Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 23 November 2022
Sec. Invertebrate Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Invertebrate Neurophysiology - of Currents, Cells, and Circuits View all 15 articles

In the sea slug Melibe leonina the posterior nerves communicate stomach distention to inhibit feeding and modify oral hood movements

Colin Anthony Lee,
Colin Anthony Lee1,2*Winsor Hays WatsonWinsor Hays Watson1
  • 1Department of Biological Sciences, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH, United States
  • 2Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, United States

The sea slug Melibe leonina is an excellent model system for the study of the neural basis of satiation, and previous studies have demonstrated that stomach distention attenuates feeding. Here we expanded on this work by examining the pathway communicating stomach distention to the central nervous system and the effects of distention on motor output. We found that the posterior nerves (PN), which extend posteriorly from the buccal ganglia and innervate the stomach, communicate stomach distention in Melibe. PN lesions led to increased feeding duration and food consumption, and PN activity increased in response to stomach distention. Additionally, the percentage of incomplete feeding movements increased with satiation, and PN stimulation had a similar impact in the nerves that innervate the oral hood. These incomplete movements may be functionally similar to the egestive, food rejecting motions seen in other gastropods and enable Melibe to remain responsive to food, yet adjust their behavior as they become satiated. Such flexibility would not be possible if the entire feeding network were inhibited.

Introduction

In recent years the sea slug Melibe leonina has emerged as a promising species in which to study the neuronal regulation of behavioral state. It feeds using rhythmic movements of its oral hood to capture both planktonic prey (Watson and Trimarchi, 1992) and organisms on the kelp and sea grass on which it tends to reside (Watson et al., 2021), and it can feed while either stationary or crawling. Both its rhythmic feeding movements (Watson and Trimarchi, 1992) and locomotor activity (Newcomb et al., 2014) can be easily quantified, and the expression of these movements changes with time of day (Newcomb et al., 2014) or hunger state (Lee and Watson, 2016). Like all heterobranch gastropods, Melibe has a simple nervous system with a small number of large, individually identifiable neurons. However, unlike most heterobranchs Melibe lacks a buccal mass and complicated chewing mechanics, leading to an exceptionally simple (∼30 neurons) buccal ganglion (Trimarchi and Watson, 1992). Finally, it is an organism about which we have transcriptomic (Cook et al., 2018) and peptidomic (Lee et al., 2021) information, and whose daily rhythms have been thoroughly documented (Newcomb et al., 2014).

Both time of day and hunger state influence behavioral arousal in Melibe, but our understanding of the neuronal mechanisms by which hunger does so remains limited. As a nocturnal animal, Melibe shows increased arousal during the night, with increased feeding and locomotion (Newcomb et al., 2014), and greater responsiveness to excitatory neurotransmitters (Watson et al., 2020). Its expression of key clock genes also peaks during this active period (Duback et al., 2018), consistent with what is seen across the metazoa (von Schantz and Archer, 2003; Nitabach and Taghert, 2008). A major regulator of hunger state in Melibe is stomach distention: as the volume in the stomach increases individuals become less likely to feed and move, until they are eventually satiated, and cease feeding altogether (Lee and Watson, 2016). This pattern has also been observed in other gastropods (e.g. Pleurobranchaea californica (Gillette et al., 2000), Lymnaea stagnalis (Dyakonova et al., 2015), and Aplysia californica (Kupfermann, 1974)) and other invertebrates (e.g. the fly Phormia meigen (Green, 1964) and the leech Hirudo medicinalis (Lent and Dickinson, 1987)). Stomach distention is conveyed by nerves that innervate the esophagus and stomach (Dethier and Gelperin, 1967; Kupfermann, 1974; Susswein and Kupfermann, 1975; Croll et al., 1987), and in several gastropods reductions in the motivation to feed are accompanied by reconfiguration of feeding networks to produce egestive, food rejecting motions (London and Gillette, 1986; Jing et al., 2007; Crossley et al., 2018). In Melibe, the most likely pathway for this signaling is via the posterior nerves (PN) of the buccal ganglia, which emanate from the buccal ganglia and innervate the esophagus and stomach (Figure 1 (Trimarchi and Watson, 1992)). The primary goal of this study was to test this hypothesis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The Melibe buccal ganglion. (A) Picture of the buccal ganglion on the esophagus. A portion of the pedal ganglion, which is part of the fused cerebral-pleural-pedal complex, is visible in the top left. (B) Diagram of the ganglion and its nerves. The PN leaves the BG posteriorly and runs towards the stomach and intestines. In neurophysiological experiments examining the effect of stomach distention on activity, recordings were made from the severed distal end of the PN (conveying afferent signals from the stomach).

In this study we demonstrated that PN activity changes in response to stomach distention, and that PN signaling is necessary for normal satiation of feeding behavior. Additionally, we found that PN signaling attenuates feeding not merely by reducing the output from the feeding network, but by shortening feeding movements, so they do not cause consumption of food. These data demonstrate that the PNs are important regulators of the motivation to feed in Melibe.

Methods

Animals

Adult Melibe leonina (21.3 ± 2.69 g) were acquired from eelgrass beds near the University of Washington’s Friday Harbor Laboratories (FHL) in the Puget Sound, WA and in Monterey Bay, California, and either maintained at FHL or shipped to the University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH. The subjects used at FHL were housed in sea tables with flow through seawater, while those used at the University of New Hampshire were maintained in an aquarium with recirculating seawater, at approximately 13°C, until needed for experiments.

Feeding assays

To determine the effect of PN signaling on the motivation to feed, experiments were performed in which feeding rate over time was assessed for both Melibe with PN lesions (described in the next section) and control animals. Individual Melibe were placed in circular buckets (30 cm diameter) located within a larger tank of aerated seawater. The buckets had small mesh “windows” that allowed water exchange with the larger tank but prevented food from escaping. Tanks were located in a 13°C cold room that was kept on a 24-h light/dark cycle, with 10–14 h of light per day, depending on the season. After subjects acclimated to the buckets for 24 h, sufficient newly hatched Artemia spp. (brine shrimp) were added to the bucket to yield a density of approximately 3,000 Artemia/L. All trials began between 10 and 11 a.m. and Melibe were allowed to feed undisturbed for approximately 24 h. A black and white low light sensitive camera suspended directly above the buckets captured feeding activity, and recordings were obtained from approximately 1 hour before Artemia addition to 24 h post-addition. Camera outputs were digitized, time-stamped, and recorded on a Macintosh computer using the video capture software Gawker, which took one picture every second and streamed the images together at a rate of ten frames per second (Supplemental Video S1).

During the subsequent video analysis, the number of feeding motions performed per minute was counted for the entire experiment. Melibe feeds using rhythmic movements termed oral hood closures (OHCs), which consist of an oral hood closing phase (in which the hood comes forward and closes, capturing a bolus of water) and a tilt and squeeze swallowing phase (in which the closed hood is tilted back and the water is squeezed out through tentacles that are used to capture prey); see Watson and Trimarchi (Watson and Trimarchi, 1992) for a complete description of these phases. Subjects routinely produced incomplete feeding movements, performing only the oral hood closing phase, and likely not ingesting captured prey (Trimarchi and Watson, 1992). For comparisons between PN lesioned and control individuals these incomplete motions were ignored, and only the complete, ingestive OHCs were quantified. In a separate analysis of control individuals, both complete (Supplemental Video S2) and incomplete OHCs (Supplemental Video S3) were quantified.

PN lesions

Melibe were pinned out dorsally on a Sylgard-coated dish, with a single pin through the foot and two through the oral hood, and viewed under a dissecting microscope. A single incision was made in the skin directly above the central nervous system (CNS), exposing the fused cerebral, pedal, and pleural ganglia, the buccal ganglia, and PNs. Both PNs were then either cut with scissors or torn with tweezers. Incisions were sewn up with sterile sutures, and the subjects were given at least 5 days to recover. After this recovery period lesioned animals were fed as described in the previous section, and their feeding activity was compared to that of control animals. Half of the controls received sham operations, in which incisions were made and sutured, but PNs were left intact. There was no difference in the feeding activity between sham-operated (n = 4) and unoperated controls (n = 8). The baseline feeding rate for controls was 0.28 ± 0.12 OHC/min and 0.66 ± 0.22 OHC/min for sham-operated Melibe (T (10) = 1.90, p = 0.087). The maximum feeding rate was 2.15 ± 0.59 OHC/min for unoperated animals vs. 2.75 ± 0.17 OHC/min for those that were sham-operated (T (10) = 0.76, p = 0.47).

Quantification of food consumed during a satiating meal

Six lesioned and six control Melibe were fed to satiation with Artemia. After their feeding rate had returned to baseline, each individual’s stomach was removed, and the number of Artemia in it was estimated. For estimates, the contents of the stomach were emptied into a known volume of seawater and stirred thoroughly to produce a homogenous concentration of Artemia. Three separate 1 ml aliquots were drawn up, and the number of Artemia in each aliquot was counted separately by two different people. Numbers were averaged between the measurements and used to estimate the total number of Artemia in the larger sample.

Neurophysiology

Extracellular neurophysiological recordings were carried out using both isolated CNS and semi-intact preparations. The semi-intact preparation was used to assess the effects of stomach distention on nervous system activity. For this preparation, the intact mouth, esophagus, stomach, intestine, and buccal ganglia were removed, pinned out in a Sylgard-coated dish, and continuously perfused with 13°C seawater. A cannula was inserted through the mouth and esophagus into the stomach, and a thread was used to tighten the junction of the mouth and esophagus to the cannula. Another thread was used to close the junction between the stomach and the intestine (Figure 2). Seawater was injected through the cannula to distend the stomach to one of four different diameters (1/4, 1/2, 3/4, maximum), and recordings were obtained from the PN (n = 6). To record the action potentials traveling via the PN from the stomach to the buccal ganglia, the nerve was severed near the connection to the buccal ganglion and recordings were obtained from the cut distal end.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. A Melibe mouth, esophagus and stomach preparation distended to the following degrees: (A) empty, (B) ¼ full, (C) ½ full, (D) ¾ full, (E) fully distended. Note the cannula held in place by a thread in the bottom right corner of image A, and the tied-off junctions between the stomach and intestines in all the images.

To assess the effects of PN activity on feeding motor output, extracellular recordings were obtained from the oral hood nerves (OHN) of the cerebral ganglion during sustained PN stimulation. For these recordings, the CNS was cut almost entirely away from the body, with only a piece of the esophagus retained to ensure that the buccal ganglia were not lost. The esophageal portion was pinned in a Sylgard-lined dish, and suction electrodes were attached to the PN and one to three OHNs. Recordings were carried out for several hours, alternating between ∼30 min without PN stimulation and 20 min with PN stimulation. The PN was stimulated at 10 Hz 5 V. Fictive feeding can be identified in isolated Melibe brains by recording OHN bursts that have the same frequency as the oral hood movements of intact animals. Bursts identified using the following criteria: 1) at least 10 s in duration; 2) spiking at a rate of at least 5 Hz and at a rate that was at least twice that of the baseline spiking rate. For activity to be defined as a spike, it needed to be at least twice the voltage of the background noise.

Recordings were made with suction electrodes made of either pulled borosilicate capillaries or polyethylene tubing. Signals were amplified and filtered with an AM Systems Microelectrode AC Amplifier (AM-systems, Sequim, WA), digitized with an AD Instruments Powerlab 4/30 (AD Instruments, Dunedin, New Zealand), and displayed with Labchart software (AD Instruments, Dunedin, New Zealand). Stimuli were triggered using Labchart’s Stimulator function. Changes in firing rate over time were determined by counting number of spikes/min using Labchart software.

Statistics

For the experiment comparing feeding rate over time in control and lesioned individuals, data were normalized by subtracting the baseline rate from the rate at a given time point. In all graphs, data are shown as the mean ± the standard error of means. All statistical tests were performed with the software Prism (Graphpad, Boston, MA). A two-way repeated measures ANOVA with Sidak post-test was used to compare feeding rate over time between control and lesioned individuals. One-way repeated measures ANOVAs with Tukey post-tests were used to assess changes in complete vs. incomplete OHCs over time and PN responses to stomach distention. Changes in OHN activity were assessed using paired t-tests, and differences in the number of Artemia consumed between lesioned and control Melibe were assessed with unpaired t-tests.

Results

Posterior nerve lesions led to increases in feeding behavior

PN lesioned Melibe (n = 6) fed more than controls (n = 12) (Figure 3). The two groups displayed statistically significant differences in normalized feeding rate by both time (F (5,80) = 6.96, p < 0.001) and treatment group (F (1,16) = 9.56, p = 0.0070), although the interaction between the two variables was not significantly different (F (5,80) = 0.13, p = 0.99, two-way repeated measures ANOVA). Lesioned Melibe fed at a significantly greater rate in the third hour of their feeding bout (1.94 ± 0.29 OHC/min vs. 0.82 ± 0.24 OHC/min; p = 0.042, Sidak’s Multiple Comparison Test). However, there was no significant difference in their maximum feeding rates (lesioned: 2.84 ± 0.30 OHC/min; controls: 1.94 ± 0.40 OHC/min; T (16) = 1.64, p = 0.12, unpaired t-test). Thus, PN lesions did not increase the initial or peak feeding intensity, but prolonged the duration of food arousal.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Normalized feeding rate over time for both control Melibe and individuals with PN lesions. For the graph rates were calculated in 10-min bins for the first hour, and for every hour thereafter; for statistical analysis rates were calculated for every hour. In the third hour the feeding rate was significantly elevated for lesioned individuals compared to controls (indicated by star).

To verify that the lesioned Melibe consumed more prey, the stomachs of six lesioned and six control Melibe were removed after they had ceased feeding (8.9 h for lesioned individuals, 4.1 h for controls), and the Artemia in the stomachs were counted (Figure 4). Lesioned Melibe consumed significantly more Artemia (7323 ± 3278 Artemia for lesioned and 1358 ± 590 Artemia for controls; p = 0.04, t-test).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Assessment of stomach contents following feeding to satiation. Melibe with posterior nerve lesions consumed significantly more Artemia than controls. Artemia were less than 24 h old, and at this age are typically 500 μm in length (Wurstbaugh and Maciej Gliwicz, 2001).

The frequency of complete, but not incomplete, OHCs decreased over time

Like many gastropods, Melibe can produce multiple feeding related movements, performing both complete OHCs, which lead to food ingestion, and incomplete ones, which likely do not lead to ingestion (Watson and Trimarchi, 1992; Wurstbaugh and Maciej Gliwicz, 2001; Crossley et al., 2018). To determine if the proportion of complete OHCs changes over the course of a feeding bout, we recorded incomplete vs. complete motions for a group of intact Melibe for 6 h (Figure 5, n = 8). While the rate of incomplete motions changed minimally over the course of the feeding period, the rate of complete motions dropped dramatically throughout the feeding bout, leading to a significantly increased ratio of incomplete motions for the final 2 hours of feeding compared to the first 3 h (F (6,42) = 5.81, p < 0.001, p < 0.05 for each comparison; one-way repeated measures ANOVA with Tukey post-test).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Changes in the types of OH movements during a feeding bout. The number of incomplete feeding motions remained constant over the course of a feeding bout, whereas the number of complete ones decreased after 3 hours. The frequency of incomplete movements was significantly greater at 6 h than for the first 3 hours.

Stomach distention increases PN activity

To determine if the PNs communicate information about stomach fullness to the CNS, extracellular recordings were obtained from the PN while the stomach was artificially inflated with seawater. Although there was activity in the PNs even when the stomach was empty, spike frequency changed in response to distention (Figures 6A, C) and was significantly increased compared to baseline rates 10–20 s after the start of distension (n = 5; F (11,44) = 2.39, p < 0.021; one-way repeated measures ANOVA with Tukey post-test). After the stomach was deflated PN activity showed a trend toward a decrease in firing rate (Figures 6B, D), although the decrease was not statistically significant (n = 6; F (11,55) = 1.853, p = 0.067; one-way repeated measures ANOVA).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Response of the Melibe PN to stomach distention. (A) Representative neurophysiological recording showing the increase in spiking starting at the onset of distention (arrow). (B) Representative decrease in activity when the stomach was deflated (arrow). (C) Average spike frequency during the onset of distension, firing was significantly elevated compared to baseline at 10–20 s. (D) Average spike frequency before and after deflation of the stomach.

To assess the effects of different levels of prolonged stomach distention on PN nerve activity we obtained PN recordings while the stomach was inflated to ¼, ½, ¾, and 100% of its maximum for at least 30 min at each distention level (Figure 7). During sustained distention multiple units burst rhythmically, and the overall firing rate increased (n = 6; F (4,25) = 5.98, p = 0.0025, one-way repeated measures ANOVA with Tukey post-test). Complete distention of the stomach caused a significant increase in posterior nerve firing compared to baseline, ½, and ¾ distention (p < 0.05 for each comparison) suggesting that partial stomach distention has a minimal impact on feeding behavior in Melibe.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 7. The relationship between PN firing rate and different levels of stomach distention. Maximal distention significantly increased firing compared to baseline, ¼, and ¾ distention.

PN signaling alters feeding-related activity in the CNS

Finally, we examined the effect of PN stimulation on patterns of OHN activity. These nerves emerge from the cerebral ganglion, cause contraction of the oral hood, the first phase of feeding behavior (Watson and Trimarchi, 1992), and produce spontaneous rhythmic bursts in isolated perparations (Figure 8A. 1.19 ± 0.21 OHCs/min, 21.70 ± 2.51 s duration). Long term recordings were performed during which we alternated between approximately 30 min without stimulation and 20 min with PN stimulation, and we found that stimulation significantly changed both the frequency (n = 4; F (2,6) = 60.24, p < 0.001, one-way repeated measures ANOVA with Tukey post-test) and duration of bursts F (2,6) = 9.74, p = 0.013, one-way repeated measures ANOVA with Tukey post-test). Specifically, stimulation of the PNs (Figure 8B) significantly reduced both the rate (0.56 ± 0.21 OHCs/min, 47.1% of baseline; p < 0.001) and duration (17.82 ± 2.12 s duration 82.1% of baseline; p = 0.049) of these bursts, with returns to baseline after stimulation ceased (Figure 8C; 1.08 ± 0.24 bursts/min, 90.8% of baseline; p = 0.98; 23.58 ± 2.44 s duration, 108.6% of baseline; p = 0.49). The shorter duration bursts that occurred during PN stimulation likely correspond to incomplete OHCs.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 8. Recordings from an OHN showing activity (A) before, (B) during, and (C) after PN stimulation. Note the PN stimulation led to distinct bursting, which likely represents a change in the movements of the oral hood.

Discussion

This study demonstrates a clear role for the PNs in the regulation of Melibe feeding behavior. Both behavioral and neurophysiological data indicate that stomach distention, a key factor in Melibe satiation (Lee and Watson, 2016), is communicated to the CNS by the PNs. The data also indicate that this signaling leads to three changes in feeding related activity: 1) a decrease in the overall number of feeding movements; 2) an increase in the ratio of incomplete, non-ingestive movements to normal complete movements; 3) a decrease in the rhythmic movements of the esophagus associated with swallowing. This suggests that satiation in Melibe is more complex than simple inhibition of the feeding network.

The PNs facilitate the inhibition of feeding by stomach distention

Loss of PN signaling, after they were lesioned, dramatically prolonged the duration of feeding-related arousal in Melibe. Lesions led to increases in both the duration of feeding bouts and the amount of food consumed. These results were very similar to those previously obtained when Melibe stomachs were lesioned, so food could not distend the stomach (Lee and Watson, 2016). The initial intensity of feeding did not increase in PN lesioned individuals, suggesting that they were not hungrier or experiencing greater food-induced arousal at the start of a meal. Rather, they simply took longer to satiate. This change did not appear to be due to behavioral deficits caused by the lesions, as individuals were fully healed from the surgery when experiments were performed, were still capable of consuming food, and consumed more than sham operated individuals. Additionally, in lesioned individuals, we observed defecation and transport of gut contents to the diverticuli that run throughout the body, indicating that the difference in final stomach volume was not a product of changes in the processing of stomach contents. Similar results have been obtained during studies involving the posterior stomatogastric nerve in Pleurobranchaea (Croll et al., 1987) and the esophageal nerve in Aplysia (Jing et al., 2007), suggesting a common function for the nerves that innervate the gut in gastropods.

Activity in the PNs signals the CNS that the stomach is filling with prey, and the information transmitted is proportional to the stimulus. Increases in stomach fullness cause immediate increases in PN activity, and deflation of the stomach causes an immediate cessation of activity. Additionally, the firing rate from the PNs during sustained (>30 min) distention roughly correlates with the degree of distention. However, a significant increase in the PN firing rate does not take place until the stomach is 100% distended, which suggests that Melibe will continue to feed until the stomach fills to this degree.

Satiation in Melibe is more complex than simple inhibition of feeding circuitry

Although satiation led to overall reductions in food-induced arousal, the feeding network still responded to food, but the response changed. Freely behaving Melibe continued to perform OHCs throughout a feeding bout, but the number of complete, ingestive ones decreased as their stomachs filled, whereas the incomplete, ineffective ones, remained unchanged. Similarly, the isolated CNS continued to produce rhythmic bursts during PN stimulation, but the duration of these bursts decreased. Therefore, as they start to become satiated, their feeding efforts become less effective and, while they do not start to egest prey, they also do not consume prey.

Other gastropods show a similar pattern as they satiate, producing egestive movements (Cropper et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2019), which are used to reject food in multiple contexts (e.g. (McManus et al., 2019)). It is unclear if these are homologous to Melibe’s incomplete OHCS, but they display functional similarities, as both are food-evoked movements of the feeding apparatus that do not result in consumption. Aplysia (Jing et al., 2007) and Lymnaea (Crossley et al., 2018) switch from ingestion to egestion as they satiate, and satiated Pleurobranchaea perform aversive behaviors in response to appetitive stimuli (Gillette et al., 2000). Mechanistic similarities also exist, as in Aplysia the shift to egestion is driven by the esophageal nerve, which, like the Melibe PN, innervates the gut and esophagus (Chiel et al., 1986). Studies in these species have also identified individual neurons involved in this shift. In both Aplysia (Jing et al., 2007) and Lymnaea (Crossley et al., 2018) an egestion specific interneuron becomes active, and in satiated Aplysia the multifunctional CBI2 interneuron, which can elicit both ingestive and egestive behaviors (Jing and Weiss, 2001), drives egestion. In Pleurobranchaea food avoidance conditioning biases individuals towards egestion, and this conditioning is accompanied by increases in the excitability of retraction phase neurons (London and Gillette, 1986), which in normal feeding behavior inhibit the protraction phase interneurons (London and Gillette, 1984). Similarly, in the land snail Limax marginatus feeding network oscillations increase following food avoidance conditioning (Kimura et al., 1998; Inoue et al., 2006). Thus, as gastropods satiate, they respond to palatable food in the same way they respond to unpalatable or toxic stimuli.

Why does satiation not merely manifest as simple inhibition of the feeding network, with no response of any type to food? Such inhibition can occur with defensive behaviors. In Lymnaea (Alania et al., 2004) and the nudibranch Clione limacina (Alania et al., 1999) the PlB neuron, which fires during defensive withdrawal, globally shuts down the feeding network, inhibiting neurons that act throughout the network, including the retraction phase ones that drive egestion. The answer likely lies in the complex and variable nature of the motivation to feed. Hunger state is not binary, but rather lies on a continuum (Susswein et al., 1976). On the hungry end of the continuum individuals feed robustly and primarily produce ingestive motions, whereas on the satiated end feeding responses are reduced and most movements are egestive or incomplete. Shifts away from ingestion allow individuals to flexibly move along this continuum. As they satiate, the number of non-ingestive movements decreases, but they can temporarily increase their feeding rate if food abundance or quality increases, in the same way that hungry individuals can temporarily perform egestive or incomplete feeding movements in response to an undesired food item.

This may be particularly important for Melibe, which filter feeds on patchy sources of planktonic prey, and which needs to be able to adaptively modify its feeding behavior in response to changes in food availability (Watson and Trimarchi, 1992). For example, a given bolus of seawater may hold toxic or unpalatable prey, so temporary expression of incomplete feeding motions allows individuals to clear undesirable material without ending a feeding bout. With inhibition as seen from the PlB neurons, feeding is inflexibly inhibited, and the feeding network cannot respond to changes in environmental condition. The existence of these separate mechanisms for feeding inhibition strongly indicates the utility of preserving some responsiveness within the feeding network, even with satiation.

How do stomach distention and circadian clocks interact to regulate the motivation to feed?

In Melibe, which are nocturnal animals, the onset of darkness initiates locomotion and bouts of feeding (Newcomb et al., 2014). This conflicts with satiation, which inhibits feeding behavior and has been shown in other species to inhibit overall activity. Studies in Melibe and other gastropods suggest that serotonin (5-HT) is involved in these processes. In gastropods it acts via the metacerebral cells and other 5-HT-ergic cerebral neurons to strongly potentiate feeding (Weiss et al., 1975; Weiss and Kupfermann, 1976; Gillette and Davis, 1977; Granzow and Kater, 1977; McCrohan and Benjamin, 1980; Delaney and Gelperin, 1990; Kobatake et al., 1992), and in Pleurobranchaea the 5-HT content of the metacerebral cells is reduced following satiation (Hatcher et al., 2008). Additionally, synaptically connected 5-HT-ergic neurons are embedded in each of the major locomotor networks (Gillette, 2006; Dyakonova et al., 2015), and act to regulate overall behavioral arousal; inhibition of 5-HT neurons in one circuit correlates with inhibition in another (Dyakonova et al., 2015). 5-HT mediates the effects of light in animals across the Metazoa (Morin, 1999; Itoh and Igarashi, 2000; Saifullah and Tomioka, 2002), including Aplysia (Koumenis and Eskin, 1992) and the marine snail Bulla gouldiana (Whitmore and Block, 1996), and is generally more abundant during an animal’s active period (Itoh and Igarashi, 2000). In Melibe, 5-HT-ergic processes project to the buccal ganglion via the cerebral-buccal connective (Newcomb et al., 2006).

Overall, hunger state, time of day, and the abundance of food act together to modulate the expression of feeding (Figure 9). During the day, 5-HT levels are likely lower, which combines with input from the circadian clock and inhibition from light, to reduce activity and responsiveness to the presence of prey in the water column. When night falls, 5-HT levels rise, circadian clock input changes, and it is dark, which leads to increased activity, and responsiveness to the same food-related stimuli that are present in the day. However, when individuals consume a satiating meal, even though they might still be in an active state, feeding is diminished.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 9. Model showing how various inputs might modulate Melibe feeding, locomotion, and overall arousal. Open circles are excitatory, closed circles are inhibitory. During the day, which is their quiescent period, 5-HT levels are reduced and light both inhibits active behaviors and reduces the expression of several clock genes. With the onset of darkness, clock genes are upregulated and 5-HT levels rise, leading to increased feeding, locomotion, and overall arousal. 5-HT may also have effects on clock gene expression (gray circle), perhaps serving to phase advance the circadian clock. When food is ingested it causes the stomach to distend, which is communicated to the NS by the PNs that innervate the stomach. This input reduces the motivation to feed, 5-HT levels, and locomotion. In the sea slug Melibe leonina the posterior nerves communicate stomach distention to inhibit feeding and modify oral hood movements.

Conclusion and future directions

This study demonstrates that stomach distension reduces the expression of rhythmic feeding behavior in Melibe and the state of distension is communicated to the NS via the posterior nerve. Satiating signals likely act alongside circadian clock cues to regulate overall behavioral arousal, and it is likely that 5-HT plays an important role. Further studies might eventually identify the neural circuits for feeding and swallowing and thus make it possible to determine how PN activity modifies their activity so there is a tendency for less feeding and more incomplete, likely egestive, movements. Additionally, although clear similarities in feeding behavior can be seen across gastropods, there is diversity in prey choice, feeding apparatus morphology, feeding mechanics, and gut morphology. Future work may also examine if these traits lead to interspecific variation in mechanisms of satiation.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

CL contributed to project and experimental design, data collection and analysis, and manuscript preparation. WW contributed to project and experimental design, and manuscript preparation.

Funding

This work was supported by the New Hampshire- INBRE through an Institutional Development Award (IDeA) from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health [grant number P20GM103506 to Jim Newcomb and WW]. CL was supported by the Conchologists of America through the grants to malacology program the University of New Hampshire Summer Teaching Assistant Fellowship Program, and the Friday Harbor Labs Summer Graduate Fellowship Program.

Acknowledgments

We thank Jim Newcomb, Leslie Curren and Megan Mahoney for their advice on this project, and Trevor Fay and Reyn Yoshioka for providing us with animals. Additionally, we thank Corey Collins for his help with video analysis and Maureen Raubach for her help with Artemia counts.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling editor JN declared a past co-authorship with the authors.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors, and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphys.2022.1047106/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

5-HT, Serotonin; CNS, Central Nervous System; OHC, Oral Hood Closure; OHN, Oral Hood Nerve; PN, Posterior Nerve.

References

Alania M. A., Panchin Y. V., Sakharov D. A. (1999). Pleural-buccal interneurons in the pteropod mollusc Clione limacina. J. Comp. Physiology A Sens. Neural Behav. Physiology 185, 267–275. doi:10.1007/s003590050386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alania M., Sakharov D. A., Elliott C. J. H. (2004). Multilevel inhibition of feeding by a peptidergic pleural interneuron in the mollusc Lymnaea stagnalis. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 190, 379–390. doi:10.1007/s00359-004-0503-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiel H. J., Weiss K. R., Kupfermann I. (1986). An identified histaminergic neuron modulates feeding motor circuitry in Aplysia. J. Neurosci. 6, 2427–2450. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.06-08-02427.1986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook G. M., Gruen A. E., Morris J., Pankey M. S., Senatore A., Katz P. S., et al. (2018). Sequences of circadian clock proteins in the nudibranch molluscs Hermissenda crassicornis, Melibe leonina, and Tritonia diomedea. Biol. Bull. 234, 207–218. doi:10.1086/698467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croll R. P., Albuquerque T., Fitzpatrick L. (1987). Hyperphagia resulting from gut denervation in the sea slug, Pleurobranchaea. Behav. Neural Biol. 47, 212–218. doi:10.1016/s0163-1047(87)90341-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cropper E. C., Evans C. G., Hurwitz I., Jing J., Proekt A., Romero A., et al. (2004). Feeding neural networks in the mollusc Aplysia. Neurosignals. 13, 70–86. doi:10.1159/000076159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crossley M., Staras K., Kemenes G. (2018). A central control circuit for encoding perceived food value. Sci. Adv. 4, eaau9180. doi:10.1126/sciadv.aau9180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delaney K., Gelperin A. (1990). Cerebral interneurons controlling fictive feeding in Limax maximus. J. Comp. Physiol. A 166, 311–326. doi:10.1007/bf00204805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dethier V. G., Gelperin A. (1967). Hyperphagia in the blowfly. J. Exp. Biol. 47, 191–200. doi:10.1242/jeb.47.1.191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duback V. E., Pankey M. S., Thomas R. I., Huyck T. L., Mbarani I. M., Bernier K. R., et al. (2018). Localization and expression of putative circadian clock transcripts in the brain of the nudibranch Melibe leonina. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 223, 52–59. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2018.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyakonova V. E., Hernadi L., Ito E., Dyakonova T. L., Chistopolsky I. A., Zakharov I. S., et al. (2015). The activity of isolated neurons and the modulatory state of an isolated nervous system represent a recent behavioural state. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 1151–1158. doi:10.1242/jeb.111930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillette R., Davis W. J. (1977). The role of the metacerebral giant neuron in the feeding behavior ofPleurobranchaea. J. Comp. Physiol. 116, 129–159. doi:10.1007/bf00605400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillette R. (2006). Evolution and function in serotonergic systems. Integr. Comp. Biol. 46, 838–846. doi:10.1093/icb/icl024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillette R., Huang R. C., Hatcher N., Moroz L. L. (2000). Cost-benefit analysis potential in feeding behavior of a predatory snail by integration of hunger, taste, and pain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97, 3585–3590. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.7.3585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Granzow B., Kater S. B. (1977). Identified higher-order neurons controlling the feeding motor program of Helisoma. Neuroscience 2, 1049–1063. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(77)90128-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green G. W. (1964). The control of spontaneous locomotor activity in Phormia regina Meigen—II. Experiments to determine the mechanism involved. J. Insect Physiology 10, 727–752. doi:10.1016/0022-1910(64)90055-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatcher N. G., Zhang X., Stuart J. N., Moroz L. L., Sweedler J. V., Gillette R. (2008). 5–HT and 5–HT-SO 4 , but not tryptophan or 5-HIAA levels in single feeding neurons track animal hunger state. J. Neurochem. 104, 1358–1363. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.05084.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inoue T., Murakami M., Watanabe S., Inokuma Y., Kirino Y. (2006). In vitro odor-aversion conditioning in a terrestrial mollusk. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 3898–3903. doi:10.1152/jn.00853.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itoh M. Y., Igarashi J. (2000). Circadian rhythm of serotonin levels in planarians. Neuroreport 11, 473–476. doi:10.1097/00001756-200002280-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jing J., Vilim F. S., Horn C. C., Alexeeva V., Hatcher N. G., Sasaki K., et al. (2007). From hunger to satiety: Reconfiguration of a feeding network by Aplysia neuropeptide Y. J. Neurosci. 27, 3490–3502. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0334-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jing J., Weiss K. R. (2001). Neural mechanisms of motor program switching in Aplysia. J. Neurosci. 21, 7349–7362. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.21-18-07349.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimura T., Toda S., Sekiguchi T., Kirino Y. (1998). Behavioral modulation induced by food odor aversive conditioning and its influence on the olfactory responses of an oscillatory brain network in the slug Limax marginatus. Learn. Mem. 4, 365–375. doi:10.1101/lm.4.5.365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobatake E., Kawahara S., Yano M., Shimizu H. (1992). Control of feeding rhythm in the terrestrial slug Incilaria-bilineata. Neurosci. Res. 13, 257–265. doi:10.1016/0168-0102(92)90038-e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koumenis C., Eskin A. (1992). The hunt for mechanisms of circadian timing in the eye of Aplysia. Chronobiol. Int. 9, 201–221. doi:10.3109/07420529209064530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kupfermann I. (1974). Feeding behavior in Aplysia: A simple system for the study of motivation. Behav. Biol. 10, 1–26. doi:10.1016/s0091-6773(74)91644-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee C. A., Watson W. H. (2016). The influence of stomach distention on feeding in the nudibranch mollusk Melibe leonina. Mar. Freshw. Behav. Physiology 49, 277–290. doi:10.1080/10236244.2016.1192305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee C. L. A., Romanova E. V., Southey B. R., Gillette R., Sweedler J. V. (2021). Comparative analysis of neuropeptides in homologous interneurons and prohormone annotation in nudipleuran sea slugs. Front. Physiol. 12, 809529. doi:10.3389/fphys.2021.809529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent C. M., Dickinson M. H. (1987). On the termination of ingestive behaviour by the medicinal leech. J. Exp. Biol. 131, 1–15. doi:10.1242/jeb.131.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

London J. A., Gillette R. (1984). Functional roles and circuitry in an inhibitory pathway to feeding command neurones in Pleurobranchaea. J. Exp. Biol. 113, 423–446. doi:10.1242/jeb.113.1.423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

London J. A., Gillette R. (1986). Mechanism for food avoidance learning in the central pattern generator of feeding behavior of Pleurobranchae californica. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 83, 4058–4062. doi:10.1073/pnas.83.11.4058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrohan C. R., Benjamin P. R. (1980). Patterns of activity and axonal projections of the cerebral giant-cells of the snail, Lymnaea-stagnalis. J. Exp. Biol. 85, 149–168. doi:10.1242/jeb.85.1.149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McManus J. M., Chiel H. J., Susswein A. J. (2019). Successful and unsuccessful attempts to swallow in a reduced Aplysia preparation regulate feeding responses and produce memory at different neural sites. Learn. Mem. 26, 151–165. doi:10.1101/lm.048983.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin L. P. (1999). Serotonin and the regulation of mammalian circadian rhythmicity. Ann. Med. 31, 12–33. doi:10.3109/07853899909019259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newcomb J. M., Fickbohm D. J., Katz P. S. (2006). Comparative mapping of serotonin-immunoreactive neurons in the central nervous systems of nudibranch molluscs. J. Comp. Neurol. 499, 485–505. doi:10.1002/cne.21111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newcomb J. M., Kirouac L. E., Naimie A. A., Bixby K. A., Lee C., Malanga S., et al. (2014). Circadian rhythms of crawling and swimming in the nudibranch mollusc Melibe leonina. Biol. Bull. 227, 263–273. doi:10.1086/BBLv227n3p263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitabach M. N., Taghert P. H. (2008). Organization of the Drosophila circadian control circuit. Curr. Biol. 18, R84–R93. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saifullah A. S. M., Tomioka K. (2002). Serotonin sets the day state in the neurons that control coupling between the optic lobe circadian pacemakers in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J. Exp. Biol. 205, 1305–1314. doi:10.1242/jeb.205.9.1305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Susswein A. J., Kupfermann I. (1975). Bulk as a stimulus for satiation in Aplysia. Behav. Biol. 13, 203–209. doi:10.1016/s0091-6773(75)91903-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Susswein A. J., Kupfermann I., Weiss K. R. (1976). The stimulus control of biting in Aplysia. J. Comp. Physiol. 108, 75–96. doi:10.1007/bf00625442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trimarchi J., Watson W. H. (1992). The role of the Melibe buccal ganglia in feeding-behavior. Mar. Behav. Physiology 19, 195–209. doi:10.1080/10236249209378808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Schantz M., Archer S. N. (2003). Clocks, genes and sleep. J. R. Soc. Med. 96, 486–489. doi:10.1258/jrsm.96.10.486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang Y. Q., Weiss K. R., Cropper E. C. (2019). Network degeneracy and the dynamics of task switching in the feeding circuit in Aplysia. J. Neurosci. 39, 8705–8716. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1454-19.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson W. H., Bourque K. M. F., Sullivan J. R., Miller M., Buell A., Kallins M. G., et al. (2021). The digestive diverticula in the carnivorous nudibranch, Melibe leonina, do not contain photosynthetic symbionts. Integr. Org. Biol. 3, 15. doi:10.1093/iob/obab015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson W. H., Nash A., Lee C., Patz M. D., Newcomb J. M. (2020). The distribution and possible roles of small cardioactive peptide in the nudibranch Melibe leonina. Integr. Org. Biol. 2, obaa016. doi:10.1093/iob/obaa016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson W. H., Trimarchi J. (1992). A quantitative description of Melibe feeding-behavior and its modification by prey density. Mar. Behav. Physiology 19, 183–194. doi:10.1080/10236249209378807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss K. R., Cohen J., Kupfermann I. (1975). Potentiation of muscle contraction: A possible modulatory function of an identified serotonergic cell in Aplysia. Brain Res. 99, 381–386. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(75)90041-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss K. R., Kupfermann I. (1976). Homology of the giant serotonergic neurons (metacerebral cells) in Aplysia and pulmonate molluscs. Brain Res. 117, 33–49. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(76)90554-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitmore D., Block G. D. (1996). Cellular aspects of molluskan biochronometry. Seminars Cell. & Dev. Biol. 7, 781–789. doi:10.1006/scdb.1996.0096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wurstbaugh W. A., Maciej Gliwicz Z. (2001). Limnological control of brine shrimp population dynamics and cyst production in the Great Salt Lake, Utah. Hydrobiologia 466, 119–132. doi:10.1023/a:1014502510903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: gastropod, nudibranch, invertebrate, motivation, arousal, satiation, feeding, behavior

Citation: Lee CA and Watson WH (2022) In the sea slug Melibe leonina the posterior nerves communicate stomach distention to inhibit feeding and modify oral hood movements. Front. Physiol. 13:1047106. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.1047106

Received: 20 September 2022; Accepted: 03 November 2022;
Published: 23 November 2022.

Edited by:

James Newcomb, New England College, United States

Reviewed by:

Akira Sakurai, Georgia State University, United States
Roger P. Croll, Dalhousie University, Canada

Copyright © 2022 Lee and Watson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Colin Anthony Lee, Y2xlZTE3MEBpbGxpbm9pcy5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.