PSDB (effect sizes −0.57 and −2.12, respectively), while RPE was influenced by both PSDB and PSDE (effect sizes 0.47 and 0.6, respectively). Speed performance was also impacted by PSDB and PSDE, with effect sizes of −0.57 and −0.78, respectively. These findings underscore the subtle effects of sleep deprivation on various components of sports performance.

Aerobic endurance performance and explosive power are particularly compromised during total sleep deprivation, likely due to increased energy expenditure and impaired resynthesis of energy substrates. During PSDE, explosive power and maximal force are diminished. Moreover, PSDE has a more pronounced effect on speed performance than PSDB. Zerouali’s research suggests that PSDE has a more pronounced impact on athletes' selective attention than PSDB, possibly due to its effect on rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and slow wave sleep (SWS), critical stages for attention restoration and various physiological functions, such as memory consolidation, immune repair, energy recovery, and hormone release (Zerouali et al., 2010; Mejri et al., 2016a). PSDB may lead to reduced skill control, potentially stemming from difficulty focusing and sustaining attention during PSDB(Boksem et al., 2005). Additionally, reduced testosterone levels following PSDB could affect dopaminergic signaling in brain regions associated with cognition, potentially resulting in an increased incidence of technical errors (Leproult and Van Cauter, 2011; Jardí et al., 2018; Moreira et al., 2018). The effect size of PSDE on RPE is larger than that of PSDB. This discrepancy may be attributed to the prolonged wakefulness and subsequent sleepiness following PSDE (Souissi et al., 2013). Furthermore, research indicates that PSDE is associated with increased inflammation, leading to greater cellular damage and, consequently, heightened subjective sleepiness (Irwin et al., 2016).

4.4 Analysis of differences in a.m. and p.m. sports performance after sleep deprivation

The effect size analysis of this study revealed a decrease in speed performance and a significant increase in RPE during the a.m. tests, with effect sizes of −0.81 and 0.44, respectively. In the p.m. tests, aerobic endurance performance, explosive power, maximal force, skill control, and RPE decreased, with effect sizes of −1.4, −0.68, −0.3, −2.12, and 0.72, respectively.

The analysis suggested that sleep deprivation induced fatigue in the subjects, leading to a decrease in alertness, alterations in energy expenditure, depletion of strength reserves, and a decline in motor control precision (Gong et al., 2024). These factors likely contributed to the observed decline in afternoon sports performance following sleep deprivation. Speed performance may be associated with an increase in RPE (Helms et al., 2016). Longer waking hours reduce alertness, prolong reaction time, and decrease speed. However, for RPE, its effect size increased in both morning and afternoon sessions, possibly due to disruption of the circadian rhythm and alteration of muscle cell function following sleep deprivation, which affects muscle contraction and relaxation. Additionally, the extended waking hours compared to a normal night’s sleep lead to a progressive increase in sleep pressure, disrupting sleep homeostasis (Gong et al., 2024). Adenosine accumulation in the brain serves as the physiological foundation for regulating sleep homeostasis (Romdhani et al., 2021). The longer one remains awake, the higher the adenosine concentration, which in turn heightens the perception of exertion following physical activity (Gong et al., 2024).

In conclusion, sleep deprivation affects the athletic performance of both athletes and healthy non-athletes. Although this study primarily focused on the effects of sleep deprivation on sports performance and perceived fatigue, sleep recovery strategies play a key role in mitigating its negative impact. Sleep recovery plays an essential role in restoring cognitive function, muscle recovery, and overall performance (Nédélec et al., 2015; Mesas et al., 2023). Current sleep interventions aimed at recovery include sleep extension, daytime naps, sleep hygiene, and post-exercise recovery strategies (Bonnar et al., 2018; Mesas et al., 2023). These strategies have varying effects on athletic performance and recovery. Therefore, to protect the sleep health and athletic performance of both athletes and non-athletes, appropriate sleep recovery strategies should be adopted following sleep deprivation. It is important to note that while this study primarily focuses on performance testing under standardized laboratory conditions, there may be differences between these tests and an athlete’s performance in real-world sports or competition settings. Laboratory tests provide a controlled environment for assessing the fundamental physiological and psychological factors affecting performance, while real-world performance is influenced by additional uncontrollable factors, such as competitive pressure and environmental conditions. Thus, while the results of this study provide valuable insights into the effects of sleep deprivation on athlete performance, the external validity of these findings in actual sporting contexts may be limited. Future research could further assess the generalizability of these results by conducting sleep deprivation tests in simulated competitive environments or real-world sports settings. For example, evaluating athlete fatigue and recovery based on task performance in actual competitions would help enhance the ecological validity of the research, providing a more accurate representation of performance in real-world athletic situations.

4.5 Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, some studies did not provide original data, which prevented the inclusion of all available research in the analysis. Second, due to the limited number of studies, the results for some outcome measures exhibited heterogeneity, making interpretation of the findings challenging. Third, due to the inclusion criteria, the majority of the studies primarily focused on the acute effects of sleep deprivation on sports performance and perceived fatigue, meaning this review is more suited for understanding the short-term physiological impacts of sleep deprivation on sports performance and lacks insights into long-term effects. The studies included in this review utilized a variety of methodologies, including differences in experimental designs, outcome measures, and assessment tools. Fourth, this heterogeneity may introduce some inconsistencies in the interpretation of the results. Despite efforts to standardize data collection and reporting, the variation in methodologies could limit the comparability of studies and the overall robustness of our conclusions. Fifth, one of the key limitations of this meta-analysis is the variability in the control of sleep quality across the included studies. While we selected studies that implemented sleep control nights with habitual sleep patterns and a sleep duration of approximately 7 h per night, there was considerable variation in how sleep quality parameters such as sleep efficiency, sleep latency, and sleep fragmentation were monitored and reported. Some studies did not provide detailed information on these factors, which could potentially influence the results related to sleep deprivation’s effects on athletic performance and fatigue perception. Finally, because there were fewer female participants in the included studies, gender differences could not be analyzed.

5 Conclusions and suggestions

Our findings indicate that sleep deprivation significantly impairs aerobic endurance, muscular strength, speed, and motor control, while also increasing perceived exertion (RPE). Specifically, athletes experience significant declines in performance, particularly in aerobic endurance, muscular strength, speed, motor control, and perceived exertion. Similarly, non-athletes also show a notable reduction in aerobic endurance. Further analysis reveals that the impact of sleep deprivation on sports performance extends beyond physical decline and is associated with disruptions in neurological, metabolic, and psychological functions. Notably, the effects of sleep deprivation on performance were more pronounced during assessments conducted in the afternoon and for the PSDE sleep deprivation type. Overall, sleep deprivation can have a broad negative impact on both athletes' and non-athletes' performance, particularly during training or competitive events. Therefore, ensuring adequate sleep is crucial for optimizing athletes' performance and overall health. Future research should explore the specific effects of sleep deprivation on sports performance across different types of exercise and athlete states.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

YK: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Writing–original draft, Writing–review and editing. BY: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Writing–review and editing. GG: Conceptualization, Writing–review and editing. YW: Conceptualization, Writing–review and editing. HH: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Writing–review and editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

Author GG was employed by DeRucci Healthy Sleep Co., Ltd.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphys.2025.1544286/full#supplementary-material

References

Abedelmalek S., Boussetta N., Chtourou H., Souissi N., Tabka Z. (2014). Effect of partial sleep deprivation and racial variation on short-term maximal performance. Biol. Rhythm Res. 45 (5), 1–10. doi:10.1080/09291016.2014.904574

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abedelmalek S., Chtourou H., Aloui A., Aouichaoui C., Souissi N., Tabka Z. (2013). Effect of time of day and partial sleep deprivation on plasma concentrations of IL-6 during a short-term maximal performance. Eur. J. Appl. Physiology 113 (1), 241–248. doi:10.1007/s00421-012-2432-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajjimaporn A., Ramyarangsi P., Siripornpanich V. (2020). Effects of a 20 - min nap after sleep deprivation on brain activity and Soccer performance. Int. J. Sports Med. 41 (14), 1009–1016. doi:10.1055/a-1192-6187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azboy O., Kaygisiz Z. (2009). Effects of sleep deprivation on cardiorespiratory functions of the runners and volleyball players during rest and exercise. Acta Physiol. Hung 96 (1), 29–36. doi:10.1556/APhysiol.96.2009.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banks S., Dinges D. F. (2007). Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J. Clin. Sleep. Med. 3 (5), 519–528. doi:10.5664/jcsm.26918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben Cheikh R., Latiri I., Dogui M., Ben Saad H. (2017). Effects of one-night sleep deprivation on selective attention and isometric force in adolescent karate athletes. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fit. 57 (6), 752–759. doi:10.23736/s0022-4707.16.06323-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumert P. A., Crum A. J., Ernsting M., Volek J. S., Hollander D. B., Haff E. E., et al. (2007). The acute effects of twenty-four hours of sleep loss on the performance of national-caliber male collegiate weightlifters. J. Strength Cond. Res. 21 (4), 1146–1154. doi:10.1519/r-21606.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem M. A., Meijman T. F., Lorist M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 25 (1), 107–116. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnar D., Bartel K., Kakoschke N., Lang C. (2018). Sleep interventions designed to improve athletic performance and recovery: a systematic review of Current Approaches. Sports Med. 48 (3), 683–703. doi:10.1007/s40279-017-0832-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotherton E. J., Moseley S. E., Langan-Evans C., Pullinger S. A., Robertson C. M., Burniston J. G., et al. (2019a). Effects of two nights partial sleep deprivation on an evening submaximal weightlifting performance; are 1 h powernaps useful on the day of competition? Chronobiol Int. 36 (3), 407–426. doi:10.1080/07420528.2018.1552702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotherton E. J., Moseley S. E., Langan-Evans C., Pullinger S. A., Robertson C. M., Burniston J. G., et al. (2019b). Effects of two nights partial sleep deprivation on an evening submaximal weightlifting performance; are 1 h powernaps useful on the day of competition? Chronobiology Int. 36 (3), 407–426. doi:10.1080/07420528.2018.1552702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaabène H., Hachana Y., Franchini E., Mkaouer B., Chamari K. (2012). Physical and physiological profile of elite karate athletes. Sports Med. 42 (10), 829–843. doi:10.1007/bf03262297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charest J., Grandner M. A. (2020). Sleep and athletic performance: impacts on physical performance, mental performance, injury risk and recovery, and mental health. Sleep. Med. Clin. 15 (1), 41–57. doi:10.1016/j.jsmc.2019.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chase J. D., Roberson P. A., Saunders M. J., Hargens T. A., Womack C. J., Luden N. D. (2017). One night of sleep restriction following heavy exercise impairs 3-km cycling time-trial performance in the morning. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 42 (9), 909–915. doi:10.1139/apnm-2016-0698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen H. I. (1991). Effects of 30-h sleep loss on cardiorespiratory functions at rest and in exercise. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc 23 (2), 193–198. doi:10.1249/00005768-199102000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng L. Y., Che T., Tomic G., Slutzky M. W., Paller K. A. (2021). Memory Reactivation during sleep Improves execution of a challenging motor skill. J. Neurosci. 41 (46), 9608–9616. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0265-21.2021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cincin A., Sari I., Oğuz M., Sert S., Bozbay M., Ataş H., et al. (2015). Effect of acute sleep deprivation on heart rate recovery in healthy young adults. Sleep. Breath. 19 (2), 631–636. doi:10.1007/s11325-014-1066-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craven J., McCartney D., Desbrow B., Sabapathy S., Bellinger P., Roberts L., et al. (2022). Effects of acute sleep loss on physical performance: a systematic and meta-Analytical review. Sports Med. 52 (11), 2669–2690. doi:10.1007/s40279-022-01706-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cullen T., Thomas G., Wadley A. J., Myers T. (2019). The effects of a single night of complete and partial sleep deprivation on physical and cognitive performance: a Bayesian analysis. J. Sports Sci. 37 (23), 2726–2734. doi:10.1080/02640414.2019.1662539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daaloul H., Souissi N., Davenne D. (2019). Effects of napping on alertness, cognitive, and physical outcomes of karate athletes. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 51 (2), 338–345. doi:10.1249/mss.0000000000001786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daanen H. A., van Ling S., Tan T. K. (2013). Subjective ratings and performance in the heat and after sleep deprivation. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 84 (7), 701–707. doi:10.3357/asem.3494.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dáttilo M., Antunes H. K. M., Galbes N. M. N., Mônico-Neto M., Dos Santos Quaresma M. V. L., Lee K. S., et al. (2020). Effects of sleep deprivation on acute skeletal muscle recovery after exercise. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc 52 (2), 507–514. doi:10.1249/mss.0000000000002137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daviaux Y., Mignardot J. B., Cornu C., Deschamps T. (2014). Effects of total sleep deprivation on the perception of action capabilities. Exp. Brain Res. 232 (7), 2243–2253. doi:10.1007/s00221-014-3915-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean B., Hartmann T., Wingfield G., Larsen P., Skein M. (2023). Sleep restriction between consecutive days of exercise impairs sprint and endurance cycling performance. J. Sleep. Res. 32 (5), e13857. doi:10.1111/jsr.13857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Corrado D., Guarnera M., Quartiroli A. (2014). Vividness and transformation of mental images in karate and ballet. Percept. Mot. Ski. 119 (3), 764–773. doi:10.2466/22.24.PMS.119c30z6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrara M., De Gennaro L. (2001). How much sleep do we need? Sleep. Med. Rev. 5 (2), 155–179. doi:10.1053/smrv.2000.0138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filipas L., Ferioli D., Banfi G., La Torre A., Vitale J. A. (2021a). Single and combined effect of acute sleep restriction and mental fatigue on basketball Free-throw performance. Int. J. Sports Physiology Perform. 16 (3), 415–420. doi:10.1123/ijspp.2020-0142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filipas L., Ferioli D., Banfi G., La Torre A., Vitale J. A. (2021b). Single and combined effect of acute sleep restriction and mental fatigue on basketball Free-throw performance. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 16 (3), 415–420. doi:10.1123/ijspp.2020-0142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao B., Dwivedi S., Milewski M. D., Cruz A. I. (2019). Lack of sleep and sports Injuries in Adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Pediatr. Orthop. 39 (5), e324–e333. doi:10.1097/bpo.0000000000001306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goel N. (2017). Neurobehavioral effects and biomarkers of sleep loss in healthy adults. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 17 (11), 89. doi:10.1007/s11910-017-0799-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong M., Sun M., Sun Y., Jin L., Li S. (2024). Effects of acute sleep deprivation on sporting performance in athletes: a comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis. Nat. Sci. Sleep. 16, 935–948. doi:10.2147/nss.S467531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

HajSalem M., Chtourou H., Aloui A., Hammouda O., Souissi N. (2013). Effects of partial sleep deprivation at the end of the night on anaerobic performances in judokas. Biol. Rhythm Res. 44 (5), 815–821. doi:10.1080/09291016.2012.756282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helms E. R., Cronin J., Storey A., Zourdos M. C. (2016). Application of the Repetitions in Reserve-based rating of perceived exertion Scale for Resistance training. Strength Cond. J. 38 (4), 42–49. doi:10.1519/ssc.0000000000000218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins J. P., Thompson S. G., Deeks J. J., Altman D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Bmj 327 (7414), 557–560. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honn K. A., Hinson J. M., Whitney P., Van Dongen H. P. A. (2019). Cognitive flexibility: a distinct element of performance impairment due to sleep deprivation. Accid. Anal. Prev. 126, 191–197. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2018.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irwin M. R., Olmstead R., Carroll J. E. (2016). Sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and inflammation: a systematic review and meta-analysis of Cohort studies and experimental sleep deprivation. Biol. Psychiatry 80 (1), 40–52. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jardí F., Laurent M. R., Kim N., Khalil R., De Bundel D., Van Eeckhaut A., et al. (2018). Testosterone boosts physical activity in male mice via dopaminergic pathways. Sci. Rep. 8 (1), 957. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-19104-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juliff L. E., Halson S. L., Peiffer J. J. (2015). Understanding sleep disturbance in athletes prior to important competitions. J. Sci. Med. Sport 18 (1), 13–18. doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2014.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kecklund G., Axelsson J. (2016). Health consequences of shift work and insufficient sleep. Bmj 355, i5210. doi:10.1136/bmj.i5210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knowles O. E., Drinkwater E. J., Roberts S. S. H., Alexander S. E., Abbott G., Garnham A., et al. (2022). Sustained sleep restriction reduces Resistance exercise quality and quantity in females. Med. and Sci. Sports and Exerc. 54 (12), 2167–2177. doi:10.1249/mss.0000000000003000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong J., Shepel P. N., Holden C. P., Mackiewicz M., Pack A. I., Geiger J. D. (2002). Brain glycogen decreases with increased periods of wakefulness: implications for homeostatic drive to sleep. J. Neurosci. 22 (13), 5581–5587. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.22-13-05581.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konishi M., Takahashi M., Endo N., Numao S., Takagi S., Miyashita M., et al. (2013). Effects of sleep deprivation on autonomic and endocrine functions throughout the day and on exercise tolerance in the evening. J. Sports Sci. 31 (3), 248–255. doi:10.1080/02640414.2012.733824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lack L. C., Wright H. R. (2007). Chronobiology of sleep in humans. Cell Mol. Life Sci. 64 (10), 1205–1215. doi:10.1007/s00018-007-6531-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lastella M., Lovell G. P., Sargent C. (2014). Athletes' precompetitive sleep behaviour and its relationship with subsequent precompetitive mood and performance. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 14 (Suppl. 1), S123–S130. doi:10.1080/17461391.2012.660505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leproult R., Van Cauter E. (2010). Role of sleep and sleep loss in hormonal release and metabolism. Endocr. Dev. 17, 11–21. doi:10.1159/000262524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leproult R., Van Cauter E. (2011). Effect of 1 week of sleep restriction on testosterone levels in young healthy men. Jama 305 (21), 2173–2174. doi:10.1001/jama.2011.710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopes T. R., Pereira H. M., Bittencourt L. R. A., Silva B. M. (2023). How much does sleep deprivation impair endurance performance? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 23 (7), 1279–1292. doi:10.1080/17461391.2022.2155583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma J., Yao Y. J., Ma R. M., Li J. Q., Wang T., Li X. J., et al. (2009). Effects of sleep deprivation on human postural control, subjective fatigue assessment and psychomotor performance. J. Int. Med. Res. 37 (5), 1311–1320. doi:10.1177/147323000903700506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnuson J. R., Kang H. J., Debenham M. I. B., McNeil C. J., Dalton B. H. (2023). Effects of sleep deprivation on perceived and performance fatigability in females: an exploratory study. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 23 (9), 1922–1931. doi:10.1080/17461391.2022.2115944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mah C. D., Sparks A. J., Samaan M. A., Souza R. B., Luke A. (2019). Sleep restriction impairs maximal jump performance and joint coordination in elite athletes. J. Sports Sci. 37 (17), 1981–1988. doi:10.1080/02640414.2019.1612504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin B. J., Chen H. I. (1984). Sleep loss and the sympathoadrenal response to exercise. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc 16 (1), 56–59. doi:10.1249/00005768-198401000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mejri M. A., Yousfi N., Mhenni T., Tayech A., Hammouda O., Driss T., et al. (2016a). Does one night of partial sleep deprivation affect the evening performance during intermittent exercise in Taekwondo players? J. Exerc Rehabil. 12 (1), 47–53. doi:10.12965/jer.150256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mejri M. A., Yousfi N., Mhenni T., Tayech A., Hammouda O., Driss T., et al. (2016b). Does one night of partial sleep deprivation affect the evening performance during intermittent exercise in Taekwondo players? J. Exerc. rehabilitation 12 (1), 47–53. doi:10.12965/jer.150256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesas A. E., Núñez de Arenas-Arroyo S., Martinez-Vizcaino V., Garrido-Miguel M., Fernández-Rodríguez R., Bizzozero-Peroni B., et al. (2023). Is daytime napping an effective strategy to improve sport-related cognitive and physical performance and reduce fatigue? A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Br. J. Sports Med. 57 (7), 417–426. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2022-106355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreira A., Aoki M. S., Franchini E., da Silva Machado D. G., Paludo A. C., Okano A. H. (2018). Mental fatigue impairs technical performance and alters neuroendocrine and autonomic responses in elite young basketball players. Physiol. Behav. 196, 112–118. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mori S., Ohtani Y., Imanaka K. (2002). Reaction times and anticipatory skills of karate athletes. Hum. Mov. Sci. 21 (2), 213–230. doi:10.1016/s0167-9457(02)00103-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mougin F., Bourdin H., Simon-Rigaud M. L., Didier J. M., Toubin G., Kantelip J. P. (1996). Effects of a selective sleep deprivation on subsequent anaerobic performance. Int. J. Sports Med. 17 (2), 115–119. doi:10.1055/s-2007-972818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mougin F., Bourdin H., Simon-Rigaud M. L., Nguyen Nhu U., Kantelip J. P., Davenne D. (2001). Hormonal responses to exercise after partial sleep deprivation and after a hypnotic drug-induced sleep. J. Sports Sci. 19 (2), 89–97. doi:10.1080/026404101300036253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nedelec M., Aloulou A., Duforez F., Meyer T., Dupont G. (2018). The variability of sleep Among elite athletes. Sports Med. Open 4 (1), 34. doi:10.1186/s40798-018-0151-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nédélec M., Halson S., Delecroix B., Abaidia A. E., Ahmaidi S., Dupont G. (2015). Sleep hygiene and recovery strategies in elite Soccer players. Sports Med. 45 (11), 1547–1559. doi:10.1007/s40279-015-0377-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliver S. J., Costa R. J., Laing S. J., Bilzon J. L., Walsh N. P. (2009). One night of sleep deprivation decreases treadmill endurance performance. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 107 (2), 155–161. doi:10.1007/s00421-009-1103-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pallesen S., Gundersen H. S., Kristoffersen M., Bjorvatn B., Thun E., Harris A. (2017). The effects of sleep deprivation on Soccer skills. Percept. Mot. Ski. 124 (4), 812–829. doi:10.1177/0031512517707412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penev P. D. (2007). Sleep deprivation and energy metabolism: to sleep, perchance to eat? Curr. Opin. Endocrinol. Diabetes Obes. 14 (5), 374–381. doi:10.1097/MED.0b013e3282be9093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilcher J. J., Huffcutt A. I. (1996). Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: a meta-analysis. Sleep 19 (4), 318–326. doi:10.1093/sleep/19.4.318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piovezan R. D., Abucham J., Dos Santos R. V., Mello M. T., Tufik S., Poyares D. (2015). The impact of sleep on age-related sarcopenia: Possible connections and clinical implications. Ageing Res. Rev. 23 (Pt B), 210–220. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2015.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Racinais S., Hue O., Blonc S., Le Gallais D. (2004). Effect of sleep deprivation on shuttle run score in middle-aged amateur athletes. Influence of initial score. J. sports Med. Phys. Fit. 44 (3), 246–248.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rae D., Chin T., Dikgomo K., Hill L., McKune A., Kohn T., et al. (2017). One night of partial sleep deprivation impairs recovery from a single exercise training session. Eur. J. Appl. Physiology 117 (4), 699–712. doi:10.1007/s00421-017-3565-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly T. (1983). Effects of partial sleep loss on subjective states, psychomotor and physical performance tests.

Google Scholar

Reyner L. A., Horne J. A. (2013). Sleep restriction and serving accuracy in performance tennis players, and effects of caffeine. Physiology and Behav. 120, 93–96. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds A. C., Banks S. (2010). Total sleep deprivation, chronic sleep restriction and sleep disruption. Prog. Brain Res. 185, 91–103. doi:10.1016/b978-0-444-53702-7.00006-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts S. S. H., Teo W. P., Aisbett B., Warmington S. A. (2019a). Effects of total sleep deprivation on endurance cycling performance and heart rate indices used for monitoring athlete readiness. J. Sports Sci. 37 (23), 2691–2701. doi:10.1080/02640414.2019.1661561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts S. S. H., Teo W. P., Aisbett B., Warmington S. A. (2019b). Effects of total sleep deprivation on endurance cycling performance and heart rate indices used for monitoring athlete readiness. J. Sports Sci. 37 (23), 2691–2701. doi:10.1080/02640414.2019.1661561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts S. S. H., Teo W. P., Aisbett B., Warmington S. A. (2019c). Extended sleep maintains endurance performance Better than normal or restricted sleep. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 51 (12), 2516–2523. doi:10.1249/mss.0000000000002071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romdhani M., Hammouda O., Chaabouni Y., Mahdouani K., Driss T., Chamari K., et al. (2019). Sleep deprivation affects post-lunch dip performances, biomarkers of muscle damage and antioxidant status. Biol. Sport 36 (1), 55–65. doi:10.5114/biolsport.2018.78907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romdhani M., Souissi N., Chaabouni Y., Mahdouani K., Driss T., Chamari K., et al. (2020). Improved physical performance and decreased muscular and oxidative damage with Postlunch napping after partial sleep deprivation in athletes. Int. J. Sports Physiology Perform. 15 (6), 874–883. doi:10.1123/ijspp.2019-0308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romdhani M., Souissi N., Moussa-Chamari I., Chaabouni Y., Mahdouani K., Sahnoun Z., et al. (2021). Caffeine Use or napping to enhance repeated sprint performance after partial sleep deprivation: Why not both? Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 16 (5), 711–718. doi:10.1123/ijspp.2019-0792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saddoud A., Rekik G., Belkhir Y., Kammoun N., Hidouri S., Chlif M., et al. (2023). One night of total sleep deprivation impairs decision-making skills, anaerobic performances, mood states, and perceptual responses in elite Kung-Fu athletes. Biol. Rhythm Res. 54 (1), 102–116. doi:10.1080/09291016.2022.2090673

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samuels C. (2008). Sleep, recovery, and performance: the new frontier in high-performance athletics. Neurol. Clin. 26 (1), 169–180. doi:10.1016/j.ncl.2007.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santos R. V., Tufik S., De Mello M. T. (2007). Exercise, sleep and cytokines: is there a relation? Sleep. Med. Rev. 11 (3), 231–239. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2007.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamseer L., Moher D., Clarke M., Ghersi D., Liberati A., Petticrew M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. Bmj 350, g7647. doi:10.1136/bmj.g7647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skein M., Duffield R., Edge J., Short M. J., Mündel T. (2011a). Intermittent-sprint performance and muscle glycogen after 30 h of sleep deprivation. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc 43 (7), 1301–1311. doi:10.1249/MSS.0b013e31820abc5a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skein M., Duffield R., Edge J., Short M. J., Mündel T. (2011b). Intermittent-Sprint performance and muscle glycogen after 30 h of sleep deprivation. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 43 (7), 1301–1311. doi:10.1249/MSS.0b013e31820abc5a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skein M., Duffield R., Minett G. M., Snape A., Murphy A. (2013). The effect of overnight sleep deprivation after competitive rugby league matches on postmatch physiological and perceptual recovery. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 8 (5), 556–564. doi:10.1123/ijspp.8.5.556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skurvydas A., Kazlauskaite D., Zlibinaite L., Cekanauskaite A., Valanciene D., Karanauskiene D., et al. (2021). Effects of two nights of sleep deprivation on executive function and central and peripheral fatigue during maximal voluntary contraction lasting 60s. Physiology and Behav. 229, 113226. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skurvydas A., Zlibinaite L., Solianik R., Brazaitis M., Valanciene D., Baranauskiene N., et al. (2020). One night of sleep deprivation impairs executive function but does not affect psychomotor or motor performance. Biol. Sport 37 (1), 7–14. doi:10.5114/biolsport.2020.89936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smithies T. D., Toth A. J., Dunican I. C., Caldwell J. A., Kowal M., Campbell M. J. (2021). The effect of sleep restriction on cognitive performance in elite cognitive performers: a systematic review. Sleep 44 (7), zsab008. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsab008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souissi N., Chtourou H., Aloui A., Hammouda O., Dogui M., Chaouachi A., et al. (2013). Effects of time-of-day and partial sleep deprivation on short-term maximal performances of judo competitors. J. Strength Cond. Res. 27 (9), 2473–2480. doi:10.1519/JSC.0b013e31827f4792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souissi N., Sesboue B., Gauthier A., Larue J., Davenne D. (2003). Effects of one night's sleep deprivation on anaerobic performance the following day. Eur. J. Appl. Physiology 89 (3-4), 359–366. doi:10.1007/s00421-003-0793-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souissi W., Hammouda O., Ayachi M., Ammar A., Khcharem A., de Marco G., et al. (2020). Partial sleep deprivation affects endurance performance and psychophysiological responses during 12-minute self-paced running exercise. Physiol. Behav. 227, 113165. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterne J. A. C., Savović J., Page M. J., Elbers R. G., Blencowe N. S., Boutron I., et al. (2019). RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. Bmj 366, l4898. doi:10.1136/bmj.l4898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taheri M., Arabameri E. (2012). The effect of sleep deprivation on choice Reaction time and anaerobic power of college student athletes. Asian J. Sports Med. 3 (1), 15–20. doi:10.5812/asjsm.34719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderlinden J., Boen F., van Uffelen J. G. Z. (2020). Effects of physical activity programs on sleep outcomes in older adults: a systematic review. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 17 (1), 11. doi:10.1186/s12966-020-0913-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VanHelder T., Symons J. D., Radomski M. W. (1993). Effects of sleep deprivation and exercise on glucose tolerance. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 64 (6), 487–492.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Vardar S. A., Oztürk L., Kurt C., Bulut E., Sut N., Vardar E. (2007). Sleep deprivation induced anxiety and anaerobic performance. J. Sports Sci. Med. 6 (4), 532–537.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Vitale J. A., Bonato M., Petrucci L., Zucca G., La Torre A., Banfi G. (2021). Acute sleep restriction affects sport-specific but not athletic performance in Junior tennis players. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 16 (8), 1154–1159–1159. doi:10.1123/ijspp.2020-0390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vondra K., Brodan V., Bass A., Kuhn E., Teisinger J., Andĕl M., et al. (1981). Effects of sleep deprivation on the activity of selected metabolic enzymes in skeletal muscle. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 47 (1), 41–46. doi:10.1007/bf00422481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang H., Yu K., Yang T., Zeng L., Li J., Dai C., et al. (2021). Altered functional Connectivity in the resting state Neostriatum after complete sleep deprivation: impairment of motor control and regulatory network. Front. Neurosci. 15, 665687. doi:10.3389/fnins.2021.665687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei Y., Kaifang L., Chong G., Chao B., Shaocong Z., Yongming L. (2024). Effects of mental fatigue on athletic performance and its potential mechanisms. J. Xi'an Phys. Educ. Univ. 41 (01), 112–129.

Google Scholar

Weinhandl E. D., Duval S. (2012). Generalization of trim and fill for application in meta-regression. Res. Synth. Methods 3 (1), 51–67. doi:10.1002/jrsm.1042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zerouali Y., Jemel B., Godbout R. (2010). The effects of early and late night partial sleep deprivation on automatic and selective attention: an ERP study. Brain Res. 1308, 87–99. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sleep deprivation, aerobic endurance performance, anaerobic sports performance, fatigue, ratings of perceived exertion

Citation: Kong Y, Yu B, Guan G, Wang Y and He H (2025) Effects of sleep deprivation on sports performance and perceived exertion in athletes and non-athletes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Physiol. 16:1544286. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2025.1544286

Received: 12 December 2024; Accepted: 20 March 2025;
Published: 01 April 2025.

Edited by:

Francis Degache, Université de Lausanne, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Thiago Teixeira Mendes, Federal University of Bahia (UFBA), Brazil
Karuppasamy Govindasamy, Symbiosis International (Deemed University), India
Salma Abedelmalek, University of Hail, Saudi Arabia

Copyright © 2025 Kong, Yu, Guan, Wang and He. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hui He, aGVfaHVpMDQwMkAxMjYuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more