Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Physiol., 19 April 2013
Sec. Integrative Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Are neurocognitive problems in chronic fatigue syndrome caused by neurovascular deficits View all 6 articles

Neurocognitive impairment in childhood chronic fatigue syndrome

  • 1Pathophysiological and Health Science Team, RIKEN Center for Life Science Technologies, Kobe City, Hyogo, Japan
  • 2Department of Physiology, Osaka City University Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka, Japan
  • 3Department of Medical Science on Fatigue, Osaka City University Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka, Japan

Neurocognitive impairment is a feature of childhood chronic fatigue syndrome (CCFS). Several studies have demonstrated reduced attention control in CCFS patients in switching and divided attention tasks. In students, the extent of deterioration in task performance depends on the level of fatigue. Poor performance in switching and divided attention is common in both fatigued students and CCFS patients. Additionally, attentional functions show dramatic development from childhood to adolescence, suggesting that abnormal development of switching and divided attention may be induced by chronic fatigue. The brain structures associated with attentional control are situated in the frontal and parietal cortices, which are the last to mature, suggesting that severe fatigue in CCFS patients and students may inhibit normal structural and functional development in these regions. A combination of treatment with cognitive behavioral therapy and antidepressant medication is effective to improve attentional control processing in CCFS patients. Studies identifying the features of neurocognitive impairment in CCFS have improved our current understanding of the neurophysiological mechanisms of CCFS.

Introduction

Myalgic encephalopathy (ME), also known as chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS), is a critical and relatively common condition in children and adolescents as well as in adults. ME/CFS is the largest single cause of long-term school absence in the UK (Dowsett and Colby, 1997), and affects between 0.1 and 2% of children under 18 years of age in the US. (Chalder et al., 2003; Rimes et al., 2007), and 2.6% of junior high and 5–10% of senior high school students suffer from childhood CFS (CCFS) in Japan (Miike et al., 2004).

For a diagnosis of CFS, most researchers use criteria that were developed by the International CFS Study Group (Fukuda et al., 1994) that was established for adults rather than children. In 2006, diagnostic criteria for children and adolescents with ME/CFS were developed by the International Association of CFS Pediatric Case Definition Working Group (Jason et al., 2006). The symptom categories included fatigue, post-exertional malaise, unrefreshing sleep or disturbance of sleep quantity, pain (myofascial, joint, abdominal, and/or head pain), two or more neurocognitive manifestations (impaired memory, difficulty focusing, difficulty finding the right word, absentmindedness, slowness of thought, difficulty recalling information, difficulty focusing on one thing at a time, trouble expressing thought, difficulty comprehending information, frequently losing train of thought, and/or new trouble with math or other educational subjects).

Among the cognitive impairments associated with CCFS, memory and concentration problems affect over 80% of patients (Miike et al., 2004). However, it is difficult to identify these impairments of CCFS using cognitive tests (Kawatani et al., 2011). Several studies have revealed that performance is reduced in attentional control tasks in CCFS patients (Tomoda et al., 2007; Haig-Ferguson et al., 2009; Kawatani et al., 2011). In this review, we discuss the neural mechanisms of cognitive impairments in CCFS from the standpoint of performance on cognitive tasks and neurophysiological responsiveness such as event-related potentials (ERP) of electroencephalogram and activations and neural circuits of brain regions using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). In addition, we discuss the effects of treatment on decreased cognitive function in CCFS patients.

Attentional Control Impairment

The number of reports related to cognitive dysfunction associated with CCFS using cognitive tests is limited (Haig-Ferguson et al., 2009; Kawatani et al., 2011). Haig-Ferguson et al. (2009) used a battery of 10 tests to measure processing speed, attention, immediate and delayed memory, working memory, and executive function in CCFS patients. They found that CCFS patients have problems with attention control, such as sustained attention, switching attention, divided attention, and auditory learning. We used a battery of 6 tests to measure motor skill, selective, switching and divided attention, and working memory of patients with CCFS (Kawatani et al., 2011). We demonstrated that performance is reduced in CCFS patients on switching attention using a modified Advanced Trail Making Test (mATMT), which is a visual search task (Kajimoto, 2008; Mizuno and Watanabe, 2008; Kawatani et al., 2011). Previously, we demonstrated that performance is impaired on a divided attention task, using the kana-pick out test (KPT), which is a dual verbal task (Kaneko, 1996; Tomoda et al., 2007). These studies indicate that attentional control is reduced in CCFS, including switching and dividing attention.

When students proceed to junior high school from elementary school, they encounter rapid environmental changes, which can cause a variety of behavioral and emotional problems (Spear, 2000). One example is the number of Japanese students with school-refusal, which is reported to be 7154 out of 1,201,134 (0.5%) in 6th-graders and 21,084 out of 1,177,557 (1.8%) in 7th-graders in 2010 (MEXT, 2010). Thus, the prevalence of CCFS included in school-refusal increases from elementary school to junior high school, indicating that the number of fatigued students increases from elementary school to junior high school (Mizuno et al., 2011a). Recent results demonstrated that performance on switching and divided attention on the mATMT and KPT, respectively, in fatigued students in junior high school was reduced relative to controls, similar to performance in CCFS patients (Mizuno et al., 2011a). Thus, decreased performance on switching and divided attention tasks appears to be a common characteristic of both fatigued students and CCFS patients.

Development of Attentional Control

Morphological analyses in children and adolescents have shown that brain maturation occurs at different rates in different brain regions. The primary sensory and motor areas are the first to mature, while the association areas, especially in the frontal and parietal regions, are the last to mature (Sowell et al., 1999; Gaillard et al., 2001). Along with structural changes in the brain, executive function (defined as the set of mental control processes that permit goal-directed behavior) develops dramatically between childhood and adolescence (Travis and Tecce, 1998). In the development of executive function, for example, age-related gain has been reported in inhibitory control (Van der Molen, 2000), working memory (Casey et al., 2000; Vuontela et al., 2003), task switching (Cepeda et al., 2000), adaptive problem solving (Chelune and Baer, 1986), and various other planning and problem solving tasks (Welsh et al., 1991). Executive function is also related to the control of attention (Cowan et al., 2005), an important element of information processing that is embodied in the central executive component in theoretical conceptions of working memory (Cowan, 1988). Attentional competency develops steadily through early and late childhood, perhaps due in part to the development of core processing resources (Bisanz et al., 1979).

For switching and divided attention, we demonstrated that performance on motor processing tasks of students improved by grade in elementary school. However, performance did not change from elementary school to junior high school (Mizuno et al., 2011b). In contrast, performance of students on working memory, switching and divided attention tasks improved from elementary to junior high school (Mizuno et al., 2011b). In junior high school, performance on divided attention tasks improved by grade (Mizuno et al., 2011b). Taken together, these results suggest that fatigue influences the age-dependent development of attention control in students and CCFS patients.

Brain Mapping for Attention Control

Neuroimaging studies using fMRI have revealed the neural structures associated with switching attention (Zakzanis et al., 2005) and divided attention (Mizuno et al., 2012a). Task E in the mATMT was reported to measure the ability to switch attention while performing task B of the trail making test (Reitan, 1955). It was observed to be comparable to task E, activation in the left prefrontal cortex, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (Zakzanis et al., 2005). Processing of divided attention in the KPT requires activation of the left frontal and parietal cortices, including the dorsal inferior frontal gyrus and superior parietal lobule, in young adults (Mizuno et al., 2012a). In addition, we observed increased synchronization between these brain regions during the KPT, suggesting that effective communication between them may be induced, which may contribute to more attentional processing (Mizuno et al., 2012a). Clinical studies have validated the use of the most extensively studied cognitive ERP component, the P300, as an indicator of perceptual, attentional, and short-term memory processes on an oddball task (Sutton et al., 1965). In CCFS patients, we found that prolonged P300 latency during a visual oddball task is related to a decrease in performance on the KPT (Tomoda et al., 2007). These results suggest that there is a delay of neural processing and/or lower synchronization between brain regions that are associated with decreased performance during divided attention. Since the frontal and parietal cortices are the last to mature from childhood to adulthood (Sowell et al., 1999; Gaillard et al., 2001), it is possible that severe fatigue in students and CCFS patients inhibits structural and functional development in these regions. In adults with CFS, a decrease in divided attention (Ross et al., 2001) and a reduction of gray matter volumes in the lateral prefrontal cortex have been observed (Okada et al., 2004; de Lange et al., 2008), which support this possibility.

Treatment Effects on Attention Control

No clearly effective therapy is available for CCFS patients (Miike et al., 2004), but data from a brain morphometry study has suggested that cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), which combines a rehabilitative approach of a graded increase in physical activity with a psychological approach, may be beneficial (de Lange et al., 2008). However, CBT has been described as ineffective, not evidence-based, and potentially harmful for CFS treatment (Twisk and Maes, 2009). Many pharmacological therapies have been used for treating CFS including anti-depressants, non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, anxiolytic drugs, anti-allergy drugs, anti-hypotensive drugs, and antidepressants such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). However, such medications are not universally beneficial (Afari and Buchwald, 2003). A recent randomized controlled study in youths with depression who had not improved during a SSRI trial showed that cognitive performance can be moderated by combined treatment with CBT and antidepressant medication, suggesting that CBT combined with medication may be more advantageous for depressive adolescents than medication or CBT alone (Asarnow et al., 2009). Recently, we investigated the effects of combination therapy with CBT and antidepressant medication on the severity of mental fatigue, which was evaluated by Chalder's fatigue scale (Chalder et al., 1993; Tanaka et al., 2008), and cognitive performance in CCFS patients (Kawatani et al., 2011). This combination therapy decreased the severity of mental fatigue (Figure 1A) and improved performance on switching attention tasks and on the mATMT (Figure 1B). The association between these changes in fatigue severity and task performance by treatment are shown in Figure 1C. These results suggest that combined treatment with CBT and medication is effective for improving poor attention characteristics associated with CCFS.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Effects of combination treatments of cognitive behavioral therapy and antidepressant medication (fluvoxamine) for 6 months on (A) the severity of mental fatigue and (B) task performance on a attention switching task in patients with childhood chronic fatigue syndrome (CCFS). (C) Correlation between changes in mental fatigue score and reaction time from before to after the treatment in CCFS patients. HC, healthy control. Values are means and error bars indicate SD. Statistically significant, *p < 0.05.

Summary and Future Study

Several studies have shown that CCFS patients have poor performance on attention control tasks such as attention switching, and divided attention on the mATMT and KPT, respectively. In students, performances on switching and divided attention tasks are also reduced, the extent of which is dependent on the fatigue level. Therefore, deterioration on switching and divided attention tasks are a common characteristic feature in both fatigued students and CCFS patients. Additionally, these attentional functions are dramatically developed from childhood to adolescence, suggesting that inappropriate development of switching and divided attention is induced by fatigue. The neural structures associated with attention switching and divided attention are regions of the frontal and parietal cortices, which are the last to mature from childhood to adulthood, suggesting that severe fatigue of CCFS patients and students may inhibit structural and functional development in these regions. For CCFS patients, combined treatment with CBT and antidepressant medication is effective to improve attentional control processing.

In addition to neurocognitive impairments associated with CCFS, impairments of autonomic function include decreased parasympathetic activity and increased relative sympathetic activity (Tomoda et al., 2007). These symptoms also occur in adults with CFS (Wyller et al., 2007; Burton et al., 2010). Sympathoexcitatory subcortical threat circuits are normally under the inhibitory control of the prefrontal cortex (Amat et al., 2005; Thayer, 2006; Thayer and Sternberg, 2006). Previous studies showed the decreased regional cerebral blood flow of the frontal, temporal, and occipital cortices in CCFS patients using N-isopropyl-p-[123I]iodoamphetamine single photon emission computed tomography and xenon-computed tomography (Tomoda et al., 1995, 2000). A neurochemical investigation of the choline concentration of the frontal white matter in patients with CCFS demonstrated an increase, as measured by magnetic resonance spectroscopy (Tomoda et al., 2000). These findings indicate abnormal cerebral blood flow, cholinergic function, and autonomic function in patients with CCFS. However, the associations among decreases in attention control and these physiological and chemical functions of the brain are still unclear, and thus, further neuroimaging study is necessary for elucidating the neural and molecular mechanisms of cognitive impairments in CCFS.

In adult CFS patients, evidence such as lowered cerebral activity in the prefrontal cortex during fatigue-inducing tasks (Caseras et al., 2008) and a bilateral reduction of gray-matter volume in the prefrontal cortices (Okada et al., 2004; de Lange et al., 2008) suggest that individuals with CFS may exhibit anatomical and functional alterations in the prefrontal cortex. In childhood and adolescence, excessive stress, such as that due to verbal and sexual abuse, is associated with alterations of the brain structure (Tomoda et al., 2009, 2011). The influence of chronic fatigue on the brains of children and adolescents including CCFS patients remains unclear. Further prospective neuroimaging studies are necessary to identify the vulnerability of the lateral prefrontal cortices and other brain regions and interactions between attention control and brain structures in CCFS.

Monitoring of cognitive function in fatigued students and CCFS patients using the mATMT and KPT is useful for the evaluation of attention control. In addition, the evaluation of attention control might aid in the development of therapeutic strategies such as the CBT and medications for treating CCFS (Kawatani et al., 2011). Our recent work found that poor performance on divided attention tasks using the KPT in junior high school students also relates to poor lifestyle choices (skipping breakfast, spending too much time watching television) and family conditions (little time spent with family and little praise from family members) (Mizuno et al., 2012b). These results suggest that further studies should investigate the effects of not only CBT and medications, but also interventions of lifestyle and family condition in CCFS patients.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Japan Science and Technology Corporation (JST)/Research Institute of Science and Technology for Society (RISTEX) (07052628) and the RIKEN Special Postdoctoral Researchers Program (K24232). We thank Forte Science Communications for editorial assistance with the manuscript.

References

Afari, N., and Buchwald, D. (2003). Chronic fatigue syndrome: a review. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 221–236.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amat, J., Baratta, M. V., Paul, E., Bland, S. T., Watkins, L. R., and Maier, S. F. (2005). Medial prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 365–371.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Asarnow, J. R., Emslie, G., Clarke, G., Wagner, K. D., Spirito, A., Vitiello, B., et al. (2009). Treatment of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor-resistant depression in adolescents: predictors and moderators of treatment response. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 48, 330–339.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bisanz, J., Danner, F., and Resnick, L. B. (1979). Changes with age in measures of processing efficiency. Child Dev. 50, 132–141.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Burton, A. R., Rahman, K., Kadota, Y., Lloyd, A., and Vollmer-Conna, U. (2010). Reduced heart rate variability predicts poor sleep quality in a case–control study of chronic fatigue syndrome. Exp. Brain Res. 204, 71–78.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Caseras, X., Mataix-Cols, D., Rimes, K. A., Giampietro, V., Brammer, M., Zelaya, F., et al. (2008). The neural correlates of fatigue: an exploratory imaginal fatigue provocation study in chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychol. Med. 38, 941–951.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., and Thomas, K. M. (2000). Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol. Psychol. 54, 241–257.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cepeda, N. J., Cepeda, M. L., and Kramer, A. F. (2000). Task switching and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 28, 213–226.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Chalder, T., Berelowitz, G., Pawlikowska, T., Watts, L., Wessely, S., Wright, D., et al. (1993). Development of a fatigue scale. J. Psychosom. Res. 37, 147–153.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Chalder, T., Goodman, R., Wessely, S., Hotopf, M., and Meltzer, H. (2003). Epidemiology of chronic fatigue syndrome and self reported myalgic encephalomyelitis in 5-15 year olds: cross sectional study. BMJ 327, 654–655.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chelune, G. J., and Baer, R. A. (1986). Developmental norms for the Wisconsin Card Sorting test. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 8, 219–228.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information- processing system. Psychol. Bull. 104, 163–191.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cowan, N., Elliott, E. M., Scott Saults, J., Morey, C. C., Mattox, S., Hismjatullina, A., et al. (2005). On the capacity of attention: its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cogn. Psychol. 51, 42–100.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Lange, F. P., Koers, A., Kalkman, J. S., Bleijenberg, G., Hagoort, P., van der Meer, J. W., et al. (2008). Increase in prefrontal cortical volume following cognitive behavioural therapy in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Brain 131, 2172–2180.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dowsett, E. J., and Colby, J. (1997). Long-term sickness absence due to ME/CFS in UK schools: an epidemiological study with medical and educational implications. J. Chronic Fatigue Syndr. 3, 29–42.

Fukuda, K., Straus, S. E., Hickie, I., Sharpe, M. C., Dobbins, J. G., and Komaroff, A. (1994). The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach to its definition and study. International Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group. Ann. Intern. Med. 121, 953–959.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gaillard, W. D., Grandin, C. B., and Xu, B. (2001). Developmental aspects of pediatric fMRI: considerations for image acquisition, analysis, and interpretation. Neuroimage 13, 239–249.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haig-Ferguson, A., Tucker, P., Eaton, N., Hunt, L., and Crawley, E. (2009). Memory and attention problems in children with chronic fatigue syndrome or myalgic encephalopathy. Arch. Dis. Child. 94, 757–762.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jason, L. A., Bell, D. S., Rowe, K., Van Hoof, E. L. S., Jordan, K., Lapp, C., et al. (2006). A pediatric case definition for ME/CFS. J. Chronic Fatigue Syndr. 13, 1–44.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kajimoto, O. (2008). “Development of a method of evaluation of fatigue and its economic impacts,” in Fatigue Science for Human Health, eds Y. Watanabe, B. Evengård, B. H. Natelson, L. A. Jason, and H. Kuratsune (New York, NY: Springer), 33–46.

Kaneko, M. (1996). A neuropsychological index of aging—data from 9000 elderly people. Nihon Ronen Igakkai Zasshi 33, 811–815.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kawatani, J., Mizuno, K., Shiraishi, S., Takao, M., Joudoi, T., Fukuda, S., et al. (2011). Cognitive dysfunction and mental fatigue in childhood chronic fatigue syndrome–a 6-month follow-up study. Brain Dev. 33, 832–841.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MEXT (Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology-Japan). (2010). Statistics on Student Guidance.

Miike, T., Tomoda, A., Jhodoi, T., Iwatani, N., and Mabe, H. (2004). Learning and memorization impairment in childhood chronic fatigue syndrome manifesting as school phobia in Japan. Brain Dev. 26, 442–447.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mizuno, K., Tanaka, M., Fukuda, S., Imai-Matsumura, K., and Watanabe, Y. (2011a). Relationship between cognitive functions and prevalence of fatigue in elementary and junior high school students. Brain Dev. 33, 470–479.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mizuno, K., Tanaka, M., Fukuda, S., Sasabe, T., Imai-Matsumura, K., and Watanabe, Y. (2011b). Changes in cognitive functions of students in the transitional period from elementary school to junior high school. Brain Dev. 33, 412–420.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mizuno, K., Tanaka, M., Tanabe, H. C., Sadato, N., and Watanabe, Y. (2012a). The neural substrates associated with attentional resources and difficulty of concurrent processing of the two verbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 50, 1998–2009.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mizuno, K., Tanaka, M., Fukuda, S., Imai-Matsumura, K., and Watanabe, Y. (2012b). Divided attention of adolescents related to lifestyles and academic and family conditions. Brain Dev. 35, 435–440.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mizuno, K., and Watanabe, Y. (2008). “Utility of an advanced trail making test as a neuropsychological tool for an objective evaluation of work efficiency during mental fatigue,” in Fatigue Science for Human Health, eds Y. Watanabe, B. Evengård, B. H. Natelson, L. A. Jason, and H. Kuratsune (New York, NY: Springer), 47–54.

Okada, T., Tanaka, M., Kuratsune, H., Watanabe, Y., and Sadato, N. (2004). Mechanisms underlying fatigue: a voxel-based morphometric study of chronic fatigue syndrome. BMC Neurol. 4:14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-4-14

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reitan, R. M. (1955). The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J. Consult. Psychol. 19, 393–394.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rimes, K. A., Goodman, R., Hotopf, M., Wessely, S., Meltzer, H., and Chalder, T. (2007). Incidence, prognosis, and risk factors for fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome in adolescents: a prospective community study. Pediatrics 119, e603–e609.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ross, S., Fantie, B., Straus, S. F., and Grafman, J. (2001). Divided attention deficits in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Appl. Neuropsychol. 8, 4–11.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Jernigan, T. L., and Toga, A. W. (1999). In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 859–861.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 417–463.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J., and John, E. R. (1965). Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science 150, 1187–1188.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tanaka, M., Fukuda, S., Mizuno, K., Imai-Matsumura, K., Joudoi, T., Kawatani, J., et al. (2008). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Chalder Fatigue Scale among youth in Japan. Psychol. Rep. 103, 682–690.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thayer, J. F. (2006). On the importance of inhibition: central and peripheral manifestations of nonlinear inhibitory processes in neural systems. Dose Response 4, 2–21.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thayer, J. F., and Sternberg, E. (2006). Beyond heart rate variability: vagal regulation of allostatic systems. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1088, 361–372.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tomoda, A., Miike, T., Honda, T., Fukuda, K., Kai, Y., Nabeshima, M., et al. (1995). Single-photon emission computed tomography for cerebral blood flow in school phobias. Curr. Ther. Res. 56, 1088–1093.

Tomoda, A., Miike, T., Yamada, E., Honda, H., Moroi, T., Ogawa, M., et al. (2000). Chronic fatigue syndrome in childhood. Brain Dev. 22, 60–64.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tomoda, A., Mizuno, K., Murayama, N., Joudoi, T., Igasaki, T., Miyazaki, M., et al. (2007). Event-related potentials in Japanese childhood chronic fatigue syndrome. J. Pediatr. Neurol. 5, 199–208.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tomoda, A., Navalta, C. P., Polcari, A., Sadato, N., and Teicher, M. H. (2009). Childhood sexual abuse is associated with reduced gray matter volume in visual cortex of young women. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 642–648.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tomoda, A., Sheu, Y. S., Rabi, K., Suzuki, H., Navalta, C. P., Polcari, A., et al. (2011). Exposure to parental verbal abuse is associated with increased gray matter volume in superior temporal gyrus. Neuroimage 54, S280–S286.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Travis, F., and Tecce, J. J. (1998). Effects of distracting stimuli on CNV amplitude and reaction time. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 31, 45–50.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Twisk, F. N., and Maes, M. (2009). A review on cognitive behavorial therapy (CBT) and graded exercise therapy (GET) in myalgic encephalomyelitis (ME)/chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS): CBT/GET is not only ineffective and not evidence-based, but also potentially harmful for many patients with ME/CFS. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 30, 284–299.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Van der Molen, M. W. (2000). Developmental changes in inhibitory processing: evidence from psychophysiological measures. Biol. Psychol. 54, 207–239.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vuontela, V., Steenari, M. R., Carlson, S., Koivisto, J., Fjallberg, M., and Aronen, E. T. (2003). Audiospatial and visuospatial working memory in 6-13 year old school children. Learn. Mem. 10, 74–81.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., and Groisser, D. B. (1991). A normative-developmental study of executive function: a window on prefrontal function in children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 7, 131–149.

Wyller, V. B., Saul, J. P., Amlie, J. P., and Thaulow, E. (2007). Sympathetic predominance of cardiovascular regulation during mild orthostatic stress in adolescents with chronic fatigue. Clin. Physiol. Funct. Imaging 27, 231–238.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zakzanis, K. K., Mraz, R., and Graham, S. J. (2005). An fMRI study of the Trail Making Test. Neuropsychologia 43, 1878–1886.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: childhood chronic fatigue syndrome, cognitive development, divided attention, event-related potential, functional magnetic resonance imaging, kana pick-out test, modified advanced trail making test, switching attention

Citation: Mizuno K and Watanabe Y (2013) Neurocognitive impairment in childhood chronic fatigue syndrome. Front. Physiol. 4:87. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00087

Received: 06 February 2013; Paper pending published: 17 March 2013;
Accepted: 03 April 2013; Published online: 19 April 2013.

Edited by:

Julian M. Stewart, New York Medical College, USA

Reviewed by:

Joseph Francis, Louisiana State University, USA
J. Thomas Cunningham, Univerity of North Texas Health Science Center, USA

Copyright © 2013 Mizuno and Watanabe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Kei Mizuno, Pathophysiological and Health Science Team, RIKEN Center for Life Science Technologies, 6-7-3 Minatojima-minamimachi, Chuo-ku, Kobe, Hyogo 650-0047, Japan. e-mail: keimizuno@riken.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.