Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pharmacol., 29 November 2021
Sec. Neuropharmacology

Growth Hormone Secretagogue Receptor 1A Antagonist JMV2959 Effectively Prevents Morphine Memory Reconsolidation and Relapse

Jing Zhao
Jing Zhao1*Xinyu DuXinyu Du2Mingzhu ChenMingzhu Chen2Shimin ZhuShimin Zhu2
  • 1Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai Jiao Tong University Affiliated Sixth People’s Hospital, Shanghai, China
  • 2Key Laboratory of Combinatorial Biosynthesis and Drug Discovery (Wuhan University), Ministry of Education, and Wuhan University School of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Wuhan, China

Relapse to drug seeking after prolonged abstinence is a major problem in the clinical treatment of drug addiction. The use of pharmacological interventions to disrupt established drug reward memories is a promising strategy for the treatment of drug addiction. A growth hormone secretagogue receptor 1 A antagonist, JMV2959, has been shown to reduce morphine-induced conditioned place preference (CPP) in rats within hours of intervention; thus, JMV2959 is a potential candidate for drug addiction treatment. However, the effect of JMV2959 on reconsolidation to disrupt drug seeking remains unknown. In this study, we assessed the effect of JMV2959 on morphine induced memory reconsolidation to inhibit drug seeking after drug withdrawal. Our results showed that the administration of JMV2959 (6 mg/kg) significantly reduced environmental cue induced CPP, which suggested a preventive effect of JMV2959 on morphine induced memory reconsolidation. Additionally, JMV2959 administration significantly altered the locomotor activity and food and water intake but did not significantly alter the natural reward preference. We concluded that JMV2959 may be an effective candidate to treat drug addiction.

Introduction

Drug addiction is a chronic relapsing brain disorder that is often caused by the persistent intake of morphine, cocaine, amphetamine, or fentanyl over a long period (Leshner, 1997; O'Brien and McLellan, 1996). These addictive drugs activate human neurological systems through neurotrophic factors to form drug reward memory. This newly learned memory can be transformed into stable memories via memory consolidation (Davis and Squire, 1984; McGaugh, 2000). When this stabilised drug memory is recalled or reactivated, it undergoes additional consolidation, known as reconsolidation. This process requires de novo protein synthesis mediated by receptors, signal transduction pathways, and proteins. The reconsolidated drug reward memory is labile and sensitive to certain disruptors, such as receptor antagonists, pharmacological techniques, and protein synthesis inhibitors (Sara, 2000; Nestler, 2001; Hyman et al., 2006; Tronson et al., 2006; Tronson and Taylor, 2007). Therefore, the disruption of drug reward reconsolidation has become a promising therapeutic strategy for the prevention of drug addiction. Protein synthesis inhibitors, such as anisomycin or cycloheximide, administered immediately after reactivation, can disrupt drug reward memory, which leads to the absence of addictive behaviour (Milekic et al., 2006; Valjent et al., 2006; Robinson and Franklin, 2007). However, during drug addiction treatment, relapse to drug seeking after prolonged abstinence is a major clinical problem (Grimm et al., 2001; Lu et al., 2005; Li et al., 2008a; Millan et al., 2013). Thus, new effective treatment strategies are of urgent need.

Ghrelin, a 28-amino acid peptide hormone endogenously expressed in the gut and brain tissues, plays a crucial role in food and addictive drug rewards by binding to and activating the growth hormone secretagogue receptor (GHSR1A) (Wu et al., 2019; Abizaid and Hougland, 2020). The activation of the ghrelin-GHSR1A signalling pathway is critical to adjust appetite and food intake in the gut (Kern et al., 2015). The ghrelin and GHSR1A expressed in areas of the brain, such as the hypothalamus, striatum, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and ventral tegmental area, have been reported to play a crucial role in drug intake and reward (Howard et al., 1996; Abizaid et al., 2006; Ferrini et al., 2009; Landgren et al., 2011; Skibicka et al., 2011).

Recently, GHSR1A antagonism has been adopted as a promising anti-addiction mechanism. Studies have demonstrated that the administration of GHSR1A antagonist JMV2959 significantly inhibits fentanyl‐ and methamphetamine-induced conditioned place preference (CPP), intravenous self-administration, and dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens in rats (Engel et al., 2015; Jerabek et al., 2017; Havlickova et al., 2018; Sustkova-Fiserova et al., 2019). However, the effect of JMV2959 on relapse to drug seeking remains unknown. In this study, we examined the effect of JMV2959 on the reconsolidation of drug reward memory and prevention of relapse to drug seeking in a morphine-induced CPP animal model. We also examined the effect of JMV2959 on natural reward preference and food and water intake.

Materials and Methods

Animals

Male Sprague–Dawley rats (weighing 220–250 g) were housed in a temperature (23 ± 2°C) and humidity (50 ± 5%) controlled animal facility. A total of 48 rats were randomized into experimental groups with free access to food and water (24 rats for CPP, 12 rats for natural reward preference, 12 rats for food, water intake and locomoter activity). Rats weighed 300–320 g when the experiments began. The experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Wuhan University Animal Care and Use Committee (Li et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2014).

Drugs

JMV2959 (1,2,4-triazole derivate, purity >99.5%) was chemically synthesised by Waterfall Biotechnologies, LLC, Shanghai, China. The compound was dissolved in 2% dimethyl sulphoxide at different concentrations for subcutaneous and intraperitoneal injections. Morphine sulphate (Qinghai Pharmaceutical Ltd., Xining, China) was prepared at different concentrations in 0.9% physiological saline for subcutaneous and intraperitoneal injection (Li et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2014). Sucrose (2%) solution was dissolved in tap water.

CPP: Effect of JMV2959 on Morphine Reward Memory Reconsolidation and Relapse to Morphine

CPP training was performed using an unbiased, counterbalanced protocol as described in the previous studies (Li et al., 2008a; Li et al., 2008b). Briefly, the CPP apparatus was assembled by five identical three-chamber polyvinyl chloride (PVC) boxes. In each box, two large side chambers (27.9 cm long, 21.0 cm wide and 20.9 cm high) were separated by a smaller one (12.1 cm long, 21.0 cm wide and 20.9 cm high with a smooth PVC floor). The two larger chambers differed in their floor texture (bar or grid) and provided distinct visual contexts that were paired with the drug or saline injection. Guillotine doors were manually installed to separate the three distinct chambers.

To determine the baseline preference, rats were initially placed in the middle chamber with the doors removed for 15 min (pre-conditioning test). A computer measured the time spent in the designated saline- or morphine-paired chambers during the 15 min session by the interruption of infrared beams by animals. The data (not shown) indicated that most rats spent approximately one-third of the time in each chamber. Approximately 5% of rats exhibited a strong unconditioned preference (540 s) and were excluded from the study.

Rats (n = 6 for each group) were assigned to one of the following treatments: 1. saline CPP + saline (with 2% dimethyl sulphoxide) (S + S); 2. saline CPP + JMV2959 (S + J); 3. morphine CPP + saline (with 2% dimethyl sulphoxide) (M + S); 4. morphine CPP + JMV2959 (S + J). On the conditioning days, each rat was trained for eight consecutive days with alternate injections of morphine (10 mg/kg, s.c., on day 2, 4, 6, and 8) or vehicle (morphine control, 1 ml/kg, s.c., on day 3, 5, 7, and 9). After each injection, rats were confined to the corresponding conditioning chambers for 45 min before returning to their home cages. The day after conditioning (day 10), rats were tested for CPP (post-conditioning test) under conditions identical to those described in the pre-conditioning test. The CPP score was defined as the time spent, in seconds, in the morphine-paired chamber minus that spent in the morphine-unpaired (saline-paired) chamber (Lin et al., 2014).

Drug-Memory Reactivation

Twenty-four hours later (day 11), rats were exposed to the morphine-paired chamber for 10 min immediately followed by JMV2959 (3, 6 mg/kg,i.p) or vehicle administration. Two groups of rats (n = 6 per group) were used.

Retesting and Priming of Morphine CPP

The drug-induced CPP was retested 1 day (post-treatment 1, Post-T1) or 7 days (post-treatment 7, Post-T7) after memory reactivation (day 12 and 18, respectively). If rats did not show significant drug CPP, they would be injected with a priming drug injection (3 mg/kg morphine, s.c.) and immediately submitted to the CPP procedures again (post-treatment 8, Post-T8).

Locomotor Activity

The behavioural locomotor activity test was performed based on the previous studies (Li et al., 2008a; Li et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2014). Briefly, the rats were placed in a photocell cage for 1 h according to the behavioural testing procedure. Then, they were injected with vehicle or JMV2959 (6 mg/kg JMV2959, i.p., respectively) and immediately placed in the chamber. Ambulation behaviour was measured for 60 min. After each trial, the chamber was carefully cleaned.

Natural Reward Preference and Food and Water Intake

To test the effect of JMV2959 on natural reward preference, the two-bottle sucrose intake test was performed as described in the previous studies (Li et al., 2008a; Li et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2014). Rats were housed singly during the testing. The procedure included: 1) Adaptation: On day 1, a bottle filled with tap water and a bottle filled with 2% sucrose solution were placed in symmetric positions on the same cage wall at 18:00 h. On day 2, the placements of the water and sucrose bottles were switched to balance side preference; 2) Grouping: The rats were divided into two groups (JMV2959 0 or 6 mg/kg) fairly according to their weight; 3) Testing: Rats were administered JMV2959 injections (0 or 6 mg/kg) on day 3 in the morning. The results were measured 24 h later.

The rats.were provided with a known quantity of water in the nostled drinking bottle, which was replenished every morning after recording daily consumption. For food intake assay, a known quantity of food were provided and intake was calculated by the quantity difference after daily consumption. The animals were weighed and their weight was recorded in grams.

Statistics

Data are expressed as mean ± S.E.M. The between-factor differences for JMV2959 treatment doses (0, 3, 6 mg/kg) and the within-factor differences for the test condition (pre- and post-conditioning and post-treatment) were analysed using a two-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). For locomotor activity, a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was used to analyse the differences in crossings. The rest of the experiments were analysed with a two-way ANOVA. Behavioural data were analysed using SPSS 15.0. Differences were statistically significant when p < 0.05.

Results

JMV2959 Disrupts the Reconsolidation of Morphine Reward Memory

In experiment 1, a three-way ANOVA conducted on CPP score using exposure (exposure to drug-paired or no exposure) and JMV2959 (0, 3 or 6 mg/kg) as the between-subjects factors and test condition (pre-conditioning, post-conditioning and post-treatment 1 or 7) as the within-subjects factor, revealed that there was a signifificant interaction between exposure × JMV2959 × test condition(F1,287 = 18.19; p < 0.01) and exposure ×JMV2959(F(3,189) = 45.43; p < 0.01) and exposure × JMV2959(F(3,189) = 34.23; p < 0.01). Post-hoc analysis showed that after morphine CPP training, all groups acquired CPP (p < 0.001) and there were no differences in CPP scores between any two groups during post-conditioning. Compared with the post-conditioning test, the CPP score was signifificantly decreased only in the group of rats administrated a dose of 6 mg/kg but not 1 mg/kg JMV2959 after re-exposure to the previously morphinepaired chamber (p < 0.01) on the post-treatment day 1 or 7, as shown in (Figures 1A,B). Thus, the inhibitory effect of JMV2959 on reconsolidation of morphine reward memory was dependent on re-exposure to drug-paired context and memory reactivation in a dose-dependent way (Figure 1B). And this absence of CPP was reinstated by a morphine priming injection (one-way repeated measures ANOVA showed F1,168 = 65.69; p < 0.05, compared to post-treatment condition) (Figure 1C).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The effect of JMV2959 for rats on morphine reconsolidation. (A) Timeline of the Experimental procedure (B) Systematic administration of JMV2959 immediately after exposure to morphine-paired context impaired the reconsolidation of morphine reward memory. The inhibitory effect of JMV2959 on the expression of morphine CPP last for 7d. When given 3 mg/kg morphine priming injection, rats in the group with 6 mg/kg JMV2959 treatment immediately after exposure reinstated the morphine CPP (n = 6-8 per group). *p < 0.01 vs. preconditioning or post-conditioning within group. #p < 0.01 vs. preconditioning or post-treatment within group. (C) Systemic administration of JMV2959 without exposure to morphine reward memory. Pre-C, preconditioning; Post-C, post-conditioning; Post-T1, post-treatment; Post-T14, post-treatment 14; Priming, injected by 3 mg/kg morphine.

JMV2959 has an Effect on Locomotor Activity

Locomotor activity was detected to explore whether JMV2959 affects rat locomotion, which could influence the CPP score. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA analyse indicated that there was significant difference between the group exposed to JMV2959 (6 mg/kg) and the saline group(ANOVA, F(1,15) = 45.46, p < 0.01), which indicated that JMV2959 was able to influence the locomotor activity of the rats and that the CPP score was valid (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. The effect of JMV2959 for rats on locomotor activity. The rats were placed in a photocell cage for 1 h according to the behavioural testing procedure. Then they were injected with vehicle or JMV2959 (0 or 6 mg/kg JMV2959, i.p., respectively) and immediately placed in the chamber. Ambulation behaviour was measured for 60 min. Values are presented as the mean ± SEM (n = 6) *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

JMV2959 May Have Weak Effect on Natural Reward Preference

The two-bottle sucrose intake test was conducted to determine the effect of JMV2959 on natural reward preference. There was no significant difference between the rats with exposure to JMV2959 and those without(F(1,39) = 0.54, p > 0.05). However, the trend suggests that JMV2959 may have weak influence on natural reward preference and there could be a significant difference should larger sample number be included (Figure 3A). Water intake was recorded at the same time and the JMV2959-injected rats showed significantly less water drinking on the second (p < 0.05) and third (p < 0.01) days than the non-JMV2959-injected rats (Figure 3B).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Effect of JMV2959 on natural reward preference. The two-bottle sucrose intake test was performed as described in methods. The difference between Saline group (n = 6) and the group with JMV2959 (6 mg/kg, n = 6) were measured by (A) 2% sucrose and (B) tap water intaking up to 3 days. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA was taken. Values are presented as the mean ± SEM (n = 4). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

JMV2959 Causes Less Food Intake and Weight Loss

Weight alteration and food intake were recorded because JMV2959 is a GHSR1A antagonist that plays a crucial role in food intake. The JMV2959-injected rats showed significantly less food intake than the saline group on the firstand second days (ANOVA, F(1,39) = 34.3, p < 0.05) (Figure 4A). The weight alteration in the JMV2959-injected group was significantly less than that in the saline group on the first day (ANOVA, F(1,39) = 59.2, p < 0.01) (Figure 4B).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Effect of JMV2959 on food intake and body weight. Weight alteration and food intake test were measured as described in methods. The difference between saline group (n = 6) and the group with JMV2959 (6 mg/kg, n = 6) were recorded by (A) food intaking and (B) body weight in 3 days. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA was taken. Values are presented as the mean ± SEM (n = 4).*p < 0.05,**p < 0.01.

Discussion

Drug addiction is a life-threatening disease and huge medical burden; however, the prevention of drug abuse is a largely unmet need. In this study, we showed the preventive effect of the GHSR1A antagonist JMV2959 on morphine memory reconsolidation and relapse to morphine over a long period. Our results are consistent with those of a previous study that suggested the inhibitory effect of JMV2959 on morphine-induced CPP(Engel et al., 2015; Jerabek et al., 2017). We also showed that the systemic administration of JMV2959 was long-lasting and effective, which suggests a crucial role of the ghrelin-GHSR1A pathway in the regulation of drug addiction. Other studies have reported the inhibitory effect of JMV2959 on addictive drug reward memory in other drugs, such as methamphetamine, fentanyl, and cocaine (Engel et al., 2015; Jerabek et al., 2017; Havlickova et al., 2018; Sustkova-Fiserova et al., 2019).

Drug seeking is a major problem in the treatment of drug addiction that is often caused by maladaptive drug-related memory (Lin et al., 2014). The prevention of relapse is a huge challenge in clinical practice owing to drug-seeking behaviours induced by drug-associated environmental cues (Li et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2014). The disruption of drug memory reconsolidation has been suggested as a promising strategy to prevent relapse (Lin et al., 2014). A number of studies have demonstrated that ghrelin and its specific receptor GHSR1A are involved in the mediation of memory reward-related neurological processes (Havlickova et al., 2018; Sustkova-Fiserova et al., 2019). Thus, targeting these neuronal processes could disrupt memories underlying addiction behaviour (Engel et al., 2015; Jerabek et al., 2017). In this study, we employed JMV2959 to antagonise GHSR1A-mediated processes to disrupt reconsolidation to prevent drug seeking.

Our result presented an effective method to prevent drug seeking by the systemic administration of JMV2959, which has also been suggested by Engel group and Jerabek group, etc (Engel et al., 2015; Jerabek et al., 2017). Other studies have reported that rapamycin may be effective in rats (Lin et al., 2014). As rapamycin inhibits the mammalian target of rapamycin signaling pathway to disrupt protein synthesis that is dependent on long-term synaptic plasticity and memory storage (Casadio et al., 1999; Stoica et al., 2011), a ghrelin-GHSR1A antagonism mechanism could be a promising strategy to prevent drug addiction. Ghrelin-GHSR1A signaling has been reported to play a crucial role in drug intake and reward (Ge et al., 2018). Thus, it would be useful to design and synthesise more potent antagonists than JMV2959 to prevent drug addiction.

Ghrelin exerts a complex spectrum of effects on systemic metabolism, such as the stimulation of gut motility and gastric acid secretion, regulation of glucose metabolism, inhibition of insulin secretion, and increase of adiposity. Furthermore, it has effects on drug addiction and manipulates sleep, stress, and anxiety. In this study, JMV2959 significantly decreased water and food intake, which suggests that ghrelin-GHSR1A antagonism may also play a role in these behavioural differences.

Conclusion

In this study, we elucidated that JMV2959 was able to inhibit morphine memory reconsolidation and relapse to drug seeking, suggesting that JMV2959 may be an effective candidate to treat drug addiction. Secondly, JMV2959 administration significantly affected the food and water consumption. Thirdly, JMV2959 may have an effect on altering the locomotor activity or natural reward preference.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by Wuhan University Animal Care and Use Committee.

Author Contributions

JZ was responsible for designing and running the experiments and writing the manuscript. XD, MC, and SZ were responsible for animal care and experiments. JZ was responsible for the conception, design, and coordination of the study, acquisition and interpretation of the statistical data, and revision of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Shanghai Municipal Planning Commission of Science and Research Fund (grant number 2015ZB0103).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Yanqin Li from the Key Laboratory of Combinatorial Biosynthesis and Drug Discovery (Wuhan University), Ministry of Education, and Wuhan University School of Pharmaceutical Sciences for her excellent help. We thank Dr. Wei Jiang from the Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai Jiao Tong University Affiliated Sixth People's Hospital for his great support.

Abbreviations

CPP, conditioned place preference; GHSR1A, growth hormone secretagogue receptor.

References

Abizaid, A., and Hougland, J. L. (2020). Ghrelin Signaling: GOAT and GHS-R1a Take a LEAP in Complexity. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 31, 107–117. doi:10.1016/j.tem.2019.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abizaid, A., Liu, Z. W., Andrews, Z. B., Shanabrough, M., Borok, E., Elsworth, J. D., et al. (2006). Ghrelin Modulates the Activity and Synaptic Input Organization of Midbrain Dopamine Neurons while Promoting Appetite. J. Clin. Invest. 116, 3229–3239. doi:10.1172/JCI29867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casadio, A., Martin, K. C., Giustetto, M., Zhu, H., Chen, M., Bartsch, D., et al. (1999). A Transient, Neuron-wide Form of CREB-Mediated Long-Term Facilitation Can Be Stabilized at Specific Synapses by Local Protein Synthesis. Cell 99, 221–237. doi:10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81653-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, H. P., and Squire, L. R. (1984). Protein Synthesis and Memory: a Review. Psychol. Bull. 96, 518–559. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.96.3.518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, J. A., Nylander, I., and Jerlhag, E. (2015). A Ghrelin Receptor (GHS-R1a) Antagonist Attenuates the Rewarding Properties of Morphine and Increases Opioid Peptide Levels in Reward Areas in Mice. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25 (12), 2364–2371. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrini, F., Salio, C., Lossi, L., and Merighi, A. (2009). Ghrelin in central Neurons. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 7, 37–49. doi:10.2174/157015909787602779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ge, X., Yang, H., Bednarek, M. A., Galon-Tilleman, H., Chen, P., Chen, M., et al. (2018). LEAP2 Is an Endogenous Antagonist of the Ghrelin Receptor. Cell Metab 27, 461–e6. doi:10.1016/j.cmet.2017.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimm, J. W., Hope, B. T., Wise, R. A., and Shaham, Y. (2001). Neuroadaptation. Incubation of Cocaine Craving after Withdrawal. Nature 412, 141–142. doi:10.1038/35084134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Havlickova, T., Charalambous, C., Lapka, M., Puskina, N., Jerabek, P., and Sustkova-Fiserova, M. (2018). Ghrelin Receptor Antagonism of Methamphetamine-Induced Conditioned Place Preference and Intravenous Self-Administration in Rats. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 19, 2925. doi:10.3390/ijms19102925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, A. D., Feighner, S. D., Cully, D. F., Arena, J. P., Liberator, P. A., Rosenblum, C. I., et al. (1996). A Receptor in Pituitary and Hypothalamus that Functions in Growth Hormone Release. Science 273, 974–977. doi:10.1126/science.273.5277.974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyman, S. E., Malenka, R. C., and Nestler, E. J. (2006). Neural Mechanisms of Addiction: the Role of Reward-Related Learning and Memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 565–598. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jerabek, P., Havlickova, T., Puskina, N., Charalambous, C., Lapka, M., Kacer, P., et al. (2017). Ghrelin Receptor Antagonism of Morphine-Induced Conditioned Place Preference and Behavioral and Accumbens Dopaminergic Sensitization in Rats. Neurochem. Int. 110, 101–113. doi:10.1016/j.neuint.2017.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kern, A., Mavrikaki, M., Ullrich, C., Albarran-Zeckler, R., Brantley, A. F., and Smith, R. G. (2015). Hippocampal Dopamine/DRD1 Signaling Dependent on the Ghrelin Receptor. Cell 163, 1176–1190. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2015.10.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landgren, S., Engel, J. A., Hyytiä, P., Zetterberg, H., Blennow, K., and Jerlhag, E. (2011). Expression of the Gene Encoding the Ghrelin Receptor in Rats Selected for Differential Alcohol Preference. Behav. Brain Res. 221, 182–188. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leshner, A. I. (1997). Addiction Is a Brain Disease, and it Matters. Science 278, 45–47. doi:10.1126/science.278.5335.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y. Q., Li, F. Q., Wang, X. Y., Wu, P., Zhao, M., Xu, C. M., et al. (2008a). Central Amygdala Extracellular Signal-Regulated Kinase Signaling Pathway Is Critical to Incubation of Opiate Craving. J. Neurosci. 28, 13248–13257. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3027-08.2008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y. Q., Wang, X. Y., Zhai, H. F., Zheng, Y. Q., Zhang, X. Y., Kosten, T., et al. (2008b). Effects of Early Postnatal Sibling Deprivation on Anxiety and Vulnerability to Cocaine in Offspring Rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 199, 245–253. doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1169-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y. Q., Xue, Y. X., He, Y. Y., Li, F. Q., Xue, L. F., Xu, C. M., et al. (2011). Inhibition of PKMzeta in Nucleus Accumbens Core Abolishes Long-Term Drug Reward Memory. J. Neurosci. 31, 5436–5446. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5884-10.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, J., Liu, L., Wen, Q., Zheng, C., Gao, Y., Peng, S., et al. (2014). Rapamycin Prevents Drug Seeking via Disrupting Reconsolidation of Reward Memory in Rats. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 17, 127–136. doi:10.1017/S1461145713001156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, L., Hope, B. T., Dempsey, J., Liu, S. Y., Bossert, J. M., and Shaham, Y. (2005). Central Amygdala ERK Signaling Pathway Is Critical to Incubation of Cocaine Craving. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 212–219. doi:10.1038/nn1383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory--A Century of Consolidation. Science 287, 248–251. doi:10.1126/science.287.5451.248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milekic, M. H., Brown, S. D., Castellini, C., and Alberini, C. M. (2006). Persistent Disruption of an Established Morphine Conditioned Place Preference. J. Neurosci. 26, 3010–3020. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4818-05.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millan, E. Z., Milligan-Saville, J., and McNally, G. P. (2013). Memory Retrieval, Extinction, and Reinstatement of Alcohol Seeking. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 101, 26–32. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2012.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nestler, E. J. (2001). Neurobiology. Total Recall-The Memory of Addiction. Science 292, 2266–2267. doi:10.1126/science.1063024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Brien, C. P., and McLellan, A. T. (1996). Myths about the Treatment of Addiction. Lancet 347, 237–240. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(96)90409-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, M. J., and Franklin, K. B. (2007). Effects of Anisomycin on Consolidation and Reconsolidation of a Morphine-Conditioned Place Preference. Behav. Brain Res. 178, 146–153. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2006.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sara, S. J. (2000). Retrieval and Reconsolidation: Toward a Neurobiology of Remembering. Learn. Mem. 7, 73–84. doi:10.1101/lm.7.2.73

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skibicka, K. P., Hansson, C., Alvarez-Crespo, M., Friberg, P. A., and Dickson, S. L. (2011). Ghrelin Directly Targets the Ventral Tegmental Area to Increase Food Motivation. Neuroscience 180, 129–137. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoica, L., Zhu, P. J., Huang, W., Zhou, H., Kozma, S. C., and Costa-Mattioli, M. (2011). Selective Pharmacogenetic Inhibition of Mammalian Target of Rapamycin Complex I (mTORC1) Blocks Long-Term Synaptic Plasticity and Memory Storage. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 108, 3791–3796. doi:10.1073/pnas.1014715108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sustkova-Fiserova, M., Puskina, N., Havlickova, T., Lapka, M., Syslova, K., Pohorala, V., et al. (2019). Ghrelin Receptor Antagonism of Fentanyl-Induced Conditioned Place Preference, Intravenous Self-Administration, and Dopamine Release in the Nucleus Accumbens in Rats. Addict. Biol. 25 e12845. doi:10.1111/adb.12845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronson, N. C., and Taylor, J. R. (2007). Molecular Mechanisms of Memory Reconsolidation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 262–275. doi:10.1038/nrn2090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronson, N. C., Wiseman, S. L., Olausson, P., and Taylor, J. R. (2006). Bidirectional Behavioral Plasticity of Memory Reconsolidation Depends on Amygdalar Protein Kinase A. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 167–169. doi:10.1038/nn1628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valjent, E., Corbillé, A. G., Bertran-Gonzalez, J., Hervé, D., and Girault, J. A. (2006). Inhibition of ERK Pathway or Protein Synthesis during Reexposure to Drugs of Abuse Erases Previously Learned Place Preference. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 103, 2932–2937. doi:10.1073/pnas.0511030103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, B., Liu, Y., Liu, F., Deng, Q., Wang, J., Han, R., et al. (2019). The Antinociceptive Effects and Molecular Mechanisms of ghrelin(1-7)-NH2 at the Supraspinal Level in Acute Pain in Mice. Brain Res. Bull. 146, 112–123. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2018.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: conditioned place preference, morphine relapse, addiction, ghrelin antagonism, JMV2959

Citation: Zhao J, Du X, Chen M and Zhu S (2021) Growth Hormone Secretagogue Receptor 1A Antagonist JMV2959 Effectively Prevents Morphine Memory Reconsolidation and Relapse. Front. Pharmacol. 12:718615. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2021.718615

Received: 02 June 2021; Accepted: 22 October 2021;
Published: 29 November 2021.

Edited by:

Giacinto Bagetta, University of Calabria, Italy

Reviewed by:

Jianfeng Liu, Texas A&M University, United States
Laura Rombolà, University of Calabria, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Zhao, Du, Chen and Zhu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jing Zhao, zhaojing@shsmu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.