Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 16 April 2021
Sec. Environmental, Aviation and Space Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Time Domains of Hypoxia Adaptation: Evolutionary Insights and Applications, Volume II View all 12 articles

Influence of Exposure at Different Altitudes on the Executive Function of Plateau Soldiers—Evidence From ERPs and Neural Oscillations

\nXin Wei&#x;Xin Wei1Xiaoli Ni
Xiaoli Ni1*Shanguang Zhao&#x;Shanguang Zhao2Aiping Chi
Aiping Chi3*
  • 1Institute of Social Psychology, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Xi'an Jiaotong University, Xi'an, China
  • 2Centre for Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of Malaya, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia
  • 3School of Sports, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China

This study investigates the changes in soldiers' brain executive function at different altitude environments and their relationship with blood oxygen saturation. Stratified sampling was conducted in different altitude 133 active-duty soldiers who were stationed in Weinan (347 m, n = 34), Nyingchi (2,950 m, n = 32), Lhasa (3,860 m, n = 33), and Nagqu (4,890 m, n = 34) for 2 years. The Go/NoGo paradigm with event-related potentials (ERPs) and event-related oscillations (EROs) was used to explore the time and neural oscillation courses of response inhibition. Behavioral results revealed that at the 4,890-m altitude area, the soldiers had the highest false alarm rate, the longest reaction time, and the slowest information transmission rate. The electrophysiological results revealed that NoGo-N2 and N2d decreased with increasing altitude, with significant changes at 3,860 m; the amplitudes of NoGo-P3 and P3d in plateau groups were significantly more negative than the plain and changed significantly at 2,950 m. The results of correlation analysis showed that NoGo-P3 was negatively correlated with altitude (r = −0.358, p = 0.000), positively correlated with SpO2 (r = 0.197, p = 0.041) and information translation rate (ITR) (r = 0.202, p = 0.036). P3d was negatively correlated with altitude (r = −0.276, p = 0.004) and positively correlated with ITR (r = 0.228, p = 0.018). N2d was negatively correlated with ITR (r = 0.204, p = 0.034). The power spectrum analysis of NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 showed that the power of δ and θ bands at the plateau area was significantly lower than the plain area and showed a significant step-by-step decrease; the α-band power increases significantly only in the area of 4,890 m. The effect of chronic hypoxia exposure at different altitudes of the plateau on the response inhibition of soldiers was manifested: 3,860 m was the altitude at which the brain response inhibition function decreased during the conflict monitoring stage, and 2,950 m was the altitude at which it dropped during the response inhibition stage. In addition, the soldier's brain's executive function was closely related to SpO2, and a reduction in SpO2 may lead to a decline in response inhibition.

Introduction

The plateau is in an important strategic military position. Hypobaric and hypoxia are the plateau environment's main characteristics. The brain is the most sensitive organ to hypobaric hypoxia exposure (Heinrich et al., 2019), which can have a significant impact on a variety of cognitive functions, such as long-term exposure that may lead to a decrease in response inhibition (Wang et al., 2014; Davis et al., 2015; Nation et al., 2017).

Neuroimaging studies have proved that high-altitude environment can have significant effects on the human brain. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shown that the activation of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is related to response inhibition (Yan et al., 2010). An analysis of the Go/NoGo task found that a significant neural network, including the right superior middle/inferior frontal gyrus, the right lower parietal lobe, and the medial frontal gyrus, was associated with response inhibition (Buchsbaum et al., 2005). Besides, in the study of chronic hypoxia using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), it was found that a group of subjects born and living at high altitudes had structural changes in the inferior frontal gyrus, middle gyrus, and anterior cingulate cortex (Yan et al., 2011). From altitude on the anterior cingulate cortex and middle frontal gyrus, the response inhibition control-related cortex will be affected, indicating that the plateau's response inhibition control was affected. Response inhibition control is an essential part of cognitive ability and a necessary executive function aspect (Schlaghecken and Eimer, 2006). It focuses on limited cognitive resources on specific target tasks, suppresses some dominant responses, and reduces the impact of habitual behavior or other interference stimuli on target tasks (Palmwood et al., 2017). Then, it improves the efficiency of completing the target task and increases the probability of success. If the individual was affected by the disorder of inhibitory control, there might be behavior out of control and a series of impulsive behaviors. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) reflect the sum of the brain's neuroelectric responses to specific events with temporal resolution on milliseconds' order (Vinet and Zhedanov, 2011). In Go/NoGo paradigms, the subjects are instructed to respond to repetitive Go stimuli and refrain from responding to infrequent NoGo stimuli (Cheng et al., 2019). There are two main electrophysiological components associated with response inhibition: N2 and P3 (Folstein and Van Petten, 2008; Pires et al., 2014; Fogarty et al., 2018).

Although some of the existing studies focus on the ERPs phase locked in the time domain, little research on the non-phase-locked nerve oscillation reflects the energy change in electroencephalogram (EEG) rhythm when the stimulus was locked. The ongoing information processing can reduce or block the brain waves' amplitude in the α and β rhythms, which is called event-related desynchronization (ERD). Studies have found a positive correlation between ERD intensity and cortical excitation (Daly et al., 2018). ERD activity indicates that the brain is in a state of activation or excitement. The α and β rhythms show increased concussion, called event-related synchronization (ERS) (Pfurtscheller et al., 1997). ERS activity indicates that the brain is in an inactive or resting state. Depending on the characteristics of work and the brain's shape, ERD and ERS can be induced by external or internal events and form a specific brain map distribution. Previous studies have confirmed that ERD and ERS phenomena appear in the process of cognitive information processing (Vázquez-Marrufo et al., 2017).

Therefore, is there no direct evidence that long-term exposure to high-altitude weather affects response inhibition control? If so, is there a regular pattern of the effect of different elevations on response inhibition control? A high-altitude environment can also lead to changes in blood oxygen saturation (SpO2) (Brutsaert et al., 2000), and the partial pressure of oxygen decreases with the increase in height. The partial pressure of oxygen in the artery also decreases, leading to a decrease in the artery's SpO2 (Bernardi et al., 2017). However, the relationship between SpO2 and executive brain function at different elevations remains to be studied.

Therefore, in this study, the Go/NoGo paradigm with event-related potentials (ERPs) and event-related oscillations (EROs) was used to investigate the dynamic changes in brain response inhibition and neuron characteristics oscillation at different altitudes, as well as the relationship between physiological indicators and cognitive components. We hypothesized that the effects of varying altitude plateaus on soldiers' inhibitory control and non-attentive tasks show a progressively decreasing trend change with increasing altitude.

Subjects and Methods

Participants

Self-compiled general demographic questionnaire was used to register the subject's demographic information, including name, gender, birth date, place of origin, place of residence before enlistment, and education level. A total of 133 soldiers, who were stationed in Weinan (n = 34), Nyingchi (n = 32), Lhasa (n = 33), and Nagqu (n = 34) for 2 years, were invited in this study. The mean age of the participants was 22.21 years (SD = 0.27). There were no statistical differences for age, body mass index (BMI), and education level among the four groups. More details for the information can be found in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Information on different altitude areas.

Common inclusion criteria were the following: (1) male, high school education, right-handed (Annett, 1970), normal eye vision or corrected vision, and normal BMI; (2) no head trauma, no physical or mental illness; and (3) no alcohol, drugs, or narcotics within 1 week; and (4) informed consent to participate.

Inclusion criteria for the plateau group (Nyingchi, Lhasa, and Nagqu) were as follows: (1) birthplace was a low-altitude area (<500 m) after stationed in the current altitude area and (2) permanent residence in the current altitude area for 24 months. Exclusion criterion was the experience of going to the plateau before stationing or returning to the plains during stationing. The plain group's inclusion criteria (Weinan) were that the birthplace and stationing place are low altitude (<500 m). Exclusion criterion was an experience of going to the plateau.

The study was conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures were carried out with the adequate understanding and written informed consent of the subjects. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Fourth Military of Medical University.

General Demographic Information Statistics

Measurement of SpO2 and HR

The participants' SpO2 and heart rate (HR) were measured by an oxygen saturation meter (Kehui Covidien, American). The participants were asked to rest for at least 15 min before measuring. The subjects' right index finger was ensured to be dry and clean without accessories before testing. When calculating, the excessive sunlight affecting the fingers' temperature was avoided, and cold was prevented to ensure the fingers' warmth. When the subjects were breathing calmly, the portable fingertip oxygen saturation meter was used to clip the right hand's index finger. The values of SpO2 and HR were read after the reading was stable for at least 10 s.

Stimuli and Procedures

Participants were seated in a comfortable experimental laboratory and exposed to limited sound and appropriate light. The stimuli were presented on a computer screen ~75 cm away from the participant, with a visual angle of 4° × 4°. Visual stimuli included single and double triangles in the gray background, presented on a computer screen (light degree = 60 cd/m2). There were four blocks with 60 Go (double triangles) and 40 NoGo (single triangle) stimuli for each. Before EEG recording, participants performed two practice blocks consisting of 40 Go and NoGo trials to ascertain that their operation was correct. The behavior data such as accuracy rate (AR), false alarm rate (FAR), and reaction time (RT) were collected using E-prime 3.0. Each stimulus was presented for 100 ms, with the mean interstimulus intervals (ISI) being 1,200 ms (randomly between 1,000 and 1,400 ms). During the experiment, participants were instructed to watch the center of the screen, relax, and minimize eye blinks or body movements.

Information transfer rate (ITR) was the standard measurement method of measuring communication and control systems. It refers to the amount of data transmitted per unit of time (Pierce and Epling, 1980). ITR has been widely used in the evaluation of the cognitive attention system. Wolpaw (2002) suggested that ITR be used to evaluate the brain–computer interface's performance. Thus, in this study, ITR was used to assess the effects of cognitive processing rate and hypoxia on soldiers' brains at different altitudes.

ITR is calculated as follows:

ITR={log2N+Plog2P+ (1-P)log21-PN-1 }/T

T is the reaction time (RT), N is the total number of stimuli, and P is the accurate rate (AR).

EEG Acquisition

Brain electric activity was measured from 34 channels using a modified 10–20 system electrode cap (Beijing Yiran Sunny Co. Ltd., www.yiransunny.com.cn). All EEGs were continuously sampled at 1,000 Hz with a left mastoid reference and a forehead ground and referenced offline to an averaged reference derivation by mathematically combining the left and right mastoid. The vertical electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded with electrodes placed above and below the left eye, and the horizontal EOG was recorded with the electrodes placed outboard of both eyes. All electrode impedances were maintained below 5 kΩ. EEG and EOG were amplified using a 0.05–100 Hz bandpass and continuously sampled at 1,000 Hz/channel for offline analysis. EEGs contaminated with artifacts due to amplifier clipping, bursts of electromyographic (EMG) activity, or peak-to-peak deflection exceeding ±75 μV were excluded from trials.

Event-Related Potential Analysis

Raw EEG data preprocessing was performed using EEGLAB software (Version R2013b, San Diego, USA), an open-source toolbox running MATLAB (Version R2013b, MathWorks, United States). The processing process consisted of bilateral mastoid reference and a bandpass filter with a low cut of 0.5 Hz and a high cut at 30 Hz. Eye movement artifacts were corrected using individual independent component analysis (ICA) by removing the corresponding components based on the particular activation curve (Jung et al., 2000; Li et al., 2006). Segments that contained excessive amplitudes (±75) were marked and rejected. The averaged epoch for ERP was 1,000 ms, including a 200-ms prestimuli baseline. The EEGs associated with each stimulus to which subjects correctly responded were overlapped and averaged for each condition.

The peak amplitudes (baseline to peak) of visual N2 and P3 waves were measured in 200–340 ms and 340–430 ms time windows. Furthermore, difference waves were computed by subtracting the average Go-ERP from the average NoGo-ERP for midline electrodes for each condition. As in previous studies (Liu et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2011), the five electrodes were chosen for statistical analysis: Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and Pz. The amplitudes were evaluated by selecting the peak amplitude and computing the mean amplitude over a time window of ±50 ms. The latency onsets were measured with fractional peak latency.

Event-Related Spectral Perturbation Analysis

The EEG data sampling rate was reduced to 500 Hz, and short-time Fourier transform (STFT) in Matlab was used to analyze EEG data's neural concussion. STFT divides the signal into many identical small-time intervals by window function and uses Fourier transform to explore each time interval to determine the time interval frequency and get a series of variation results of signals in the frequency domain. The baseline of (−200 0) ms corrects the oscillation energy value before stimulation. This study focused on the successful inhibition of N2 and P3 components in NoGo stimulation. STFT was used to calculate the power in five frequency bands: δ (1–4 Hz), θ (4–8 Hz), α (8–14 Hz), β (14–30 Hz), and γ (30–45 Hz).

Statistical Analysis

All the statistical analyses were performed by SPSS (23.0; SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used to test the normal distribution of each group of data. Comparisons data from the four altitude groups were made using mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) for normally distributed data. One-way analysis of variance with elevation (Weinan/Nyingchi/Lhasa/Naqu) as a between-subjects factor was conducted for behavior results and physiology indexes. Three-way analysis of variance with elevation (Weinan/Nyingchi/Lhasa/Naqu) as a between-subjects factor and condition (Go, NoGo, NoGo–Go), and electrodeposition (Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and Pz) as a within-subjects factor for ERP results. Three-way analysis of variance with elevation (Weinan/Nyingchi/Lhasa/Naqu) as a between-subjects factor and five frequencies bands (δ, θ, α, β, and γ) and electrodeposition (Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and Pz) as a within-subjects factor for event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) results. A p-value of 0.05 was considered a significant consequence. The Bonferroni-corrected method was performed for post-hoc testing of significant main effects. Effect size in all ANOVA analyses was reported by partial eta-squared (η2), where 0.05 represents a small effect, 0.10 represents a medium effect, and 0.20 represents a large effect (Faul et al., 2007).

Results

Behavior Results and Physiology Measures

The behavioral outcomes can be seen in Table 1. For the Go stimuli, the RT was significantly increased in the Naqu group compared with the plain area (p < 0.01). Meanwhile, the false alarm rate and ITR were markedly enhanced in the Naqu group than in the plain group for the NoGo stimuli (p < 0.01). The false alarm rate, RT, and ITR in the Naqu group increased than that in the Nyingchi or Lhasa groups (p < 0.05 or 0.01) for the NoGo stimuli.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Behavior results of participants for Go and NoGo stimulations.

The SpO2 and HR of soldiers from four areas are shown in Figure 2. The SpO2 percentage was significantly decreased in plateau groups (Nyingchi, Lhasa, and Naqu) compared with the plain group (p < 0.001). On the contrary, the soldiers' HR in the three plateau groups markedly increased than that in the plain group (p < 0.001). In addition, the SpO2 percentage and HR of soldiers in Lhasa and Naqu presented the same changes as Nyingchi (p < 0.001). However, no significant difference was found between Lhasa and Naqu.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. SpO2 and HR index of soldiers at different altitudes.

ERP Results

The Amplitude and Peak Latency of N2

N2 Amplitude

A 4(altitude) × 3(trial type) × 5(electrode distribution) mixed analysis of variance was conducted for the peak amplitudes of N200. The main effect of the electrodes was significant [F(4, 1,455) = 9.352, p < 0.000, MSE = 86.49, η2 = 0.025]. Post-hoc analysis revealed that the amplitude of N2 were larger in electrode Fz showing the minimum amplitude (M = 2.443 μV, SE = 0.273 μV) and electrode CPz showing the maximum amplitude (M = 4.600 μV, SE = 0.259 μV).

The results revealed interaction effects between altitude and trial type [F(6, 1,455) = 15.50, p < 0.000, MSE = 143.38, η2 = 0.05]. Simple main effect analysis found that there was no significant difference under Go condition (p = 0.527), but there was difference under NoGo trial [F(24, 1,455) = 2.923, p = 0.033, MSE = 27.036, η2 = 0.006] and a significant difference in difference wave N2d [F(24, 1,455) = 42.73, p < 0.000, MSE = 394.95, η2 = 0.081]. Further analyses revealed that at NoGo trial, the amplitudes of N2 was positive for Naqu [M = 2.671 μV, 95% CL (1.997, 3.064), p = 0.019] and Lhasa [M = 2.528 μV, 95% CL (2.033, 4.720), p = 0.022] regions than that for the plain [M = 1.557 μV, 95% CL (1.023, 2.09)]. The amplitudes of N2d for the plain group [M = −3.617 μV, 95% CL (−4.239, −3.103)] were negative than that for Lhasa [M = −2.235 μV, 95% CL (−2.730, −1.704), p = 0.017] and Naqu [M = −1.906 μV, 95% CL (−2.44, −1.373), p < 0.000].

There was no interaction effect between altitude and electrodes [F(12, 1,455) = 0.618, p = 0.829, MSE = 5.714, η2 = 0.005], no interaction effect between electrodes and trial type [F(8, 1,455) = 1.588, p = 0.124, MSE = 14.683, η2 = 0.009], or no triple interaction effects among three variables [F(24, 1,455) = 0.276, p = 1.00, MSE = 2.548, η2 = 0.005] (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Total average ERP waveform at different altitudes.

N2 Latency

A 4(altitude) × 3(trial type) × 5(electrode distribution) mixed analysis of variance analysis revealed a significant main effect of electrode on the latencies of N2 [F(24, 1,455) = 3.702, p = 0.040, η2 = 0.049]. The post-hoc test suggested that the latency of N2 at Cz electrode was significantly earlier than that of Fz and FCz electrode (265.46 ± 3.48 vs. 270.52 ± 3.43, p = 0.034; 265.46 ± 3.48 vs. 269.08 ± 3.25, p < 0.001).

The analysis also revealed significant interaction effects between the altitude and trial type [F(4, 1,455) = 2.910, p = 0.040, η2 = 0.108]. Simple effect analysis indicated that there was a significant difference in the NoGo trial [F(12, 1,455) = 3.913, p = 0.012, η2 = 0.140]. The latency of N2 in the Nyingchi group was significantly earlier than that in the plain and the Lhasa groups.

The Amplitude and Peak Latency for P3

P3 Amplitude

A 4(altitude) × 3(trial type) × 5(electrode distribution) mixed analysis of variance was conducted for the peak amplitudes of P3. The main effect of electrodes was significant [F(4, 1, 037) = 7.602, p < 0.000, η2 = 0.028]. The amplitudes of P3 were positive, with electrode FCz showing the maximum amplitude (M = 8.813 μV, SE = 0.402 μV) and electrode Pz showing the minimum amplitude (M = 6.250, SE = 0.405 μV). P3d amplitude were larger, with electrode Fz showing the maximum amplitude (M = 6.979 μV, SE = 0.337 μV) and electrode Pz showing the minimum amplitude (M = 3.409, SE = 0.334 μV).

The results also showed that the interaction effects between altitude and stimulation type were significantly different [F(6, 1,455) = 17.361, p < 0.000, MSE = 280.611, η2 = 0.067]. The simple main effect analysis found that there was a significant difference for three conditions at different altitudes [F(3, 1,455) = 4.583/74.857/13.88, p < 0.01]. A further analysis revealed that at Go trial, the amplitude of P3 in the plain group [M = 4.716 μV, 95% CL (4.01, 5.421)] was significantly positive than that in Nyingchi [M = 3.202 μV, 95% CL (2.45, 3.954)] and Naqu [M = 3.225 μV, 95% CL (2.54, 3.93), p = 0.024] groups. In NoGo trial, the amplitude of P3 in the plain group [M = 10.295 μV, 95% CL (9.59, 11.00)] was significantly positive than that in Nyingchi [M = 7.720 μV, 95% CL (6.968, 8.472)], Lhasa [M = 7.799 μV, 95% CL (6.652, 8.37)], and Naqu groups [M = 7.464 μV, 95% CL (6.759, 8.17)], p < 0.000. The amplitude of P3d were significant positive for the plain [M = 6.114 μV, 95% CL (5.408, 6.819)] than for the Nyingchi [M = 7.720 μV, 95% CL (6.968, 8.472), p = 0.013], Lhasa [M = 7.799 μV, 95% CL (6.652, 8.37), p = 0.013], and Naqu [M = 4.574 μV, 95% CL (3.868, 5.279), p = 0.009] (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Topographic map of ERP composition at different altitudes.

There was no interaction effect between altitude and electrodes [F(12, 1,455) = 0.963, p = 0.403, MSE = 6.51, η2 = 0.003), no interaction effect between trial type and electrodes [F(8, 1,455) = 1.435, p = 0.177, MSE = 23.191, η2 = 0.008], or no triple interaction effects among three variables [F(24, 1,455) = 0.059, p = 1.00, MSE = 0.956, η2 = 0.005].

P3 Peak Latency

A 4(altitude) × 3(trial type) × 5 (electrode distribution) mixed analysis of variance analysis revealed a significant main effect of altitude for the latencies of P3 [F(6, 14, 455) = 3.399, p = 0.038, η2 = 0.124]. The post-hoc analysis test suggested that the latency of P3 in the Nyingchi group was significantly earlier than that of the plain, Lhasa, and Naqu groups (355.51 ± 8.15 vs. 386.13 ± 7.52, 378.53 ± 7.52, 387.23 ± 7.71, p = 0.007, p = 0.042, p = 0.006). The analysis also revealed a significant interaction effects between altitude and trial type [F(24, 14, 455) = 4.210, p = 0.008, η2 = 0.149]. Further analysis indicated that at the Go task, the latency of P3 in the Nyingzhi group was significantly earlier than that in the other three groups (p < 0.001, p = 0.011, p = 0.026). At the NoGo task, the latency of P3 in the Nyingzhi group was significantly earlier than that in the other three groups (p < 0.001). Other main effects and interaction effects were not significant.

Correlation Analyses Between Behavioral Data and ERP Components

Pearson correlation analysis revealed that NoGo-P3 and P3d were negatively related to altitudes (r = −0.358, p < 0.000) and (r = −0.276, p = 0.004), respectively, and the NoGo-P3 was positively related to SpO2 (r = 0.197, p = 0.041). Correlation analysis also revealed that Go-P3, NoGo-P3, N2d, and P3d were positively related to ITR (r = 0.203, p = 0.036), (r = 0.202, p = 0.036), (r = 0.204, p = 0.034), (r = 0.228, p = 0.018). The results are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of correlation analysis.

The Results of Frequency Domain (ERO)

N2

A 4(altitude) × 5(frequency band) × 5(electrodes) mixed ANOVA yielded no triple interaction effects [F(48, 2,275) = 1.252, p = 0.116, MSE = 0.449, η2 = 0.026]. The results of analysis revealed a significant interaction effect between the altitude and frequency band [F(12, 2,275) = 24.599, p < 0.000, MSE = 8.821, η2 = 0.559]. Simple effect analysis found that in the δ, θ, and α bands, there was a significant difference for N2 at different altitudes [F(3, 2,275) = 16.359/78.98/24.672, p < 0.000]. A further analysis indicated that the power of δ band in the plain group was the highest [M = 1.258, 95% CL (1.153, 1.363)], p < 0.000], which was significantly larger than that in the Nyingchi [M = 0.805, 95% CL (0.693, 0.917)], Lhasa [M = 0.926, 95% CL (0.817, 1.036)], and Naqu [M = 0.786, 95% CL (0.681, 0.891)] groups. The power of θ band in plain group was the highest [M = 1.453, 95% CL (1.347, 1.558), p < 0.000], which was significantly larger than that in Nyingchi [M = 1.062, 95% CL (0.952, 1.171)], Lhasa [M = 0.871, 95% CL (0.759, 0.983)], and Naqu [M = 0.307, 95% CL (0.202, 0.412)]. However, the power of α band in the Naqu group was the highest [M = 0.9, 95% CL (0.795, 1.01), p < 0.000], which was significantly higher than that in the plain group [M = 0.479, 95% CL (0.374, 0.584)], Nyingzhi [M = 0.329, 95% CL (0.217, 0.441)], and Lhasa [M = 0.324, 95% CL (0.215, 0.434)]. The results of analysis also revealed a significant interaction effect between the electrodeposition and frequency band [F(16, 2,275) = 14.453, p < 0.000, MSE = 5.183, η2 = 0.092]. Simple effect analysis found that the power of δ, θ, and α bands was the highest at the FCz electrode, decreased gradually at the Fz, Cz, and CPz electrode, and dropped up to the minimum Pz electrode (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Time-frequency diagram of Nogo stimulation at different altitudes.

P3

A 4(altitude) × 5(frequency band) × 5(electrodes) mixed ANOVA yielded no triple interaction effect [F(48, 2,275) = 1.073, p = 0.341, MSE = 0.143, η2 = 0.022]. The results of the analysis yielded significant interaction effects between the altitude and frequency band [F(12, 2,275) = 29.044, p < 0.000, MSE = 3.874, η2 = 0.133]. Simple main effect analysis found that in the δ, θ, and α bands, there was a significant difference for P3 at different altitudes [F(3, 2,275) = 14.741/58.743/59.699, p < 0.000]. The power of δ band in the plain group [M = 0.657, 95% CL (0.592, 0.721)] was significantly larger than that in the Nyingchi [M = 0.38, 95% CL (0.311, 0.448)], and Lhasa [M = 0.419, 95% CL (0.353, 0.486)], p < 0.000. Naqu [M = 0.38, 95% CL (0.311, 0.448)] was significantly larger than Nyingchi and Lhasa, p < 0.05. The power of θ band in the plain group was the highest [M = 0.589, 95% CL (0.525, 0.653), p < 0.000], which was significantly larger than that in Nyingzhi [M = 0.261, 95% CL (0.193, 0.33)], Lhasa [M = 0.323, 95% CL (0.256, 0.389)], and Naqu [M = −0.021, 95% CL (−0.085, 0.043)]. The power of θ band in Naqu was the lowest, which is significantly lower than that in Nyingchi and Lhasa. However, the power of α in Naqu [M = 0.453, 95% CL (0.389, 0.517), p < 0.000], which was significantly higher than that in Weinan [M = 0.035, 95% CL (−0.029, 0.099)], Nyingzhi [M = −0.091, 95% CL (−0.159, −0.023)], and Lhasa [M = −0.083, 95% CL (−0.149, −0.016)].

Discussion

Changes in Response Inhibition Function in Soldiers From Different Areas

In this study, physiological indexes and ERPs were used to explore the dynamic brain changes and neurons' oscillating hypoxia hypobaric environment exposure characteristics to varying altitudes on soldiers' inhibition control ability.

The results showed that the SpO2 of soldiers in all three plateau areas was significantly lower than the plain group. The results of HR analysis showed an opposite trend to SpO2. Studies have shown that SpO2 was negatively correlated with stationing altitude, and HR was positively correlated with stationing altitude (Rivera-Ch et al., 2008; Hilty et al., 2016; Ottestad et al., 2017). These study results are consistent with previous studies' results, and the overall SpO2 level and HR of the participants were in the normal range.

The classical Go/NoGo task used in this study presented button stimuli as simple visual stimuli. The RT results showed that both for Go and NoGo stimulation, there was a highly significant increase in the Naqu group compared to the plain group. However, there was no significant difference between the Nyingchi and Lhasa and the plain. The FARs of soldiers in the plateau groups were all higher than that of the plain group, but only the Nagqu and the plain group had significant differences. The analysis of ITR shows that there is no substantial change in the transmission rate from 347 to 3,860 m (Lhasa), but it decreases significantly at the altitude of 4,890 m (Naqu). Previous long-term exposure studies using different cognitive tasks have found that reaction times appear shortened at high altitudes (Richardson et al., 2011). The disappearance of the effects of prolonged exposure on behavior may be due to adaptation supported by compensatory mechanisms. However, the significant increase in the Nagqu area may be due to the decline in executive function caused by prolonged exposure. One study found that the FAR and RT increased in the highland region, indicating that hypoxia exposure led to the decline in executive function (Trompetero et al., 2015). The findings are consistent with the earlier results that the plateau's hypoxic environment has a relatively low effect on simple cognitive tasks. In contrast, the hypoxic environment may impact complex cognitive functions dominated by coordination and accuracy.

The ERPs can reflect information on the neural mechanisms of response inhibition in warriors exposed to high-altitude environments for long periods. In this study, significant response inhibition effects were successfully evoked at four groups, that is, NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 components induced by NoGo stimulation were successfully inhibited. Comparing the differences between the three plateau groups and one plain group revealed that the three plateau groups' soldiers showed a significant decrease in amplitude both in the N2 component of conflict monitoring and the P3 component of late processing of information. Studies have indicated that N2 and P3 are considered to function as separable processes in Go/NoGo tasks (Randall and Smith, 2011; Burkhard et al., 2018). The NoGo-N2 component response conflict detection and response inhibition can effectively represent the soldiers' cognitive control degree (Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2010; Burkhard et al., 2018). The NoGo-P3 component amplitude is associated with late processing of response inhibition (Groom and Cragg, 2015). It shows that with the increase in altitude, functional declines in soldiers' response inhibition control become more and more serious, manifested by the decrease in conflict monitoring ability and inhibition control ability in the inhibition processing stage. The amplitude of P3d also decreased significantly in the three plateau groups, especially at the Naqu group. The study showed that the amplitude of P3d was weakened considerably or even missing, suggesting that the function of inhibition control (i.e., response inhibition or movement inhibition) was declined at high altitude. In addition to the response inhibition domain, the P3 component is usually considered the late stage of information processing: the allocated attentional resources (Meinhardt and Pekrun, 2003). It has been shown that high-altitude populations have higher cognitive demands that limit attentional resources for inhibitory control (Hill et al., 2014; Hu et al., 2016). A high-altitude study using an attention task also found lower P3 amplitudes in the high-altitude group than in the low-altitude group (Wang et al., 2014). Chronic high-altitude exposure leads to reduced attentional resource availability (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005). This study proved that the attention resources of the plateau group decreased compared with the plain control group.

Notably, among the three groups of high-altitude regions, the hypoxic environment in the Nagqu region had the most pronounced effect on the soldiers' inhibitory control function. The primary manifestation was the smallest amplitude of the P3 component of the difference wave, which is considered the neural activity induced by afferent stimuli and reflects the degree of perceptual information processing and an alteration (Godde et al., 2010; Martin et al., 2012; Walsh et al., 2020). This result suggests that a higher altitude leads to a lower degree of inhibitory control activity on information processing due to hypoxia. Studies also show that people's mental and physical working ability in the highland environment is significantly reduced along with the increase in altitude (Tong et al., 2007; Zhu and Fan, 2017). Although the P3 latency is thought to reflect a slowdown in signal processing caused by hypoxia (Ramautar et al., 2006), there was no difference in P3 latency among groups. It may be that the relatively long exposure to high altitude in the subjects of this study may have acclimated to hypoxia. Therefore, the changes in P3 latency at high altitudes found in previous studies may depend on how long individuals live at high altitudes (Mazzeo, 2008). The amplitude of the N2 component, which is a conflict monitor, also tends to weaken, but significant differences are observed in Lhasa at the height of 3,160 m. The results are similar to previous studies' findings that under mild hypoxic conditions, the organism showed increased neural activity, but reduced availability of attentional resources produced neuroexcitatory inhibition, increased impulsivity, and reduced conflict monitoring sensitivity post-conflict processing (Pontifex et al., 2009; Turner et al., 2015; Lefferts et al., 2016; McMorris et al., 2017). The results suggest that cognitive function effects are not easily perceived under mild hypoxic conditions and when complex tasks are not performed. Mental health maintenance should be done in advance, with particular attention to strengthening emotional stability regulation.

The Variation in δ-, θ-, and α-Band Power in Response Inhibition Function

This study also explored the effects of different altitude plateau on the soldier's response inhibition function near convulsions, focusing on the conflict monitoring and response inhibition phases. Hypoxic exposure can change the neural network's spatial representation and be reflected by EEG's different nervous oscillations (Cvetkovic et al., 2004). The results showed decreased the δ- and θ-band powers of soldiers in three high-altitude areas than those in the plain area. One study showed reduced brain wave activity in the δ and θ bands in a hypoxic environment (more than 30 days) (Zhao et al., 2016). However, the opposite result was found in the simulation module simulating the anoxic environment at high altitude (23 min): the δ and θ bands' activity increased (Papadelis et al., 2007). The reason may be related to the time of hypoxic exposure. This study's subjects are soldiers stationed in various areas for 2 years, indicating that prolonged exposure to a plateau environment can affect brain activity changes. Some studies have explored brainwave activity characteristics in adolescents living at high altitudes and found that δ- and β-frequency activity decreased (Richardson et al., 2011). In this study, there is no difference in the β-frequency activity of soldiers in different regions.

Studies have shown that EEG activity decreases at the beginning of an acute hypoxic exposure and increases with the emergence of α-band activity (Schellart and Reits, 2001; Papadelis et al., 2007). This study shows that the α-band power activity in the conflict monitoring phase of response inhibition function decreases at Nyingchi and Lhasa. There was no significant difference compared with the plain area. However, the α-band power at Naqu was significantly higher than that at the other three groups. This change of power in the specific frequency band of EEG is caused by a decreased synchronization of related nerve oscillations in the cortex (Pfurtscheller and Aranibar, 1979). α-Rhythm synchronization means that this region of the brain is resting or dormant (Pfurtscheller and Aranibar, 1979; Pfurtscheller et al., 1997). α-Rhythmic ERD phenomenon can reveal some pathological states to some extent: some studies have found that Parkinson's patients have α- and β-band ERD phenomenon, showing a hyperactive motor cortex (Heida et al., 2014). Combined with the behavioral results, the above studies further show that soldiers' low information transmission speed in the Naqu was related to weakening of the α-band ERD phenomenon. The study also found that the α-band power in the conflict detection stage of response inhibition was more active than that in the response inhibition stage, indicating that long-term hypobaric hypoxia (HH) exposure caused long-term excitation of the central nervous system, resulting in increased brain loss and gradual decline inactivity. This further reveals that soldiers need to consume more cognitive resources to complete tasks in an anoxic environment.

Several limitations of the present study should be noted. In this study, all the subjects lived at the site for more than 24 months and completed the process of acclimatization at a high altitude. As the experiment was carried out in spring, Nyingchi and Lhasa's oxygen contents are relatively abundant. The sample size involved in the study is relatively small, so the research results may be affected and have some limitations. Second, we did not measure any cerebral oxygen delivery parameters, such as cerebral oxygenation and cerebral blood flow. Future studies should directly measure cerebral oxygenation, cerebral blood flow, and cerebral oxygen delivery and clarify the mechanism of decreased executive function under hypoxia (Ochi et al., 2018).

Conclusion

In summary, this study found that chronic hypoxia exposure at different altitudes of plateau affects response inhibition, mainly in the conflict monitoring phase and post-processing phase. Further analysis revealed that 3,860 m was the height of decline in the brain's response inhibition function during the conflict monitoring phase, and 2,950 m was the height of decrease during the response inhibition phase. In addition, the soldier's brain executive function was closely related to SpO2, and a reduction in SpO2 may lead to a decline in response inhibition.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The study was conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures were carried out with the adequate understanding and written informed consent of the subjects. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Fourth Military of Medical University.

Author Contributions

XW and SZ were involved in the field for the data collection, troubleshooting, manuscript finalization, organized the data, carried out the analyses, drafted the manuscript, and took the lead in the manuscript finalization and submission. XN and AC were involved in the conceptualization, design, and planning of the study. All authors went through all the versions of the manuscript and approved them.

Funding

This project was supported by the National Social Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 15BSH023).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Annett, M. (1970). A classification of hand preference by association analysis. Br. J. Psychol. 61, 303–321. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1970.tb01248.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernardi, E., Pratali, L., Mandolesi, G., Spiridonova, M., Roi, G. S., and Cogo, A. (2017). Thoraco-abdominal coordination and performance during uphill running at altitude. PLoS ONE 12:e0174927. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0174927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brutsaert, T. D., Araoz, M., Soria, R., Spielvogel, H., and Haas, J. D. (2000). Higher arterial oxygen saturation during submaximal exercise in Bolivian Aymara compared to European sojourners and Europeans born and raised at high altitude. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 113, 169–181. doi: 10.1002/1096-8644(200010)113:2<169::AID-AJPA3>3.0.CO;2-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchsbaum, B. R., Greer, S., Chang, W. L., and Berman, K. F. (2005). Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin card-sorting task and component processes. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 35–45. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkhard, A., Elmer, S., Kara, D., Brauchli, C., and Jäncke, L. (2018). The effect of background music on inhibitory functions: an ERP study. Front. Hum. Neuroscience 12:293. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, C. H., Tsai, H. Y., and Cheng, H. N. (2019). The effect of age on N2 and P3 components: a meta-analysis of Go/Nogo tasks. Brain Cognit. 135, 103574. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cvetkovic, D., Djuwari, D., and Cosic, I. (2004). “The induced rhythmic oscillations of neural activity in the human brain,” in Proceedings of the IASTED International Conference on Biomedical Engineering (Innsbruck), 306–311.

Google Scholar

Daly, I., Blanchard, C., and Holmes, N. P. (2018). Cortical excitability correlates with the event-related desynchronization during brain-computer interface control. J. Neural Eng. 15:026022. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aa9c8c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, J. E., Wagner, D. R., Garvin, N., Moilanen, D., Thorington, J., and Schall, C. (2015). Cognitive and psychomotor responses to high-altitude exposure in sea level and high-altitude residents of Ecuador. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 34:2. doi: 10.1186/s40101-014-0039-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enriquez-Geppert, S., Konrad, C., Pantev, C., and Huster, R. J. (2010). Conflict and inhibition differentially affect the N200/P300 complex in a combined go/nogo and stop-signal task. NeuroImage 51, 877–887. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fogarty, J. S., Barry, R. J., De Blasio, F. M., and Steiner, G. Z. (2018). Manipulating the focus of attention in working memory go/no-go task: inhibition in young adults. Psychophysiology 55:e13065. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, J. R., and Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology 45, 152–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godde, B., Diamond, M. E., and Braun, C. (2010). Feeling for space or for time: task-dependent modulation of the cortical representation of identical vibrotactile stimuli. Neurosci. Lett. 480, 143–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.06.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groom, M. J., and Cragg, L. (2015). Differential modulation of the N2 and P3 event-related potentials by response conflict and inhibition. Brain Cognit. 97, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heida, T., Poppe, N. R., de Vos, C. C., van Putten, M. J., and van Vugt, J. P. (2014). Event-related mu-rhythm desynchronization during movement observation is impaired in Parkinson's disease. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 1819–1825. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrich, E. C., Djokic, M. A., Gilbertson, D., DeYoung, P. N., Bosompra, N. O., Wu, L., et al. (2019). Cognitive function and mood at high altitude following acclimatization and use of supplemental oxygen and adaptive servo-ventilation sleep treatments. PLoS ONE 14:e0217089. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, C. M., Dimitriou, D., Baya, A., Webster, R., Gavlak-Dingle, J., Lesperance, V., et al. (2014). Cognitive performance in high-altitude Andean residents compared with low-altitude populations: from childhood to older age. Neuropsychology 28, 752–760. doi: 10.1037/neu0000065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilty, M. P., Müller, A., Flück, D., Siebenmann, C., Rasmussen, P., Keiser, S., et al. (2016). Effect of increased blood flow on pulmonary circulation before and during high altitude acclimatization. High Altit. Med. Biol. 17, 305–314. doi: 10.1089/ham.2016.0004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, S. L., Xiong, W., Dai, Z. Q., Zhao, H. L., and Feng, H. (2016). Cognitive changes during prolonged stay at high altitude and its correlation with c-reactive protein. PLoS ONE 11:e0146290. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, T. P., Makeig, S., Humphries, C., Lee, T. W., McKeown, M. J., Iragui, V., et al. (2000). Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology 37, 163–178. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefferts, W. K., Babcock, M. C., Tiss, M. J., Ives, S. J., White, C. N., Brutsaert, T. D., et al. (2016). Effect of hypoxia on cerebrovascular and cognitive function during moderate-intensity exercise. Physiol. Behav. 165, 108–118. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Ma, Z., Lu, W., and Li, Y. (2006). Automatic removal of the eye blink artifact from EEG using an ICA-based template matching approach. Physiol. Measure. 27, 425–436. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/27/4/008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, D., Meltzoff, A. N., and Wellman, H. M. (2009). Neural correlates of belief- and desire-reasoning. Child Dev. 80, 1163–1171. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01323.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, T., Das, A., and Huxlin, K. R. (2012). Visual cortical activity reflects faster accumulation of information from cortically blind fields. Brain 135(Pt 11), 3440–3452. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzeo, R. S. (2008). Physiological responses to exercise at altitude: An update. Sports Med. 38, 1–8. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200838010-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMorris, T., Hale, B. J., Barwood, M., Costello, J., and Corbett, J. (2017). Effect of acute hypoxia on cognition: a systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74(Pt A), 225–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meinhardt, J., and Pekrun, R. (2003). Attentional resource allocation to emotional events: An ERP study. Cognit. Emot. 17, 477–500. doi: 10.1080/02699930244000039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nation, D. A., Bondi, M. W., Gayles, E., and Delis, D. C. (2017). Mechanisms of memory dysfunction during high altitude hypoxia training in military aircrew. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 23, 1–10. doi: 10.1017/S1355617716000965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. Psychol. Bull. 131, 510–532. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochi, G., Kanazawa, Y., Hyodo, K., Suwabe, K., Shimizu, T., Fukuie, T., et al. (2018). Hypoxia-induced lowered executive function depends on arterial oxygen desaturation. J. Physiol. Sci. 68, 847–853. doi: 10.1007/s12576-018-0603-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ottestad, W., Hansen, T. A., Pradhan, G., Stepanek, J., Høiseth, L. Ø., and Kåsin, J. I. (2017). Acute hypoxia in a simulated high-altitude airdrop scenario due to oxygen system failure. J. Appl. Physiol. 123, 1443–1450. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00169.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmwood, E. N., Krompinger, J. W., and Simons, R. F. (2017). Electrophysiological indicators of inhibitory control deficits in depression. Biol. Psychol. 130, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papadelis, C., Kourtidou-Papadeli, C., Bamidis, P. D., Maglaveras, N., and Pappas, K. (2007). The effect of hypobaric hypoxia on multichannel EEG signal complexity. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 31–52. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., and Aranibar, A. (1979). Evaluation of event-related desynchronization (ERD) preceding and following voluntary self-paced movement. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 46, 138–146. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(79)90063-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., Neuper, C., Flotzinger, D., and Pregenzer, M. (1997). EEG-based discrimination between imagination of right and left-hand movement. Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 103, 642–651. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(97)00080-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, W. D., and Epling, W. F. (1980). What happened to analysis in applied behavior analysis? Behav. Analyst 3, 1–9. doi: 10.1007/BF03392373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pires, L., Leitão, J., Guerrini, C., and Simões, M. R. (2014). Event-related brain potentials in the study of inhibition: cognitive control, source localization and age-related modulations. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 461–490. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9275-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontifex, M. B., Hillman, C. H., Fernhall, B., Thompson, K. M., and Valentini, T. A. (2009). The effect of acute aerobic and resistance exercise on working memory. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 41, 927–934. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181907d69

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramautar, J. R., Kok, A., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2006). Effects of stop-signal modality on the N2/P3 complex elicited in the stop-signal paradigm. Biol. Psychol. 72, 96–109. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randall, W. M., and Smith, J. L. (2011). Conflict and inhibition in the cued-Go/NoGo task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 2400–2407. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, C., Hogan, A. M., Bucks, R. S., Baya, A., Virues-Ortega, J., Holloway, J. W., et al. (2011). Neurophysiological evidence for cognitive and brain functional adaptation in adolescents living at high altitude. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 1726–1734. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivera-Ch, M., Huicho, L., Bouchet, P., Richalet, J. P., and León-Velarde, F. (2008). Effect of acetazolamide on ventilatory response in subjects with chronic mountain sickness. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 162, 184–189. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2008.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schellart, N. A., and Reits, D. (2001). Transient and maintained changes of the spontaneous occipital EEG during acute systemic hypoxia. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 72, 462–470.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schlaghecken, F., and Eimer, M. (2006). Active masks and active inhibition: a comment on Lleras and Enns (2004) and on Verleger, Jaskowski, Aydemir, van der Lubbe, and Groen (2004). J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 135, 484–494. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, C.-Q., Zhao, J., Bo, H., and Xiang, X.-H. (2007). Influence of isometric exercise on vascular functions of recruits from the Chinese People's armed police forces during hypoxic training. J. Clin. Rehabil. Tissue Eng. Res. 11, 6424–6427.

Trompetero, A. C., Cristancho, E., Benavides, W., and Ramírez, M. H. (2015). Behavior of the indicators of the erythropoiesis and the state of the iron in Colombian University population at different heights. Physiotherapy 101, e1537–e1538. doi: 10.1016/j.physio.2015.03.1529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, C. E., Barker-Collo, S. L., Connell, C. J., and Gant, N. (2015). Acute hypoxic gas breathing severely impairs cognition and task learning in humans. Physiol. Behav. 142, 104–110. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vázquez-Marrufo, M., Galvao-Carmona, A., Benítez Lugo, M. L., Ruíz-Peña, J. L., Borges Guerra, M., and Izquierdo Ayuso, G. (2017). Retest reliability of individual alpha ERD topography assessed by human electroencephalography. PLoS ONE 12:e0187244. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vinet, L., and Zhedanov, A. (2011). A “missing” family of classical orthogonal polynomials. J. Phys. A 44:085201. doi: 10.1088/1751-8113/44/8/085201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, K. S., McGovern, D. P., Clark, A., and O'Connell, R. G. (2020). Evaluating the neurophysiological evidence for predictive processing as a model of perception. Ann. New York Acad. Sci. 1464, 242–268. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Ma, H., Fu, S., Guo, S., Yang, X., Luo, P., et al. (2014). Long-term exposure to high altitude affects voluntary spatial attention at early and late processing stages. Sci. Rep. 4:4443. doi: 10.1038/srep04443

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpaw, J. R. (2002). Memory in neuroscience: rhetoric versus reality. Behav. Cognit. Neurosci. Rev. 1, 130–163. doi: 10.1177/1534582302001002003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, X., Zhang, J., Gong, Q., and Weng, X. (2011). Prolonged high-altitude residence impacts verbal working memory: an fMRI study. Exper. Brain Res. 208, 437–445. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2494-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, X., Zhang, J., Shi, J., Gong, Q., and Weng, X. (2010). Cerebral and functional adaptation with chronic hypoxia exposure: a multi-modal MRI study. Brain Res. 1348, 21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Wang, J., Wu, Q., Kuang, W., Huang, X., He, Y., et al. (2011). Disrupted brain connectivity networks in drug-naive, first-episode major depressive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 334–342. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, J. P., Zhang, R., Yu, Q., and Zhang, J. X. (2016). Characteristics of EEG activity during high altitude hypoxia and lowland reoxygenation. Brain Res. 1648(Pt A), 243–249. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, L. L., and Fan, M. (2017). Impact of plateau environment hypoxia on human cognitive function and intervention measures. Chin. J. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 11, 1114–1119. doi: 10.3867/j.issn.1000-3002.2017.11.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: high altitude, hypobaric hypoxia, SpO2, response inhibition, military personnel

Citation: Wei X, Ni X, Zhao S and Chi A (2021) Influence of Exposure at Different Altitudes on the Executive Function of Plateau Soldiers—Evidence From ERPs and Neural Oscillations. Front. Physiol. 12:632058. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.632058

Received: 22 November 2020; Accepted: 02 March 2021;
Published: 16 April 2021.

Edited by:

Francisco C. Villafuerte, Universidad Peruana Cayetano Heredia, Peru

Reviewed by:

Jorge G. Farias, University of La Frontera, Chile
Gloria Rakita Leon, University of Minnesota Twin Cities, United States

Copyright © 2021 Wei, Ni, Zhao and Chi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiaoli Ni, bml4aWFvbGkmI3gwMDA0MDt4anR1LmVkdS5jbg==; Aiping Chi, Y2hpbXUmI3gwMDA0MDtzbm51LmVkdS5jbg==

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.