Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 02 July 2013
Sec. Integrative Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic How nature shaped echolocation in animals View all 21 articles

Echolocation of static and moving objects in two-dimensional space using bat-like frequency-modulation sound

  • 1Department of Information Science, Tohoku Gakuin University, Sendai, Japan
  • 2Neurosensing and Bionavigation Research Center, Doshisha University, Kyotanabe, Kyoto, Japan

Bats use frequency-modulated echolocation to identify and capture moving objects in real three-dimensional space. The big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus, emits linear period modulation sound, and is capable of locating static objects with a range accuracy of less than 1 μs. A previously introduced model can estimate ranges of multiple, static objects using linear frequency modulation (LFM) sound and Gaussian chirplets with a carrier frequency compatible with bat emission sweep rates. The delay time for a single object was estimated with an accuracy of about 1.3 μs by measuring the echo at a low signal-to-noise ratio. This model could estimate the location of each moving object in two-dimensional space. In this study, the linear period modulation sounds, mimicking the emitting pulse of big brown bats, were introduced as the emitted signals. Echoes were measured from moving objects at two receiving points by intermittently emitting these sounds. It was clarified that this model could localize moving objects in two-dimensional space by accurately estimating the object ranges.

Introduction

Bats emit high-frequency sound waves, allowing them to track and catch flying insects (Griffin, 1958; Simmons et al., 1995). Bats perceive the location of moving objects in three-dimensional (3D) space using frequency modulation. Experimental evidence indicates that bats are capable of locating static objects at high signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs) achieving sub-microsecond accuracy (Simmons, 1979; Menne et al., 1989; Moss and Schnitzler, 1989; Simmons et al., 1990). In echolocation, many kinds of bats, including Eptesicus fuscus and Noctilio leporinus, emit linear period modulation (LPM) sound, the instantaneous period of which increases linearly with time. It was clarified that the LPM signal is useful for the range estimation of moving objects because of its Doppler tolerance using matched filters (Altes and Titlebaum, 1970; Altes and Skinner, 1977). Several previously proposed models estimate the delay times of multiple objects from an echo spectrogram, which is computed by IIR filters or short-time Fourier transform, which corresponds to convolution of the constant-frequency (CF) carrier wave at each frequency (Saillant et al., 1993; Matsuo et al., 2001; Neretti et al., 2003). However, it is difficult to accurately determine the delay time for each object using the peak time, because the integration time of the cochlear filters is long. An echolocation model was proposed to estimate the delay times of multiple objects from the time–frequency pattern using linear frequency modulation (LFM) sound (Matsuo and Yano, 2004; Matsuo et al., 2004; Matsuo, 2011, 2013). In this model, the time–frequency pattern is computed through the convolution of Gaussian chirplet filters for which the carrier frequency agrees with the sweep rate of emission (Matsuo and Yano, 2004; Matsuo et al., 2004; Matsuo, 2011, 2013). It was demonstrated that this proposed model could estimate the range of the moving object or accurately localize the moving object in two-dimensional (2D) space using the interaural range difference (IRD), computed as the difference between the object's range at two receiving points. In addition, Gaussian chirplet filters have been proposed for LPM sounds (Guarato et al., 2011). The present study examines whether this model can localize moving objects in 2D space from echoes, which are measured from static and moving objects at two receiving points by intermittently emitting LPM sounds, corresponding to the emitting pulse of big brown bats.

Methods

Acoustic data were recorded in a soundproof chamber (length × width × height = 2.8 m × 1.7 m × 1.8 m). The measuring system, including one loudspeaker, two microphones, and objects to be detected were located on an optical base (Chuo Precision Industrial, TT-D6090), as shown in Figure 1. The loudspeaker and microphones were placed at a height of 70 cm, and the distance between them was 4 cm. The origin was defined as the center of the speaker's surface. The reflecting objects used were erect poles (radius of 8 mm) set on a rotating table controlled by a computer via an electric rotary actuator (Taiyo, ESR1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Measuring system (top view) with a pole set on a rotating table.

The emitted signal was generated by a computer (National Instruments, PXI-8106), digital-to-analog (DA) converted (PXI-5412), amplified (TDT, ED1), and emitted by the loudspeaker (TDT, ES1). The sampling frequency of the DA converter was 1 MHz, and the resolution was 16 bits. The echoes reflected by the objects were recorded using a 1/8-inch condenser microphone (Brüel & Kjær, 4138), amplified (Brüel & Kjær, NEXUS 2670, 2690), bandpass-filtered (NF, CF-4BL, CF-4BH), and analog-to-digital (AD) converted (NI, PXI-6133). The sampling frequency of the AD converter was 400 kHz, and the resolution was 14 bits. The temperature was measured in the chamber to compute the sound velocity. To estimate the characteristics of the measuring system, LFM sound, sweeping from 135 to 5 kHz over 2 ms, was used and the waveform was measured when the loudspeaker and microphone were positioned face-to-face. Figure 2 shows the spectrum computed by taking the Fourier transform of the measured waveform. At a distance of 40 cm, the maximum and average values for the amplitude spectrum in the range of 30–100 kHz were 98.6 and 89.3 decibels sound pressure level (dB SPL), respectively, and the half-power (3-dB) bandwidth was 32 kHz (Matsuo, 2013).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Characteristics of the loudspeaker. The solid curve shows the amplitude spectrum computed from the measured waveform with the loudspeaker facing the microphone. The dotted curve shows the noise level.

In this paper, the bat-like LPM sound signal was synthesized by referring to the sound emitted by a big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus, during approach of an object. The sound duration was almost 1.9 ms and the LPM signal started at 53 kHz and swept down to 25 kHz. Figure 3A shows the emitted waveform. The echoes were measured from the rotating pole for two situations. The first measurement was of the echo from the object moving back and forth, and the second was of the echo from the object moving from side to side. In addition, to clarify the effect of the Doppler shift on the accuracy, echoes from the static object were measured.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Emission waveform. (B) Emission spectrogram, computed by convolution of the Gaussian chirplets. (C) The temporal emission pattern Wemi corresponds to the spectrogram for one bandpass filter (with a center frequency of 30 kHz).

Model

Transformation of the Waveform into a Spectrogram Using Chirplet Filters

The waveforms of the object echoes were entered into the echolocation model discussed in the Introduction. They were transformed into spectrograms in a manner that simulated the process in the mammalian cochlea. The temporal changes in the interference pattern were extracted using Gaussian chirplet filters with a carrier frequency consistent with the sweep rate of emission (Matsuo and Yano, 2004; Matsuo et al., 2004; Guarato et al., 2011). The temporal characteristics of the filter can be described by

F(fj,t)=exp(t2αj)exp(2πjln(kt+l)k)(1)

Here fj (kHz) is the center frequency for the jth bandpass filter, t is time (s), and αj is a parameter that describes the width of the window function:

αj=w/2ln(0.7),w=bw(f1fj),

where f1 is the start frequency of the signal and bw is the filter's base bandwidth fixed as 160 μs. Constants k and l are defined by

k=fstafenddur·fsta·fend,l=1k·tsta·fstafsta,

where dur is the duration of the signal, fsta (53 kHz) and fend (25 kHz) are the starting and end frequencies of the signal, and tsta is the start time of the signal. The bandpass filter bank comprised 24 filters with center frequencies ranging 27–50 kHz, positioned at regular intervals. The quality factor at 10 dB (Q10 dB) values ranges from 1.7 at 27 kHz to 3.1 at 50 kHz.

The waveforms for both the emitted waves and the echoes were transformed into a spectrogram P(f, t) through convolution with the filters as shown in Equation 1. Figure 3B shows the outputs of the cochlear filters for the emitted waveform. Figure 3C shows the temporal pattern corresponding to the spectrogram P(f, t) of the emission for one filter (with a center frequency of 30 kHz). The shapes of the temporal patterns corresponding to the spectrogram P(f, t) for all filters were the same because the window lengths were set dependent on center frequencies.

To demonstrate the output from the cochlear filters, we considered the situation of a static object with position (x, y) of (0 mm, 450 mm) and range of 901.8 mm. Figure 4A shows the measured waveforms including the object's echo as well as the sound transmitted from the loudspeaker. The spectrogram P(f, t), which was computed from the outputs of the Gaussian chirplets, was transformed into a range-frequency pattern Secho (f, τ) with 10-μs intervals by compensating for the sweep rate, as shown in Figure 4B. The compensation time is denoted τ, and it is implied that the range corresponds to the delay time since the start time of emission was zero.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Echo waveform. (B) Range–frequency pattern computed from the spectrogram, through convolution of the Gaussian chirplets. (C) Averaged pattern computed by integration of bandpass filter channels.

Determination of the Object's Range and Location in 2D Space

The delay time for one object, T1, was estimated from the range–frequency pattern around the onset (Matsuo et al., 2004; Matsuo, 2011, 2013). First, the averaged pattern was computed by the integration of bandpass filter channels. The delay times for the onset and offset were determined using a threshold corresponding to almost four times the noise level (Matsuo, 2011, 2013). T1 and the corresponding reflectivity, r1, were uniquely determined from the averages of the two spectra at the onset delay time τon and 10 μs later. Figure 4C shows the averaged pattern, which was computed from the range–frequency pattern shown in Figure 4B. In this case, the delay time τ of the onset was estimated using a threshold of 2630 μs. The black curve in Figure 5 shows candidates for T1 according to the reflected intensity distribution estimated from the average of Secho at the onset delay τon (2630 μs). The red curve shows candidates estimated from the average of Secho 10 μs after τon (2640 μs). The delay time for T1 was determined to be 2678 μs, corresponding to 908.6 mm, by comparing the correspondence between the reflected intensities of the two candidates.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Determination of T1. The solid curve shows candidates for the reflected intensity distribution estimated from Secho at onset delay time τon. The dotted curve shows candidates for the reflected intensity distribution estimated from Secho at 10 μs after τon. T1 was determined by estimating the correspondence between the reflectivity of candidates at each delay timepoint.

The location of the object in 2D space was determined by the difference between the object's ranges at the two microphones. The object's position (x, y) is represented by polar coordinates (r, θ):

x=rsin(θ),y=rcos(θ),

where r is the distance between the speaker and the object, and θ the direction of the object with respect to the horizontal axis. The distance r between the speaker and object was obtained from the mean of the ranges at the two microphones. If r >> d, corresponding to the distance between two microphones, the direction of the object, θ, was computed from this difference, Δr, using the approximation

θ=sin1(Δrd)

The object was continuously tracked by estimating its position at each timepoint.

Results

Localization of One Pole Moving Back and Forth

To evaluate the effect of the Doppler shift on the localization accuracy, the echoes from one object moving back and forth were measured and analyzed. The center of rotation was fixed at (250 mm, 450 mm) and the radius of rotation was 250 mm. The circles in Figure 6A show the estimated range along the time axis. The object's range could be estimated accurately using the temporal changes of echo spectra at the onset time. Figure 6B shows the IRD at each timepoint when the pole was moving back to forth. As shown in Figure 6C, the errors of the IRD were less than 4 mm. The circles and curves in Figure 6D show the estimated location and position of the object in 2D space. The locations of one pole could be estimated using the object's ranges for two microphones.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Outputs for one object, moving back and forth with a rotation radius of 250 mm. (A) Estimated and actual ranges. (B) Estimated IRDs. (C) Estimation errors of IRDs. (D) Estimated and actual positions in 2D space.

Localization of One Pole Moving From Side to Side

To evaluate the model's performance for different movements, the echoes from one pole moving from side to side were measured and analyzed. First, the center of rotation was fixed at (0 mm, 575 mm) and the radius of rotation was 125 mm. The circles and curves in Figure 7A show the estimated range and the object's actual range along the time axis. The object's range could be estimated using the temporal changes of echo spectra at the onset time. The circles and curves in Figure 7B show the estimated location and object's position in 2D space, respectively. One pole could be localized except for side positions.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Outputs for one object, moving side to side with a rotation radius of 125 mm. (A) Estimated and actual ranges. (B) Estimated and actual positions in 2D space.

In the second measurement scenario, the center of rotation was fixed at (0 mm, 700 mm) and the radius of rotation was 250 mm. The circles and curves in Figure 8A show the estimated range and the object's actual range along the time axis. The object's range could be estimated using the temporal changes of echo spectra at the onset time. The circles and curves in Figure 8B show the estimated location and object's position in 2D space, respectively. One pole could be localized except for side positions.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Outputs for one object, moving from side to side with a rotation radius of 250 mm. (A) Estimated and actual ranges. (B) Estimated and actual positions in 2D space.

Discussion and Conclusion

Bats can locate and discriminate between individual objects even when the objects are moving (Griffin, 1958; Webster and Griffin, 1962; Griffin et al., 1965; Simmons et al., 1995). In a previous study, echoes were measured from a moving object while emitting (LFM) sound intermittently. The object's range and location in 2D space was estimated by extracting the temporal changes of echo spectra. In this paper, bat-like LPM sound was used to localize a moving object. It was demonstrated that this model could extend the localization of the moving object from echoes using the LPM signal. For this model, the errors in the IRD were less than 4 mm, corresponding to 12 μs, as shown in Figure 6, while the errors in the IRD using the LFM signal (Matsuo, 2013) were less than 2 mm, corresponding to 6 μs. The range accuracy was dependent on the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) and the frequency bandwidth (Burdic, 1968; Menne and Hackbarth, 1986; Simmons et al., 2004; Boonman and Ostwald, 2007). The frequency bandwidths were 23 kHz in this model using the LPM signal, and 70 kHz in the previous model using the LFM signal. It is thought that the difference of errors is due to differences in frequency bandwidths of the emitted sound.

Bat can perceive the object in 3D space by localizing object's distance and direction. Directional information by real bats has previously been investigated by measuring the head-related transfer function (Wotton et al., 1995; Aytekin et al., 2004; Mey et al., 2008). Therefore, it is necessary to extend to localize objects in 3D space using the IRD in combination with the interaural level difference and the transfer function.

In this paper, only the first harmonics of the LPM signal were used. Eptesicus fuscus emits ultrasonic frequency modulation sounds containing two prominent downward-sweeping harmonics. In behavioral studies, echo-delay perception was disrupted by small temporal misalignments of echo harmonics (Bates and Simmons, 2011; Bates et al., 2011). Thus, the temporal cues for two harmonics are important to echolocation in nature. In future work, it will be necessary to extend this model to describe these results using harmonic sound signals.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Ms. K. Wagatsuma for helpful experimental assistance. This work was partly supported by JST, CREST.

References

Altes, R. A., and Skinner, D. P. (1977). Sonar velocity resolution with a linear-period-modulated pulse. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 61, 1019–1030. doi: 10.1121/1.381375

CrossRef Full Text

Altes, R. A., and Titlebaum, E. L. (1970). Bat signals as optimally doppler tolerant waveforms. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 48, 1014–1020. doi: 10.1121/1.1912222

CrossRef Full Text

Aytekin, M., Grassi, E., Sahota, M., and Moss, C. F. (2004). The bat head-related transfer function reveals binaural cues for sound localization in azimuth and elevation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116, 3594–3605. doi: 10.1121/1.1811412

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bates, M. E., and Simmons, J. A. (2011). Perception of echo delay is disrupted by small temporal misalignments of echo harmonics in bat sonar. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 394–401. doi: 10.1242/jeb.048983

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bates, M. E., Simmons, J. A., and Zorikov, T. V. (2011). Bats use echo harmonic structure to distinguish their targets from background clutter. Science 333, 627–630. doi: 10.1126/science.1202065

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boonman, A., and Ostwald, J. (2007). A modeling approach to explain pulse design in bats. Biol. Cybern. 97, 159–172. doi: 10.1007/s00422-007-0164-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burdic, W. S. (1968). Radar Signal Analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Griffin, D. R. (1958). Listening in the Dark. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. (Reprinted by Cornell University Press: Ithaca, N.Y., 1986).

Griffin, D. R., Friend, J. H., and Webster, F. A. (1965). Target discrimination by the echolocation of bats. J. Exp. Zool. 158, 155–168. doi: 10.1002/jez.1401580204

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guarato, F., Hallam, J., and Matsuo, I. (2011). Reconstruction of the signal produced by a directional sound source from remote multi-microphone recordings. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 130, 1689–1699. doi: 10.1121/1.3619792

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matsuo, I. (2011). Evaluation of the echolocation model for range estimation of multiple closely spaced objects. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 130, 1030–1037. doi: 10.1121/1.3608119

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matsuo, I. (2013). Localization and tracking of moving objects in two-dimensional space by echolocation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 133, 1151–1157. doi: 10.1121/1.4773254

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matsuo, I., Kunugiyama, K., and Yano, M. (2004). An echolocation model for range discrimination of multiple closely spaced objects: transformation of spectrogram into the reflected intensity distribution. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 115, 920–928. doi: 10.1121/1.1642626

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matsuo, I., Tani, J., and Yano, M. (2001). A model of echolocation of multiple targets in 3D space from a single emission. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110, 607–624. doi: 10.1121/1.1377294

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matsuo, I., and Yano, M. (2004). An echolocation model for the restoration of an acoustic image from a single-emission echo. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116, 3782–3788. doi: 10.1121/1.1811411

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Menne, D., and Hackbarth, H. (1986). Accuracy of distance measurement in the bat Eptesicus fuscus: theoretical aspects and computer simulation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 79, 386–397. doi: 10.1121/1.393578

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Menne, D., Kaipf, I., Wagner, I., Ostwald, J., and Schnitzler, H. U. (1989). Range estimation by echolocation in the bat Eptesicus fuscus: trading of phase versus time cues. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 2642–2650. doi: 10.1121/1.397758

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mey, F. D., Reijniers, J., Peremans, H., Otani, M., and Firzlaff, U. (2008). Simulated head related transfer function of the phyllostomid bat Phyllostomus discolor. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 124, 2123–2132. doi: 10.1121/1.2968703

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moss, C. F., and Schnitzler, H. U. (1989). Accuracy of target ranging in echolocating bats: acoustic information processing. J. Comp. Physiol. A 165, 383–393. doi: 10.1007/BF00619357

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neretti, N., Sanderson, M. I., Intrator, N., and Simmons, J. A. (2003). Time-frequency model for echo-delay resolution in wideband biosonar. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113, 2137–2145. doi: 10.1121/1.1554693

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saillant, P. A., Simmons, J. A., Dear, S. P., and McMullen, T. A. (1993). A computational model of echo processing and acoustic imaging in frequency-modulated echolocating bats: the spectrogram correlation and transformation receiver. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 94, 2691–2712. doi: 10.1121/1.407353

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A. (1979). Perception of echo phase information in bat sonar. Science 207, 1336–1338. doi: 10.1126/science.451543

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A., Ferragamo, M. J., Moss, C. F., Stevenson, S. B., and Altes, R. A. (1990). Discrimination of jittered sonar echoes by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus: the shape of target images in echolocation. J. Comp. Physiol. A 167, 589–616. doi: 10.1007/BF00192654

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Simmons, J. A., Ferragamo, M. J., Saillant, P. A., Haresign, T., Wotton, J. M., Dear, S. P., et al. (1995). “Auditory dimensions of acoustic images in echolocation,” in Hearing by Bats, eds A. N. Popper and R. R. Fay (New York, NY: Springer), 146–190.

Simmons, J. A., Neretti, N., Intrator, N., Altes, R. A., Ferragamo, M. J., and Sanderson, M. I. (2004). Delay accuracy in bat sonar is related to the reciprocal of normalized echo bandwidth, or Q. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 3638–3643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308279101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Webster, F. A., and Griffin, D. R. (1962). The role of the flight membranes in insect capture by bats. Anim. Behav. 10, 332–340. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(62)90056-8

CrossRef Full Text

Wotton, J. M., Haresigan, T., and Simonns, J. A. (1995). Spatially dependent acoustic cues generated by the external ear of the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 98, 1423–1445. doi: 10.1121/1.413410

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: bat, echolocation, model, localization, linear period modulation

Citation: Matsuo I (2013) Echolocation of static and moving objects in two-dimensional space using bat-like frequency-modulation sound. Front. Physiol. 4:149. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00149

Received: 28 February 2013; Accepted: 04 June 2013;
Published online: 02 July 2013.

Edited by:

Cynthia F. Moss, University of Maryland, USA

Reviewed by:

Abhijit Patwardhan, University of Kentucky, USA
James A. Simmons, Brown University, USA

Copyright © 2013 Matsuo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Ikuo Matsuo, Neurosensing and Bionavigation Research Center, Doshisha University, Kyotanabe, Kyoto, Japan e-mail: matsuo@cs.tohoku-gakuin.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.